Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2003, 66, 893–902

doi:10.1006/anbe.2003.2250

The energy cost of song in the canary, Serinus canaria

SALLY WARD*, JOHN R. SPEAKMAN† & PETER J. B. SLATER*


*School of Biology, University of St Andrews
†School of Biological Sciences, University of Aberdeen and Rowett Research Institute

(Received 20 November 2002; initial acceptance 6 January 2003;


final acceptance 26 February 2003; MS. number: 7535)

Although sound production requires energy, it has been unclear how much singing increases metabolic
rate in passerine birds. We measured the rate of oxygen consumption of two breeds of canary that sang
inside a respirometry chamber. Metabolic rate increased with the proportion of time that birds spent
singing. Average metabolic rate during singing at 15–20C was 1.05–1.07 times that of standing quietly in
the same temperature range or 2.2–2.6 times basal metabolic rate (BMR). Whether an increase in
metabolic rate during song of this order would represent a fitness cost to free-living passerine birds would
depend upon the circumstances. Singing rather than perching during the day would raise metabolic rate
only slightly. Singing at night or at dawn, instead of sleeping with a metabolic rate closer to BMR, would
cause a greater increase in metabolism. Birdsong could act as a condition-dependent signal, since birds
that are easily able to achieve energy balance could afford the cost of singing, but those close to their
energy limits might not.
 2003 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Birdsong is a conspicuous example of a display that 2001). Field observations of birds imply that singing has
can function both to deter rivals and to attract mates an energy cost. Birds sing more when in good body
(Catchpole & Slater 1995). The possible costs of birdsong condition or following supplementary feeding and less
are not well understood. Most models of animal signal- after cold nights or following adverse experimental treat-
ling assume that displays should be costly if they are to ments (Lambrechts 1996). However, these data do not
carry honest information about the quality of the signal- show whether singing itself is energetically costly or
ler (Zahavi 1975; Clutton-Brock & Albon 1979; Grafen whether time that could have been spent singing, at
1990a, b; Godfray 1991). Hypotheses on how animal however low a rate of energy expenditure, was better
displays may have evolved assume explicitly or implicitly spent foraging during periods of energy shortage.
that signals should be costly (Fisher 1930; Zahavi 1975; Measurements of the rate of oxygen consumption of
Grafen 1990a, b). The costliness of signals is also an captive animals show that sound production can cause
important assumption of stochastic dynamic program- large factorial increases in metabolism for insects,
ming models of animal signalling (e.g. Hutchinson et al. anurans and bats (Ryan 1988; Speakman et al. 1989;
1993). Prestwich 1994; Kotiaho et al. 1998). However, one can-
Song production obviously requires energy. Muscles not infer from these data how energetically costly singing
associated with control of respiration, the syrinx, the should be for passerine birds, because resting metabolic
upper vocal tract and movements of the bill are all used rates and sound production mechanisms differ between
during singing (Goller & Larsen 1997; Suthers et al. 1999; taxa. Singing inside respirometry chambers appears to be
Larsen & Goller 2002). Singing could also be costly in energetically costly in Carolina wrens, Thryothorus ludovi-
other ways: time spent singing is not available for other cianus (Eberhardt 1994), but not in zebra finches, Taen-
activities, singing might attract predators, and song learn- iopygia guttata, Waterslager canaries, Serinus canaria, or
ing and production involves specialized areas of the brain European starlings, Sturnus vulgaris (Oberweger & Goller
that could be costly to develop or maintain (Gil & Gahr 2001; Franz & Goller 2003). Singing by free-living com-
mon nightingales, Luscinia megarhynchos, increases their
Correspondence: S. Ward, School of Biology, Bute Medical Buildings, overnight rates of mass loss, implying that song has an
University of St Andrews, St Andrews, Fife KY16 9TS, U.K. (email:
energy cost in this species (Thomas 2002). In contrast,
sw29@st-andrews.ac.uk). J. R. Speakman is at the Aberdeen Centre for
Energy Regulation and Obesity, School of Biological Sciences, Zoology
begging by nestling birds is thought to be energetically
Building, Tillydrone Avenue, Aberdeen B24 2TZ, U.K. and Division of cheap (Chappell & Bachman 2002) as is crowing by
Appetite and Energy Balance, Rowett Research Institute, Greenburn cockerels, Gallus gallus domesticus, and junglefowl, G.
Road, Bucksburn, Aberdeen, AB21 9SB, U.K. gallus spadiceus (Chappell et al. 1995; Horn et al. 1995).
893
0003–3472/03/$30.00/0  2003 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
894 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 66, 5

Conclusions that bird vocalization is energetically singly (to prevent aggression between birds) in cages
costly (Eberhardt 1994) and that it is energetically cheap measuring 6030 cm and 35 cm high except during
(McCarty 1996) have both been questioned (Gaunt et al. breeding, when a pair of birds shared a double cage. The
1996; Verhulst & Wiersma 1997; Weathers et al. 1997). male birds could see and hear each other. The Fife
Whether singing is energetically costly or not therefore canaries could also see females. The diet was mixed seed
remains controversial, both because some of the empiri- (Super Canary for Fife canaries and Roller Mix for roller
cal data are contradictory and because it is unclear how canaries, Haiths, Cleethorpes, U.K.) fed ad libitum and
much energy expenditure must increase during an supplemented with broccoli, egg food (Sluis, Woodlea
activity before this will impose a fitness cost. Verhulst & Birds, Maldon, U.K.) mixed with hardboiled egg, soaked
Wiersma (1997) proposed that whether a particular seed (Easysoak, Haiths, Cleethorpes, U.K.) and vitamins
activity is energetically costly should best be considered (Daily Essentials, The Birdcare Company, Nailsworth,
by calculating its marginal cost, that is, how much of the U.K.). The birds had constant access to water and crushed
available energy that activity requires. A problem with oyster shell. Water baths were provided once a week. The
this approach, however, is that it is unclear how to photoperiod (daylight fluorescent strip lights) was
determine how much energy is available to an animal. 10:14 h light:dark in October, was increased evenly to
Previous studies of the energy cost of singing inside 16:8 h in January, remained constant until June and was
respirometry chambers have not included measurement decreased evenly to 10:14 h by October. Room air tem-
of a possible anaerobic component to metabolism during perature was 15–20C. The two breeds of canary were kept
vocalization (Weathers et al. 1997), for which the oxygen in separate rooms. Data were collected between December
debt would be repaid by increased aerobic metabolism and May. Bird husbandry and our experimental pro-
between songs. Nor has it been assessed whether birds cedures were approved by the Home Office, as was
sang as loudly inside respirometry chambers as when they returning the birds to local canary breeders at the end of
were not enclosed. It is important that song volume is not our work.
reduced inside respirometry chambers, since louder
sounds contain more energy. The energy cost of singing
Respirometry
was found to increase with song amplitude in a single
European starling that sang both quiet and loud songs We used as subjects the 12 male Fife canaries that sang
(Oberweger & Goller 2001). most frequently in their home cages, and all nine male
We measured the energy cost of singing by two breeds roller canaries. Each bird spent 30–90 min inside the
of canary (roller and Fife Fancy; body masses given in respirometry chamber during an experiment. The
Table 1) from the rate of oxygen consumption of birds respirometry chamber was placed in the room in which
singing inside a respirometry chamber. Each song bout the birds were normally kept. Food (broccoli and mixed
lasted several minutes and included both singing and seed) was provided inside the chamber. Eight Fife canaries
pauses between songs, so our measurements would and six roller canaries sang inside the chamber.
include any increase in gas exchange between songs that To measure the rate of oxygen consumption of the
compensated for an anaerobic component to metabolism canaries, we used an open-flow respirometry system con-
during song. The structure of the song differs substan- nected to an oxygen analyser (model 1100A or Xentra,
tially between the two breeds of canary that we studied. Servomex, Crowborough, U.K.). Air was pumped (Charles
The song of the Fife Fancy is louder, higher-pitched and Austen, Byfleet, U.K., diaphragm pump) through an
contains more phrases than that of the roller (Güttinger ABS plastic chamber (1712 cm and 15 cm high, Ensto,
1985; Mundinger 1995). We compared the cost of singing Briticent, Christchurch, U.K.) with a 1-cm-diameter
between these two breeds to assess whether song com- wooden perch 3 cm from the floor. The upper 9 cm of the
plexity influenced the energy cost of singing. To ensure chamber was transparent. We used a wet type gas flow
that the measurements were representative of song in meter (model DM3A, Zeal, London, U.K.) to measure the
more normal situations, we compared the characteristics flow rate of ambient air into the chamber. The meanSD
of song recorded inside the respirometry chamber and in flow rate of dry air at standard temperature and pressure
the birds’ home cages. We recorded the rate at which (STPD) across experiments was 1480450 ml/min
birds moved so that we could separate the energy costs of (N=206). Flow rates varied by less than 0.5% within
singing from those of moving. We also measured basal experiments, and these changes were taken into account
metabolic rate (BMR) so that we could express metab- in the calculations of the rate of oxygen consumption.
olism during singing as a multiple of BMR. Our results Gases were dried (silica gel) before and after passing
allow us to assess whether singing is energetically costly through the flow meter and after passing though the
or cheap in passerine birds. chamber. The zero point of the oxygen analyser was set
each week using oxygen-free dry nitrogen gas (BOC,
Guildford, U.K.) and the span was set before each exper-
METHODS iment using ambient air. The output from the oxygen
Birds and Husbandry analyser was sampled at 30 Hz using a microcomputer
with an analogue-to-digital converter (PC-ADH24, Bede
We purchased nine male roller canaries from local Technology, Sunderland, U.K.) and averaged every 2 s
breeders and purchased or bred 49 male Fife Fancy by customized software written in BASIC. The ambient
canaries (hereafter called Fife canaries). We kept the birds oxygen content of the air that was pumped through the
Table 1. Metabolic rates (W) of Fife canaries and roller canaries at night in the thermoneutral zone (BMR), and during the day while sitting (on a perch with the tarsi covered by the body
feathers and the body in a relatively horizontal posture), standing (in the singing posture with the tarsi exposed and the body held relatively vertically), eating and singing
Metabolic rate (W) Factorial increase t test, sing versus stand

Mass Proportion Sing/ Sing/ Sing/ Effect


Bird (g) BMR Sit Stand Eat Sing song BMR Sit Stand t df P size, r

Fife canaries
Dor31 20.1 0.25 — 0.72±0.06 0.74±0.07 0.84±0.10 0.39±0.09 3.41 — 1.18 3.91 22 0.0007 0.64
(13) (4) (14) (14)
DP37 17.1 0.28 — 0.55±0.08 0.57±0.04 0.50±0.03 0.21±0.09 1.82 — 0.92 −1.37 6 0.22 −0.49
(6) (4) (6) (6)
DW2 17.7 0.33 — 0.60±0.07 0.70±0.09 0.62±0.09 0.40±0.09 1.89 — 1.03 0.41 5 0.70 0.18
(3) (11) (7) (7)
J6 22.4 0.31 0.54±0.05 0.60±0.05 0.62±0.07 0.61±0.10 0.24±0.09 1.98 1.14 1.02 0.22 2 0.85 0.15
(2) (8) (11) (3) (3)
J9 21.5 0.23 — — 0.59±0.11 0.73±0.07 0.35±0.07 3.13 — — — — — —
(17) (7) (7)
J22 23.7 0.35 0.52 — 0.73±0.09 0.80±0.06 0.18±0.06 2.28 1.54 — — — — —
(1) (21) (2) (2)
LY4 21.5 0.37 — 0.62±0.05 0.77±0.07 0.71±0.09 0.33±0.11 1.95 — 1.14 2.91 15 0.01 0.60
(6) (4) (19) (19)
TW47 28.3 0.25 0.46±0.05 0.66±0.02 0.78±0.09 0.73±0.04 0.56±0.17 2.93 1.59 1.11 4.19 14 0.0002 0.80
(2) (7) (15) (10) (10)
Mean 21.5±3.5 0.30±0.05 0.51±0.04 0.62±0.06 0.69±0.08 0.69±0.11 0.33±0.12 2.46±0.67 1.42±0.25 1.07±0.10 (6) — — — 0.31±0.47
(8) (8) (3) (6) (8) (8) (8) (8) (3) (6) (6)
Roller canaries
Var 19.8 0.29 — 0.73±0.16 0.75±0.17 0.68±0.11 0.39±0.09 2.32 — 0.93 −0.69 7 0.51 −0.25
(10) (19) (21) (21)
208 21.9 0.35 — 0.69±0.08 0.72±0.09 0.71±0.08 0.54±0.15 2.00 — 1.02 0.3 7 0.8 0.11
(5) (7) (17) (17)
1725 19.9 0.32 0.43±0.05 0.51±0.09 0.58±0.06 0.62±0.01 0.36±0.10 1.92 1.44 1.21 1.23 4 0.3 0.52
(5) (4) (11) (2) (5)
109 18.7 0.25 0.40±0.03 0.52±0.06 0.53±0.06 0.56±0.06 0.32±0.07 2.20 1.40 1.06 1.08 6 0.32 0.40
(5) (17) (11) (5) (5)
23 21.7 0.22 — 0.56±0.06 0.56 0.62±0.11 0.54±0.17 — — 1.11 0.63 5 0.56 0.27
(16) (1) (5) (5)
209 17.9 0.38 — 0.64±0.09 0.75 0.71±0.08 0.44±0.18 — — 1.11 2.43 23 0.02 0.45
(14) (1) (24) (24)
Mean 20.0±1.6 0.30±0.06 0.41±0.02 0.61±0.11 0.65±0.10 0.64±0.07 0.43±0.09 2.11±0.15 1.42±0.02 1.05±0.07 — — — 0.25±0.29
(6) (6) (2) (4) (6) (4) (6) (4) (2) (6) (6)

Only data from bouts of sitting, standing and singing during which the birds did not move are included, except for the data given in italics for two roller canaries (birds 23 and 209) for which
movement rates did not differ between singing and standing birds. Proportion of song refers to the proportion of time for which a bird sang during a song bout. The t tests compare metabolic
rate between perching and singing within individual birds. Sample sizes are given in parentheses. Means are given±SD. The mean values given for each breed of canary are the means of the
mean values for each bird. The durations of measurements of metabolic rate across Fife canaries were 3.0±2.4, 6.6±1.5, 6.8±1.6 and 5.9±2.1 min for sitting, standing, eating and singing,
respectively. The durations of measurements of metabolic rate across roller canaries were 7.5±2.6, 5.2±3.0, 6.4±5.2 and 6.5±1.0 min for sitting, standing, eating and singing, respectively.
WARD ET AL. THE ENERGY COST OF CANARY SONG
895
896 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 66, 5

chamber was measured before and after each bird was categorized as singing, standing (in the singing posture
placed in the chamber. We used these measurements to with the tarsi exposed and the body held relatively
compensate for any drift in the output of the oxygen vertical), sitting (with the legs and feet covered by the
analyser during an experiment. body feathers and the body in a relatively horizontal
We calculated the rate of oxygen consumption by posture) or eating. We defined song duration as the
multiplying the difference between the fractional oxygen period during which a bird produced a series of phrases in
content of ambient and excurrent air by the incurrent rapid succession. Song duration was always shorter than
flow rate (corrected to STPD). Carbon dioxide was not the equilibration time of the chamber. Our measure-
absorbed; this maximizes the accuracy of calculated ments of the energy cost of singing therefore refer to the
energy expenditure when the respiratory quotient is not average metabolic rate during bouts of songs. Song bouts
measured (Koteja 1996; Speakman 2000). We converted began or ended when the bird perched, sat, ate, moved (if
rates of oxygen consumption to energy equivalents using the bird had not otherwise moved during the singing
an oxycalorific value of 20.92 J/ml (derived from Brody bout) or sang at a rate that differed by more than 50%
1945 for a respiratory quotient of 1). We measured basal from that during the rest of the bout. Movement was
metabolic rate (BMR) by placing each bird in the assessed from the rate at which birds moved their feet (i.e.
respirometry chamber overnight at 30C (within the to hop between the perch and the chamber floor or to
thermoneutral zone for passerine birds of similar mass to rotate by 180 upon the perch).
canaries; Calder & King 1974) inside a constant tempera-
ture incubator (model INL-401N-010, Gallenkamp,
Song Duration and Amplitude
Loughborough, U.K.). BMR was calculated from the
lowest metabolic rate over 5 min. We recorded Fife canary song using a Tandberg TM6
To determine the washout characteristics of the microphone connected to a computer (Dell Dimension
respirometry chamber, we reduced the oxygen content of 4100, Creative Sound Blaster AudioPCI 64V soundcard)
the gases inside the chamber to 20.8% (a level similar to running Goldwave (version 4.23, Goldwave Inc., St
that experienced by the birds during experiments) using John’s, Canada) software with a sampling rate of 22 kHz
nitrogen gas (BOC). We then pumped ambient air and 16-bit resolution. The centre of the microphone was
through the chamber at rates within the range used 5 cm in front of and 2 cm below the bird’s bill when the
during experiments until the oxygen content of the bird sang facing the microphone while standing on
excurrent gases returned to that of ambient air. A Fife the perch inside the respirometry chamber. We used the
canary that had died of natural causes was placed on the video equipment described above to assess when the birds
perch inside the chamber to make the internal volume sang in the same position relative to the microphone
and air flow characteristics of the chamber as similar as inside their home cages. We recorded song from eight
possible to those during experiments with living birds. A Fife canaries standing in this position relative to the
meanSD of 5200200 ml (N=3) of ambient air was microphone inside the respirometry chamber and from
passed through the chamber before the oxygen content seven of these birds in their home cages.
of the gases leaving the chamber recovered by 95% from We calculated the proportion of time spent singing
the level to which it had been perturbed. We therefore during each bout of song from the summed durations
assumed that the equilibrium rate of oxygen consump- of all songs divided by the duration of the bout. The
tion of a bird for a given behaviour was reached after that duration of each Fife canary song was measured from the
behaviour had been performed consistently for long digitized recordings to the nearest 0.1 s using Avisoft-
enough for 5200 ml of gas to have passed through the SASLab Pro (Berlin, Germany, version 3.94c). The dur-
chamber (hereafter called the chamber equilibration ation of each roller canary song was measured from the
time). The meanSD chamber equilibration time was videos, with a stopwatch. Comparison of the meanSD
3.81.1 min (N=206 experiments). The combined time duration of Fife canary songs that were measured both
taken for gases to pass from the respirometry chamber to with the stopwatch and from the digitized recordings
the oxygen analyser and for the analyser to respond to showed that song duration was measured to the nearest
any change in concentration of the gases was 3–5 s, 0.40.1 s (N=230 songs) using the stopwatch. The pro-
depending on the flow rate. This response time was portion of singing during song bouts measured using the
taken into account when we matched the data on the stopwatch differed by 23% (N=8 bouts) from that
oxygen content gases leaving the chamber with bird measured from the digitized recordings.
behaviour. We did not use an instantaneous correction All eight Fife canaries included three types of phrase
(Bartholemew et al. 1981) to calculate the rate of oxygen during the first 5 min of song that we recorded from each
consumption, because this was unnecessary when bouts bird in his home cage. These were a series of elements
of behaviour were long enough to allow us to measure with continuously descending pitch, an ‘A’ phrase (Vallet
equilibrium rates of oxygen consumption. & Kreutzer 1995; Vallet et al. 1998), and a series of
elements in which descending and low pitch notes alter-
nated (Fig. 1). To measure the peak amplitude and the
Bird Behaviour
frequency of the peak amplitude of these elements, we
We recorded bird behaviour on VHS video (JVC GR-A used the spectral characteristics function from the log-
X280 video camera connected to a Panasonic NV-FS90 arithmic power spectrum in Avisoft-SASLab Pro. We
video recorder) during experiments. Bird behaviour was sampled 3–15 repetitions of each element of the three
WARD ET AL. THE ENERGY COST OF CANARY SONG 897

a paired t test (with song element as the unit of indepen-


dent data) to test whether differences in volume between
song inside the respirometry chamber and in the birds’
home cages were the same as the differences in volume
between song played from a speaker with and without the
lid on the respirometry chamber. We used a Bonferroni
correction to adjust  when more than one test was
performed on related data. All statistical tests used were
two tailed. All means are presentedSD unless otherwise
stated.
Figure 1. The three phrases sung by Fife canaries for which
characteristics were analysed. We used least-square regression to assess whether meta-
bolic rate was related to the proportion of time spent
singing for individual birds (Zar 1996). All computations
phrases on six randomly selected occasions from record- were carried out using arcsine-transformed proportion of
ings of Fife canaries singing inside the respirometry time spent singing. This transformation is appropriate
chamber and in their home cages. We calculated the before analysis of data that are expressed as a proportion
mean peak amplitude, the mean frequency at the peak (Sokal & Rohlf 1995, page 421). The regression coef-
amplitude and the mean repetition frequency for each ficients were back-transformed before presentation. We
bird singing in both situations. used GLM ANCOVA in which bird was entered as a
We compared the volume of recordings of Fife canary random factor, and the proportion of time spent singing
song played with and without the lid on the respirometry was a covariate to assess whether the relation between
chamber to determine the effect of enclosure inside the metabolic rate and the proportion of time spent singing
chamber upon the apparent volume of canary song. We differed between birds (Zar 1996). The gradients of the
used three to five repetitions of a randomly selected relations between metabolism and the proportion of time
example of each of the three Fife canary phrases shown in spent singing did not vary between birds, but the inter-
Fig. 1 sung by each of seven birds. We played the songs cepts differed. We therefore present the relations for
through a general-purpose driver speaker from a Sony individual birds. We used these relations to calculate
SRS-P3 speaker (60 cm diameter, 0.4–20 kHz output) metabolic rate during continuous singing, with an associ-
placed in a box (88 cm and 5.5 cm high) from which ated 95% prediction interval (Zar 1996, equation 16.29).
sound was directed upwards into a plastic funnel (7 cm Analyses were carried out with Minitab 12.22 (Minitab
diameter initially, narrowing gradually to 1 cm diameter Inc, State College, Pennsylvania, U.S.A.).
over 4.5 cm, 1 mm wall thickness). The sound from the
speaker was directed horizontally for 2.5 cm before being
broadcast through an opening of 0.5 cm internal diam- RESULTS
eter, 11.5 cm from the floor of the chamber (a similar
height to the head of a canary). The volume of the songs Song Characteristics
played from the speaker was similar to that of singing Fife canaries sang slightly shorter songs inside the
canaries and was recorded with the same equipment as respirometry chamber than in their home cages
was used to record the birds. (respirometry chamber: 5.92.2 s, range 0.4–56.0 s, N=8
birds; home cage: 7.32.5 s, range 0.2–49.9 s, N=7 birds;
Data Analysis Z test: Z=5.9, N=7 birds, P<0.001). Mean roller canary
song duration inside the respirometry chamber was
We used t tests to compare metabolic rate during two 8.72.6 s (range 0.7–68.5 s, N=6 birds). The phrase rep-
behaviours in individual birds, song characteristics under etition frequency and the frequency at the peak ampli-
two circumstances or characteristics of the two breeds of tude of the elements of these phrases did not differ
canary. All but two (of 40) data sets on the metabolic rate between Fife canary song in the respirometry chamber
of individual birds during a particular activity (Table 1) and in the birds’ home cages (repetition frequency,
were normally distributed (Kolmogorov–Smirnov nor- Z=0.9, N=3 phrases, P=0.36; frequency at peak ampli-
mality tests). The data sets that were not shown to be tude, Z=1.7, N=5 elements, P=0.08). The peak amplitude
normally distributed both had N<5. We assumed that the of song recorded inside the respirometry chamber was
inferences drawn from t tests of metabolism during dif- 5.53.6 dB greater than that recorded inside the birds’
ferent activities would be robust to these deviations from home cages (Z test: Z=2.8, N=5 elements, P=0.007). The
normal distributions of data (Zar 1996, page 97). The volume of Fife canary song played from the speaker was
overall significance of tests across birds was assessed with 3.72.9 dB greater when the lid was on the chamber
Z tests following Rosenthal (1991). We calculated Z, the than when it was removed (Z=5.1, N=5 elements,
standard normal deviate, (Rosenthal 1991, equation 5.14) P<0.001). The difference in recording volume between
and r, the effect size, (Rosenthal 1991, equation 2.16) for Fife canaries singing inside the respirometry chamber and
each bird. We determined the overall value of Z across in their home cages did not differ from the difference in
birds from the unweighted values of Z from each bird volume between recordings of song played through the
(Rosenthal 1991, equation 5.4) and calculated the overall speaker with and without the lid on the respirometry
effect size from the mean value of r across birds. We used chamber (paired t test: t4 =0.9, N=5 elements, P=0.4).
898 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 66, 5

1 t12 =1.96, P=0.07; bird 23: t6 =0.1, P=0.9). When the data
(a) from these two birds were included in the analysis,
0.8 metabolism during singing was 1.050.07 times greater
than that during standing (Z=1.8, N=6 birds, P=0.03;
0.6 Table 1, Fig. 2b). Metabolism during singing without
moving did not differ from that during eating (Z=0.2,
0.4 N=4 birds, P=0.4). Metabolic rate during singing without
moving was 2.110.15 (N=4 birds) times BMR and
Metabolic rate (W)

0.2 1.420.02 (N=2 birds) times that during sitting.


8 3 6 8 8
0
Metabolism and Proportion of Time Spent
1 Singing
(b)
0.8 Metabolic rate increased with the proportion of time
spent singing for the three Fife canaries from which we
0.6 collected most data (Table 2). Metabolic rate was not
related to the proportion of time spent singing in the
0.4 other birds (regression: P>0.08 in all cases, sample sizes in
Table 1). The intercepts, but not the gradients, of the
0.2 relationships between metabolic rate and the proportion
of time spent singing varied between the three birds
6 2 5 5 6
0 shown in Table 2 (GLM ANCOVA: bird: F2,68 =13.5,
BMR Sit Stand Sing Eat P<0.001; proportion of time spent singing: F1,68 =23.1,
Figure 2. Mean+SD metabolic rate (W) of (a) male Fife canaries and P<0.001; birdproportion of time spent singing:
(b) male roller canaries at night in the thermoneutral zone (BMR), F2,68 =1.0, P=0.4). We extrapolated the relationships
and during the day when sitting, standing without moving, singing between metabolic rate and the proportion of time
without moving and eating. Numbers inside columns show the spent singing (measured range 0–0.75) to predict
number of birds that performed each behaviour.
metabolic rate during continuous singing (Table 2, Fig. 3).
The mean metabolic rate during continuous singing
Metabolism During Singing and Other Activities would be 0.880.10 W (equivalent to 1.300.12 times
metabolic rate during standing or 3.140.78BMR,
Some of the Fife canaries moved at different rates N=3 birds).
during bouts of singing and standing (t tests: two birds
that moved at different rates: t33 =2.1, P=0.04, t24 =2.8,
P=0.01; other birds: t3–20 =0.2–2.8, P=0.07–0.9), so in our Comparison Between Breeds
analyses we used only data from periods during which the
birds did not move (Table 1). Fife canary metabolism Fife canaries and roller canaries did not differ in mass,
during singing was 1.070.10 times that during standing BMR or the proportion of time spent singing (t tests of
(Z test: Z=3.6, N=6 birds, P<0.0001) but did not differ mean values for each bird of each breed: t12 =0.2–1.6,
from that during eating (Z=0.3, N=8 birds, P=0.7; Table P>0.1 in all cases), or metabolic rate during singing
1, Fig. 2a). Metabolic rate during singing was without moving or factorial increase in metabolism
2.460.67BMR (N=8 birds) or 1.420.25sitting between BMR and singing without moving (t tests:
metabolic rate (N=3 birds; Table 1). t11 =0.5–0.7, P>0.4 in all cases) or metabolic rate during
The metabolic rate of four roller canaries that sang and standing without moving (t test: t9 =0.6, P=0.6). Our
stood without moving did not differ between singing and measurements of metabolism during standing without
standing (Z test: Z=0.8, N=4 birds, P=0.2; Table 1). The moving, singing without moving and the factorial
rates of movement of two additional roller canaries did increase in metabolism during singing over that during
not differ between singing and standing (t tests: bird 209: standing by roller canaries did not differ from those of

Table 2. Relations between the proportion of time spent singing and the metabolic rate of three male Fife canaries
Metabolic rate for continuous song
Mean (95% prediction range)

Intercept Gradient Metabolism Metabolism


Bird ±SE ±SE R2 P N (W) (×stand)

Dor31 0.73±0.02 0.25±0.09 0.26 0.007 27 0.98 (0.75–1.22) 1.35 (1.02–1.67)


LY4 0.63±0.03 0.25±0.10 0.20 0.02 25 0.88 (0.64–1.12) 1.39 (1.01–1.77)
TW47 0.67±0.01 0.11±0.03 0.48 0.002 17 0.78 (0.69–0.87) 1.16 (1.03–1.30)
WARD ET AL. THE ENERGY COST OF CANARY SONG 899

1 roller canaries, was consistent with the greater proportion


of the time for which Fife canaries sang. The substantial
differences between the volume, frequency and structure
0.8
of the songs of Fife canaries and roller canaries (Güttinger
Metabolic rate (W)

1985; Mundinger 1995) did not lead to measurable differ-


0.6 ences in the metabolic cost of singing. Our data thus
confirm the conclusions of Oberweger & Goller (2001)
0.4 and Franz & Goller (2003), that changes in song structure
and complexity do not alter the energetic cost of singing.
Female preferences for greater song complexity (Searcy
0.2 & Yasukawa 1996) are not therefore because singers of
more complex songs are producing a more energetically
0 demanding display.
0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 Weathers et al. (1997) suggested that bird vocalization
Proportion of time spent singing may have an anaerobic component so that measurements
Figure 3. Metabolic rate (W) in relation to the proportion of time of oxygen consumption made during singing may under-
spent singing in eight Fife canaries (shown by different symbols). estimate the energy cost of song production. We think
Solid lines and points show the relations for the three birds shown in that it is unlikely that canaries would use anaerobic
Table 2. Dotted lines show extrapolation of these relations to metabolism during song, because they sing for such a
continuous singing. Open points show data from the other five
high proportion of the time that there would be little
birds.
opportunity to pay back any oxygen debt incurred during
singing. Oberweger & Goller (2001) calculated the cost of
Waterslager canaries (a different strain of roller canary) singing by comparing the metabolic rate of Waterslager
measured by Oberweger & Goller (2001; t tests: t6 <1.2, canaries immediately before they sang with that during
P>0.3). song. Any anaerobic component to metabolism during
singing might not have been included in their measure-
ments of the cost of song, since the measurement period
DISCUSSION ended before any oxygen debt would have been repaid
Canary Metabolism During Singing during the subsequent intersong interval. The metabolic
rates of our roller canaries did not differ from those
Recordings of Fife canary song from inside the measured by Oberweger & Goller (2001). The way in
respirometry chamber were louder than those from the which we calculated the energy cost of singing would
birds’ home cages. This was due to acoustic effects of have included repayment of any oxygen debt incurred
enclosure inside the chamber, since the volume of canary from anaerobic metabolism during song, since intervals
song played from a speaker increased to similar extent between songs were also included in our bouts of singing
when the lid was placed on the chamber. The volume, behaviour. Canaries therefore do not have a measurable
frequency and repetition frequency of a representative anaerobic component to metabolism during song. Our
sample of Fife canary song phrases did not alter inside the measurements of the energy cost of singing also represent
respirometry chamber, although songs were slightly the overall effect upon metabolism of short-term fluctu-
shorter than in birds’ home cages. This difference was ations in gas exchange caused by changes in the
unlikely to influence the metabolic cost of singing. Our respiratory patterns of singing birds (Franz & Goller
measurements of the cost of singing are therefore 2003).
representative of song in more normal situations. The metabolic rate during song bouts did not differ
Metabolic rate increased with the proportion of time from that during eating in our canaries, despite what
spent singing in Fife canaries from which we had most intuitively appeared to be much greater effort while
data, as would be expected if standing between songs singing (the whole of the bird shook vigorously) than
required slightly less energy than singing. Variation while eating (the bird leaned forwards occasionally to
between birds in the gradients of the relations between pick up food which was manipulated in the bill while
metabolic rate and the proportion of time spent singing otherwise remaining stationary). Singing by both breeds
would have suggested that some birds produced song of canary raised metabolism only slightly (mean factorial
more efficiently than others. Since the gradients of these increase 1.05–1.07) over that of birds that stood in the
relations did not vary between birds, there was no evi- same posture as was used during song. Metabolic rate
dence for individual variation in the efficiency of song during singing was a greater multiple of BMR (a factorial
production. The variation between birds in the intercepts increase of 2.1–2.5). Singing was more costly in relation
of these relations reflects individual differences in meta- to BMR than in relation to standing, partly because
bolic rate while standing. Such differences may be related metabolic rate increases as temperature falls below the
to a combination of factors such as mass (Calder & thermoneutral zone (Calder & King 1974), and we
King 1974), body composition (Daan et al. 1990) and measured the cost of singing at lower temperatures (15–
dominance (Buchanan et al. 2001). 20C) than we measured BMR (30C). The factorial
The slightly greater mean factorial increase in metab- increase in metabolism during song relative to BMR is
olism during singing by Fife canaries, compared with likely to vary with temperature, because the energy cost
900 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 66, 5

of thermoregulation changes with temperature. Singing F4,27 =4.3, P=0.01 with Tukey post hoc multiple compari-
caused a factorial increase in metabolism over sitting that sons). The cost of singing measured for Carolina wrens
was intermediate between those relative to standing and may be greater than that of the other species because of
BMR partly because the thermoregulatory costs of sitting difficulties in measuring transient changes in oxygen
are likely to be lower than those of standing birds, consumption in a relatively large respirometry chamber,
because sitting birds covered their legs. The legs can make because the birds moved as well as sang during measure-
a substantial contribution to heat loss (Ward et al. 1999). ments, or because of insufficiently detailed mathematical
Metabolic rate during sitting was also likely to be lower treatment of the data (Frappell et al. 1989; Gaunt et al.
than that of singing or standing because the birds 1996; Ortigues et al. 1997). Movements during song bouts
appeared to be less alert during sitting. The energy cost of were not monitored during Eberhardt’s (1994) study of
song production itself should therefore be assessed by Carolina wrens, so the costs of any movements would
comparing metabolism during song with that during have been included in the measured cost of singing (Horn
standing in the same posture (mean factorial increase et al. 1995; Gaunt et al. 1996). If free-living Carolina
1.05–1.07). Our data thus support the hypothesis that wrens move during song bouts, the cost of movements
song production by canaries is energetically cheap. should be included in the cost of the display, but in this
case the cost of the display would clearly be greater than
Does Singing Generally Increase Metabolic Rate in that of song alone. The increased energy expenditure of
sage grouse, Centrocercus urophasianus, that displayed at
Birds?
higher rates was attributed partially to higher movement
Singing increases the rate of oxygen consumption of rates (Vehrencamp et al. 1989). Song production is likely
Carolina wrens, European starlings, zebra finches and two to make a minor contribution to the cost of display by
breeds of canary singing inside respirometry chambers birds such as skylarks, Alauda arvensis, that sing while
(Eberhardt 1994; Oberweger & Goller 2001; Franz & flying (Hedenström & Alerstam 1996).
Goller 2003; this study). Singing also increases the rate Because consistent results have been obtained from
of overnight mass loss (which is presumably correlated more than one breed of canary and two other passerine
with the rate of consumption of body reserves and there- species (Oberweger & Goller 2001; Franz & Goller 2003;
fore with energy expenditure) of free-living nightingales this study), it is likely that the modest cost of singing,
(Thomas 2002). In contrast, crowing by junglefowl or relative to that of standing during the day, in these
cockerels inside a respirometry chamber does not cause a species is representative of most passerine birds. Winter
measurable increase in the rate of oxygen consumption wren song is louder in relation to the mass of the bird
(Chappell et al. 1995; Horn et al. 1995). Although cock- than that of many other passerines (Brackenbury 1979),
erel crows are loud and inefficient (Brackenbury 1977), and singing by wrens of all genera is considered to be
they were performed for a much lower proportion of the particularly loud (Brewer 2001). Wren song may therefore
time than that for which our canaries sang (0.05–0.06 for be energetically more costly than that of most other
crowing: Chappell et al. 1995; Horn et al. 1995; 0.33–0.43 passerine birds, but further empirical data are required to
for singing: this study). Cockerel crows also contain determine whether this is the case.
substantially less acoustic power in relation to the mass of
the bird (60 mW/kg) than the songs of typical passerine
The Energy Cost of Song and the Dawn Chorus
birds such as the song thrush, Turdus philomelos, winter
wren, Troglodytes troglodytes, and European robin, Erith- Singing appears to increase metabolism only slightly
acus rubecula (870, 600 and 300 mW/kg, respectively; over that of a bird standing during the day, but the
Brackenbury 1979). The substantially higher proportion factorial increase over BMR is much greater. The
of time spent singing and greater volume of passerine additional energy cost of singing at night (rather than
song in relation to their body mass probably explain why roosting with a metabolic rate closer to BMR) would be
the cost of passerine song has been detected over other substantially greater than that of singing during the day
fluctuations in metabolism, but the cost of crowing by (rather than standing). This is because the energy cost of
junglefowl and cockerels has not. the activity that singing would replace would be much
The measurements of metabolic rate of singing lower at night than during the day. Similarly, singing
Carolina wrens (Eberhardt 1994) have generally been during the dawn chorus could cause a greater factorial
regarded as anomalously high (Gaunt et al. 1996; increase in energy costs than singing during the day if
Oberweger & Goller 2001; Thomas 2002). The reported birds sing rather than sleep at this time. The energy cost
factorial increase in the metabolism of Carolina wrens of singing at night or at dawn would presumably be a
during song (3.61.1BMR, with a maximum value of factorial increase of between 1.4 (the difference between
9BMR: Eberhardt 1994) was greater than that of Fife sitting and singing during the day in canaries) and 2.2–
canaries (2.40.6BMR, this study), roller canaries 2.6 (the difference between BMR and singing in canaries).
(2.40.4BMR: this study; Oberweger & Goller 2001), The extent of the increase in metabolism during singing
zebra finches (2.00.2BMR: Oberweger & Goller 2001) at night would depend on the difference in temperature
or European starlings (2.10.06BMR: Oberweger & (and hence thermoregulatory costs) between the micro-
Goller 2001; ANOVA with species, or breed of canary, as a habitats in which birds would roost or sing, changes in
factor and the mean factorial increase in metabolism thermoregulatory costs due to altering posture and
during singing of individual birds as independent data: altered alertness. Sound production itself would make a
WARD ET AL. THE ENERGY COST OF CANARY SONG 901

minor contribution to metabolism. Our results are there- also be less effective than singing, since a bird can only be
fore consistent with the energetic cost hypothesis for the in one place at a time, but woodland birds can often
evolution of the dawn chorus (McNamara et al. 1987) be heard simultaneously in all directions for greater
although they do not exclude other explanations for the distances than they can be seen.
dawn chorus such as enhanced sound transmission
(Brown & Handford 2003) or reduced foraging success
(Kacelnik & Krebs 1983) at this time. Acknowledgments

We are very grateful to R. Donaldson, H. Hodge, D.


Is Singing Energetically Costly for Passerine Birds? Lumsden, D. Pointon and W. Turnbull for their advice on
canary husbandry, to I. Maynard, V. Murray, A. Oliver, G.
Whether singing is energetically costly for a passerine Thomson and R. Webster who helped to care for the
bird will depend on two factors: how much song produc- birds, to M. Coutts and G. Taylor for electronic support,
tion increases metabolic rate and the proportion of the to Lucy Gilbert, Vincent Janik, Natasha LeBas and Joe
energy available to a bird that is used by singing. It is this Tomkins for discussions, to two anonymous referees for
marginal energy cost (Verhulst & Wiersma 1997) that will suggesting improvements to the manuscript and to the
influence whether song production imposes a fitness cost BBSRC for funding our work.
upon the singer. We have shown that singing causes only
a small increase in the metabolic rate of canaries and that
this result is probably typical of most passerine birds. References
Despite this, whether the energy cost of singing
Bartholemew, G. A., Vleck, D. & Vleck, C. M. 1981. Instantaneous
imposes a fitness cost upon the singer will depend upon
measurements of oxygen consumption during pre-flight warm-up
the circumstances. If a bird is close to starvation, foraging and post-flight cooling in sphingid and saturnid moths. Journal of
during the day or energy conservation by roosting at Experimental Biology, 90, 17–32.
night is clearly more important than singing. The mar- Brackenbury, J. H. 1977. Physiological energetics of cock-crow.
ginal cost of singing by a bird under such circumstances Nature, 270, 433–435.
would be very great. At the other extreme, a bird with an Brackenbury, J. H. 1979. Power capabilities of the avian sound
ample food supply and time for foraging, as well as producing system. Journal of Experimental Biology, 78, 163–166.
negligible predation risk either during singing or forag- Brewer, D. 2001. Wrens, Dippers and Thrashers. London:
ing, would not experience a reduction in fitness by Christopher Helm.
Brody, S. 1945. Bioenergetics and Growth. New York: Reinhold.
singing. Singing would have a low marginal cost for such
Brown, T. J. & Hanford, P. 2003. Why birds sing at dawn: the role
an individual. The time and energy constraints for most
of consistent song transmission. Ibis, 145, 120–129.
birds will be intermediate between these two extremes. It Buchanan, K. L., Evans, M. R., Goldsmith, A. R., Bryant, D. M. &
is presently unclear how to assess how much energy is Rowe, L. V. 2001. Testosterone influences basal metabolic rate in
potentially available to an individual bird for singing. The male house sparrows: a new cost of dominance signalling? Pro-
marginal costs of singing for different individuals will ceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 268, 1337–1344.
remain unknown without this information, but we can Butler, P. J. & Bishop, C. 2000. Flight. In: Sturkie’s Avian Physiology
still conclude that singing is a condition-dependent sig- (Ed. by G. C. Whittow), pp. 391–436. London: Academic Press.
nal. Singing will impose a greater fitness cost upon birds Calder, W. A. & King, J. R. 1974. Thermal and caloric relations of
that have difficulty in achieving energy balance, or in birds. In: Avian Biology. Vol. 4 (Ed. by D. S. Farner & J. R. King),
foraging without increasing their predation risk, even pp. 259–413. New York: Academic Press.
though song production itself has a modest energy cost. Catchpole, C. K. & Slater, P. J. B. 1995. Bird Song: Biological Themes
and Variations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Birds potentially compensate for an increase in metab-
Chappell, M. A. & Bachman, G. C. 2002. Energetic costs of
olism caused by song production by reducing other
begging behaviour. In: The Evolution of Begging: Competition,
aspects of their metabolism such as self-repair (Sheldon & Co-operation and Communication (Ed. by J. Wright & M. L.
Verhulst 1996). Birds are able to perform metabolic com- Leonard), pp. 143–162. Netherlands: Kluwer.
pensation in other circumstances such as during flight Chappell, M. A., Zuk, M., Kwan, T. H. & Johnsen, T. S. 1995.
(Deerenberg et al. 1998). Since singing leads to only a Energy cost of an avian display: crowing in red junglefowl. Animal
small overall increase in metabolism compared with that Behaviour, 49, 255–257.
required during flight, there would appear to be less need Clutton-Brock, T. L. & Albon, S. D. 1979. The roaring of red deer
for compensatory reductions in metabolism during sing- and the evolution of honest advertisement. Behaviour, 69, 145–
ing than while flying. However, compensatory reductions 170.
in other components of metabolism during song remain a Daan, S., Masman, D. & Groenewold, A. 1990. Avian basal
possibility. metabolic rates: their association with body composition and
energy expenditure in nature. American Journal of Physiology, 259,
Although singing has an energy cost, it is an energeti-
R333–R340.
cally cheap signal if compared with alternative behaviour
Deerenberg, C., Overkamp, G. J. F., Visser, G. H. & Daan, S. 1998.
that might replace the functions of song. Flying through- Compensation in resting metabolism for experimentally increased
out the area within which song could have been heard to activity. Journal of Comparative Physiology, B, 168, 507–512.
locate potential mates or rivals would be much more Eberhardt, L. S. 1994. Oxygen consumption during singing by
energetically costly than singing. Flight costs 10BMR male Carolina wrens Thryothorus ludovicianus. Auk, 111, 124–130.
for a bird of the mass of a canary (Butler & Bishop 2000) Fisher, R. A. 1930. The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection. Oxford:
compared with 2.2–2.6BMR for singing. Flying could Clarendon.
902 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 66, 5

Franz, M. & Goller, F. 2003. Respiratory patterns and oxygen Oberweger, K. & Goller, F. 2001. The metabolic cost of bird-
consumption in singing zebra finches. Journal of Experimental song production. Journal of Experimental Biology, 204, 3379–
Biology, 206, 967–978. 3388.
Frappell, P. B. R., Levin, H. A. & Baudinette, R. V. 1989. Under- Ortigues, I., Dussap, C-G. & Anglaret, Y. 1997. Comparison
standing respirometry chambers: what goes in must come out. of various methods of calculating the instantaneous res-
Journal of Theoretical Biology, 138, 479–494. piratory gaseous exchanges from discrete measurements in
Gaunt, A. S., Bucher, T. L., Gaunt, S. L. L. & Baptista, L. F. 1996. respirometry chambers. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 185, 489–
Is singing costly? Auk, 113, 718–721. 501.
Gil, D. & Gahr, M. 2002. The honesty of bird song: multiple Prestwich, K. N. 1994. The energetics of acoustic signalling in
constraints for multiple traits. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 17, anurans and insects. American Zoologist, 34, 625–643.
133–141. Rosenthal, R. 1991. Meta-analytic Procedures for Social Research.
Godfray, H. C. J. 1991. Signalling of need by offspring to their Thousand Oaks, California: Sage.
parents. Nature, 352, 328–330. Ryan, M. J. 1988. Energy, calling, and selection. Am. Zool., 28,
Goller, F. & Larsen, O. N. 1997. A new mechanism of sound 885–898.
generation in songbirds. Proceedings of the National Academy of Searcy, W. A. & Yasukawa, K. 1996. Organization of bird song and
Sciences, U.S.A., 94, 14787–14791. constraints on performance. In: Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic
Grafen, A. 1990a. Sexual selection unhandicapped by the Fisher Communication in Birds (Ed. by D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller),
process. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 144, 473–516. pp. 454–473. Ithaca, New York: Cornell University Press.
Grafen, A. 1990b. Biological signals as handicaps. Journal of Theor- Sheldon, B. C. & Verhulst, S. 1996. Ecological immunology: costly
etical Biology, 144, 517–546. parasite defences and trade-offs in evolutionary ecology. Trends in
Güttinger, H. R. 1985. Consequences of domestication on the song Ecology and Evolution, 11, 317–321.
structures of the canary. Behaviour, 94, 254–278. Sokal, R. R. & Rohlf, F. J. 1995. Biometry. New York: W. H. Freeman.
Hedenström, A. & Alerstam, T. 1996. Skylark optimal flight speeds Speakman, J. R. 2000. The cost of living: field metabolic rates
for flying nowhere and somewhere. Behavioral Ecology, 7, 121– of small mammals. Advances in Ecological Research, 30, 177–
126. 297.
Horn, A. G., Leonard, M. L. & Weary, D. M. 1995. Oxygen Speakman, J. R., Anderson, M. E. & Racey, P. A. 1989. The energy
consumption during crowing by roosters: talk is cheap. Animal cost of echolocation in pipistrelle bats Pipistrellus pipistrellus.
Behaviour, 50, 1171–1175. Journal of Comparative Physiology, A, 165, 679–685.
Hutchinson, J. M. C., McNamara, J. M. & Cuthill, I. C. 1993. Song, Suthers, R. A., Goller, F. & Pytte, C. 1999. The neuromuscular
sexual selection, starvation and strategic handicaps. Animal Behav- control of birdsong. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society
iour, 45, 1153–1177. of London, B, 354, 927–939.
Kacelnik, A. & Krebs, J. R. 1983. The dawn chorus in the great tit Thomas, R. J. 2002. The costs of singing in nightingales. Animal
(Parus major): proximate and ultimate causes. Behaviour, 83, Behaviour, 63, 959–966.
287–309. Vallet, E. & Kreutzer, M. 1995. Female canaries are sexually
Koteja, P. 1996. Measuring energy metabolism with open-flow responsive to special song phrases. Animal Behaviour, 49, 1603–
respirometric systems: which design to choose? Functional Ecology, 1610.
10, 675–677. Vallet, E., Beme, I. & Kreutzer, M. 1998. Two-note syllables in
Kotiaho, J. S., Alatalo, R. V., Mappes, J., Nielsen, M. G., Parri, S. & canary songs elicit high levels of sexual display. Animal Behaviour,
Rivero, A. 1998. Energetic costs of size and sexual signalling in a 55, 291–297.
wolf spider. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, Vehrencamp, S. L., Bradbury, J. W. & Gibson, R. M. 1989. The
265, 2203–2209. energetic cost of display in male sage grouse. Animal Behaviour,
Lambrechts, M. M. 1996. Organization of bird song and constraints 38, 885–896.
on performance. In: Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communi- Verhulst, S. & Wiersma, P. 1997. Is begging cheap? Auk, 114,
cation in Birds (Ed. by D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller), pp. 305–320. 134–134.
Ithaca, New York: Cornell University Press. Ward, S., Rayner, J. M. V., Möller, U., Jackson, D. M., Nachtigall,
Larsen, O. N. & Goller, F. 2002. Direct observation of syringeal W. & Speakman, J. R. 1999. Heat transfer from starlings Sturnus
muscle function in songbirds and a parrot. Journal of Experimental vulgaris during flight. Journal of Experimental Biology, 202, 1589–
Biology, 205, 25–35. 1602.
McCarty, J. P. 1996. The energetic cost of begging in nestling Weathers, W. W., Hodum, P. J. & Anderson, D. J. 1997. Is the
passerines. Auk, 113, 178–188. energy cost of begging by nestling passerines surprisingly low?
McNamara, J. M., Mace, R. H. & Houston, A. I. 1987. Optimal daily Auk, 114, 133–133.
routines of singing and foraging in birds singing to attract a mate. Zahavi, A. 1975. Mate choice: a selection for a handicap. Journal of
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 20, 399–405. Theoretical Biology, 53, 205–214.
Mundinger, P. C. 1995. Behaviour-genetic analysis of canary song: Zar, J. H. 1996. Biostatistical Analysis. New York: Prentice Hall.
inter-strain differences in sensory learning, and epigenetic rules.
Animal Behaviour, 50, 1491–1511.

You might also like