Personality and Individual Differences: Rachel E. Avery, Luke D. Smillie, Chris R. Fife-Schaw

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

Personality and Individual Differences 76 (2015) 56–61

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Employee achievement orientations and personality as predictors of job


satisfaction facets
Rachel E. Avery a,⇑, Luke D. Smillie b, Chris R. Fife-Schaw a
a
University of Surrey, AD Building, Guildford, Surrey GU2 7XH, UK
b
Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, Victoria 3010, Australia

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This paper examines the incremental value of achievement orientations (Mastery-Approach; Mastery-
Received 17 June 2014 Avoid; Performance-Approach; Performance-Avoid), above Extraversion and Neuroticism, in predicting
Received in revised form 31 October 2014 two different types of satisfaction outcomes; expectation-based-job-satisfaction (EX-JS) and satisfac-
Accepted 16 November 2014
tion-with-one’s-own-job-performance (P-JS). Using structural equation modelling, data from 242 UK
government body employees showed that only Extraversion shared a (positive) relationship with EX-JS.
Whereas, the strongest relations with P-JS were found for Neuroticism and Mastery-Approach with both
Keywords:
sharing positive relationships with this satisfaction outcome. Analyses indicated that Mastery-Approach
Achievement orientation
Employee motivation
accounted for unique variance in P-JS beyond Extraversion and Neuroticism. Findings show that there
Personality is scope for experiences of satisfaction at work to be traced to stable approach competence specific
Job satisfaction motivational tendencies.
Ó 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction policy (Churchill, Ford, & Walker, 1979). Personality taxonomies


have provided vital support for the role of dispositions in job satis-
Achievement Goal Theory (Dweck, 1986) proposes that individ- faction (see Connolly & Viswesvaran, 2000; Furnham, Petrides,
uals engage in behaviour with a competence-specific purpose. This Jackson, & Cotter, 2002; Judge, Heller, & Mount, 2002). The current
purpose, an achievement orientation, thus describes the pattern of research addresses dispositional relations not to global job satis-
cognition and action that results from pursuing various goals faction but to two important conceptual distinctions, that of expec-
(DeShon & Gillespie, 2005). These orientations have been theorised tation-based-job-satisfaction (EX-JS), and, satisfaction-with-one’s-
to reflect dispositional, trait-like motivational characteristics own-job-performance (P-JS). Inclusion of these distinct forms of
(Nicholls, 1989) that are distinct from basic personality traits job satisfaction allows for exploration of multiple facets of job sat-
(Payne, Youngcourt, & Beaubien, 2007). Importantly, they have also isfaction, which, could be differentially affected by personality and/
been found to relate to workplace outcomes such as learning, or achievement orientations. The current satisfaction facets relat-
training and job performance (Payne et al., 2007; Steele-Johnson, ing to ‘expectations’ and ‘own performance’ are investigated given
Beauregard, Hoover, & Schmidt, 2000). In the present study, we theoretical relevance to achievement orientations, as described
investigate the extent to which an employee’s achievement orien- below. EX-JS is concerned with an employee’s satisfaction in rela-
tation may also predict different forms of satisfaction in the work- tion to whether they feel their job expectations are being fulfilled
place, beyond that of two major personality traits already known in their current position and ‘‘emphasises the match between
to impact satisfaction – Extraversion and Neuroticism. expectations and perceived reality for aspects of the job taken as
a whole’’ (Bacharach, Bamberger, & Conley, 1991, p. 45). Research
1.1. Satisfaction in the workplace has shown such satisfaction to negatively relate to role conflict
(Bacharach et al., 1991) and role overload (Bacharach & Mitchell,
Job satisfaction is typically conceptualised as a multidimen- 1982). EX-JS is useful in assessing and understanding the extent
sional construct encompassing satisfaction with one’s manager, to which employee dispositions might differentially influence
peers, pay, promotional opportunities, job security and company attitudes towards job expectations currently being met. EX-JS is
thus an externally referenced measure of satisfaction in the
⇑ Corresponding author. Tel.: +44 (0)1483686862; fax: +44 (0)1483689553. workplace.
E-mail addresses: r.e.avery@surrey.ac.uk (R.E. Avery), lsmillie@unimelb.edu.au
The second outcome of interest, P-JS (see Steele-Johnson,
(L.D. Smillie), c.fife-schaw@surrey.ac.uk (C.R. Fife-Schaw). Heintz, & Miller, 2008), concerns the extent to which an individual

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.11.037
0191-8869/Ó 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
R.E. Avery et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 76 (2015) 56–61 57

is content with their own performance. P-JS is a more self-referen- 1988). Dweck (1986) suggested that mastery-oriented individuals
tial measure of satisfaction in the workplace compared to EX-JS. tend to hold an incremental theory reflecting beliefs that self-attri-
Measures of P-JS have been previously used in laboratory settings butes such as intelligence and skills are developable entities.
with P-JS often increasing in a linear fashion during the course of a Whereas, performance-orientated individuals tend to believe that
laboratory task and higher P-JS being reported under low, relative self-attributes are fixed entities (Dweck, 1999; VandeWalle,
to high, cognitive load conditions (Steele-Johnson et al., 2000). 2003). Since this two-factor conceptualisation, both orientations
There is no evident research which has addressed possible rela- have been suggested to vary in motivational direction. Perfor-
tionships between satisfaction with one’s own performance and mance-orientated individuals can be motivated either to demon-
dispositional tendencies within an organisational framework. By strate superior competence relative to others (Performance-
considering these two specific satisfaction outcomes, one can take Approach), or to avoid demonstrating inferior competence relative
into account the possible non-similarity of expectations satisfac- to others (Performance-Avoid) (Elliot, 1999). Mastery-orientated
tion versus performance judgment satisfaction across individual individuals are assumed to be motivated to either focus on the
personality and achievement characteristics. development of competence through task mastery (Mastery-
Approach) or to strive to avoid deterioration/leaving the task
1.2. Extraversion and Neuroticism, and satisfaction at work incomplete (Mastery-Avoid) (Baranik, Barron, & Finney, 2007;
Elliot & McGregor, 2001). Although there is a relative paucity of
In examining the relation between achievement orientations research directly addressing the achievement orientation-job satis-
and job satisfaction, it is important to consider incremental validity faction relationship, Joo and Park (2010) found performance-orien-
beyond broad aspects of personality that are already known to pre- tation predicted career satisfaction (a workplace-satisfaction
dict this outcome. Extraversion and Neuroticism – the ‘Big Two’ variable) but no relations for mastery-orientation. Janssen and
personality traits that appear in most major personality taxono- Van Yreren (2004), and Van Yperen and Janssen (2002) found a
mies – play significant roles in explaining job satisfaction positive correlation between a mastery-orientation, but no relation
(Barrick & Mount, 1991). Indeed, in their influential review of per- for performance-orientation, and global job satisfaction. These
sonality and significant life outcomes, Ozer and Benet-Martinez researchers only examined the original two-factor (mastery/per-
(2006) note that, while other traits are more salient predictors of formance) framework without considering motivational directions
‘‘how well one does at work’’, Extraversion and Neuroticism are (approach/avoid). Furthermore, these researchers examined satis-
the most salient predictors of ‘‘how one feels about work’’. Extra- faction at the global level. Some job characteristics encompassed
version positively relates, and Neuroticism negatively relates, to at this level may not be equally important to individuals with dif-
global satisfaction at work (Connolly & Viswesvaran, 2000; ferent achievement orientations. Considering the dimensions of
Furnham et al., 2002; Judge & Bono, 2001; Judge, Higgins, EX-JS and P-JS addresses this; how achievement orientations might
Thoresen, & Barrick, 1999). Judge et al. (2002) estimated true score differentially relate to satisfaction concerned with ones ‘job expec-
correlations with job satisfaction of .29 for Neuroticism and .25 tations’ (EX-JS) compared to a more self-orientated satisfaction
for Extraversion, generalising across studies. These observations dimension (P-JS), is now outlined.
are highly consistent with findings in the subjective well-being lit- Mastery-Approach has been found to beneficially relate to
erature that Extraversion positively relates, and Neuroticism nega- intrinsic interest in learning and training (Fisher & Ford, 1998),
tively relates, to overall satisfaction with life (e.g., DeNeve & higher worker self-efficacy (Phillips & Gully, 1997) and job perfor-
Cooper, 1998). A comprehensive theoretical explanation for these mance (Payne et al., 2007). With an incremental view on skill
associations is currently lacking, however these associations are development, Mastery-Approach individuals view the exertion of
potentially explained by the susceptibility of extraverts to positive effort (in the pursuit of competence development) as an indication
affective experiences and of highly-neurotic individuals to nega- of success in itself, allowing one to gain enjoyment from their
tive affective experiences (Elliot & Thrash, 2002; Larsen & efforts (Duda, 2001; Dweck, 1999; Elliot, 1999). As such, it is
Ketelaar, 1991; Watson, Weise, Vaidya & Tellegen, 1999). Our expected that such individuals will be disposed to experience high
aim in this study is not to evaluate a particular theory of the rela- self-referential satisfaction (P-JS), and, given their approach like
tion between personality and job satisfaction, but rather to exam- temperament, to make more positive judgments of expectation
ine the predictive validity of achievement orientation above and fulfilment (EX-JS).
beyond personality variables that have a known link with job sat- Mastery-Avoid has been found to be associated with less adap-
isfaction. For our first hypotheses, we predict that the associations tive outcomes including worry and test anxiety (Elliot & McGregor,
of Extraversion and Neuroticism with global job satisfaction will 2001). Mastery-Avoid, like Mastery-Approach, stems from the per-
generalise to the more specific conceptualisations of job satisfac- ception that skills are malleable (incremental view) and predomi-
tion, EX-JS and P-JS. nantly under one’s control (Elliot & McGregor, 2001). Mastery-
Avoid individuals can also be considered to view the maintenance
Hypothesis 1. Extraversion is positively related, whereas Neurot- of their competence (and ultimately their own performance) to
icism is negatively related, to EX-JS. result from their exertion of effort (Elliot & McGregor, 2001) sug-
gesting Mastery-Avoid individuals would be likely to perceive their
own exerted effort as indicative of success. This encourages the
Hypothesis 2. Extraversion is positively related, whereas Neuroti-
perception that Mastery-Avoid individuals would score high on a
cism is negatively related, to P-JS.
self-referential satisfaction variable like P-JS. However, with the
observed associations between Mastery-Avoid and less affective
1.3. Employee achievement orientations and satisfaction at work outcomes, and with other research finding theoretically meaning-
ful links between negative affectivity and avoidance like tempera-
Dweck (1986) proposed individuals can be mastery or perfor- ments (Elliot & Thrash, 2002), one might expect that the avoidance
mance orientated; those with a mastery-orientation are typically nature of this dimension would generally contribute to the ten-
concerned with developing their competence and acquiring new dency to have more diminished levels of satisfaction (at a less
skills; those with a performance-orientation are focused on the self-oriented level) when thinking about expectations being met
demonstration and verification of their ability (Elliot & Dweck, at work (EX-JS).
58 R.E. Avery et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 76 (2015) 56–61

Performance-Approach has been found to relate to various out- 2.2. Measures


comes including the use of adaptive learning strategies in situations
where this is necessary to outperform others (Linnenbrink & 2.2.1. Personality
Pintrich, 2002), persistence where success is likely (Harackiewicz, The Eysenck Personality Questionnaire-Revised (short form)
Barron, Carter, Lehto, & Elliott, 1997), and job performance (EPQ-RS; Eysenck, Eysenck, & Barrett, 1985) was used to measure
(Payne et al., 2007). Given that those high in this motivational ori- Extraversion (11-items) and Neuroticism (12-items). The Extraver-
entation view abilities as fixed (entity view), for them working sion subscale (a = .81in the present data set) includes items such as
hard actually signifies low ability in the attempt to meet normative ‘Are you a talkative person?’, and the Neuroticism subscale (a = .85
standards (Dweck, 1999). One might expect as such, that Perfor- in the present data set) includes items such as ‘Does your mood often
mance-Approach individuals would be likely to experience self- go up and down?’. Participants were required to respond yes or no
orientated satisfaction at work (P-JS) as means of endorsing their to items presented.
competence to have met required standards of performance. Such
individuals, given their approach like temperament, are also
2.2.2. Achievement orientation
expected (alike Mastery-Approach individuals) to make more posi-
Elliot and McGregor’s (2001) Achievement Goals Questionnaire
tive judgments on expectation fulfilment (EX-JS).
(3 items per achievement dimension) was used to assess mastery-
Performance-Avoid refers to the withdrawal of effort in the face
approach (a = .85 in the present data set; e.g., ‘it is important for me
of failure (Dweck & Leggett, 1988), the experience of negative
to understand things as thoroughly as possible’), mastery-avoidance
affect following failure (Turner & Meyer, 1999), and lower intrinsic
(a = .75 in the present data set; e.g., ‘I worry that I may not learn
motivation and poor performance (Elliot & Church, 1997;
all that I possibly could’), performance-approach (a = .79 in the pres-
Middleton & Midgley, 1997). It is the undesirable combination of
ent data set; e.g., ‘it is important for me to do better than others’), and
the avoidance orientation along with the theorised roots to view-
performance-avoidance scale (a = .69 in the present data set; e.g.,
ing skills as being fixed and out of one’s control (entity view) that
‘my fear of performing poorly is often what motivates me’). For the
contributes to the observed association with negative outcomes.
purpose of the current research all items were adapted with the
On the basis of this one might expect those high in this disposition
word ‘work’ where appropriate in order to make items more con-
to generally experience dissatisfaction with their own perfor-
text specific. Participants responded on a scale from 1 (strongly
mance, and expectations having been met, at work.
disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).
In sum, the following hypotheses are suggested:

Hypothesis 3. The approach dimensions of mastery and perfor- 2.2.3. Expectation based job satisfaction
mance orientation are positively related, whereas the avoidance A 5-item scale developed by Bacharach et al. (1991) (a = .85 in
dimensions of mastery and performance orientation are negatively the present data set) was used with participants responding on a
related, to EX-JS. scale from 1 (very dissatisfied) to 7 (very satisfied), to the items
such as ‘your present job in light of your career expectations’ and
‘your present job when you compare it to jobs in other organisations’
Hypothesis 4. The approach and avoidance dimensions of mastery following the headed question ‘how satisfied or dissatisfied are you
orientation, and performance-approach, are positively related, with. . .’.
whereas performance-avoidance is negatively related, to P-JS.
Personality only partly accounts for criterion variance in work-
related outcomes (Paunonen & Ashton, 2001). Extraversion and 2.2.4. Satisfaction with one’s own job performance
Neuroticism are predictors of satisfaction at work, however these A 2-item scale developed by Steele-Johnson et al. (2000) (a = .73
traits have also been viewed as components of broad motivational in the present data set) was used with items being taken form a lar-
(achievement) constructs (see Elliot & Thrash, 2002). Given that ger performance perception scale developed by Steele-Johnson
the current research is drawing upon a social-cognitive perspective et al. (2000) previously found to demonstrate good internal reli-
to explain satisfaction in the workplace, the extent to which more ability (Steele-Johnson et al., 2008; a = .80). So as to make the
comprehensive measures of achievement orientations can explain items specific to the context of the current research, items were
unique variance in both satisfaction outcomes beyond that adapted for example, ‘I am satisfied with my overall performance
explained by Extraversion and Neuroticism is of interest. Accord- on the task during this session’ became ‘I am satisfied with my overall
ingly, the following two hypotheses are suggested: job performance at present’. Items were scored on a scale from 1
(strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).
Hypothesis 5. Achievement orientation relates incrementally to
EX-JS over and above Extraversion and Neuroticism. 2.3. Procedure

Hypothesis 6. Achievement orientation relates incrementally to P- Paper copies of questionnaires were distributed and displayed
JS over and above Extraversion and Neuroticism. (along with a poster outlining voluntary survey, all welcome) in
participating departments. A total of 400 paper questionnaires
were initially distributed, and thus a response rate of 60.5% was
2. Method achieved. Participants were instructed in the questionnaire docu-
ment to post completions to an allocated collector within the orga-
2.1. Participants nisation’s internal mail system (envelopes were provided).
Participation was voluntary and the opportunity to participate
Participants were 242 UK government employees (50.4% male). lasted for 2 weeks. Completion of a questionnaire took approxi-
Age was recorded in 1 of 6 possible ranges with a modal age range mately 15 min and employees were given the opportunity to com-
of 46–55 years, accounting for 36.4% of the sample (M = 3.21, plete the questionnaire during working hours. Each questionnaire
SD = 1.18). On average, participants had worked for their organisa- contained a detachable information sheet assuring participants of
tion for 9.1 years (SD = 7.45) (85.1% full time, 14.9% part time confidentiality and providing them with full contact details of
workers). the primary researchers.
R.E. Avery et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 76 (2015) 56–61 59

3. Results of v2(171) = 326.77, p < 0.01, NNFI = 0.92, CFI = 0.94, RMSEA = 0.06,
SRMR = 0.07, suggesting moderate levels of fit. The reduced form
Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics and correlations for squared multiple correlations for EX-JS and P-JS respectively were
all variables. To assess the relationships between the personality 0.13 and 0.19.
variables, the achievement orientation variables and the two job At the second stage the direct paths between the four achieve-
satisfaction variables a structural model was proposed with and ment orientation measures and the two job satisfaction measures
tested in two stages. Analyses were conducted on the covariance were freely estimated yielding model fit statistics of
matrix using ML estimation (see Fig. 1). Given the large number v2(163) = 304.39, p < 0.01, Dv2(8) = 22.38, p < 0.005, NNFI = 0.92,
of indicators of the EPQ-RS variables Extraversion and Neuroticism, CFI = 0.94, RMSEA = 0.06, SRMR = 0.07 suggesting an improvement
composite scores for these variables were calculated and their in fit as a result of including the achievement orientation variables.
known reliabilities used to calculate their respective disturbance The reduced form squared multiple correlations for EX-JS and P-JS
terms. Table 2 outlines the correlations between all the observed increased to 0.14 and 0.27 respectively with Mastery-Approach
variables presented in Fig. 1. In the first stage only contributing to the increase in explained variance in P-JS only.
Extraversion and Neuroticism were allowed to predict the two Fig. 1 presents this model. Extraversion predicts both forms of
job satisfaction variables with the paths relating the achievement job satisfaction, as predicted, and Neuroticism predicts P-JS though
orientation variables fixed to be zero. This model yielded fit indices not EX-JS. Of note is the unanticipated positive relationship

Table 1
Descriptive statistics and correlations among study variables.

Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. Extraversion 6.89 3.18 –
2. Neuroticism 4.34 3.53 .25 –
3. Mastery-approach 10.69 2.97 .25 .01 –
4. Mastery-avoid 9.09 2.77 .09 .22 .14 –
5. Performance-approach 8.93 2.91 .18 .01 .24 .05 –
6. Performance-avoid 9.24 2.62 .10 .18 .04 .23 .07 –
7. P-JS 9.10 3.42 .27 .08 .35 .02 .13 .06 –
8. EX-JS 20.90 7.50 .30 .02 .20 .04 .18 .03 .51 –

Note. Correlations r P |.13|, p < .05, r P |.17|, p < .01, r P |.21|, p < .001.

Fig. 1. Structural model relating personality and achievement orientation variables with job satisfaction factors. [Figures are standardised coefficients. Paths denoting
correlations between exogenous variables have been omitted for clarity. Extrav = Extraversion; neurot = Neuroticism; em1 = Elliot and McGregor (2001) item1; satperf1 = P-
JS item 1; js1 = EX-JS item 1; MASTAPP = Mastery-Approach; MASTAV = Mastery-Avoid; PERFAPP = Performance-Approach; PERFAV = Performance-Avoid].
60 R.E. Avery et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 76 (2015) 56–61

Table 2
Correlations among observed variables (as presented in Fig. 1).

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21
1. Extraversion –
2. Neuroticism .245 –
3. EM1 .184 .072 –
4. EM2 .174 .020 .569 –
5. EM3 .293 .071 .636 .759 –
6. EM4 .215 .073 .133 .165 .315 –
7. EM5 .170 .049 .130 .149 .270 .649 –
8. EM6 .092 .140 .154 .080 .199 .532 .543 –
9. EM7 .097 .158 .104 .059 .054 .023 .039 .016 –
10. EM8 .087 .176 .103 .142 .106 .014 .054 .012 .496 –
11. EM9 .029 .219 .146 .128 .067 .079 .106 .092 .424 .618 –
12. EM10 .032 .131 .022 .098 .039 .023 .072 .000 .067 .131 .252 –
13. EM11 .068 .088 .020 .061 .001 .015 .026 .133 .146 .148 .211 .323 –
14. EM12 .123 .209 .018 .006 .008 .101 .061 .159 .144 .054 .203 .370 .512 –
15. P-JS 1 .293 .024 .256 .340 .327 .199 .140 .131 .074 .028 .008 .007 .073 .100 –
16. P-JS 2 .177 .113 .187 .234 .257 .046 .085 .010 .052 .039 .034 .012 .042 .007 .562 –
17. EX-JS 1 .159 .062 .122 .121 .144 .118 .042 .020 .034 .040 .027 .014 .056 .138 .403 .465 –
18. EX-JS 2 .267 .084 .148 .134 .099 .035 .003 .083 .004 .021 .028 .120 .053 .018 .291 .287 .600 –
19. EX-JS 3 .250 .083 .068 .118 .084 .112 .005 .036 .076 .009 .014 .060 .068 .044 .357 .311 .560 .684 –
20. EX-JS 4 .209 .050 .164 .134 .167 .279 .364 .251 .032 .066 .044 .150 .024 .054 .364 .296 .421 .440 .524 –
21. EX-JS 5 .288 .068 .209 .169 .259 .209 .120 .069 .071 .031 .009 .060 .062 .051 .419 .381 .573 .590 .572 .449 –

Note. EM1 = Elliot and McGregor (2001) item 1; P-JS 1 = P-JS item 1; EX-JS 1 = EX-JS item 1. Correlations r P |.13|, p < .05, r P |.17|, p < .01.

between Neuroticism and P-JS which is discussed below. The their efforts to increase competence. No significant relations for
model suggests that of the achievement orientation variables only any of the other achievement orientations (Mastery-Avoid, Perfor-
Mastery-Approach has an independent contribution to the vari- mance-Approach, Performance-Avoid) with either satisfaction out-
ance in P-JS over and above that of Extraversion and Neuroticism. come were found. Thus the previously supported links between
Mastery-Avoid and Performance-Avoid with less affective out-
comes (Baranik et al., 2007; VandeWalle, 2003), were not sup-
4. Discussion ported when considering two different facets of job satisfaction.
Likewise, there was no support for previous findings for Perfor-
The current research aimed to extend research into personality mance-Approach being positively related to more affective out-
and job satisfaction by exploring the additive value of achievement comes in the workplace (Harackiewicz et al., 1997; Linnenbrink
orientations in explaining different forms of satisfaction at work. & Pintrich, 2002; Payne et al., 2007). Analyses did however further
Only partial support was found for the predicted relations. First, reveal that after controlling for Extraversion and Neuroticism, Mas-
Extraversion was found to positively relate to both EX-JS and P- tery-Approach was incrementally related to P-JS. This relation was
JS, which is in line with the robust association this trait has with modest, but provides some support in favour of the previous prop-
overall satisfaction at work dimensions (Connolly & Viswesvaran, ositions of researchers (Judge & Locke, 1993), that moving beyond
2000; Furnham et al., 2002; Judge & Bono, 2001; Judge et al., broad personality domains and drawing upon an existing social-
1999). Second, no support was found for the predicted negative cognitive perspective to understand the dispositional source of sat-
relation between Neuroticism and both of our satisfaction mea- isfaction in the workplace, is beneficial.
sures. This finding contradicts previous findings of a negative rela-
tion between Neuroticism and satisfaction in the workplace
(Connolly & Viswesvaran, 2000; Furnham et al., 2002; Judge & 4.1. Limitations, implications and future directions
Bono, 2001; Judge et al., 1999). Neuroticism may not relate to
how satisfied one is in the context of their expectations about Current findings confirm that Extraversion and Neuroticism
work, simply because their expectations are not positive or opti- play a role in the experience of satisfaction in the workplace, but
mistic in the first place. In addition, low satisfaction with one’s furthermore that the achievement orientation of Mastery-
own performance might reflect the belief that one has scope for Approach plays an incremental role in explaining who is more or
harder work and effort. As neurotic individuals tend to score lower less likely to be satisfied with their job performance. The present
on measures of organisational commitment and higher on mea- research also investigated, for the first time, both approach and
sures of burnout and fatigue (Ozer & Benet-Martinez, 2006), they avoidance forms of achievement orientations as antecedents of sat-
may be less likely to endorse this belief. This may explain why isfaction at work. A novel finding was the observed positive rela-
more neurotic individuals indicated that they were more satisfied tion of Neuroticism to P-JS. Identification of this relationship in
with their own performance in the current sample. Future research the current research illustrates that when different forms of satis-
would benefit from exploration of this these possibilities. faction are considered, a deeper understanding of the dispositional
Mastery-Approach was only positively related to P-JS, sharing source of satisfaction at work can be considered. Importantly,
no relationship with EX-JS. The dispositional tendency to focus although the value of considering EX-JS and P-JS rather than global
on development of competence through task mastery appears to job satisfaction are implied in the current work, no global measure
play a more significant role in the experience of self-referential sat- was taken to substantiate this. Empirical evidence for the distinc-
isfaction at work. This observed relationship with P-JS is consistent tiveness of EX-JS and P-JS relative to global satisfaction is neces-
with the theoretical propositions of Dweck (1986) that because sary. It is important to note that more empirical evidence for the
mastery-orientated individuals hold an incremental theory about reliability and validity of the two item P-JS measure within an
their self-attributes being developable and under their own con- organisational rather than lab context, in particular, is also
trol, they are more likely to derive enjoyment from exertion of essential.
R.E. Avery et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 76 (2015) 56–61 61

Findings specific to Mastery-Approach in the current research Eysenck, S. B. G., Eysenck, H. J., & Barrett, P. (1985). A revised version of the
psychoticism scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 21–29.
contribute to the growing consensus of this construct’s practical
Fisher, S. L., & Ford, J. K. (1998). Differential effects of learner effort and goal
value in understanding behavioural and emotional manifestations orientation on two learning outcomes. Personnel Psychology, 51, 397–420.
in the workplace (Steele-Johnson et al., 2000). On the basis of such Furnham, A., Petrides, K. V., Jackson, C. J., & Cotter, T. (2002). Do personality factors
an understanding and given that one’s achievement orientation is predict job satisfaction? Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 1325–1342.
Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Carter, S. M., Lehto, A. T., & Elliot, A. J. (1997).
found to be fairly stable over time (Chen, Gully, Whiteman, & Predictors and consequences of achievement goals in the college classroom:
Kilcullen, 2000), practical implications can be considered to also Maintaining interest and making the grade. Journal of Personality and Social
hold at a personnel selection level. With the aim of selecting Psychology, 73, 1284–1295.
Janssen, O., & Van Yreren, N. W. (2004). Employees goal orientations, the quality of
employees more likely to experience the affective state of satisfac- leader–member exchange and the outcomes of job performance and
tion with their own performance, organisations might benefit from satisfaction. Academy of Management Journal, 47, 368–384.
using selection methods that measure a prospective employee’s Joo, B., & Park, S. (2010). Career satisfaction, organizational commitment, and
turnover intention. The effects of goal orientation, organizational learning
Mastery-Approach tendencies. In sum, the current findings provide culture and developmental feedback. Leadership and Organization Development
an important illustration of how satisfaction at work can be linked Journal, 31, 482–500.
with stable competence specific motivational tendencies. Judge, T. A., & Bono, J. E. (2001). Relationship of core self-evaluations traits, self-
esteem, generalized self-efficacy, locus of control, and emotional stability with
job satisfaction and job performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied
References Psychology, 86, 80–92.
Judge, T. A., Heller, D., & Mount, M. K. (2002). Five-factor model of personality and
Bacharach, B. P., Bamberger, P., & Conley, S. (1991). Work-home conflict among job satisfaction: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 530–541.
nurses and engineers: Mediating the impact of role stress on burnout and Judge, T. A., Higgins, C. A., Thoresen, C. J., & Barrick, M. R. (1999). The big five
satisfaction at work. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 12, 1239–1253. personality traits, general mental ability, and career success across the life span.
Bacharach, S., & Mitchell, S. (1982). The quality of working life of professional, Personnel Psychology, 52, 621–652.
technical, and scientific employees of New York State. NYSSILR, Cornell University, Judge, T. A., & Locke, E. A. (1993). Effect of dysfunctional thought processes on
Ithaca, N.Y: Report to the Joint Committee on Professional Development and subjective well-being and job satisfaction. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78,
QWL. 475–490.
Baranik, L. E., Barron, K. E., & Finney, S. J. (2007). Measuring goal orientation in a Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and susceptibility to positive and
work domain: Construct validity evidence for the 2  2 framework. Educational negative emotional states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61(1),
and Psychological Measurement, 67, 697–718. 132.
Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The big five personality dimensions and job Linnenbrink, E. A., & Pintrich, P. R. (2002). Motivation as an enabler for academic
performance. Personnel Psychology, 44, 1–26. success. School Psychology Review, 31, 313–327.
Chen, G. M., Gully, S. M., Whiteman, J., & Kilcullen, R. N. (2000). Examination of Middleton, M., & Midgley, C. (1997). Avoiding the demonstration of lack of ability:
relationships among trait-like individual differences, state-like individual An under-explored aspect of goal theory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89,
differences, and learning performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 710–718.
835–847. Nicholls, J. G. (1989). The competitive ethos and democratic education. Cambridge,
Churchill, G., Ford, N., & Walker, J. (1979). Personal characteristics of salespeople MA: Harvard University Press.
and the attractiveness of alternative rewards. Journal of Business Research, 7, Ozer, D. J., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2006). Personality and the prediction of
25–50. consequential outcomes. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 401–421.
Connolly, J. J., & Viswesvaran, C. (2000). The role of affectivity in job satisfaction: A Paunonen, S. V., & Ashton, M. C. (2001). Big five factors and facets and the prediction
meta-analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 265–281. of behaviour. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 524–539.
DeShon, R. P., & Gillespie, J. Z. (2005). A motivated action theory account of goal Payne, S. C., Youngcourt, S., & Beaubien, J. M. (2007). A meta-analytic examination of
orientation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 1096–1127. the goal orientation nomological net. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 128–150.
DeNeve, K. M., & Cooper, H. (1998). The happy personality: A meta-analysis of 137 Phillips, J. M., & Gully, S. M. (1997). Role of goal orientation, ability, need for
personality traits and subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 124(2), 197. achievement, and locus of control in the self-efficacy and goal-setting process.
Duda, J. L. (2001). Achievement goal research in sport: Pushing the boundaries and Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 792–802.
clarifying some misunderstandings. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Advances in motivation Steele-Johnson, D., Beauregard, R. S., Hoover, P. D., & Schmidt, A. M. (2000). Goal
in sport and exercise (pp. 129–182). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics Books. orientation and task demand effects on motivation, affect and performance.
Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 724–738.
Psychologist, 41, 1040–1048. Steele-Johnson, D., Heintz, P., & Miller, C. E. (2008). Examining situationally induced
Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-Theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and state goal orientation effects on task perceptions, performance, and satisfaction:
development. Philadelphia, PA: The Psychology Press, Taylor and Francis Group. A two-dimensional conceptualization. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 38,
Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and 334–365.
personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256–273. Turner, J., & Meyer, D. (1999). Integrating classroom context into motivation theory
Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation and achievement goals. and research: Rationales, methods and implications. Advances in Motivation and
Educational Psychologist, 34, 169–189. Achievement, 11, 87–121.
Elliot, A. J., & Church, M. A. (1997). A hierarchical model of approach and avoidance Van Yperen, N. W., & Janssen, O. (2002). Fatigued and dissatisfied or fatigued but
achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, satisfied? Goal orientations and responses to high job demands. Academy of
218–232. Management Journal, 45, 1161–1171.
Elliot, E. S., & Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach to motivation and VandeWalle, D. (2003). A goal orientation model of feedback-seeking behaviour.
achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 5–12. Human Resource Management Review, 13, 581–604.
Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2  2 achievement goal framework. Journal Watson, D., Weise, D., Vaidya, J., & Tellegen, A. (1999). The two general activation
of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 501–519. systems of affect: Structural findings, evolutionary considerations, and
Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Approach-avoidance motivation in personality psychobiological evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(5),
approach and avoidance temperaments and goals. Journal of Personality and 820–838.
Social Psychology, 82, 804–818.

You might also like