Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 18

Sijun Wang, Sharon E.

Beatty, & Jeanny Liu

Employees’ Decision Making in the


Face of Customers’ Fuzzy Return
Requests
Frontline service employees frequently encounter customers’ fuzzy requests, defined as requests that are slightly
or somewhat outside company policy but not completely unacceptable or detrimental to the company. Employees’
compliance decisions can profoundly affect customers, organizations, and employees themselves. However, the
complex decision process in which service employees engage is largely unexplored. The authors draw from script
and motivated reasoning theories, as well as qualitative interviews, to model employees’ responses to customers’
fuzzy requests in a retail setting. The results, which are based on a national survey of retail employees, indicate
that employees with higher customer orientation and higher conflict avoidance tend to handle fuzzy return requests
in a friendlier, more effortful manner, especially when customers demonstrate an affiliative style. In contrast, when
customers display a dominant style, employees engage in motivated reasoning and perceive the request to be less
legitimate, reducing their likelihood of complying. In addition, the employees’ perceived flexibility influences their
compliance decisions, but punishment expectations do not. The authors conclude with some managerial
implications, including better identification of these requests and more training of employees to handle them
appropriately.

T
Keywords: fuzzy requests, compliance, script theory, role theory, motivated reasoning, retail returns, legitimacy

he marketing literature has long recognized the impor- a table with a view. While some requests may be innocent
tant roles of frontline employees in service delivery problems (e.g., returning a failed product bought from the
and recovery (e.g., Bitner, Booms, and Tetreault store), others may involve intentionally taking advantage of
1990; Schneider 1991). According to Langeard et al.’s a service provider (e.g., returning a dress after wearing it for
(1981) Servuction Service Model, the quality of service a special occasion [e.g., Schmidt et al. 1999]).
performance depends on the interaction and performance of In contrast to both the common organization assumption
three key players in the delivery process: the service organi- that customers are always right (e.g., Ringberg, Odekerken-
zation system, the contact employee, and the customer. In Schroder, and Christensen 2007) and the emphasis in cur-
this relationship, customers are motivated to compete with rent literature on problem or fraudulent customers, in which
the firm and its employees to seize control of the service customers or their actions are completely wrong (e.g., Har-
process to increase their utility and personal satisfaction ris 2010), many customers’ requests lie between these
(Bateson 1985). One way customers do this is by asking for extremes. These requests often are somewhat or slightly
special requests that require the employee or the firm to outside company policy, not addressed by company policy,
adapt the service to these requests (Bitner, Booms, and or outside the employee’s job description or job expecta-
Tetreault 1990). tions, but they are not unacceptable or detrimental to the
Some requests greatly deviate from the normal service company. We label these requests as “fuzzy requests,”
scope and employee expectations, while others are routine, implying that how and what employees should do regarding
such as a customer asking the host to seat his or her party at these requests are not always clear.
Although the marketing literature has not identified or
examined employees’ reactions to customers’ fuzzy requests,
their occurrence is unquestionable; many excellent service
Sijun Wang is Associate Professor of Marketing, Department of Marketing

firms, such as Ritz-Carlton and the Mayo Clinic, focus on


and Business Law, College of Business Administration, Loyola Marymount

meeting unconventional or unexpected customer requests


University (e-mail: swang15@lmu.edu). Sharon E. Beatty is Reese Phifer

(Berry and Bendapudi 2003; Michel, Bowen, and Johnston


Professor of Marketing, Department of Management and Marketing, Cul-

2008). Furthermore, several studies indicate the ubiquity of


verhouse College of Commerce and Business Administration, University

these incidents and their challenges to frontline employees


of Alabama (sbeatty@cba.ua.edu). Jeanny Liu is Assistant Professor of
Marketing, College of Business & Public Management, University of La

(e.g., Beatty et al. 1996; Bitner, Booms, and Mohr 1994). In


Verne (e-mail: jliu@laverne.edu). The authors gratefully acknowledge the

contrast to the strong presence of fuzzy requests in the mar-


comments and suggestions provided by the following people on previous

ketplace, a Strativity Group (2008) survey shows that only


versions of this article: Rich Lutz, Mark Arnold, Gianfranco Walsh, and

29% of executives believe that employees have the tools


Mark Leach. In addition, they acknowledge the helpful comments from the
three anonymous JM reviewers.

© 2012, American Marketing Association Journal of Marketing


ISSN: 0022-2429 (print), 1547-7185 (electronic) 69 Volume 76 (November 2012), 69–86
and authority to solve customer problems and to delight reasoning theory (Kunda 1990). Finally, we find that
customers. employee gender and organizational tenure moderate sev-
The current study focuses on a specific type of customer eral key relationships in the model. The findings of this
fuzzy request: fuzzy return requests in a retail context. study offer significant managerial insights for service orga-
While retail returns were reported to reach 8.9% of all nizations with regard to employees’ compliance decisions
goods sold in a year and merchandise returned to stores with customers’ fuzzy return requests. We first discuss our
reached $217 billion in 2011, only 6.6% of these returns qualitative work and literature review; then, the model and
were considered fraudulent returns (National Retail Federa- hypotheses; and finally, the study procedure and findings.
tion 2011). Thus, a significant proportion of customer
return requests are likely to be classified as fuzzy return
requests (i.e., outside policy but not completely unaccept- Theory Building and Hypotheses
able or fraudulent1), as evidenced by 19.7% of returns Qualitative Research and Theory Development
being made without receipts, even though almost every
retailer requires a receipt as proof of purchase (National The qualitative research consisted of 21 in-depth interviews
Retail Federation 2011). When customers present a fuzzy with a group of customer service representatives from a
return request to a contact employee, the employee must variety of retailers, ranging from one-store, family-owned
decide on the spot whether and how to comply with this shops to national chain retailers. In our interviews, we
request, while taking into account several important issues asked informants to recall several return incidents in which
and concerns instantaneously. What drives a contact the customer’s return request was outside the store policy
employee’s approach and decision making when dealing but not completely unacceptable or fraudulent. In the inter-
with such a customer request? What are the important fac- views, informants elaborated on why they chose to comply
tors affecting how he or she chooses to handle these or not comply with these requests. Informants had no trou-
requests and whether he or she decides to bend the rules ble retrieving one or more return incidents falling into our
(i.e., comply or not)? Understanding employee decision definition of a fuzzy request. The average length of the
making regarding these important decisions is critical to interviews was 90 minutes.
firms in their efforts to manage service encounters effec- Following Strauss and Corbin’s (1998) recommendations,
tively (Bitner, Booms, and Tetreault 1990). we first analyzed interview transcripts, preparing memos for
In addressing these questions, our study makes several each. Then, we reviewed and discussed the interviews and
important contributions. First, we model contact employees’ notes extensively. We used open-coding methods to identify
compliance decisions (both compliance process and out- concepts with common properties and dimensions. We then
come) with regard to customers’ fuzzy return requests, the clustered data pertaining to the same category together and
first such attempt in the literature. Noting that compliance developed a set of antecedents relative to employee’s com-
behaviors should be studied from both process and outcome pliance behavior (with a customer’s fuzzy return request).
perspectives, we identify antecedents relevant to the compli- We also conducted an extensive literature review focus-
ance behaviors of frontline employees. In particular, while ing on service quality issues (e.g., Parasuraman, Zeithaml,
much research effort has focused on customers’ reactions/ and Berry 1985), service failure and recovery (e.g., Smith,
perceptions to employee behaviors in service encounters Bolton, and Wagner 1999), organizational behavior (e.g.,
(e.g., Mohr and Bitner 1995; Parasuraman, Zeithaml, and Vecchio 1995), and communication (e.g., Webster and
Berry 1985), our study sheds light on how and what condi- Driskell 1983). This literature review and our qualitative
tions are conducive to contact employees acting in certain research led to the identification of four bundles of ante-
ways toward customers. In the context of fuzzy return cedents important to compliance behavior: customer factors,
requests, which conceptually resemble a service failure or request factors, organizational factors, and several individual
customer complaint encounter, we find that employees with difference factors of the employee (which we simply call
higher customer orientation (CO) and higher conflict avoid- “employee factors”). Appendix A presents the list of con-
ance (CA) tend to handle customers’ fuzzy requests in a structs and example excerpts from the in-depth interviews.
friendlier, more effortful manner, especially when cus- Our model of employee compliance decision making is
tomers demonstrate a more affiliative interaction style dur- a middle-range theory (Merton 1968), which falls between
ing the encounter. In terms of the actual outcome, we find substantive and more formal theories (Glaser and Strauss
that employees are more likely to comply with fuzzy 1967). Our goal was to develop a framework through
requests when they have more flexibility but that punish- grounded theory (Strauss and Corbin 1998) that represents
ment expectations do not influence these decisions. a view of the important variables and their interrelation-
Furthermore, a customer’s dominant style reduces the ships relative to the topic.
employee’s perceptions of the request’s legitimacy and his
or her willingness to comply with it. Differential findings Theoretical Background: Compliance Outcome
regarding customers’ affiliative versus dominant styles and Process
enrich the literature based on script theory and motivated Researchers have paid little attention to compliance behav-
ior in marketing, and the term has widely varying meanings
1We do not include requests outside the employee’s job descrip- (e.g., Joshi 2010; Payan and McFarland 2005). Our study
tion as part of customer return requests, because these requests are examines service employees’ compliance behavior with
likely to be part of the frontline employee’s job description. respect to customers’ fuzzy return requests. By definition,

70 / Journal of Marketing, November 2012


compliance is demonstrated when a party displays “overt Script Theory and Overview of Framework
behavioral adherence” (Brill 1994, p. 212). Overt behav-
Role and script theories explain many frontline employees’
ioral adherence to a customer’s request (e.g., exchanging an
behaviors (e.g., Walker, Churchill, and Ford 1975). In an
item, giving store credit or cash back), which is a compli-
organizational context, an employee’s role is his or her per-
ance outcome, is an important component of service
ceptions of what the role senders expect of the employee.
employees’ reactions to customers’ requests. Moreover, it is
Role senders, such as organizations (e.g., supervisors,
equally important to understand how employees actually
coworkers) and customers, provide input to individuals,
interact (i.e., the compliance process) with the customer
allowing them to understand or interpret their roles more
during service encounters (Bitner, Booms, and Tetreault
accurately. In particular, service firms use service scripts to
1990); however, little research systematically studies what
standardize customer–firm interactions. However, in our
drives these critical behaviors.
study context, organizations have difficulty prescribing
In the service encounter literature, researchers view ser-
vice quality as being composed of two dimensions: the ser- exactly how an employee should handle a customer’s
vice outcome and service process (e.g., Mohr and Bitner request given the variance possible (Hartline and Ferrell
1995; Parasuraman, Zeithaml, and Berry 1985). Broadly 1996). Thus, the compliance process is largely discretionary
speaking, the service outcome encompasses all results or with regard to the employee’s interaction with a customer
consequences associated with a service encounter, while the involving a fuzzy request (Blancero and Johnson 2001).
service process refers to the way service transactions are The employee–customer dyad interaction affects how
accomplished (Mohr and Bitner 1995). Similarly, service the employee handles the return given the proximal one-on-
recovery is understood to include both process and outcome one contact involved. Thus, customer interaction style
variables (e.g., Smith and Bolton 2002; Smith, Bolton, and (affiliation) and several employee factors (e.g., CO, CA)
Wagner 1999). Researchers have connected process variables should jointly determine the employee’s level of effort and
to procedural and interactional justice and outcome variables friendliness with regard to the request (see Figure 1).
to distributive justice. Thus, distinguishing a compliance In contrast to the processing of the return request, the
outcome and compliance process behaviors is important. employee’s decision making with regard to complying with
For our study, “compliance outcome” is the extent to the request (i.e., compliance outcome) is scripted more eas-
which an employee believes that he or she has met the ily and involves less discretionary behavior. Therefore, the
expectations of the customer rather than simply whether the expectations of relevant role senders, including the organi-
return was accepted (e.g., a customer may want a refund but zation and the customer, along with the employee’s percep-
the employee only provides store credit, thus not fully meet- tions of the specific request (and its legitimacy), will jointly
ing the customer’s expectations). Consistent with Mohr and influence the extent to which an employee fulfills the
Bitner’s (1995) broader definition of service process, “com- request (though employee factors will not play a significant
pliance process” is the way the employee handles the return role). Thus, our model indicates that a customers’ interac-
transaction and, for this study, is composed of two key tion style (affiliation), the legitimacy of the request, and
behavioral factors: effort and friendliness. Researchers have several organizational factors (both formal and informal)
consistently suggested that these factors are important com- produce the employee’s compliance outcome. We discuss
ponents of service delivery and/or the recovery process the antecedents for the compliance process and compliance
(e.g., Hui et al. 2004; Mohr and Bitner 1995). outcome next.
Employee effort. According to Mohr and Bitner (1995), Employee Factors
employee effort represents the amount of energy put into a
behavior or series of behaviors. Customers, as observers in As we noted previously, employee factors are important dri-
the service encounter, use employee effort to derive their vers of the discretionary compliance process (Hartline and
attribution of the service experience, as well as their percep- Ferrell 1996). An increasing number of studies consistently
tions of the service quality of the firm and their satisfaction show that employees with certain individual difference
with the experience. As Mohr and Bitner (1995, p. 242) characteristics display common behaviors when delivering
note, “when a service outcome is favorable, a high level of customer service (e.g., Auh et al. 2011). Thus, on the basis
employee effort may enhance satisfaction; on the other of our qualitative work and literature review, two important
hand, when the customer does not get the desired outcome, individual difference variables emerged as important in the
a low level of employee effort may cause additional aggra- context of this study: CO and CA. The following subsec-
vation to an already unsatisfactory encounter.” tions elaborate on these variables.
Employee friendliness. Emotional contagion unques- Customer orientation (CO). Brown et al. (2002, p. 111)
tionably exists in customer–employee encounters. This define employees’ CO as an “employee’s tendency or pre-
theory suggests that employee friendliness is an important disposition to meet customer needs in an on-the-job con-
component of the customers’ overall experience. Indeed, text.” It is positively related to customer satisfaction (e.g.,
research in the communication field indicates that employ- Reynierse and Harker 1992), worker performance (Brown
ees’ friendly, helpful styles elicit greater customer satisfac- et al. 2002), and job responses. Compared with employees
tion with the employee and firm (e.g., Buller and Buller with lower CO, employees with higher CO are more intrin-
1987), and researchers link it to the service recovery sically motivated to make customers happy and to go the
process (Blodgett, Hill, and Tax 1997; Hui et al. 2004). extra mile to meet their needs (Brown et al. 2002). Thus,

Customers’ Fuzzy Return Requests / 71


FIGURE 1
Employee Compliance Decision-Making Model

Emp
Employee
loyee F
Factors
actors Compliance
Compliance Be
Behaviors
haviors

• Customer Orientation
• Conflict Avoidance
H1

Compliance
Process
C
Customer
ustomer IInteraction
nteraction S
Styles
tyles

H2
Affiliation

R equest Le
Request gitimacy
Legitimacy
Dominance
H3
• Regulative
O rganizational F
Organizational actors
Factors H4a-c •H3 Normative H5 Compliance
• Cognitive Outcome
H6
• Norm of Flexibility
• !
Punishment Expectations

employees with higher CO will work harder to please the such as attentiveness, relaxed, open, and friendly styles. In
customer, regardless of the compliance outcome. the context of an employee–customer interaction, “affilia-
tive style” refers to the employee’s perception that a cus-
H1a: There is a positive relationship between an employee’s
CO and his or her friendliness and effort in handling the tomer engages with him or her in a warm, friendly manner.
fuzzy request (i.e., compliance process behaviors). In contrast, “dominant style” refers to the employee’s per-
ception that a customer is trying to control or dominate the
Conflict avoidance (CA). Researchers in psychology interaction. Emotional contagion theory (Hatfield, Cacioppo,
have extensively studied CA, which is an attempt to protect and Rapson 1994) proposes a direct link between employee
the self from conflict, disapproval, and negative attention and customer by affective transfer during interpersonal con-
(Rahim 1983). People who have a CA personality prefer not tact. In the service encounter literature, Dallimore, Sparks,
to assert themselves to preserve rapport and smooth rela- and Butcher (2007) demonstrate that angry customers often
tionships with others (Schroeder 1965). Oddly enough, produce similar facial displays and affective states in ser-
although marketers have studied group CA in the context of vice workers through an emotional contagion process. In an
marketing strategy decision making (Atuahene-Gima and even broader sense, each party of the client–service
Murray 2004), researchers have not linked individual provider dyad influences the other party’s behavior (Ma and
employees’ CA to their handling of customer complaints or Dubé 2011). For example, Webster (2005) finds that a cus-
requests (including return requests). Yet in our qualitative tomer evaluates an employee with a more affiliative style
interviews, informants frequently talked about trying to more favorably and is more satisfied, whereas an employee
avoid arguments with customers if possible. Thus, a posi- displaying more dominance receives less favorable cus-
tomer evaluations (Korsch and Negrete 1972). While cus-
tive relationship between an employee’s tendency to avoid
tomer interaction styles have not been associated with
conflicts and his or her compliance process is likely:
employee reactions to customers, given the dyadic, interac-
H1b: There is a positive relationship between an employee’s tive nature of customer–employee dyads, the findings
CA and his or her friendliness and effort in handling the between employee interaction styles and customer evalua-
fuzzy request (i.e., compliance process behaviors). tions should work in reverse. That is, the employee is likely
to more positively handle the request (i.e., compliance
Customer Interaction Styles process) when the customer approaches him or her with a
The social interaction model (Ben-Sira 1980) identifies two friendly, warm manner.
major interaction styles: affiliation and dominance. An affil- H2: The employee’s perceptions of the customer’s affiliative
iative style parallels Norton’s (1978) communication styles, style are related positively to his or her friendliness and

72 / Journal of Marketing, November 2012


effort in handling the fuzzy request (i.e., compliance ible, desirable, and reasonable. Consistent with Autry, Hill,
process behaviors). and O’Brien (2007) and Krapfel (1988), legitimacy draws
In addition, the employee who perceives that a customer from institutional theory and represents the extent to which
is being friendly and warm is more likely to comply with an employee perceives that a return complies with regulatory,
the customer’s request. This proposition lies in the similar normative, and cognitive expectations (Meyer and Zucker
logic of interdependency between customer and employee 1989). Thus, legitimacy encompasses three dimensions:
(or an echoing effect) as Ma and Dubé (2011) report. This regulative, normative, and cognitive legitimacy. “Regula-
idea follows from the work of Cialdini (2001), who sug- tive legitimacy” refers to the conformance of the request to
gests liking as an important persuasive tool. That is, the established return policies and procedures (i.e., whether the
employee likes the customer more (due to emotional conta- return request falls within the return period, conditions, and
gion) and so may be more willing to comply. terms set by the retailer). This type of legitimacy is closest
to the idea of whether it is or is not a fuzzy request. “Nor-
H3: The employee’s perceptions of the customer’s affiliative mative legitimacy” addresses whether the return request
style are related positively to his or her likelihood to com- conforms to commonly held social values and norms of
ply relative to the customer’s fuzzy request (i.e., his or her
compliance outcome). acceptable behavior. “Cognitive legitimacy” refers to the
appropriateness, interpretability, and comprehensibility of
In contrast, when a customer uses a dominant approach, the return request (i.e., whether it makes sense). The
the employee will need to regulate his or her outward reac- employee must judge the legitimacy of the customer’s
tions to such an approach, reducing the likelihood that he or claim according to the rationale the customer offers when
she will show anger or dismay in the situation. Thus, the returning a product, the condition of the product, and other
echoing effect occurring when the customer uses an affilia- contextual factors, such as price and the resale ability of the
tive approach is not likely to hold when the customer item.
approaches the employee in a dominant manner. Recent Although legitimacy has been studied in the context of
theorizing in dual process models has suggested that people returns in retailing (e.g., Autry, Hill, and O’Brien 2007;
will engage in various defense mechanisms to cope with Krapfel 1988) and other customer–employee interaction
uncomfortable or negative situations. As part of that contexts (e.g., Hui, Au, and Fock 2004a, b), an accurate
process, people rely on selective information and heuristics understanding of legitimacy and its impact on employees’
that help them reach a desirable conclusion while discount- decisions is still missing. For example, Hui, Au, and Fock
ing heuristics that endorse the opposing view (e.g., Giner- (2004a, b) address only cognitive legitimacy, finding a posi-
Sorolla and Chaiken 1997). Researchers refer to this goal- tive relationship between service-return legitimacy and
directed process as “motivated reasoning” (Kunda 1990). In employee compliance intention. In contrast, Krapfel (1988,
our study context, when an employee perceives that a cus- see p. 189), focusing on normative legitimacy, reports a
tomer is displaying a dominant style, which will tend to positive link between return request legitimacy and
make an employee uncomfortable, the employee may be employee attitude toward the customer. Finally, Autry, Hill,
motivated to discount the positive or even objective aspects and O’Brien (2007) and O’Brien, Hill, and Autry (2009)
of the fuzzy request (i.e., engage in biased, motivated rea- use single items for the three legitimacy aspects in their
soning). As a result, the employee will perceive the request studies on retail return episodes and the impact of legiti-
as less legitimate (i.e., farther away from policies and/or macy on employee attitude formation and role conflict.
less acceptable to the company). Such an unfavorable judg- Thus, given the inconsistent findings and weak measure-
ment of the request’s legitimacy allows the employee to ment issues in this area, further examination of the role of
decline the request more easily, which would be more legitimacy in employees’ compliance decisions is worth-
preferable to him or her. In contrast, when a customer while.
adopts an affiliative style, the motivated reasoning process Even though no previous research examines the impact
is not triggered (i.e., legitimacy perceptions are not invoked of legitimacy on actual behavior, it is clear from the previous
as a justification for their behaviors); rather, as we noted research and logic that when an employee calls into question
previously, behavior is directly affected (i.e., the compli- the legitimacy of the request (relative to any aspect), the
ance process and outcome). We now turn to a discussion of employee is less likely to comply with the request. Combin-
legitimacy. ing the previously mentioned arguments and the theory of
motivated reasoning, when a customer engages in a domi-
Legitimacy of the Request nant style in presenting a request, the employee will view
Employees’ judgments of the request’s legitimacy are the request as less legitimate and be less likely to comply
important because their interpretation of the request vis à with the request. Thus, legitimacy perceptions serve as a
vis their interpretation of company policy is the driving mediating mechanism between a customer displaying a
force behind their reactions to these requests. Previous stud- dominant style and the employee’s compliance decision.
ies on retail returns show the important role of legitimacy in Furthermore, as we noted previously, the customer’s affilia-
employee’s interpretations and reactions, though researchers tive style does not call into question the legitimacy of the
have generally defined and measured it simplistically (e.g., request (i.e., the employee does not engage in motivated rea-
Autry, Hill, and O’Brien 2007; Krapfel 1988). soning); instead, the affiliative style of the customer directly
Unless a return meets all requirements of store policy, influences the employee’s compliance behaviors. The pre-
the employee will need to judge whether the request is cred- ceding discussion leads us to hypothesize the following:

Customers’ Fuzzy Return Requests / 73


H4(a–c): The employee’s perceptions of the customer’s domi- severity increases, the level of illegal behavior should
nant style are related negatively to the employee’s per- decrease. In our study, the employee’s expectations of pun-
ceptions of the request’s (a) regulative legitimacy, (b) ishment capture his or her expectations of the negative con-
normative legitimacy, and (c) cognitive legitimacy.
sequences of complying with a fuzzy request. In the organi-
H4(d–f): The employee’s perceptions of the customer’s affilia- zational behavior literature, reward/punishment systems are
tive style are not related to the employee’s perceptions
of the request’s (d) regulative legitimacy, (e) norma-
strong communicators of desired behavior (Lawler 1987).
tive legitimacy, and (f) cognitive legitimacy. According to control theory, punishment influences and
H5: The employee’s perceptions of the (a) regulative legitimacy, shapes employee behavior (Jaworski 1988). Indeed,
(b) normative legitimacy, and (c) cognitive legitimacy of employees’ punishment expectations have been linked to
the request are related positively to his or her likelihood of reduction in various negative behaviors, such as software
complying (i.e., his or her compliance outcome). piracy, computer abuse, and Internet misuse (Liao et al.
2010; Peace, Galletta, and Thong 2003; Straub 1990).
Organizational Factors H6b: There is a positive relationship between an employee’s
In addition to employees’ personal evaluative beliefs expectations of punishment from complying with a fuzzy
regarding the customer and the request, the organization’s request and his or her likelihood of complying with the
request (i.e., his or her compliance outcome).
formal and informal control systems also affect the
employee’s motivations and behaviors. An organization’s As two interrelated aspects of a service encounter, com-
formal control system is defined as the written procedures pliance outcome and process should also be interdependent.
and policies that direct employee behavior to achieve the From the event order perspective, employees may be likely
organization’s goals and/or detect misconduct (Leatherwood to decide on their compliance outcome (i.e., to what extent
and Lee 1991), whereas an informal system molds employee customer expectations will be met) before they engage in
behavior through common values, beliefs, and traditions the process of compliance. This also follows according to
(Ouchi 1980). The informal control system allows employees self-perception theory (Bem 1967). Therefore, we add a
to develop responses to customers according to a maze of link from compliance outcome to process in the model.
unspoken and/or unwritten rules existing in the organization. Because it is not particularly interesting, this link is a con-
Our qualitative research uncovered both types of orga- trol relationship.
nizational control mechanisms. In particular, the employee’s
perceptions of the firm’s norms of flexibility are part of the Moderating Effects of Employee Gender and
informal control system, while expectations of punishment Organizational Tenure
(for going outside company policy) are part of the formal The preceding discussion presents a set of linear relation-
control system. Interestingly, this organizational classifica- ships to aid in predicting employees’ compliance behavior
tion echoes Homburg and Furst’s (2005) ideas of mechanis- with a fuzzy request. To enhance the managerial implica-
tic versus organic. The expectation of punishment repre- tions of this study, we address two highly relevant and
sents the mechanical guidelines, while the employee’s actionable moderators, employee gender and organizational
perceptions of flexibility provide the organic environment tenure, of some of the previously mentioned relationships.
with regard to the employee’s responses. Employee gender. Researchers often find gender differ-
Perceived norm of flexibility. The norm of flexibility ences in organizational settings, especially for service
involves a management style that gives employees the dis- employees (e.g., Babin and Boles 1998; Iacobucci and
cretion to make day-to-day or case-by-case decisions about Ostrom 1993). Employee gender influences how they inter-
job-related activities (Heide and John 1992) and relates to act with customers, as well as their development and main-
employee empowerment (Bowen and Lawler 1992). tenance of customer relationships (e.g., Fiske and Stevens
Whereas previous researchers have addressed the influence 1993; Iacobucci and Ostrom 1993). The most resilient sex
of empowerment on employee behavior (e.g., Hartline and role difference is a gender-based difference in schema in
Ferrell 1996), we focus on the perceived norm of flexibility which men and women evaluate their own behaviors to reg-
to capture employees’ perceptions of how much freedom ulate their attitudes and behaviors (Bem 1981). Men tend to
they believe they have in handling customer requests day to be assertive, task oriented, dominating, and authoritative
day, including fuzzy return requests. Chebat and Kollias and exhibit independent construal; women tend to be pas-
(2000) note that the increased discretion and flexibility sive, relationship oriented, submissive, and noncompetitive
derived from more empowerment is positively associated (e.g., Fiske and Stevens 1993). Moreover, women tend to
with an employee’s confidence in serving customers. Thus, maintain and nurture the interpersonal aspects of their rela-
employees who believe that they have more flexibility in tionships more than men and attempt to avoid conflicts
their jobs are more likely to comply with fuzzy requests. (Babin and Boles 1998). These sex role differences will
produce differential weights for some antecedents for male
H6a: There is a positive relationship between an employee’s versus female employees’ with regard to their compliance
perceived flexibility in handling customer requests and process and outcome.
his or her likelihood of complying with the fuzzy request
(i.e., his or her compliance outcome). H7a: The relationship between CA and employees’ friendliness
and effort in handling the fuzzy request (i.e., compliance
Expectations of punishment. Deterrence theory (Tittle process behaviors) is stronger for female employees than
1980) posits that as punishment certainty and punishment for male employees.

74 / Journal of Marketing, November 2012


Furthermore, meta-analytic evidence (e.g., Taylor and pliance process behaviors) is stronger for longer-tenure
Hall 1982) suggests gender differences in the saliency of employees than for shorter-tenure employees.
instrumentality in decision-making processes; that is, society H8b: The relationship between CA and an employee’s friendli-
socializes men to value the outcome, focusing more on instru- ness and effort in handling the fuzzy request (i.e., com-
mental behaviors and impersonal tasks and goals (Baird 1976; pliance process behaviors) is stronger for longer-tenure
employees than for shorter-tenure employees.
Rotter and Portugal 1969). In contrast, women tend to focus
on the method used to accomplish a task, suggesting a In addition, as we reasoned previously, longer-tenured
greater process orientation (Hennig and Jardim 1977; employees should have larger resource endowments. These
Iacobucci and Ostrom 1993). Thus, the echoing effect of the endowments should enable them to engage in a more delib-
customer’s affiliative style may have a stronger effect on erate reasoning process (producing greater motivated rea-
what the employee focuses on most (i.e., process for women soning) than shorter-term employees. Thus:
and outcome for men), leading to the following hypotheses:
H7b: Female employees’ perception of the customer’s affilia- H8: The perception of the customer’s dominant style is related
tive style is related more strongly to their friendliness and more strongly to the employee’s perceptions of the
effort in handling the fuzzy request (i.e., compliance request’s (c) regulative legitimacy, (d) normative legiti-
process behaviors) than male employees’ perceptions. macy, and (e) cognitive legitimacy for longer-tenure
employees than for shorter-tenure employees.
H7c: Male employees’ perception of the customer’s affiliative
style is related more strongly to their likelihood to com-
ply with the fuzzy request (i.e., compliance outcome) Control Variables
than female employees’ perceptions.
We exercise caution with regard to possible spurious and
In addition, known gender differences involving com- attenuating variables that could influence our proposed rela-
munication styles suggest that men tend to demonstrate tionships by incorporating several relevant covariates.
visual dominance and ignore communications of others These covariates include employee demographics (age and
more so than women (e.g., Dovidio et al. 1988). Thus, male education), two company variables (type of retailer and
employees are more likely to engage in dominant styles amount of returns training), and presence versus absence of
themselves, which do not complement customers’ dominant supervisor during the encounter.
styles. Therefore, compared with women, men may react
more negatively and strongly to a customer acting in a dom- Method
inant manner (i.e., butting heads with the employee), thus
causing the male employee to engage in greater motivated Sample
reasoning to decline the request.
The sample consists of employees whose job duties include
H7(d–f): The employee’s perceptions of the customer’s domi- handling customer returns in the retail industry in the
nant style is related more strongly to the employee’s United States. We tested our conceptual framework in the
perceptions of the request’s (d) regulative legitimacy,
context of retail returns because of the ubiquity and rele-
(e) normative legitimacy, and (f) cognitive legitimacy
for male than for female employees. vance of fuzzy requests in retail return encounters; how-
ever, the model could have broad applicability to fuzzy
Organizational tenure. “Organizational tenure” refers to requests beyond a returns situation.
the length of time that an individual employee has worked The researchers recruited a web panel provided by
for the organization. A study of fraudulent consumers Qualtrics, an online survey hosting company that maintains
reports that customers intentionally choose to return items an online panel of approximately 53,000 retail employees in
to younger employees because they believe that they are the United States from all 50 states. For the current study,
less knowledgeable about return policies, less experienced, Qualtrics randomly selected more than 3000 of these pan-
and thus more accommodating to these requests (Harris elists and invited them to answer the first screening ques-
2010). Indeed, previous studies have shown that longer- tion: “Are you currently employed by a retailing company?”
tenured employees have more learning and practice oppor- One thousand sixty-six panelists responded “yes” to this
tunities to engage in serving customers as job-related question. A second question asked, “Is it part of your job to
knowledge increases (Schmidt, Hunter, and Outerbridge handle customer returns?” to which 811 panelists responded
1986). Such improved capacity may enhance their motiva- “yes” and proceeded to the survey. Following the current
tions for customer-oriented tasks, such as organizational cit- best practices of preventing respondent cheating in online
izenship behaviors (e.g., helping customers and making surveys (Rogers and Michael 2009), we dropped 369
constructive suggestions), as evidenced by a recent meta- respondents (i.e., did not allow them to continue responding
analysis (Ng and Feldman 2010). Therefore, longer-tenured to survey questions and eliminated all their answers) due to
employees, who need fewer cognitive resources to perform careless responding to the first attention-checking question,
their jobs, may have more resources available to initiate and we dropped 31 more with the second attention-checking
their own actions and to engage in greater discretionary question. Thus, the final sample included 411 respondents.
efforts (e.g., Wang et al. 2011).Therefore, we hypothesize A wide variety of retailer types appear in the responses
the following: provided by respondents, from image department stores
H8a: The relationship between CO and an employee’s friendli- (23%) to specialty stores (15%), covering almost every cate-
ness and effort in handling the fuzzy request (i.e., com- gory of retailer. The respondents’ job titles were as follows:

Customers’ Fuzzy Return Requests / 75


51% of respondents were sales associates/representatives, methodology used in the main study.2 We used or adapted
35% were managers or supervisors, and the rest did not established scales whenever possible with items and
indicate their position in the company. Approximately 30% sources identified in Appendix B.
of respondents worked less than 30 hours a week (i.e., part- All items contain multiple statements with one excep-
time), and the rest worked full-time. The sample is skewed tion: A single item assesses compliance outcome, or the
in gender (69% female) due to the retail employment real- degree to which the employee believed that he or she met
ity. All age categories (approximately half younger than 40 the customer’s expectations. In this case, a single item is
years old), ethnicity (86% Caucasians), and education lev- justified because the concept is concrete and represents the
els (49% with some college education) are well represented idea well (Bergkvist and Rossiter 2007).
and similar to recent retail statistics (U.S. Census Bureau Recall that the sample involves two groups, those who
2010). With regard to job tenure, 63% had worked for their accepted the return and those who did not.3 Thus, we
current retail firm for fewer than five years, while the rest checked the covariance and structural equivalence before
had worked for the firm for more than five years. pooling the two subsamples for model testing. Following
To aid respondents in identifying a past incident fitting Steenkamp and Baumgartner (1998), we first evaluated the
a fuzzy return request, we presented explanations of three invariance of the factor loadings, factor variances, and
categories of customer return requests to the respondents. covariances across the two groups, finding evidence for the
They included the following: (1) returns that clearly fit equality of the covariances of the two subsamples.4 We then
within the store’s return policy, (2) completely unacceptable evaluated the path coefficient equivalence by comparing the
or fraudulent returns, and (3) somewhat outside the store’s free-estimated model against an equal path coefficient
return policy (e.g., a week or two past the return deadline, model in which the subsamples are forced to have equal
within the return period but without receipt). We then asked path coefficients, as hypothesized in the model. Finding no
respondents to assess the percentage of times they believed significant chi-square change (2(15) = 23.29, p = .08)
that the return requests they received belonged to each of suggests that the measures of the focal constructs and their
these categories. More than one-third of our respondents relationships are not substantively different between the
indicated that more than 20% of the returns they handle fit subsamples, allowing us to pool groups.
into this third category (i.e., “somewhat outside the store’s 2We conducted pretesting with an online survey using Qualtrics
return policy”), exemplifying the relevance of this topic.
with the goals of item reduction and scale purification. Students in
We then asked respondents to recall a recent, memo- several upper-level undergraduate marketing classes at two south-
rable incident falling into this third category (i.e., a fuzzy eastern universities earned extra credit by recruiting nonstudent
return request). To generate variance of the dependent participants for our online survey. We subjected items to exploratory
variable (compliance behavior), we randomly assigned half and confirmatory factor analyses to initially reduce the item pool.
3As we expected, the “accept” group report higher mean com-
the respondents to recall a fuzzy return incident in which
pliance outcome scores (i.e., the extent to which the employee met
they accepted the return, while we asked the other half to the customer’s expectations on a five-point scale; see Appendix A)
recall a fuzzy return in which they did not take back the than that reported by the “not accept” group (4.11 vs. 3.14, p <
item. Researchers typically use this approach in critical .001). However, we found no other contextual and employee dif-
incident technique studies to obtain a broad range of situa- ferences on the basis of t-tests between the groups.
tions (e.g., Bitner, Booms, and Mohr 1994; Holloway and 4We first estimated a baseline measurement model assuming the

Beatty 2008). To aid in more vivid recall, respondents same pattern of fixed and free parameters across the two subsam-
ples. This model fit the data well (2 = 1122.66, d.f. = 760; com-
described the reason for the return, price of the returned parative fit index [CFI] = .97, nonnormed fit index [NNFI] = .96,
item, and time the incident occurred. The top three cate- and root mean square error of approximation [RMSEA] = .048).
gories of inappropriate customer return requests were “no Next, we estimated a model that constrained the factor loadings to
receipt” (22%), “beyond the return period” (20%), and “box/ be invariant across groups and found the fit indexes for this model
packaging opened or missing” (10%). In terms of the item to be acceptable (2 = 1149.71, d.f. = 780; CFI = .97; NNFI = .96;
value involved, 35% were valued at less than $25, 26% and RMSEA = .048), with the difference in fit between this model
and the baseline measurement model insignificant (2 = 27.05,
were $25–$50, 20% were $51–$100, and the rest were d.f. = 20, p = .13). Thus, the loadings for all measurement items
higher than $100. With regard to recency, 53% reported that were invariant across samples. The third model estimated specified
the incident had occurred in the past month, 25% of the invariant factor variance and covariances in addition to invariant
incidents occurred in the past one to three months, and the factor loadings. It has acceptable fit indexes (2 = 1188.91, d.f. =
rest more than three months previously. Respondents had 845; CFI = .96; NNFI = .96; and RMSEA = .051), and the differ-
no difficulty recalling such a return request incident. ence in fit between this model and the invariant factor model is not
significant (2 = 39.20, d.f. = 65, p = .99). Thus, all correlational
Measures (covariance) relationships among model constructs, as well as
construct (factor) variances, are equal across samples. Finally, we
Appendix B presents the scales measuring the constructs estimated a fully invariant measurement model (i.e., invariant fac-
central to the study. We achieved content validity through tor loadings, invariant factor variance and covariances, and invari-
an extensive literature search and a thorough analysis of ant item error loadings). Although the indexes indicate adequate fit
(2 = 1447.68, d.f. = 875; CFI = .957; NNFI = .95; and RMSEA =
interview scripts, which allowed us to define the domains of
.057), this model’s fit differed from the fit of the covariances’
constructs of interest and to generate the items. In addition, a measurement model (2 = 258.77, d.f. = 30, p < .001). Still, the
pretest with a convenience sample of retail employees (N = invariance of the factor loadings, factor variances, and covariances
430) provided a thorough assessment of the scales and suggest that pooling the two subsamples is appropriate.

76 / Journal of Marketing, November 2012


As Bagozzi and Heatherton(1994) suggest, when deal- tomer and the employee (Crosby, Evans, and Cowles 1990)
ing with a large number of scales and items, large scales can (Cronbach’s  = .89) and selected the lowest positive corre-
be disaggregated into subscales and the composites of the lation (r = .01) between the MV and other variables to adjust
subscales treated as indicators. Thus, we conceptualize the construct correlations and statistical significance (Lindell
compliance process, composed of employee effort and and Whitney 2001). None of the significant correlations
friendliness, as a higher-order factor. Treating the compli- became insignificant after the adjustment (see Table 1), indi-
ance process as second-order constructs is justifiable con- cating that common method bias is not a serious concern here.
ceptually. First, compliance process is a superordinate con- Covariates. To account for the potential impact of
cept, meaning it is a general concept manifested by its variables not identified in our model on our hypothesized
subdimensions. Moreover, aggregating its subdimensions in relationships, we first regressed all dependent variables
our second-order model also matches its levels of abstrac- (including compliance process, compliance outcome, and
tion with the other concepts in the model (Edwards 2001).5 three aspects of legitimacy) in our model on the covariates
Next, we subjected all measures, including the second- mentioned previously, obtained the residuals (i.e., the items
order construct, to confirmatory factor analyses to assess their with covariate effects parceled out), and used these residu-
psychometric properties; the results indicate a good model als to test our model. Other researchers have adopted this
fit (2 = 721.85, d.f. = 380, p < .01; CFI = .97; NNFI = .97; method in the past (e.g., Ahearne, Bhattachary, and Gruen
and RMSEA = .047). Construct reliability and average vari- 2005). The results and conclusions remain largely
ance extracted were strong for all latent variables (see unchanged, regardless of whether we control for covariates.
Appendix B). In addition, all constructs achieved discriminant The results in Table 2 and subsequent analyses use the
validity as assessed with the variance-extracted test (Fornell residuals of focal constructs in the analysis.
and Larcker 1981). In this test, we compared the variance-
Test of Hypotheses: Main Effects
extracted estimates for the two constructs of interest with
the square of the correlation between the two constructs. Table 1 provides pairwise correlations and descriptive sta-
Two constructs have discriminant validity if their variance- tistics of our main constructs. We derived the full structural
extracted estimates are greater than their squared correlations. model from our hypotheses using the conceptual model pre-
sented in Figure 1. Table 2 presents the standardized path
Common method bias. To assess the potential for common coefficients for all relationships in the structural model. As
method bias, we applied the marker variable (MV) method Table 2 shows, our data support the proposed model (2 =
and used a scale theoretically unrelated to at least one scale 899.35, d.f. = 407, p < .01; CFI = .96; NNFI = .96; and
in the analysis as the MV, which offers a proxy for common RMSEA = .054). The hypothesized model explains 43.6% of
method variance (Lindell and Whitney 2001). We used a the variance in the current compliance process and 44.9%
three-item scale that measured the similarity between the cus- of the compliance outcome variance. Although the negative
impact of expectations of punishment on compliance out-
5Following Hair et al.’s (2009) criterion, we ran a separate con-
come did not materialize (H6b), the analyses support the
firmatory factor analysis second-order model; this model showed other main effect hypotheses.
superior predictive validity over the lower-order factor model.
Compared with the lower-order factor model, the second-order
In addition, to test the hypothesized nonsignificant
model for compliance process (2 = 120.83, d.f. = 72, p < .05) fit effects of the customer’s affiliative interaction style on
the data better; therefore, the subsequent structural model uses the employees’ perceptions of the legitimacy of the request, we
second-order measurement model for compliance process. ran a new structural equation model adding paths from affil-

TABlE 1
Correlations and Descriptive Statistics
Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
1. Compliance process .76 .12** .04 .02 –.05 –.01 –.02 .09 .04 .44 .32**
2. Compliance outcome .13** — .36** –.40** .33** .46** .40** .24** –.18** .06 –.02
3. Affiliation .05 .37** .96 –.65** .16** .33** .45** .06 –.09 .00 –.07
4. Dominance .03 –.39** –.63** .89 –.23** –.42** –.48** –.05 .05 .07 .07
5. Regulative legitimacy –.04 .34** .17** –.22** .93 .32 .17** .00 .08 –.18** –.10
6. Normative legitimacy .01 .47** .34** –.41** .33** .89 .61** .00 .00 –.05 –.11*
7. Cognitive legitimacy –.01 .41** .46** –.47** .18** .61** .88 –.04 .02 .00 –.06
8. Norm of flexibility .10 .25** .07 –.04 .01 .01 –.03 .88 –.40** .16** .17**
9. Expectations of punishment .05 –.17** –.08 .06 .09 .01 .03 –.39** .96 –.03 .08
10. Customer orientation .45** .07 .01 .08 –.17** –.04 .01 .17** –.02 .90 .42**
11. Conflict avoidance .33** –.01 –.06 .08 –.09 –.10* –.05 .18** .09 .43** .87
12. MV (fairness) –.11* –.10* –.03 –.01 .05 .09 .01 –.34** .23** –.23** –.18**
M 5.71 3.62 4.47 4.26 2.49 2.68 2.98 4.97 3.94 6.34 5.65
SD .84 1.41 1.72 1.62 1.52 1.24 1.19 1.35 1.78 .81 1.07
*p < .05.
**p < .01.
Notes: Zero-order correlations are below the diagonal; adjusted correlations for potential common method variances (Lindell and Whitney
2001) are above the diagonal. Bold numbers in the diagonal are average variance extracted for constructs.

Customers’ Fuzzy Return Requests / 77


TABlE 2
Results of Hypothesized Relationships
Main Model Organizational Tenure Moderations

78 / Journal of Marketing, November 2012


Gender Moderations
Standardized Shorter longer
Estimate Male Female (N = 177; (N = 234;
Hypothesized Relationships (t-Value) (N = 127) (N = 284) 2(1) ≤3 Years) >3 Years) 2(1)
H1a: CO Æ compliance process .52 (7.71)** H8a .46 .63 10.33**
H1b: CA Æ compliance process .19 (2.85)** H7a –.11 n.s. .39 7.42** H8b .13 .20 n.s.
H2: Affiliation Æ compliance process .16 (3.09)** H7b .18 .17 n.s.
H3: Affiliation Æ compliance outcome .12 (2.44)* H7c .21 –.09 n.s. 4.28*
H4a: Dominance Æ regulative legitimacy –.27 (–5.04)** H7d –.20 –.30 n.s. H8c –.11 –.41 6.98**
H4b: Dominance Æ normative legitimacy –.52 (–10.43)** H7e –.67 –.46 n.s. H8d –.48 –.56 n.s.
H4c: Dominance Æ cognitive legitimacy –.60 (–11.74)** H7f –.81 –.52 4.80* H8e –.65 –.56 n.s.
H5a: Regulative legitimacy Æ compliance outcome .24 (5.40)**
H5b: Normative legitimacy Æ compliance outcome .28 (5.94)**
H5c: Cognitive legitimacy Æ compliance outcome .17 (3.26)**
H6a: Flexibility Æ compliance outcome .21 (4.06)**
H6b: Expectation of punish Æ compliance outcome –.06 (–1.22)
Controlled Path:
Compliance outcome Æ compliance process .23 (4.26)**
2
Main model fit:  (407) = 899.35, p < .01; CFI = .96; NNFI = .96; and RMSEA = .054. Squared multiple correlation for compliance outcome: 44.9%; squared multiple correlation
for compliance process: 43.6%.
*p < .05.
**p < .01.
Notes: n.s. = not significant. Supported moderation hypotheses are in boldface.
iation to the three aspects of legitimacy to the primary Gender assessments. To test the gender differences pro-
model. We found that all three paths were insignificant posed, we tested a baseline model in which we freely esti-
(standard path coefficients were .03, –.08, and .11, respec- mated all hypothesized parameters for both men and
tively; p > .10) and that this model does not fit better than women. Table 2 shows separate path estimates for hypothe-
the primary model (2(3) = 1.74, p = .63), in support of sized relationships resulting from this analysis in separate
our “no effect” hypotheses (H4d–f.) columns. We then conducted nested structural equation
models in which we constrained the paths corresponding to
Follow-Up Analyses our proposed links to be equal across men and women.
Furthermore, we performed formal tests of mediation to Table 2 presents the resulting chi-square differences with 1
examine the full mediation roles of three aspects of legiti- degree of freedom between each constrained model and its
macy in the negative impacts of dominance on compliance associated freely estimated model. We applied similar
outcome. All correlation coefficients between dominance multigroup analysis procedures to the tenure variable, in
and the legitimacy elements are positive and significant which we divided tenure groups as previously noted.
(.34, .47, and .41, respectively; p < .01). Following the pro- Consistent with expectations, CA is more strongly asso-
cedures for testing full mediation using structural equation ciated with the compliance process for female than for male
modeling, as Iacobucci, Saldanha, and Deng (2007) suggest, employees (women = .39 vs. men = –.11; 2 = 7.42, p <
we first fit the direct and indirect paths simultaneously to .001), while the effect of customer affiliation on the compli-
estimate both the direct and indirect impacts of dominance ance outcome is stronger for men (men = .21 vs. women =
on compliance outcome. We then computed the z-scores for –.09; 2 = 4.28, p < .05), in support of H7a and H7c. In
the indirect impacts and checked the significance of our addition, the customer’s dominant style is more strongly
direct path coefficients. The direct path coefficient from associated with male employees’ judgments of the request’s
dominance to compliance outcome was not significant, and cognitive legitimacy than with female employees’ judg-
the z scores were significant for all hypothesized full medi- ments (men = –.81 vs. women = –52; 2 = 4.80, p < .01),
ations: dominance Æ regulative legitimacy Æ compliance in support of H7f, but the analysis did not reveal differences
outcome, dominance Æ normative legitimacy Æ compliance regarding regulative and normative legitimacy (providing
outcome, and dominance Æ cognitive legitimacy Æ com- no support for H7d and H7e).
pliance outcome. Thus, the analyses support the hypothe- Tenure assessments. Finally, a stronger association
sized full mediation role of legitimacy in the links from between employees’ CO and their compliance process
dominance to compliance outcome, in accordance with exists (longer = .63 vs. shorter = .46; 2 = 10.33, p < .001)
motivated reasoning theory. Finally, Sobel tests confirm and between customer dominance and regulative legitimacy
mediation by showing the significant indirect impact of (longer = –.41 vs. shorter = –.11; 2 = 6.98, p < .01) for
dominance on compliance outcome through regulative longer-tenure employees versus those with less tenure, in
legitimacy, normative legitimacy, and cognitive legitimacy support of both H8a and H8c. However, the proposed tenure
(z-scores were 3.68, 5.16, and 3.14, respectively). differences between the employees’ CA and their compli-
ance process and the effect of customer dominance on cog-
Tests of Moderation Effects
nitive and normative legitimacy did not materialize, provid-
Invariance assessment. Before conducting multigroup ing no support for H8b, H8d, and H8e.
analyses, we first tested whether the measures were invari-
ant across male (N = 127) and female (N = 284) employees
by estimating a hierarchy of multigroup measurement Discussion
invariance models (Steenkamp and Baumgartner 1998). We
carried out a similar procedure for the shorter-tenured Summary of Findings and Academic Contribution
groups (N = 177 with no more than three years of organiza- The current study is a first attempt at addressing the
tional tenure) versus longer-tenured groups (N = 234 with employee decision-making process when facing customers’
more than three years of organizational tenure). Our analy- fuzzy return requests. From a theory-building perspective,
ses support invariance across the groups, allowing for the this study contributes to the literature by fully modeling
subsequent moderation assessments.6 important antecedents to employees’ compliance process

6The first model estimated was a baseline measurement model RMSEA = .047), and the difference in fit between this model and
of the same pattern of fixed and free parameters. This model fit the the invariant factor model is not significant (2 = 47.64, d.f. =
data well (2 = 1163.35, d.f. = 760; CFI = .97; NNFI = .96; and 65, p = .95). Thus, all correlational (covariance) relationships
RMSEA = .051). Next, we estimated a model that constrained the among model constructs, as well as construct (factor) variances,
factor loadings to be invariant across groups and found the fit are equal across samples. Finally, we estimated a fully invariant
indexes for this model to be acceptable (2 = 1182.75, d.f. = 780; measurement model (i.e., invariant factor loadings, invariant fac-
CFI = .97; NNFI = .96; and RMSEA =.050) and the difference in tor variance and covariances, and invariant item error loadings).
fit between this model and the baseline measurement model to be Although the indexes indicate adequate fit (2 = 1313.39, d.f. =
insignificant (2 = 19.4, d.f. = 20, p = .50). Thus, the loadings for 875; CFI = .967, NNFI = .96, and RMSEA = .049), this model’s fit
all measurement items are invariant across samples. The third differs from the fit of the baseline measurement model (2 =
model estimated specified invariant factor variance and covari- 83.0, d.f. = 30, p < .01). Still, the invariance of the factor loadings,
ances in addition to invariant factor loadings. It has acceptable fit factor variances, and covariances suggest that comparing model
indexes (2 = 1230.39, d.f. = 845; CFI = .96; NNFI = .96; and paths across samples is appropriate.

Customers’ Fuzzy Return Requests / 79


behaviors and outcome decisions, including customer, suggesting that men may rely more heavily on motivated
request, employee, and organizational factors. Specifically, reasoning in assessing the request’s cognitive legitimacy,
the analyses support the positive influence of employee CO while their reported compliance outcome choices were more
and CA on employee’s compliance process behaviors, positively affected by the customers’ affiliation style (due to
including friendliness and effort, suggesting the important emotional contagion). Thus, notably, customers’ interaction
role of these individual difference variables. In addition, the styles generally influenced men more than women in their
positive influence of customers’ affiliative style on employee decision making. In contrast, CA had a lower effect on men
reactions (relative to both process and outcome) to fuzzy than women with regard to providing a friendlier, more
return requests indicates that indeed liking begets liking, effortful treatment of customers. Such rich contingency
with positive emotions being contagious or echoed back. views of employee gender differences in the presence of
However, when customers exhibit a dominant style, it fuzzy requests offer valuable management implications.
appears to trigger the employee to engage in a motivated In addition, the findings indicate that longer-tenured
reasoning process, with the employee adopting a defensive employees, who need fewer mental resources to engage in
stance and judging the customer’s request to be less legiti- typical task behaviors (given their past practice), have more
mate, thereby reducing his or her likelihood to comply with resources left over to act on their own volition than those
the request. This strong and consistent finding, across all with a shorter organizational tenure. For example, employ-
three types of legitimacy, extends the field’s understanding of ees with longer tenure (vs. shorter tenure) relied more on
employee decision making in customer–employee encoun- their own CO in handling the request (i.e., the process).
ters beyond what emotional contagion theory suggests— Furthermore, a customer’s dominant style had a stronger
namely, that the echoing mechanism primarily operates effect on their assessment of the regulative legitimacy of the
(i.e., customers’ interaction styles directly affect employ- claims. This first finding is similar to Wang et al.’s (2011)
ees’ reactions to the customers). finding that longer-tenured employees allocate more
We also find strong support for the three aspects of per- resources to regulate their service-related emotions and
ceived legitimacy in predicting employees’ compliance out- behaviors in the face of negative customer behaviors.
come choices, which fits with script theory. In addition to Notably, the stronger influence of the dominant customer
assessing the degree to which a fuzzy request violates com- style in our study extends the field’s understanding of
pany rules (regulatory legitimacy), employees also use social employees’ reactions to negative customer behaviors by
norms (i.e., normative legitimacy) and cognitive sense-making showing how longer-tenured employees surreptitiously
(cognitive legitimacy) to judge these requests. Our study achieve their goal of punishing the offending customer (by
provides a more complete picture of this relationship than reinterpreting their view of the situation).7
those offered in previous studies of legitimacy and returns.
In this study, employees’ feelings of empowerment Managerial Implications
(assessed by perceived flexibility) influenced compliance The frequency of fuzzy returns and the percentage of accep-
outcomes as expected, while employees’ expectations of tance of such returns reported by our national sample of
punishment did not affect outcome decisions. This lack of retail employees indicate the significance of this neglected
effect may have occurred because employees believed that phenomenon. While 36% of the respondents indicated that
punishment was not relevant in these types of return situa- at least 20% of their returns belong in this fuzzy request
tions; that is, perhaps they believed they could safely bend category, they chose to accept these types of returns on
the rules with these generally inexpensive return requests or average 48% of the time. Moreover, the importance of this
even that it was worth the risk to treat the customer fairly issue is reflected by the degree to which firms embrace giv-
(e.g., Kennedy and Corliss 2008). ing employees empowerment and flexibility in their jobs
Not only do we offer conceptual and empirical support (e.g., Ritz-Carlton allows its employees to spend up to
for the separation of compliance outcome and process $2,000 to solve a customer’s problem (Michel, Bowen, and
behaviors in this study, we also predict and find a different Johnston 2008). It is evident that employees deal with these
set of antecedents for each. We find that the compliance types of requests and decisions relative to them on an
process, which is more discretionary, is mainly influenced almost daily basis. Employee decisions about whether to
by interpersonal factors (i.e., the customer and the break the rules and how to handle these issues diplomati-
employee factors), while the outcome, a more scripted cally can have a large impact on customer satisfaction, loy-
activity, is influenced by factors pertinent to multiple role alty, and, ultimately, the bottom line. Thus, our findings
senders, including a customer factor (affiliation), a request indicate several important managerial implications.
factor (legitimacy), and an organizational factor (norm of
flexibility). Therefore, this nuanced view of employee 7To exclude the possible confounding effects of longer-term
behaviors enhances the managerial relevance of this study. employees tending to hold supervisory positions, we compared all
Moreover, this work contributes to the service marketing the five differentiated impacts across employees with supervisor
literature by showing that the antecedents of employees’ com- title (N = 133) with those without supervisor title (N = 101) for
pliance outcome and employee process weigh differently for those with more than three years of tenure. We conducted a nested
structural equation model comparison to test whether the relation-
male versus female employees, as well as for employees
ship strengths of the five moderations differ across the supervisor
with shorter versus longer organizational tenure. In particular, and nonsupervisor subgroups. The results indicate that the find-
male employees in our study were more negatively affected ings noted here were not driven by whether the longer-term
by their customers’ dominant style than female employees, employee was a supervisor.

80 / Journal of Marketing, November 2012


First, service firms should recognize that several factors seem to have little impact on employee’s decision making
influence employees’ on-the-spot compliance behaviors relative to these fuzzy requests. Thus, reliance on informal
other than the objective aspects of the requests. Thus, ser- control systems, such as empowerment and flexibility, is likely
vice firms should ensure that the job designs and job to net more positive benefits than more formal control systems.
descriptions of frontline employees accurately reflect what
management wants to accomplish relative to fuzzy requests. Limitations and Directions for Further Research
If employees do not clearly know when and how to imple- Although our study’s cognitive focus is consistent with
ment (or bend) the firm’s policies and procedures, manage- research in social psychology, which indicates that a rational-
ment should train employees in this area. Furthermore, firms cognitive level of analysis explains behavior better than emo-
must understand the types of fuzzy requests employees are tions (Rabinowitz, Karuza and Zevon 1984), the main and
likely to encounter to identify acceptable deviations from interaction effects of employees’ affective or subjective reac-
the rules, as well as to provide ways to help employees tions to fuzzy requests is another fruitful path for exploration.
reject the customer’s claim diplomatically when necessary. Given that frontline employees are emotional laborers
Second, our findings suggest that beyond whether the (Hochschild 1983), further research should investigate the
return violates the rules (regulatory legitimacy), the firm effect that customer requests and employee decision making
should tap into frontline employees’ understanding of nor- relative to these requests have on role ambiguity and role
mative and cognitive legitimacy with regard to these deci- strain, as well as their subsequent effects on employee job
sions. Knowing that employees also attempt to make sense performance and satisfaction (Chung and Schneider 2002).
of the request from social norm (whether it is socially Our model paints the landscape of employee decision
acceptable) and personal norm (whether it is comprehensi- making with regard to customer fuzzy requests without
ble) perspectives suggests that service firms should commu- considering customer reactions to these decisions and
nicate the rationales of the (return) policies from both social process. Further studies should explore customers’ expecta-
logic and sense-making perspectives, as well as provide the tions (e.g., the extent to which they expect the employee to
regulations and rules to follow. break the rules and what criteria they use to develop expec-
The strong effect of the customer’s dominant style on all tations), as well as their reactions to employee compliance
three forms of legitimacy suggests that employees need addi- process and outcome in these situations. Moreover, we do
tional training aimed at not letting their subjective and negative not know the veracity of these retrospective self-reports;
feelings toward a customer’s approach affect their objective customers may have a different perspective on the incident.
assessment of the return’s legitimacy. The strong association Thus, conducting dyadic research could be fruitful.
of CO and CA with employees’ compliance process behav- Our findings should be applicable to other customer–
iors provides insights for training and hiring. That is, even if employee return or complaint situations, as well as to other
the employee cannot comply, he or she can make the process customer request encounters. However, further study is
less painful for the customer. Moreover, this positive treatment needed to establish that claim because retail returns are a
seems enhanced for longer-term employees because they rely unique type of customer request. For example, they fall within
more on CO and for women because they desire to avoid the typical job scope of the employee, while often customer
conflicts. These findings suggest the importance of hiring requests do not fall within this scope (e.g., when a customer
people with high CO (or training for that characteristic) to asks an employee to do something completely unrelated to
reduce the sting of refusing a customer’s return request. his or her job). In addition, our findings rely heavily on self-
With regard to hiring people with greater CA, this reported retrospectives of employees, which may involve
should be done cautiously, considering that the positive role inaccurate recall. Although we took precautions to reduce
of CA in the compliance process was only relevant for social desirability and other biases, it would be worthwhile
female (not male) employees. Moreover, female employees for researchers to use scenario-based and field experiments.
reported delivering compliance more positively than male Employees’ decisions to comply with fuzzy requests
employees (5.78 vs. 5.55, p < .01). These findings suggest have ethical or moral implications; excessively complying
that service firms should select employees with desirable with customer fuzzy requests may hurt the firm’s bottom
personality traits (e.g., high CO) and get female employees line, while rigidly rejecting these requests will anger cus-
more involved with sensitive return situations. tomers who are likely to expect some flexibility on the
Finally, service firms should acknowledge the differential rules. Employees need to know how to handle situations
routes of formal and informal control systems in encouraging that go beyond the rule book, and management needs to
desirable employee behavior. Expectations of punishment understand the realities employees face every day with
(relative to taking a return back that is outside company policy) regard to these situations.

REFERENCES
Ahearne, Michael, C.B. Bhattacharya, and Thomas Gruen (2005), Auh, Seigyoung, Bulent Menguc, Michelle Fisher, and Abeer
“Antecedents and Consequences of Customer–Company Iden- Haddad (2011), “The Contingency Effect of Service Employee
tification: Expanding the Role of Relationship Marketing,” Personalities on Service Climate: Getting Employee Percep-
tions Aligned Can Reduce Personality Effects,” Journal of Ser-
Journal of Applied Psychology, 90 (3), 574–85. vice Research, 14 (4), 426–41.
Atuahene-Gima, Kwaku and Janet Y. Murray (2004), Autry, Chad W., Donna J. Hill, and Matthew O’Brien (2007),
“Antecedents and Outcomes of Marketing Strategy Compre- “Attitudes Toward the Customer: A Study of Product Returns
hensiveness,” Journal of Marketing, 68 (October), 33–46. Episodes,” Journal of Managerial Issues, 19 (1), 315–39.

Customers’ Fuzzy Return Requests / 81


Babin, Barry J. and James S. Boles (1998), “Employee Behavior Cialdini, Robert B. (2001), Influence: Science and Practice.
in a Service Environment: A Model and Test of Potential Dif- Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
ferences Between Men and Women,” Journal of Marketing, 62 Crosby, Lawrence A., Kenneth R. Evans, and Deborah Cowles
(April), 77–91. (1990), “Relationship Quality in Services Selling: An Interper-
Bagozzi, Richard P. and Todd F. Heatherton (1994), “A General sonal Influence Perspective,” Journal of Marketing, 54 (July),
Approach to Representing Multifaceted Personality Con- 68–81.
structs: Application to State Self-Esteem,” Structural Equation Dallimore, Karen, Beverley A. Sparks, and Ken Butcher (2007),
Modeling, 1 (1), 35–67. “The Influence of Angry Customer Outbursts on Service
Baird, Lloyd S. (1976), “Relationship of Performance to Satisfac- Providers’ Facial Displays and Affective States,” Journal of
tion in Stimulating and Nonstimulating Jobs,” Journal of Service Research, 10 (1), 78–92.
Applied Psychology, 61 (6), 721–27. Dovidio, John F., Clifford E. Brown, Karen Heltman, Steve L.
Bateson, John E.G. (1985), Perceived Control and the Service Ellyson, and C.F. Keating (1988), “Power Displays Between
Encounter. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. Men and Women in Discussions of Gender-Linked Tasks: A
Beatty, Sharon E., Morris Mayer, James E. Coleman, Kristy Ellis Multichannel Study,” Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Reynolds, and Jungki Lee (1996), “Customer–Sales Associate chology, 55 (4), 580–87.
Retail Relationships,” Journal of Retailing, 72 (3), 223–47. Edwards, Jeffrey R. (2001), “Multidimensional Constructs in
Bem, Daryl J. (1967), “Self-Perception: An Alternative Interpreta- Organizational Behavior Research: An Integrative Analytical
tion of Cognitive Dissonance Theory,” Psychological Review, Framework,” Organizational Research Methods, 4 (2), 144–92.
74 (3), 183–200. Fiske, Susan T. and Laura E. Stevens (1993), “What’s So Special
Bem, Sandra L. (1981), Bem Sex-Role Inventory: Professional About Sex? Gender Stereotyping and Discrimination,” in Gen-
Manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. der Issues in Contemporary Society: Applied Social Psychol-
Ben-Sira, Zeev (1980), “Affective and Instrumental Components ogy Annual, Stuart Oskamp and Mark Costanzo, eds. Newbury
in the Physician–Patient Relationship: An Additional Dimen- Park, CA: Sage Publications, 173–96.
sion of Interaction Theory,” Journal of Health and Social Fornell, Claes and David F. Larcker (1981), “Structural Equation
Behavior, 21 (2), 170–80. Models with Unobservable Variables and Measurement Error:
Algebra and Statistics,” Journal of Marketing Research, 18
Bergkvist, Lars and John R. Rossiter (2007), “The Predictive
(August), 382–88.
Validity of Multiple-Item Versus Single-Item Measures of the
Giner-Sorolla, Roger and Shelly Chaiken (1997), “Selective Use
Same Constructs,” Journal of Marketing Research, 44 (May),
of Heuristic and Systematic Processing Under Defense Motiva-
175–84.
tion,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23 (Janu-
Berry, Leonard L. and Neeli Bendapudi (2003), “Clueing in Cus-
ary), 84–97.
tomers,” Harvard Business Review, 81 (February), 100–106.
Glaser, Barney and Anselm L. Strauss (1967), The Discovery of
Bitner, Mary Jo, Bernard H. Booms, and Lois A. Mohr (1994), Grounded Theory: Strategies for Qualitative Research.
“Critical Service Encounters: The Employee’s Viewpoint,” Chicago: Aldine Publishing Company.
Journal of Marketing, 58 (October), 95–106. Hair, Joseph F., William C. Black, Barry J. Babin, and Rolph E.
———, ———, and Mary Stanfield Tetreault (1990), “The Service Anderson (2009), Multivariate Data Analysis, 7th ed. Upper
Encounter: Diagnosing Favorable and Unfavorable Incidents,” Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Journal of Marketing, 54 (January), 71–84. Harris, Lloyd C. (2010), “Fraudulent Consumer Returns: Exploit-
Blancero, Donna Maria and Scott A. Johnson (2001), “A Process ing Retailers’ Return Policies,” European Journal of Market-
Model of Discretionary Service Behavior, Integrating Psycho- ing, 44 (6), 730–47.
logical Contracts, Organizational Justice, and Customer Feed- Hartline, Michael D. and O.C. Ferrell (1996), “The Management
back to Manage Service Agents,” Journal of Quality Manage- of Customer-Contact Service Employees: An Empirical Inves-
ment, 6 (2), 307–329. tigation,” Journal of Marketing, 60 (October), 52–70.
Blodgett, Jeffrey G., Donna J. Hill, and Stephen S. Tax (1997), Hatfield, Elaine, John T. Cacioppo, and Richard L. Rapson (1994),
“The Effects of Distributive, Procedural, and Interactional Jus- Emotional Contagion. New York: Cambridge University Press.
tice on Postcomplaint Behavior,” Journal of Retailing, 73 (3), Heide, Jan B. and George John (1992), “Do Norms Really Mat-
185–210. ter?” Journal of Marketing, 56 (April), 32–44.
Bowen, David E. and Edward E. Lawler III (1992), “The Empow- Hennig, Margaret and Anne Jardim (1977), The Managerial
erment of Services Workers: What, Why, How, and When,” Women. Garden City, NY: Anchor Press.
Sloan Management Review, 33 (3), 31–39. Hochschild, Arlie (1983), The Managed Heart: Commercialization
Brill, Jonathan E. (1994), “Beyond Managerial Opportunism: Sup- of Human Feeling. Berkeley: University of California Press.
plier Power and Managerial Compliance in a Franchised Mar- Holloway, Betsy and Sharon E. Beatty (2003), “Service Failure
keting Channel,” Journal of Business Research, 30 (3), Online: A Service Recovery Opportunity,” Journal of Service
211–23. Research, 6 (1), 92–105.
Brown, Tom J., John C. Mowen, D. Todd Donavan, and Jane W. Homburg, Christian and Andreas Furst (2005), “How Organiza-
Licata (2002), “The Customer Orientation of Service Workers: tional Compliant Handling Drives Customer Loyalty: An
Personality Trait Determinants and Effects on Self and Super- Analysis of the Mechanistic and the Organic Approach,” Jour-
visor Performance Ratings,” Journal of Marketing Research, nal of Marketing, 69 (July), 95–114.
39 (February), 110–19. Hui, Michael K., Kevin Au, and Henry Fock (2004a), “Empower-
Buller, Mary Klein and David B. Buller (1987), “Physicians’ ment Effects Across Cultures,” Journal of International Busi-
Communication Style and Patient Satisfaction,” Journal of ness Studies, 336 (35), 46–60.
Health and Social Behavior, 28 (4), 375–88. ———, ———, and ——— (2004b), “Reactions of Service Employ-
Chebat, Jean-Charles and Paul Kollias (2000), “The Impact of ees to Organization–Customer Conflict: A Cross-Cultural
Empowerment on Customer Contact Employees’ Roles in Ser- Comparison,” International Journal of Research in Marketing,
vice Organizations,” Journal of Service Research, 3 (1), 66–81. 21 (2), 107–121.
Chung, Beth G. and Benjamin Schneider (2002), “Serving Multiple ———, Xiande Zhao, Xiucheng Fan, and Kevin Au (2004), “When
Masters: Role Conflict Experienced by Service Employees,” Does the Service Process Matter? A Test of Two Competing
The Journal of Services Marketing, 16 (1), 70–87. Theories,” Journal of Consumer Research, 31 (2), 464–75.

82 / Journal of Marketing, November 2012


Iacobucci, Dawn and Amy Ostrom (1993), “Gender Differences in O’Brien, Matthew, Donna J. Hill, and Chad W. Autry (2009),
the Impact of ‘Core’ and ‘Relational’ Aspects of Services on “Customer Behavioral Legitimacy in Retail Returns Episodes:
Evaluation of Service Encounters,” Journal of Customer Psy- Effects on Retail Salesperson Role Conflict,” Journal of Mar-
chology, 2 (3), 257–86. keting Theory and Practice, 17 (3), 251–66.
———, Neela Saldanha, and Xiaoyan Deng (2007), “A Meditation Ouchi, William G (1980), “Markets, Bureaucracies, and Clans,”
on Mediation: Evidence that Structural Equations Models Per- Administrative Science Quarterly, 25 (1), 129–41.
form Better than Regressions,” Journal of Consumer Psychol- Parasuraman, A., Valarie A. Zeithaml, and Leonard L. Berry
ogy, 17 (2), 139–53. (1985), “A Conceptual Model of Service Quality and Its Impli-
Jaworski, Bernard J. (1988), “Toward a Theory of Marketing Con- cations for Future Research,” Journal of Marketing, 49 (Fall),
trol: Environmental Context, Control Types, and Consequences,” 41–50.
Journal of Marketing, 52 (July), 23–39. Payan, Janice M. and Richard G. McFarland (2005), “Decompos-
Joshi, Ashwin W. (2010), “Salesperson Influence on Product ing Influence Strategies: Argument Structure and Dependence
Development: Insights from a Study of Small Manufacturing as Determinants of the Effectiveness of Influence Strategies in
Organizations,” Journal of Marketing, 74 (January), 94–107. Gaining Channel Member Compliance,” Journal of Marketing,
Kennedy, Monica and Michael Corliss (2008), “Tension at the 69 (July), 66–79.
Interface: Exploring Employee Deviation,” Journal of Man- Peace, Graham A., Dennis F. Galletta, and James Y.L. Thong
agement and Organization, 14 (4), 424–37. (2003), “Software Piracy in the Workplace: A Model and
Korsch, Barbara M. and Vida Francis Negrete (1972), “Doctor– Empirical Test,” Journal of Management Information Systems,
Patient Communication,” Scientific American, 227 (2), 66–74. 20 (1), 153–77.
Krapfel, Robert E., Jr. (1988), “Customer Complaint and Sales- Rabinowitz, Vita C., Jurgis Karuza Jr., and Michael A. Zevon
person Response: The Effect of the Communication Source,” (1984), “Fairness and Effectiveness in Premeditated Helping,”
Journal of Retailing, 64 (2), 181–98. in The Sense of Injustice, R. Folger, ed. New York: Plenum
Kunda, Ziva (1990), “The Case for Motivated Reasoning,” Psy- Press.
chological Bulletin, 108 (3), 480–98. Rahim, Afzalur (1983), “A Measure of Styles of Handling Inter-
Langeard, Eric, John E.G. Bateson, Christopher H. Lovelock, and personal Conflict,” Academy of Management Journal, 26 (2),
Pierre Eiglier (1981), Marketing of Services: New Insights from 368–76.
Consumers and Managers. Cambridge, MA: Marketing Sci- Reynierse, James H. and John B. Harker (1992), “Employee and
ence Institute. Customer Perceptions of Service in Banks: Teller and Cus-
Lawler, Edward E. (1987), The Design of Effective Reward Sys- tomer Service Representative Ratings,” Human Resource Plan-
tems. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. ning, 15 (4), 31–46.
Leatherwood, Mayra L. and Spector C. Lee (1991), “Enforce-
Ringberg, Torsten, Gaby Odekerken-Schroder, and Glenn L.
ments, Inducements, Expected Utility and Employee Miscon-
Christensen (2007), “A Cultural Models Approach to Service
duct,” Journal of Management, 17 (3), 553–69.
Recovery,” Journal of Marketing, 71 (July), 194–214.
Liao, Hui (2007), “Do It Right This Time: The Role of Employee
Rogers, Felicia and Richarme Michael (2009), “The Honesty of
Service Recovery Performance in Customer-Perceived Justice
Online Survey Respondents: Lessons Learned and Prescriptive
and Customer Loyalty After Service Failures,” Journal of
Remedies,” Decision Analyst white paper, (accessed July 2,
Applied Psychology, 92 (2), 475–89.
2012), [available at http://www.decisionanalyst.com/publ_art/
Liao, Qinyu, Xin Luo, Anil Gurung, and Long Li (2010), “Work-
onlinerespondents.dai].
place Monitoring and Employee Misuse Avoidance: Does Pun-
ishment Matter?” Journal of Computer Information Systems, Rotter, George S. and Stephen M. Portugal (1969), “Group and
Individual Effects in Problem Solving,” Journal of Applied
50 (2), 49–59.
Psychology, 53 (4), 338–41.
Lindell, Michael K. and David J. Whitney (2001), “Accounting for
Common Method Variance in Cross-Sectional Research Schmidt, F.L., John E. Hunter, and A.N. Outerbridge (1986), “The
Designs,” Journal of Applied Psychology, 86 (1), 114–21. Impact of Job Experience, and Supervisory Ratings of Job Per-
Ma, Zhenfeng and Laurette Dubé (2011), “Process and Outcomes formance,” Journal of Applied Psychology, 71 (3), 432–39.
Interdependency in Frontline Service Encounters,” Journal of Schmidt, Ruth A., Fiona Sturrock, Philippa Ward, and Gaynor
Marketing, 75 (May), 83–98. Lea-Greenwood (1999), “Deshopping: The Art of Illicit Con-
Merton, Robert K. (1968), Social Theory and Social Structure. sumption,” The International Journal of Retail and Distribu-
New York: Simon and Schuster. tion Management, 27 (8), 209–301.
Meyer, Marshall W. and Lynne G. Zucker (1989), Permanently Schneider, Benjamin (1991), “Service Quality and Profits: Can
Failing Organizations. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications. You Have Your Cake and Eat It Too?” HR Human Resource
Michel, Stefan, David Bowen, and Robert Johnston (2008), “Cus- Planning, 14 (2), 151–57.
tomer Service: Making the Most of Customer Complaints; Schroeder, Pearl (1965), “Relationship of Kuder’s Conflict Avoid-
Dealing with Service Failures Means a Lot More Than Just ance and Dominance to Academic Accomplishment,” Journal
Fixing the Immediate Problem; Here’s How to Do It Right,” of Counseling Psychology, 12 (4), 395–99.
The Wall Street Journal, (September 22), 4–5. Smith, Amy K. and Ruth N. Bolton (2002), “The Effect of Cus-
Mohr, Lois A. and Mary Jo Bitner (1995), “The Role of Employee tomers’ Emotional Responses to Service Failures on Their
Effort in Satisfaction with Service Transactions,” Interpersonal Recovery Effort Evaluations and Satisfaction Judgments,”
Buyer Behavior in Marketing, 32 (3), 239–52. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 30 (1), 5–23.
National Retail Federation (2011), Consumer Returns in the Retail ———, ———, and Janet Wagner (1999), “A Model of Customer
Industry. Irvine, CA: The Retail Equation. Satisfaction with Service Encounters Involving Failure and
Ng, Thomas W.H. and Daniel C. Feldman (2010), “The Effects of Recovery,” Journal of Marketing Research, 36 (August),
Organizational Embeddedness on Development of Social and 356–72.
Human Capital,” Journal of Applied Psychology, 95 (4), Snavely, William B. and Ellen V. Walters (1983), “Differences in
696–712. Communication Competence Among Administrator Social
Norton, Robert W. (1978), “Foundation of a Communicator Style Styles,” Journal of Applied Communication Research, 11 (2),
Construct,” Human Communication Research, 4 (2), 99–112. 120–36.

Customers’ Fuzzy Return Requests / 83


Staub, Ervin (1990), “Moral Exclusion, Personal Goal Theory and U.S. Census Bureau (2010), “2010 U.S. Census,” (accessed July 2,
Extreme Destructiveness,” Journal of Social Issues, 46 (1), 2012), [available at http://2010.census.gov/2010census/].
47–64. Vecchio, Robert P. (1995), Organizational Behavior. Hinsdale, IL:
Steenkamp, Jan-Benedict E.M. and Hans Baumgartner (1998), Dryden Press.
“Assessing Measurement Invariance in Cross-National Con- Walker, Orville C., Jr., Gilbert A. Churchill Jr., and Neil M. Ford
sumer Research,” Journal of Consumer Research, 25 (June), (1975), “Organizational Determinants of the Industrial Sales-
78–90. man’s Role Conflict and Ambiguity,” Journal of Marketing, 39
Strativity Group (2008), Customer Experience Management. Par- (January), 32–39.
sippany, NJ: Strativity Group. Wang, Mo, Hui Liao, Yujie Zhan, and Junqi Shi (2011), “Daily
Strauss, Anselm L. and Juliet Corbin (1998), Basics of Qualitative Customer Mistreatment and Employee Sabotage Against Cus-
Research: Techniques and Procedures for Developing tomers: Examining Emotion and Resource Perspectives,”
Grounded Theory. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Academy of Management Journal, 54 (2), 312–34.
Taylor, Marylee C. and Judith A. Hall (1982), “Psychological Webster, Cynthia (2005), “Service Providers’ Communication
Androgyny: Theories, Methods, and Conclusions,” Psycho- Style and Customer Satisfaction,” The Business Review, Cam-
logical Bulletin, 92 (2), 347–66. bridge, 3 (2), 291–97.
Tittle, Charles R. (1980), Sanctions and Social Deviance. New Webster, Murray, Jr., and James E. Driskell (1983), “Beauty as
York: Praeger Publishers. Status,” American Journal of Sociology, 89 (1), 140–65.

84 / Journal of Marketing, November 2012


APPENDIX A
Qualitative Research Findings
Categories Constructs Example Quotes (Name, Retailer Type, Organizational Tenure)
Customer factors Interaction styles: affiliation
•Depending on the person, if they’re being really polite and stuff like that, then we’ll do something about it. [Erick,
specialty store, 4 years]
•For a legitimate return,… they feel they’re doing something wrong or they are not sure what they are going to do;…
sometimes they hide it or their little kids hold it or something like that;… very embarrassed … very apologetic. [Kelly,
specialty store, 4 years]
Interaction styles: dominance •Normally they (the customers) came in with the attitude of, “You have to take this return.” And I was like, “No, I don’t
have to do it.” [Amber, specialty store, 5 years]
•When a customer starts acting rude, I guess that is when our own egos come into play;… the ego gets involved,
and you kind of abide by the company policy. [Erick, specialty store, 4 years]
Request factors Regulative legitimacy •I know we are supposed to please the customer, but we should follow the policy before we let them have whatever
they want. [Cassandra, department store, 1 year]
•They (return requests) weren’t within our guidelines for the return policy; they have no proof of purchase; or for
some reason, we couldn’t verify that that item was purchased in our store;… we have to pretty much stick to the
rules and regulations. [Kathy, department store, 3 years]
Normative legitimacy •It depends whether they’re trying to take advantage or not. If they’re truly trying to return a product because it did
not work out…sometimes that happens…I would feel bad to reject them. [Chris, specialty store, 2 years]
Cognitive legitimacy •I was very open to making exceptions if there was a valid reason or if I feel like we gained a customer more than
we lost. [Renee, department store, 3.5 years]
Organizational factors Perceived norm of flexibility
•We’re allowed to break any rule in the book to keep a customer satisfied…. I had authority to do whatever I needed
to do to make the customer happy. [Kelly, specialty store, 4 years]
•On our rule book, it said no returns, final. You say “No.” [Darlene, outlet, 3 years]
Expectations of punishment •Bending the rules isn’t something that’s really going to benefit me at all. The customer may be happy in the end, but
if someone above me catches that, that would be my job there. For me, just sticking to the rules and regulations of
it, it’s probably the smartest way to go. [Greg, chain store, 4 years]
•You might want to bend the rules for some people, but if you do, it’s your job on the line. [Richard, department store,
1 year]
Employee factors CA •I really only chose to let it go because … I just didn’t want to deal with hassle of this guy. “No I can’t.” Then having him
be all like, “Oh come on,” or “Let me talk to your manager,” something that would escalate. And often at work I would
try to do the least work as possible. That’s why I just gave the guy store credit. [David, department store, 4 years]
CO •Customer satisfaction is the most important thing. For them (customers) to have that peace of mind of being able to
return what they want. I guess it would be convenient for customers. It (rejecting customers) makes me feel bad
because I wouldn’t be able to satisfy them as an employee. [David, specialty store, 1.5 years]
•I don’t mind refunding a customer’s money. The customer needs to be happy. That’s my philosophy, that’s the bot-
tom line, that’s the most important thing to me. [Sarah, specialty store, 1 year]
Compliance Process Friendliness •I was by myself and I started really calm (recalling an incident when she dealt with an angry customer), I was
explaining everything to her. Finally she left and her daughter came back and apologized. The daughter said,
“Thank you, you were being super nice. It’s just that my mom has been having a bad day.” [Darlene, outlet, 3 years]
Effort •They [a pair of shoes] had dirt on the bottom, so technically, according to policy, we couldn’t return them. But, I just
said, “You know what? I’ll clean them in the back.” I went to back and I wiped to dirt off from underneath. So, my
manager didn’t know and it was fine. We returned it. [Chris, specialty store, 2 years]
Compliance outcome •If you had a receipt, you were able to get it put back on your credit card, or get cash. If you brought it back without

Customers’ Fuzzy Return Requests / 85


a receipt, it was store credit. [Greg, specialty store, 4 years]
APPENDIX B
Focal Construct Measures
Construct and Source Items (Description) Factor loading
Affiliation 1. When making the return request, the customer seemed tactful. .90
(Norton 1978) 2. When making the return request, the customer seemed friendly. .99
CR = .96 3. When making the return request, the customer seemed nice. .95
Dominance 1. When making the return request, the customer seemed aggressive. .90
(Snavely and Walters 2. When making the return request, the customer seemed too assertive. .82
1983) 3. When making the return request, the customer seemed to want to control .72
CR = .87 the situation.
CO 1. I enjoy delivering quality service to our customers. .84
(Brown et al. 2002) 2. I get a great deal of satisfaction in completing tasks asked of me by our .76
CR = .86 customers.
3. I enjoy having the confidence to provide good service to customers. .87
CA 1. I try to avoid unpleasant exchange with others. .66
(Rahim 1983) 2. I generally avoid an argument with others. .82
CR = .83 3. I try to stay away from disagreements with others. .87
Regulative legitimacy 1. The customer’s return request … slightly violated ... seriously violated our .88
(self-developed: first item store policies (R)
is semantic differential) 2. It did not fit with our return policies. (R) .89
CR = .92 3. It went well beyond our return policies. (R) .90
Normative legitimacy Given the customer’s return request in this situation, most people I know would
(self-developed: semantic think that accepting the return would be:
differential) 1. Completely unacceptable … completely acceptable .91
CR = .88 2. Unreasonable….reasonable .99
3. Justified … unjustified .60
Cognitive legitimacy 1. For this return request, the customer’s explanation made no sense to me… .87
(self-developed: semantic made perfect sense.
differential) 2. For this return request, the customer’s explanation was not .81
CR = .86 comprehensible….was totally comprehensible
3. For this return request, the customer’s explanation was clearly .78
inappropriate…was clearly appropriate
Perceived norm of flexibility 1. In our store employees are expected to strictly follow the store’s return .80
(Heide and John 1992) policies.(R)
CR = .86 2. Flexibility in response to customers’ return request is normal here. .80
3. In our store, it is important to satisfy the customer even if it means bending .87
the rules.
Expectations of punishment 1. If I accept a return outside company policy, I might get into trouble. .90
(self-developed) 2. If I accept a return outside company policy, my supervisor might not approve. .95
CR = .95 3. If I accept a return outside company policy, I might get reprimanded. .94
Compliance process: 1. During the interaction, I was friendly to the customer. .78
employee friendliness 2. During the interaction, I was polite to the customer. .91
(Liao 2007) 3. During the interaction, I was patient with the customer. .76
CR = .86
Compliance process: Please recall how you actually handled the return by responding to the
employee effort following statements.
(Liao 2007) 1. I tried very hard to meet the customer’s return expectations. .81
CR = .87 2. I put a lot of effort into this return situation in order to meet the customer’s .92
expectations.
3. I went the extra mile to help this customer. .74
Compliance outcome To what extent do you feel that you met the customer’s return expectations?
(Payan and McFarland Did not meet expectations at all … fully met expectations
2005)
Notes: All scales are seven-point Likert scales unless otherwise noted (1 = “strongly disagree,” and 7 = “strongly agree”). (R) = reverse scored.

86 / Journal of Marketing, November 2012

You might also like