Bickel Moody Higgins 2016 Some Current Dimensions

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/303829918

Some Current Dimensions of the Behavioral Economics of Health-Related


Behavior Change

Article  in  Preventive Medicine · June 2016


DOI: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.06.002

CITATIONS READS

35 762

3 authors:

Warren K Bickel Lara Coughlin


Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University University of Michigan
547 PUBLICATIONS   27,492 CITATIONS    33 PUBLICATIONS   636 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Stephen T Higgins
University of Vermont
506 PUBLICATIONS   16,357 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Effects on infusion rate on the discriminative effects of opioids View project

Reinforcer Pathology View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Warren K Bickel on 12 June 2020.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


YPMED-04651; No of Pages 8
Preventive Medicine xxx (2016) xxx–xxx

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Preventive Medicine

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ypmed

Some current dimensions of the behavioral economics of health-related


behavior change
Warren K. Bickel a,⁎, Lara Moody a, Stephen T. Higgins b,c
a
Virginia Tech Carilion Research Institute, Department of Psychology, Roanoke, VA, USA
b
Vermont Center on Behavior and Health, University of Vermont, Burlington, VT, USA
c
University of Vermont, Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Health-related behaviors such as tobacco, alcohol and other substance use, poor diet and physical inactivity, and
Received 19 February 2016 risky sexual practices are important targets for research and intervention. Health-related behaviors are especially
Received in revised form 31 May 2016 pertinent targets in the United States, which lags behind most other developed nations on common markers of
Accepted 4 June 2016
population health. In this essay we examine the application of behavioral economics, a scientific discipline that
Available online xxxx
represents the intersection of economics and psychology, to the study and promotion of health-related behavior
Keywords:
change. More specifically, we review what we consider to be some core dimensions of this discipline when ap-
Behavioral economics plied to the study health-related behavior change. Behavioral economics (1) provides novel conceptual systems
Health behaviors to inform scientific understanding of health behaviors, (2) translates scientific understanding into practical and
Trans disease processes effective behavior-change interventions, (3) leverages varied aspects of behavior change beyond increases or de-
creases in frequency, (4) recognizes and exploits trans-disease processes and interventions, and (5) leverages
technology in efforts to maximize efficacy, cost effectiveness, and reach. These dimensions are overviewed and
their implications for the future of the field discussed.
© 2016 Published by Elsevier Inc.

1. Introduction effective in other areas of behavior change research and application in-
cluding behavior analysis (e.g., Baer et al., 1968).
Efforts to understand and improve health-related behavior change is The field of behavioral economics and health-related behavior
of increasing importance as the United States (US) continues to under- change can be described through at least five dimensions; that is, behav-
achieve on important markers of population health (Henningfield, ioral economics of health-related behavior changhe (1) provides novel
2014; Higgins, 2014; Kaplan, 2014; Schroeder, 2007; Schroeder, conceptual systems to inform scientific understanding of health behav-
2016). Behavioral economics, a field at the intersection of economics iors, (2) translates scientific understanding into practical and effective
and psychology, has been studying health-related behavior change for behavior-change interventions, (3) leverages varied aspects of behavior
more than 20 years (Bickel and Vuchinich, 2000; Kessler and Zhang, change beyond increases or decreases in frequency, (4) recognizes and
2014). However, behavioral economics is not a singular field but rather exploits trans-disease processes and interventions, and (5) leverages
is composed of overlapping fields derived from constitutive disciplines; technology in efforts to maximize efficacy, cost effectiveness, and
that is, some behavioral economic perspectives are more closely aligned reach. Note that we are not suggesting these dimensions are unique to
with economics, while other are more closely aligned with psychology. the behavioral economics of health-related behavior change. Indeed,
The latter brings a greater emphasis on decision science. The form of be- an emerging discipline that is derived from other disciplines would be
havioral economics that we address in this essay comes with an empha- expected to share some dimensions with them. Each of these dimen-
sis from a specific area of psychology, namely, behavior analysis (Hursh sions is discussed in further detail below.
et al. 1988). Despite these various forms, we believe there may be utility
in delineating some core aspects or dimensions of behavioral economics 1.1. Rationale for studying health-related behavior change
in the study of health-related behavior change. Establishing similar di-
mensions as a framework to advance study has previously been Health-related behavior change is a fundamentally important area
for research and intervention because despite spending more on
⁎ Corresponding author at: Addiction Recovery Research Center, Virginia Tech Carilion
healthcare than any other nation, US population health lags far behind
Research Institute, 2 Riverside Circle, Roanoke, VA 24016, USA. most other developed nations on important health indicators such as in-
E-mail address: wkbickel@vtc.vt.edu (W.K. Bickel). fant and maternal mortality rates, life expectancy, and rates of illness

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.06.002
0091-7435/© 2016 Published by Elsevier Inc.

Please cite this article as: Bickel, W.K., et al., Some current dimensions of the behavioral economics of health-related behavior change, Prev. Med.
(2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.06.002
2 W.K. Bickel et al. / Preventive Medicine xxx (2016) xxx–xxx

and injury (Schroeder, 2007; Higgins, 2014). Indeed, the U.S. National demonstrated are integral to understanding these suboptimal choice
Research Council's Institute of Medicine undertook a study comparing patterns are delay discounting and elasticity of demand.
US population health to other developed countries and uncovered an
overarching pattern that became the subtitle of their report, “poorer 2.1. Novel models and applications of delay discounting and consumer de-
health and shorter lives” (Woolf & Aron, 2013). That report examined mand theory
the individual behavior patterns contributing to excess morbidity and
mortality, including tobacco use and abuse of alcohol and other sub- Delay discounting refers to the reduction in the value of a reinforcer
stances, poor diet and inadequate physical activity, risky sexual prac- as a function of delays to its delivery. Classical economics stipulated that
tices, and high injury and accident rates. Poor adherence with delay discounting should be a rational process characterized by an ex-
medication and preventive medical regimens is another major contrib- ponential function, but that conceptualization of discounting can not
utor to the relatively poor health status of the US (World Health easily accommodate the common phenomenon of preference reversals
Organization, 2009). (Samuelson 1937). Preference reversal refers to a change in preference
Consider some additional specific details associated with US popula- from a larger later reward to a smaller sooner reward as the time to the
tion health. Tobacco use in the US serves as a great exemplar that sub- receipt of both rewards decreases (Green et al. 1994) In contrast to the
stantial changes in health behaviors are achievable having moved classical model of discounting, behavioral economics demonstrates that
from being the country with the highest prevalence rates among devel- discounting curves are best described by a hyperbolic function that can
oped countries in the 1960s to presently having the lowest rates other readily accommodate preference reversals (Rachlin et al. 1991). To date,
than Sweden (Higgins, 2014). Unfortunately, prevalence of daily the hyperbolic function has been shown to be the most universally ap-
smoking in the US still exceeds 16% of the population over 15 years plicable characterization of discounting, underscoring the value of this
old (OECD, 2009). That continuation of cigarette smoking in combina- behavioral economic perspective in accommodating irrational choice.
tion with the time-lagged adverse impacts of earlier smoking practices Importantly, the rate by which a commodity is discounted can be quan-
leaves the US among the highest in annual rates of tobacco-related dis- tified and compared between and within individuals. Indeed, individual
ease and premature death (Jemal et al., 2011), a trend that many predict differences in rates of discounting future rewards is associated with nu-
will turn for the better in the near future (Schroeder, 2016). merous negative health behaviors, and associated conditions including
Alcohol and other substance abuse, of course, are important contrib- almost every form of drug dependence (Bickel et al., 1999; Bickel et
utors to global rates of violence, traffic accidents, poisonings, drownings, al., 2014b; Yi et al., 2010), overweight and obesity (Bickel et al., 2014c;
and self-inflicted injuries (World Health Organization, 2011). While the Epstein et al., 2010; Privitera et al., 2015; Weller et al., 2008), problem
US does not consume as much alcohol or drugs as other comparable gambling (Alessi & Petry, 2003; Andrade & Petry, 2012; Reynolds,
countries, Americans lose more years of life as a result of alcohol and 2006), risky sexual practices (Chesson, 2012; Meade et al., 2011), and
substance use (Woolf & Aron, 2013). This observation of lower con- health disparities (Bickel & Marsch, 2001; Bickel et al., 2014a). Indeed,
sumption associated with worse health outcomes may be explained the broad generality of excessive delay discounting across health behav-
by the unhealthy patterns of consumption in the form of binge drinking iors and associated conditions is the basis for its designation as a trans-
and other drug misuse. Causal data, however, are lacking on the reason disease process (Bickel et al., 2012a) and proposals that excessive
for worsened health outcomes related to alcohol and drug use (Woolf & discounting may represent an endophenotype of negative health be-
Aron, 2013). haviors (e.g., Bickel, 2015). Other studies have shown that pretreatment
Poor diet and excessive caloric intake are major contributors to obe- discounting rates can be predictive of therapeutic outcomes (Sheffer et
sity and obesity-related disease (cardiovascular disease, type-2 diabe- al., 2014; Sheffer et al., 2012). Moreover, several efforts are ongoing to
tes, colon and other site-specific cancers). The US leads the world in examine whether producing greater valuation of delayed outcomes
per capita number of daily calories consumed (OECD, 2009). In addition would, in turn, result in therapeutic changes in corresponding health
to poor diet, inadequate physical activity contributes to obesity-risk and behaviors, such as reduction or cessation of substance use. Delay
these same chronic diseases. Rates of physical activity are consistently discounting also may exert nuanced influence in the developmental
lower in countries with higher income (Hallal et al., 2012). In the US, course of health-related behavior patterns. With regard to cigarette
rates of moderate to vigorous physical activity are generally similar to smoking among women, for example, a clear and robust association ex-
other high-income countries; however, average time spent engaging ists between discounting rates of monetary rewards and the likelihood
in sedentary activities In the US such as watching TV far exceeds pat- of being a smoker (Chivers et al., under review), a modest but significant
terns in other countries (Brunello et al., 2008). association between discounting and the likelihood of quitting smoking
Finally, risky sexual behaviors are associated with the spread of in- without formal treatment upon learning of a pregnancy (White et al.,
fectious disease including HIV and AIDS. Indeed, the US has the second 2014), but no discernible association with the likelihood of responding
highest rate of HIV infection when compared to 17 comparable devel- to formal treatment among the subset of women who continue smoking
oped countries and the single highest incidence of AIDS (Woolf & after learning they are pregnant (Lopez et al., 2015).
Aron, 2013). Demand refers to the relationship between price and consumption
Overall, a broad scientific consensus considers the above patterns of of a commodity and can be used as a measure of value of the commodity
unhealthy behaviors as among the leading, although certainly not the in question (Bickel et al., 1993). Relatively high valuation of specific re-
only, contributors to the unsettling disparities in health when compar- inforcers, such as drugs or food, is associated with negative health be-
ing the US to other developed countries (Woolf & Aron, 2013) and haviors (Bickel et al., 2014d). Converging studies show that among
when comparing health indicators across differing socioeconomic levels those with addictions and obesity, for example, the health-defeating
within the US. (Higgins, 2014; Higgins, 2015a; Higgins, 2015b; commodities (i.e., drugs and food, respectively) are excessively valued
Schroeder, 2007; Schroeder, 2016). and inflexible to change relative to individuals without those problems.
Stated differently, addicted and obese individuals will pay (e.g., with
2. Novel conceptual systems time, money, or effort) disproportionately for these commodities, indi-
cating inelastic demand characteristics (Epstein & Saelens, 2000;
Behavioral economics underscores how the various unhealthy be- Epstein et al., 2010; Jacobs & Bickel, 1999; Murphy et al., 2014). Quanti-
havior patterns outlined above can all be conceptualized in terms of be- tative markers of inelastic demand for these commodities may indicate
havioral choice wherein relatively health-defeating options are clinically relevant differences in the severity of such health-related pa-
repeatedly chosen over health-promoting alternatives. Two decision- thologies and may be useful in individualizing treatments as well as in-
making processes that behavioral economics research has dicators of successful treatment response (MacKillop & Murphy, 2007).

Please cite this article as: Bickel, W.K., et al., Some current dimensions of the behavioral economics of health-related behavior change, Prev. Med.
(2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.06.002
W.K. Bickel et al. / Preventive Medicine xxx (2016) xxx–xxx 3

Taken together, delay discounting and demand provide two power- increase organ donations (Ugur, 2015). Together, these diverse concep-
ful conceptual tools for assessing and potentially intervening on health- tual systems that build on dual systems with multi-axis models of be-
related behavioral pathologies. Excessive delay discounting and persis- havior change may serve as the foundation for experimental designs
tently inelastic demand, together, can be theoretically characterized as and the development of behavior change interventions.
core parts of what is discussed below as reinforcement pathology, a
trans-disease process that underpins clinical states like addiction and 3. Translates knowledge into effective behavior-change interventions
obesity, and their associated increases in risk for chronic disease and
premature death (Bickel et al., 2012b; Bickel et al., 2014d; Carr et al., Behavioral economics is an inherently translational discipline con-
2011). sidering that it is a hybrid of economics, psychology, as well as behavior-
al pharmacology, cognitive neuroscience and neuroeconomics. It is not
2.2. Other conceptual systems alone in this regard as much of contemporary science is focused on
translational approaches that cross the divide between more basic or
Behavioral economics employs a variety of conceptual systems that fundamental and applied science. Translational science brings knowl-
can guide development of behavior-change methods. These conceptual edge gained in controlled laboratory studies to the clinical treatment
systems take into account behavior analysis principles such as rein- of pathologies, but, importantly, is also bidirectional with observations
forcement, cognitive psychology principles such as heuristics and biases made under less-controlled clinical conditions also informing and set-
in decision-making, and weave them into models that elucidate pat- ting the occasion for more basic investigation.
terns of interrelatedness and are sufficiently dynamic and change-ori-
ented so as to be practically useful in supporting development of 3.1. Financial incentives
behavior-change strategies. Below we briefly review some additional
conceptual models employed in the behavioral economics of health-re- Here we use incentive programs as an exemplar of translation of
lated behavior change. basic knowledge into effective interventions. Systematic use of financial
One conceptual framework that has considerable influence in this incentives (referred to as contingency management in the area of addic-
area is dual-system models. Numerous dual-system models exist (see tions, and conditional cash transfers in global anti-poverty efforts) is
Hofmann et al., 2009 for a review); however, only two closely aligned clearly a translational intervention (e.g., Higgins et al., 2012). These in-
models have specifically addressed addiction and other health-related centive programs entail the systematic delivery of reinforcers contin-
behaviors (i.e., Bickel et al., 2007; Noël et al., 2013). Here, we focus on gent (or conditional) upon achieving some predetermined behavioral
the Competing Neurobehavioral Decision Systems model of health-re- target or goal. Use of incentives was initially explored across a wide-
lated behaviors (Bickel et al., 2007; Bickel & Yi, 2007). range of different types of behavioral disorders as part of the develop-
The Competing Neurobehavioral Decision Systems view suggests ment of behavior modification, but was most thoroughly and systemat-
that choices include two neurobiological decision systems. One is the ically researched in the area of drug dependence (e.g., Higgins et al.,
impulsive decision system that is embodied in the evolutionarily old 2008). More specifically, the field of behavioral pharmacology had
limbic and paralimbic brain regions and functions to obtain biologically long established that drugs of dependence function as reinforcers; that
important reinforcers and to engender emotions and motivation. The is, drug naive laboratory animals readily self-administer drugs of depen-
other is the executive decision system that is embodied in the evolu- dence. Extensions of that work in both the non-human and human lab-
tionarily younger prefrontal cortices and is fundamental to considering oratory demonstrated that arranging contingent availability of
the future, remembering important goals, and planning. When in a alternative drug reinforcers could decrease the frequency of drug con-
healthy state, these two decision systems function in regulatory balance sumption (Higgins, 1997). To the extent that availability of these alter-
such that they are responsive to circumstance and can adapt to situa- native non-drug reinforcers was mutually incompatible with drug use,
tions that require immediate focus, deferred consideration, or anywhere it functionally represented an increase in the price of continued drug
in between. However, with disease states these systems are dysregulat- use. More specifically, the intervention changes the opportunity cost
ed; for example, behavior can be biased towards the short term if the of using the substance and forces an interchange between the drug
executive system is relatively hypoactive and the impulsive system rel- and the incentive (e.g., Higgins, 1996; Higgins 1997; Higgins et al.,
atively hyperactive. This model can serve to elucidate behavioral and 2004).
neural systems that need to be targeted to reestablish some form of reg- Financial incentives in the form of vouchers exchangeable for retail
ulatory balance and improve health (Bickel et al., 2014e). items garnered considerable attention when they were demonstrated
Building on dual-decision models, Codagnone and colleagues' in controlled trials to significantly reduce cocaine use in dependent out-
(Codagnone et al., 2014) incorporate ‘nudging’ techniques (Thaler & patients when virtually everything else that was investigated with this
Sunstein, 2008) and a multi-axis model for behavior change. The population failed miserably (Higgins et al., 1994; Silverman et al.,
resulting nudging taxonomy provides guidance on how to influence 1996). Thereafter that incentives model was extended to a wide range
choice architecture across two axes which span automatic versus reflec- of different types of substance use disorders and different populations,
tive and hot versus cold affect dimensions of choice. This model is in- with a striking level of efficacy. Indeed, a programmatic series of litera-
structive and consistent with the Competing Neurobehavioral ture reviews on the use of this model with substance abusers identified
Decision System in its ability to point to counterbalances, such as acti- 177 controlled studies published between 1991 and 2014, with 155
vating more rational and analytical processes when individuals, groups, (88%) of them supporting efficacy (Davis et al., in press; Higgins et al.,
or communities tend towards impulsive and unhealthy choices. Nudges 2011; Lussier et al., 2006). Examples of substance abusing populations
can be applied to choices made across many different domains and to which the contingent incentives approach has been applied include
levels from individual health to the practices of healthcare systems, smoking cessation in the general adult population (Halpern et al.,
product marketing, and public policy (e.g., Thaler & Sunstein, 2008; 2015) as well as pregnant (Higgins & Solomon, 2016) and adolescent
Sunstein, 2014). For example, in health care settings, the use of defaults (Krishnan-Sarin et al., 2013) smokers, but also to a wide range of
(one form of nudging), specifically for end-of-life plans, changed choice other health problems including weight loss (Jeffery, 2012) and medica-
behavior and may be used to improve important outcomes such as re- tion adherence (DeFulio & Silverman, 2012).
ceipt of wanted and unwanted end-of-life care (Haplpern et al., 2013). Behavioral economic interventions have become increasingly well
So too, nudges have been shown to increase childhood health food known and accepted because of their track record for effectiveness.
choices (Cravener et al., 2015; Anzman-Frasca et al., 2015; Radnitz et The incentives and choice architecture interventions discussed above
al., 2013), optimize physician medical orders (Olson et al., 2015), and are currently widely used strategies for changing health behaviors. In

Please cite this article as: Bickel, W.K., et al., Some current dimensions of the behavioral economics of health-related behavior change, Prev. Med.
(2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.06.002
4 W.K. Bickel et al. / Preventive Medicine xxx (2016) xxx–xxx

addition, interventions to reduce delay discounting such as episodic fu- Taken together, the field of behavioral economics has already con-
ture thought and working memory training are being studied as addi- tributed well-validated, highly efficacious interventions, such as incen-
tional candidate approaches to enact change. tives for the treatment of negative health behaviors. Others strategies
such as defaults and choice architecture are earlier in the stages of de-
velopment and validation, but, as discussed above, have shown efficacy
3.2. Other translations and applications in controlled studies and are growing in popularity. More recently de-
veloped or emerging interventions such as episodic future thinking
Psychology lends many principles to behavioral economics to lever- and working memory-training interventions show promise in laborato-
age in developing efficacious behavior-change interventions. The psy- ry experiments and await further translational applications to the clinic.
chological foundations underpinning incentives, as discussed above in
the section on translation, was first observed by early behavioral psy- 4. Leveraging varied aspects of behavior change
chologists such as B.F. Skinner and Edward Thorndike (Skinner, 1953;
Thorndike, 1911) and then applied to health behaviors such as drug Of course, change is the goal of behavior economic interventions, but
abuse (e.g., Higgins et al., 2008) and overeating (e.g., Jeffery, 2012). what kind of change is being targeted or promoted should be clearly
Choice architecture and defaults, as mentioned above in the section on elucidated or specified. The two most familiar aspects of change are in-
conceptual models, are lower intensity strategies for influencing choice. creases and decreases in the frequency of particular health behaviors as
These behavior change strategies, stemming from cognitive psychology discussed above. Behavioral economics also addresses other important
and the study of decision-making heuristics and biases (Tversky & aspects of behavior change discussed below in this emerging area of
Kahneman, 1974; Tversky & Kahneman, 1981; Kahneman & Tversky, study.
1982), may be leveraged to influence health behaviors. Nudges include: One aspect of change is referred to as rate or baseline dependent
a) setting defaults, such as opt out programs for organ donations, b) change (Bickel et al., 2016), which was first observed in behavioral
leveraging social norms, such as emphasizing what choice the majority pharmacology research (Dews, 1977). Although rate dependence can
of people make, c) making healthy choices more convenient, such as plac- take several forms, the most typical is an inverse relationship between
ing healthy foods in more visible and accessible places, d) providing baseline rate of responding and the proportion of change. With this typ-
warnings, such as labels and graphic images on cigarette packages, and ical form of rate dependence, the same intervention may increase, de-
e) encouraging pre-commitments, such as making a commitment to crease, and have no effect on the target behavior depending on the
treatment in the future (Thaler & Sunstein, 2008; Sunstein, 2014). baseline frequency of the behavior. For example, the impact of multiple
While the degree of influence of these lower-intensity manipulations interventions, including working memory training and substance de-
of choice may have on population health are still debated pendence treatments, on degree of delay discounting have been
(Loewenstein et al., 2012), their conceptual value for understanding ir- shown to be rate dependent, with individuals exhibiting relatively
rationalities in health-related choice patterns and relatively low costs high discounting rates at baseline showing the greatest reductions in
are generating considerable interest. the rate of discounting (Bickel et al., 2014f). Also important to note
Emerging areas for behavioral economic interventions include ef- about this pattern of results is that the trials with the largest treatment
forts to change value over time to influence individual choices towards effects also showed the greatest rate-dependent relationships. More-
more health-promoting behaviors as opposed to choices that are easy or over, a recent review and reanalysis of the rate-dependent effects of
pleasurable in the moment (Murphy and MacKillop, 2006; Murphy et medications, such as modafinal could account for 67% of the variance
al., 2007). Two candidate interventions show promise at changing in the results examined (Bickel et al., 2016). Together these findings
delay-discounting values. One intervention to change delay discounting suggest that rate dependence may operate across diverse behavior-
is the use of episodic future thought. Episodic future thinking requires change interventions. Rate dependence may also permit understanding
individuals to pre-experience future events by vividly imagining realis- individual differences in treatment, but also could be used to personal-
tic events that may happen. Initial evidence for this potential change- ize treatment; that is, for conditions where rate dependence has been
oriented intervention showed promise in reducing discounting of future shown to operate, careful assessment at baseline could identify individ-
rewards (Peters & Büchel, 2010). Episodic future thinking interventions uals who are more or less likely to benefit from an intervention.
have been successful in reducing discounting of the future among the Another aspect of behavior change is stability of change. When the
obese (Daniel et al., 2013a, 2013b), nicotine-dependent (Stein et al., changed behavior meets a challenging circumstance, that behavior
under review) and alcohol-dependent (Snider et al., 2016), as well as could be fragile and fall apart or become less frequent, it could be robust
reducing consumption of food (Daniel et al., 2013a) and cigarettes and be insensitive to the effects of the challenge, or it could be anti-frag-
(Stein et al., under review) in obese and nicotine-dependent samples, ile where the challenge actually strengthens the behavior (Taleb, 2012).
respectively. These findings suggest that it may be a successful interven- This approach to health-related behavior change has been less frequent-
tion for pathologies characterized by self-control failures. ly explored but should be an important area of future investigation con-
Another candidate treatment is working memory training. In this in- sidering that it has the potential to elucidate not only if we can change a
tervention participants are exposed to sequential working memory ses- health behavior but also the likihood that change will be sustained over
sions with tasks increasing in difficulty from session to session. Working time.
memory training has been studied in a variety of disorders ranging from One example of a study modeling the importance of an initial period
children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Klingberg et al., of sustained abstinence to longer-term success in quitting smoking is
2005) to adults with traumatic brain injuries (Björkdahl et al., 2013). the work by Higgins and colleagues where cigarette smokers were in-
Of specific interest to health behavior change, several studies have in- centivized to be abstinent for one day, one week, or two weeks. Follow-
vestigated working memory training as an intervention to reduce ing these differing duration abstinence periods, participants were given
delay discounting, increase self control, and subsequently aid in health a relapse test; that is a choice to receive either money ($0.25/choice) or
behavior change. Decreases in delay discounting following working opportunities to smoke (two puffs/cigarette). The results showed a sys-
memory training have been observed in treatment-seeking stimulant tematic decrease in preference for the smoking option (i.e., less relapse)
users (Bickel et al., 2011) as well as decreases in alcohol consumption as a function of increasing duration of abstinence indicating that an ini-
in problem drinkers (Houben et al., 2011) and a longer duration of treat- tial period of sustained abstinence decreases the relative reinforcing ef-
ment effects among obese children (Verbeken et al., 2013). However, fects of cigarette smoking compared to a common non-drug alternative
we note that some studies have failed to find a working memory train- reinforce (Bradstreet et al., 2014; Lussier et al., 2005; Yoon et al., 2009).
ing effect (e.g., Rass et al., 2015). Such knowledge has been used in designing treatment interventions to

Please cite this article as: Bickel, W.K., et al., Some current dimensions of the behavioral economics of health-related behavior change, Prev. Med.
(2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.06.002
W.K. Bickel et al. / Preventive Medicine xxx (2016) xxx–xxx 5

promote longer-term behavior change in such challenging populations 5.1. Leveraging technology
as cocaine-dependent outpatients (Higgins et al., 2007) and is part of
the general rationale underpinning the widely used escalating sched- According to Baer et al. (1968) when defining dimensions of behav-
ules of reinforcement with reset contingencies in incentives-based in- ior analysis, technological refers to the “techniques making up a partic-
terventions (Roll and Higgins, 2000; Higgins et al., 2007; Romanowich ular behavioral application [being] completely identified and described”
& Lamb, 2015). Of course, despite these instructive studies on how to (p. 95). While we agree with this distinction, especially as it applies to a
promote more lasting behavior change following discontinuation of in- treatment being both effective and generalizable, we wish to extend this
centive-based interventions, relapse remains a challenge with this dimension to also encapsulate technological advancements that in-
treatment approach (e.g., John et al., 2011; Volpp et al., 2008) as it is crease the reach of interventions or increase treatment efficiency or
with other behavior change strategies underscoring the need for contin- cost effectiveness (Kurti et al., in press). Technological advancements
ued study and a more complete understanding of how to sustain behav- such as computerized-delivery of assessments and interventions allow
ior change (e.g., Leahey et al., 2016). for highly sensitive measurement of change and near identical replica-
Behavioral economics underscores the importance of recognizing tion of experimental manipulations oftentimes without the burden of
that there are important aspects of behavior change beyond producing traveling to a laboratory or clinic. Note that technological advancements
increases and decreases in the frequency of behavior. We have illustrat- are not unique to behavioral economics or to health-related behavior
ed this point with observations on rate dependence and stability of change, but rather the domain of technological advancements is fur-
change. Recognizing the contributions of these and other processes thering this field among others. Importantly, technological advances
that can render behavior change fragile or sturdy in the face of experi- can be key to reducing the burden and cost of participating in research
mental or real world challenge has the potential to extend basic under- and/or receiving treatment and hence a boon to cost-effectiveness.
standing as well as technological aspects of process and mechanisms of One promising behavioral economic intervention uses advancement
change. in voice recognition software, Interactive Voice Response, to provide reg-
ular assessment of drinking patterns (Tucker et al., 2012; Tucker et al.,
5. Trans-disease processes and therapeutics 2009). Through regular remote assessment of drinking, the experi-
menters can use the collected data in a money allocation algorithm called
Behavioral economics brings what might be considered a contrary the Alcohol-Savings Discretionary Expenditure index to compare previ-
view to the current scientific approaches to health behaviors and asso- ous years' savings compared to alcohol expenditures. The Alcohol-Savings
ciated diseases (see Bickel & Mueller, 2009, for a more in depth discus- Discretionary Expenditure index can then be used to prospectively pre-
sion of this topic). Most contemporary approaches assume that each dict drinking outcomes following natural resolutions to stop or moderate
disease is unique. This is evidenced by the organizational structure of drinking (Tucker et al., 2008; Tucker et al., 2009; Tucker et al., 2012).
the National Institutes of Health, disease specific journals, and disease Another application of technology in behavioral economic interven-
specific professional societies. Consider the consequences when we tions is the use of remote behavior-monitoring systems in incentives-
add to this assumption two other aspects of contemporary science. based interventions (Kurti et al., in press). For example, having partici-
The first aspect is the highly successful reductionist approach that re- pants use a web camera to submit time-stamped videos of breath car-
mains highly evident in contemporary science; that is, the assumption bon monoxide (CO) testing to verify smoking status in to reduce
that the solution to a disease process resides in some smaller, underly- cigarette smoking, allows for convenient sampling of smoking status
ing, part of the process. The second aspect is the growth in the size of obviating any need for smokers to visit the clinic for that purpose
the scientific literature about which scientists are expected to keep ap- (Stoops et al., 2009; Dallery & Raiff, 2011). So too, breath-alcohol sam-
prised. Collectively, scientists may learn more and more about a smaller pling for alcohol drinkers is currently undergoing feasibility studies for
aspect of the disease process and, this, in turn, may lead to the problem remotely monitored incentives-based interventions. Using these re-
of intellectual silos that risk obscuring recognition of commonalities mote technologies, incentives studies are able to have a greater reach,
across diverse diseases and disorders. more frequent objective verification of treatment targets, and greater
In contrast, behavioral economics of health-related behavior change cost-effectiveness.
is exploring and underscoring processes and interventions that have Another intervention, based on the community reinforcement ap-
generality across multiple diseases. Delay discounting as we noted proach (Higgins et al., 2003), is the Therapeutic Education System, an
above has been recognized as a decision process that is operating in a adjunctive computer-delivered intervention developed to help with
wide variety of diseases and disorders. Indeed, the pattern of excessive opioid dependence. The goal of the community reinforcement approach
discounting across these health-defeating behaviors provides evidence is to bring the patients in contact with pro-social reinforcer that may ef-
that the process of delay discounting is a trans-disease process (Bickel fectively compete with drug use. A novel extension employed in the
et al., 2012a). More recent evidence suggests that it is a behavioral Therapeutic Education System is the use of fluency training, where ac-
marker of pathology (Bickel et al., 2014b) and may also be an curacy and speed of responding is required to move onto the next mod-
endophenotype of addiction (Bickel, 2015) and other health- ule. Fluency training helps to ensure optimization for mastery of the
compromising behaviors. cognitive-behavioral skills and information presented during comput-
Interventions may also be efficacious across multiple disorders. For erized modules. Randomized clinical trials have assessed the efficacy
example, as we noted above, the efficacy of financial incentives has of computer-delivered compared to therapist-delivered modules, find-
broad generality across weight loss, tobacco and other substance ing that the Therapeutic Education System was associated with signifi-
abuse, and adherence with medications and other medical regimens. cantly greater rates of abstinence as well as significantly lower
The emerging evidence on the intervention of episodic future thinking dropout rates (Bickel et al., 2008; Campbell et al., 2014; Christensen et
also supports generality across multiple disorders and can engender al., 2014). Leveraging state-of-the-art technologies in the further devel-
greater valuation of the future rewards and decrease valuation and con- opment of behavioral economic interventions will allow for more fre-
sumption of brief intense reinforcers such as drugs or food. quent and accurate assessment as well expanding the reach for
Collectively, a conceptual framework that includes trans-disease behavioral economic interventions.
processes and interventions distinguishes behavioral economics from
many contemporary scientific approaches. The scope of trans-disease 6. Conclusion
processes and of interventions remains an open question that will re-
quire additional research in order to understand the scope and breadth Over the past 20 years, the behavioral economics of health-related
of trans disease processes. behavior change has developed into a growing and robust field that is

Please cite this article as: Bickel, W.K., et al., Some current dimensions of the behavioral economics of health-related behavior change, Prev. Med.
(2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.06.002
6 W.K. Bickel et al. / Preventive Medicine xxx (2016) xxx–xxx

continuing to develop new and refine existing interventions to promote Bickel, W.K., Koffarnus, M.N., Moody, L., Wilson, A.G., 2014b. The behavioral-and neuro-
economic process of temporal discounting: a candidate behavioral marker of addic-
change over an ever-expanding range of health behaviors. As we have tion. Neuropharmacology 76, 518–527.
outlined above, this field is informed by novel conceptual systems, is in- Bickel, W.K., Landes, R.D., Kurth-Nelson, Z., Redish, A.D., 2014f. A quantitative signature of
herently translational, focuses on developing efficacious trans-disease self-control repair rate-dependent effects of successful addiction treatment. Clin.
Psycholl. Sci. 3, 140–153. http://dx.doi.org/10.177/2167702614528162.
interventions, researches a variety of different aspects of change in Bickel, W.K., DeGrandpre, R.J., Higgins, S.T., 1993. Behavioral economics: a novel experimen-
health behaviors, promotes recognition of trans-disease processes, and tal approach to the study of drug dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 33, 173–192.
leverages technology to extend reach and cost-effectiveness. Without Bickel, W.K., Marsch, L.A., Buchhalter, A.R., Badger, G.J., 2008. Computerized behavior
therapy for opioid-dependent outpatients: a randomized controlled trial. Exp. Clin.
question the magnitude and adverse impacts of highly prevalent and Psychopharm. 16 (2), 132.
unhealthy behavior patterns remain large and challenging. The ways Bickel, W.K., Miller, M.L., Yi, R., Kowal, B.P., Lindquist, D.M., Pitcock, J.A., 2007. Behavioral
that behavioral economics may change and evolve to assist in meeting and neuroeconomics of drug addiction: competing neural systems and temporal
discounting processes. Drug Alcohol Depend. 90, S85–S91.
those challenges over the next 20 years will define the future of the
Bickel, W.K., Moody, L., Quisenberry, A.J., Ramey, C.T., Sheffer, C.E., 2014a. A competing
field and may contribute to improved population health in the U.S. neurobehavioral decision system model of SES-related health and behavioral dispar-
and globally. We look forward to seeing the field's evolution and to ities. Prev. Med. 68, 37–43.
assisting in meeting emerging challenges. Bickel, W.K., Odum, A.L., Madden, G.J., 1999. Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay
discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology 146 (4), 447–454.
Bickel, W.K., Quisenberry, A.J., Moody, L., Wilson, A.G., 2014e. Therapeutic opportunities
for self-control repair in addiction and related disorders change and the limits of
Funding
change in trans-disease processes. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 3, 39–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1177/2167702614541260.
Preparation of this report was supported in part by, R01AA021529 Bickel, W.K., Quisenberry, A.J., Snider, S.E., 2016. Does impulsivity change rate dependent-
and F31AA024368 from the National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Al- ly following stimulant administration? A translational selective review and re-analy-
sis. Psychopharmacology 233 (1), 1–18.
coholism, U19CA157345 and R01DA034755 from the National Institute Bickel, W.K., Wilson, A.G., Franck, C.T., Mueller, E.T., Jarmolowicz, D.P., Koffarnus, M.N.,
of Drug Abuse, UH2DK109543 from the National Institute of Diabetes Fede, S.J., 2014c. Using crowdsourcing to compare temporal, social temporal, and
and Digestive and Kidney Diseases, R01HD075669 and R01HD078332 probability discounting among obese and non-obese individuals. Appetite 75,
82–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.12.018.
from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, Bickel, W.K., Yi, R., Landes, R.D., Hill, P.F., Baxter, C., 2011. Remember the future: working
and Center of Biomedical Research Excellence award P20GM103644 memory training decreases delay discounting among stimulant addicts. Biol. Psychi-
from the National Institute of General Medical Sciences. atry 69 (3), 260–265.
Björkdahl, A., Åkerlund, E., Svensson, S., Esbjörnsson, E., 2013. A randomized study of
computerized working memory training and effects on functioning in everyday life
Declaration on conflicts of interest for patients with brain injury. Brain Inj. 27 (13–14), 1658–1665.
Bradstreet, M.P., Higgins, S.T., McClernon, F.J., Kozink, R.V., Skelly, J.M., Washio, Y., Lopez,
A.A., Parry, M.A., 2014. Examining the effects of initial smoking abstinence on re-
The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare. sponse to smoking-related stimuli and response inhibition in a human laboratory
model. Psychopharmacology 231 (10), 2145–2158. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
s00213-013-3360-x (Epub 2013 Dec 15).
Transparency document Brunello, G., Michaud, P., Sanz-de-Galdeano, A., 2008. The Rise in Obesity Across the At-
lantic: An Economic Perspective. Institute for the Study of Labor, Bonn, Germany.
Campbell, A.N., Nunes, E.V., Matthews, A.G., Stitzer, M., Miele, G.M., Polsky, D., ... Ghitza,
The Transparency document related to this article can be found in U.E., 2014. Internet-delivered treatment for substance abuse: a multisite randomized
the online version. controlled trial. Am. J. Psychiatry 171 (6), 683–690. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.
ajp.2014.13081055.
Carr, K.A., Daniel, T.O., Lin, H., Epstein, L.H., 2011. Reinforcement pathology and obesity.
References Curr. Drug Abuse Rev. 4 (3), 190.
Chesson, H.W., 2012. Sexonomics: a commentary and review of selected sexually trans-
Alessi, S.M., Petry, N.M., 2003. Pathological gambling severity is associated with impulsiv- mitted disease studies in the economics literature. Sex. Transm. Dis. 39 (3), 161–166.
ity in a delay discounting procedure. Behav. Process. 64 (3), 345–354. Chivers, L.L., Hand, D.J., Priest, J.S., Higgins, S.T., 2016. E-cigarette use among women of re-
Andrade, L.F., Petry, N.M., 2012. Delay and probability discounting in pathological gam- productive age: cigarette smoking status, impulsivity, and other predictors. Prev.
blers with and without a history of substance use problems. Psychopharmacology Med. (in press).
219 (2), 491–499. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-011-2508-9. Christensen, D.R., Landes, R.D., Jackson, L., Marsch, L.A., Mancino, M.J., Chopra, M.P., Bickel,
Anzman-Frasca, S., Mueller, M.P., Sliwa, S., Dolan, P.R., Harelick, L., Roberts, S.B., W.K., 2014. Adding an internet-delivered treatment to an efficacious treatment pack-
Washburn, K., Economos, C.D., 2015. Changes in children's meal orders following age for opioid dependence. J. Consult. Clin. Psych. 82 (6), 964–972.
healthy menu modifications at a regional U.S. restaurant chain. Obesity 23 (5), Codagnone, C., Bogliacino, F., Veltri, G.A., Lupiáñez-Villanueva, F., Gaskell, G., 2014. Nudg-
1055–1062. ing in the World of International Policymaking.
Baer, D.M., Wolf, M.M., Risley, T.R., 1968. Some current dimensions of applied behavior Cravener, T.L., Schlechter, H., Loeb, K.L., Radnitz, C., Schwartz, M., Zucker, N., Finkelstein, S.,
analysis. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 1 (1), 91–97. Wang, Y.C., Rolls, B.J., Keller, K.L., 2015. Feeding strategies derived from behavioral eco-
Bickel, W.K., 2015. Discounting of delayed rewards as an endophenotype. Biol. Psychiatry nomics and psychology can increase vegetable intake in children as part of a home-
77 (10), 846–847. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.03.003. based intervention: results of a pilot study. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 115 (11), 1798–1807.
Bickel, W.K., Marsch, L.A., 2001. Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug de- Dallery, J., Raiff, B.R., 2011. Contingency management in the 21st century: technological
pendence: delay discounting processes. Addiction 96 (1), 73–86. innovations to promote smoking cessation. Subst. Use Misuse 46 (1), 10–22.
Bickel, W.K., Mueller, E.T., 2009. Toward the study of trans-disease processes: a novel ap- Daniel, T.O., Stanton, C.M., Epstein, L.H., 2013a. The future is now reducing impulsivity
proach with special reference to the study of co-morbidity. J. Dual Diagn. 5 (2), and energy intake using episodic future thinking. Psychol. Sci. http://dx.doi.org/10.
131–138. 1177/0956797613488780.
Bickel, W.K., Vuchinich, R.E., 2000. Reframing Health Behavior Change With Behavioral Daniel, T.O., Stanton, C.M., Epstein, L.H., 2013b. The future is now: comparing the effect of
Economics. Psychology Press. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Mahwah, NJ. episodic future thinking on impulsivity in lean and obese individuals. Appetite 71,
Bickel, W.K., Yi, R., 2007. Temporal discounting as a measure of executive function: in- 120–125.
sights from the competing neuro-behavioral decision system hypothesis of addiction. Davis, R.D., Kurti, A.N., Redner, R., White, T.J., Higgins, S.T., 2016. A review of th literature
Adv. Health Econ. Health Serv. Res. 20, 289–309. on contingency management in the treatment of substance use disorders, 2009–
Bickel, W.K., Jarmolowicz, D.P., MacKillop, J., Epstein, L.H., Carr, K., Mueller, E.T., Waltz, T.J., 2015. Prev. Med. (in press).
2012b. The behavioral economics of reinforcement pathologies: Novel approaches to DeFulio, A., Silverman, K., 2012. The use of incentives to reinforce medication adherence.
addictive disorders. In: Shaffer, H., LaPlante, D., Nelson, S. (Eds.), APA Addiction Syn- Prev Med. 55S1, S86–S94.
drome Handbook, Vol. 2: Recovery, Prevention, and Other Issues. APA hanDbooks in Dews, P., 1977. Rate-dependency hypothesis. Science 80 (198), 1182–1183. http://dx.doi.
Psychology. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, US http://dx.doi. org/10.1126/science.563103.
org/10.1037/13750-014 (vii). Epstein, L.H., Saelens, B.E., 2000. Behavioral economics of obesity: food intake and energy
Bickel, W.K., Jarmolowicz, D.P., Mueller, E.T., Koffarnus, M.N., Gatchalian, K.M., 2012a. Ex- expenditure. Reframing Health Behavior Change With Behavioral Economics,
cessive discounting of delayed reinforcers as a trans-disease process contributing to pp. 293–311.
addiction and other disease-related vulnerabilities: emerging evidence. Pharmacol. Epstein, L.H., Salvy, S.J., Carr, K.A., Dearing, K.K., Bickel, W.K., 2010. Food reinforcement,
Therapeut. 134 (3), 287–297. delay discounting and obesity. Physiol. Behav. 100 (5), 438–445.
Bickel, W.K., Johnson, M.W., Koffarnus, M.N., MacKillop, J., Murphy, J.G., 2014d. The be- Green, L., Fristoe, N., Myerson, J., 1994. Temporal discounting and preference reversals in
havioral economics of substance use disorders: reinforcement pathologies and their choice between delayed outcomes. Psych Bull Rev. Sep;1 (3), 383–389. http://dx.doi.
repair. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 10, 641–677. org/10.3758/BF03213979.

Please cite this article as: Bickel, W.K., et al., Some current dimensions of the behavioral economics of health-related behavior change, Prev. Med.
(2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.06.002
W.K. Bickel et al. / Preventive Medicine xxx (2016) xxx–xxx 7

Hallal, P.C., Andersen, L.B., Bull, F.C., Guthold, R., Haskell, W., Ekelund, U., Group, Lancet benefit approach to weight loss maintenance. Prev. Med. (16), 30068 http://dx.doi.
Physical Activity Series Working, 2012. Global physical activity levels: surveillance org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.04.013 pii: S0091-7435.
progress, pitfalls, and prospects. Lancet 380 (9838), 247–257. Loewenstein, G., Asch, D.A., Friedman, J.Y., Melichar, L.A., Volpp, K.G., 2012. Can behav-
Halpern, S.D., French, B., Small, D.S., Saulsgiver, K., Harhay, M.O., Audrain-McGovern, J., ioural economics make us healthier? BMJ 344.
Loewenstein, G., Brennan, T.A., Asch, D.A., Volpp, K.G., 2015. Randomized trial of Lopez, A.A., Skelly, J.M., White, T.J., Higgins, S.T., 2015. Does impulsiveness moderate re-
four financial-incentive programs for smoking cessation. N. Engl. J. Med. 372, sponse to financial incentives for smoking cessation among pregnant and newly
2108–2117. postpartum women? Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 23 (2), 97–108. http://dx.doi.org/
Haplpern, S.D., Loewenstein, G., Volpp, K.G., Cooney, E., Vranas, K., Quill, C.M., McKenzie, 10.1037/a0038810 (Epub 2015 Mar 2).
M.S., Harhay, M.O., Gabler, N.B., Silva, T., Arnold, R., Angus, D.C., Bryce, C., 2013. De- Lussier, J.P., Heil, S.H., Mongeon, J.A., Badger, G.J., Higgins, S.T., 2006. A meta-analysis of
fault options in advance directives influence how patients set goals for end-of-life voucher-based reinforcement therapy for substance use disorders. Addiction 101
care. Health Aff. 32, 408–417. http://dx.doi.org/10.1377/hlthaff.2012.0895. (2), 192–203.
Henningfield, J.E., 2014. The tobacco endgame: it's all about behavior. Prev. Med. 68, Lussier, J.P., Higgins, S.T., Badger, G.J., 2005. Influence of the duration of abstinence on the
11–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2014.09.003. relative reinforcing effects of cigarette smoking. Psychopharmacology 181 (3),
Higgins, S.T., 1996. Some potential contributions of reinforcement and consumer-demand 486–495.
theory to reducing cocaine use. Addict. Behav. 21 (6), 803–816. MacKillop, J., Murphy, J.G., 2007. A behavioral economic measure of demand for alcohol
Higgins, S.T., 1997. The influence of alternative reinforcers on cocaine use and abuse: a predicts brief intervention outcomes. Drug Alcohol Depend. 89 (2), 227–233.
brief review. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 57 (3), 419–427. Meade, C.S., Lowen, S.B., MacLean, R.R., Key, M.D., Lukas, S.E., 2011. fMRI brain activation
Higgins, S.T., 2014. Behavior change, health, and health disparities: an introduction. Prev. during a delay discounting task in HIV-positive adults with and without cocaine de-
Med. 68, 1–4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2014.10.007. pendence. Psychiatry Res. 192 (3), 167–175.
Higgins, S.T., 2015a. Editorial: 2nd special issue on behavior change, health, and health Murphy, J.G., MacKillop, J., 2006. Relative reinforcing efficacy of alcohol among college
disparities. Prev. Med. 80, 1–4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2015.07.020 student drinkers. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 14 (2), 219. http://dx.doi.org/10.
(Epub 2015 Aug 6). 1037/1064-1297.14.2.219.
Higgins, S.T., 2015b. Borrowing from tobacco control to curtail the overweight and obesity Murphy, J.G., Correia, C.J., Barnett, N.P., 2007. Behavioral economic approaches to reduce
epidemic: leveraging the U.S. Surgeon General's Report. Prev. Med. 80, 47–49. http:// college student drinking. Addict. Behav. 32 (11), 2573–2585. http://dx.doi.org/10.
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2014.12.021 (Epub 2015 Jan 3). 1016/j.addbeh.2007.05.015.
Higgins, S.T., Budney, A.J., Bickel, W.K., Foerg, F.E., Donham, R., Badger, G.J., 1994. Incen- Murphy, J.G., Yurasek, A.M., Meshesha, L.Z., Dennhardt, A.A., Mackillop, J., Skidmore, J.R.,
tives improve outcome in outpatient behavioral treatment of cocaine dependence. Martens, M.P., 2014. Family history of problem drinking is associated with less sensi-
Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 51 (7), 568–576. tivity of alcohol demand to a next-day responsibility. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 75 (4),
Higgins, S.T., Heil, S.H., Dantona, R., Donham, R., Matthews, M., Badger, G.J., 2007. Effects of 653–663. http://dx.doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2014.75.653.
varying the monetary value of voucher-based incentives on abstinence achieved dur- Noël, X., Brevers, D., Bechara, A., 2013. A neurocognitive approach to understanding the
ing and following treatment among cocaine-dependent outpatients. Addiction 102 neurobiology of addiction. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23 (4), 632–638.
(2), 271–281. OECD, 2009. Health at a Glance.
Higgins, S.T., Heil, S.H., Lussier, J.P., 2004. Clinical implications of reinforcement as a deter- Olson, J., Hollenbeak, C., Donaldson, K., Abendroth, T., Castellani, W., 2015. Default setting
minant of substance use disorders. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 55, 431–436. of computerized physician order entry system order sets drive ordering habits.
Higgins, S.T., Sigmon, S.C., Heil, S.H., 2011. Contingency management in the treatment of J. Pathol. Inform. 6, 16.
substance use disorders: trends in the literature. In: Ruiz, P., Strain, E. (Eds.), Peters, J., Büchel, C., 2010. Episodic future thinking reduces reward delay discounting
Lowinson and Ruiz's Substance Abuse: A Comprehensive Textbook. Lippincott Wil- through an enhancement of prefrontal-mediotemporal interactions. Neuron 66 (1),
liams & Wilkins, Philadelphia, PA, pp. 603–621. 138–148.
Higgins, S.T., Sigmon, S.C., Wong, C.J., Heil, S.H., Badger, G.J., Donham, R., ... Anthony, S., Privitera, G.J., McGrath, H.K., Windus, B.A., Doraiswamy, P.M., 2015. Eat now or later: self-
2003. Community reinforcement therapy for cocaine-dependent outpatients. Arch. control as an overlapping cognitive mechanism of depression and obesity. PLoS One
Gen. Psychiatry 60 (10), 1043–1052. 10 (3), e0123136. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0123136.
Higgins, S.T., Silverman, K., Heil, S.H., 2008. Contingency Management in Substance Abuse Rachlin, H., Raineri, A., Cross, D., 1991. Subjective probability of delay. J. Exp. Anal. Behav.
Treatment. Guilford Press, New York, NY. 55 (2), 233–244.
Higgins, S.T., Silverman, K., Sigmon, S.C., Naito, N.A., 2012. Incentives and health: an intro- Radnitz, C., Loeb, K.L., DiMatteo, J., Keller, K.L., Zucker, N., Schwartz, M.B., 2013. Optimal
duction. Prev. Med. 55 (Suppl), S2–S6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2012.04. defaults in the prevention of pediatric obesity: from platform to practice. J. Food
008 (Epub 2012 Apr 20). Nutr. Disord. 2 (5).
Higgins, S.T., Solomon, L.J., 2016. Some recent developments on financial incentives for Rass, O., Schacht, R.L., Buckheit, K., Johnson, M.W., Strain, E.C., Mintzer, M.Z., 2015. A ran-
smoking cessation among pregnant and newly postpartum women. Curr Addict domized controlled trial of the effects of working memory training in methadone
Rep 3 (1), 9–18. maintenance patients. Drug Alcohol Depend. 156, 38–46.
Hofmann, W., Friese, M., Strack, F., 2009. Impulse and self-control from a dual-systems Reynolds, B., 2006. A review of delay-discounting research with humans: relations to
perspective. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 4 (2), 162–176. drug use and gambling. Behav. Pharmacol. 17 (8), 651–667.
Houben, K., Wiers, R.W., Jansen, A., 2011. Getting a grip on drinking behavior training Roll, J.M., Higgins, S.T., 2000. A within-subject comparison of three different schedules of
working memory to reduce alcohol abuse. Psychol. Sci. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ reinforcement of drug abstinence using cigarette smoking as an exemplar. Drug Alco-
0956797611412392. hol Depend. 58 (1–2), 103–109.
Hursh, S.R., Raslear, T.G., Shurtleff, D., Bauman, R., Simmons, L., 1988. A cost-benefit anal- Romanowich, P., Lamb, R.J., 2015. The effects of fixed versus escalating reinforcement
ysis of demand for food. J. Exper. Anal. Behav. 50 (3), 419–440. schedules on smoking abstinence. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 48 (1), 25–37.
Jacobs, E.A., Bickel, W.K., 1999. Modeling drug consumption in the clinic via simulation Samuelson, P.A., 1937. A note on measurement of utility. Rev. Econ. Stud. 4 (2), 155–161.
procedures: demand for heroin and cigarettes in opioid-dependent outpatients. Schroeder, S.A., 2007. We can do better—improving the health of the American people.
Exp. Clin. Psychopharm. 7, 412–426. New Engl. J. Med. 357 (12), 1221–1228.
Jeffery, R.W., 2012. Financial incentives and weight control. Prev. Med. 55S1, S61–S67. Schroeder, S.A., 2016. American health improvement depends upon addressing class dis-
Jemal, A., Bray, F., Center, M.M., Ferlay, J., Ward, E., Forman, D., 2011. Global cancer statis- parities. Prev. Med. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.02.024.
tics. CA Cancer J. Clin. 61 (2), 69–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.3322/caac.20107. Sheffer, C.E., Christensen, D.R., Landes, R., Carter, L.P., Jackson, L., Bickel, W.K., 2014. Delay
John, L.K., Loewenstein, G., Troxel, A.B., Norton, L., Fassbender, J.E., Volpp, K.G., Jun 1 2011. discounting rates: a strong prognostic indicator of smoking relapse. Addict. Behav. 39
Financial incentives for extended weight loss: a randomized, controlled trial. J. Gen. (11), 1682–1689. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.04.019.
Intern. Med. 26 (6), 621–626. Sheffer, C., MacKillop, J., McGeary, J., Landes, R., Carter, L., Yi, R., ... Jackson, L., 2012. Delay
Kahneman, D., Tversky, A., 1982. Variants of uncertainty. Cognition 11 (2), 143–157. discounting, locus of control, and cognitive impulsiveness independently predict to-
Kaplan, R.M., 2014. Behavior change and reducing health disparities. Prev. Med. 68, 5–10. bacco dependence treatment outcomes in a highly dependent, lower socioeconomic
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2014.04.014. group of smokers. Am. J. Addict. 21 (3), 221–232.
Kessler, J.B., Zhang, C.Y., 2014. Behavioral Economics and Health. Paper for Oxford Text- Silverman, K., Higgins, S.T., Brooner, R.K., Montoya, I.D., Cone, E.J., Schuster, C.R.,
book of Public Health(Available at:) http://assets.wharton.upenn.edu/~czhan/ Preston, K.L., 1996. Sustained cocaine abstinence in methadone maintenance pa-
KesslerZhang_BehavioralEconomicsHealth.pdf. (Accessed on: 3/23/2016). tients through voucher-based reinforcement therapy. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 53
Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P.J., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., Dahlström, K., Gillberg, (5), 409–415.
C.G., Forssberg, H., Westerberg, H., 2005. Computerized training of working memory Skinner, B.F., 1953. Science and Human Behavior: Simon and Schuster.
in children with ADHD-a randomized, controlled trial. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psy- Snider, S.E., LaConte, S.M., Bickel, W.K., 2016. Episodic future thinking: expansion of the
chiatry 44 (2), 177–186. temporal window in alcohol dependence. Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res.
Krishnan-Sarin, S., Cavallo, D.A., Cooney, J.L., Schepis, T.S., Kong, G., Liss, T.B., McMahon, Stein, J.S., Wilson, A.G., Koffarnus, M.N., Daniel, T.O., Epstein, L.H., Bickel, W.K., 2016. Un-
T.J., Nich, C., 2013. An exploratory randomized controlled trial of a novel high- stuck in time: episodic future thinking reduces delay discounting and cigarette
school-based smoking cessation intervention for adolescent smokers using absti- smoking. Psychol. Sci. (under review).
nence-contingent incentives and cognitive behavioral therapy. Drug Alcohol Depend. Stoops, W.W., Dallery, J., Fields, N.M., Nuzzo, P.A., Schoenberg, N.E., Martin, C.A., ... Wong,
132 (1–2), 346–351. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.03.002. C.J., 2009. An internet-based abstinence reinforcement smoking cessation interven-
Kurti, A.N., Davis, D., Redner, R., Zavorski, I., Keith, D.R., White, T.J., Rippberger, P., tion in rural smokers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 105 (1), 56–62.
Markeisch, C., Higgins, S.T., 2016. A review of the literature on remote monitoring Sunstein, C.R., 2014. Nudging: a very short guide. J. Consum. Policy 583 (37).
technology in incentive-based interventions for health-related behavior change. Taleb, N.N., 2012. Antifragile: Things That Gain From Disorder. 3. Random House
Trans. Issues Psychol. Sci. (in press). Incorporated.
Leahey, T.M., Fava, J.L., Seiden, A., Fernandes, D., Doyle, C., Kent, K., LaRue, M., Mitchell, M., Thaler, R., Sunstein, C., 2008. Nudge: The Gentle Power of Choice Architecture. Yale, New
Wing, R.R., 2016. A randomized controlled trial testing an internet delivered cost- Haven, Conn.

Please cite this article as: Bickel, W.K., et al., Some current dimensions of the behavioral economics of health-related behavior change, Prev. Med.
(2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.06.002
8 W.K. Bickel et al. / Preventive Medicine xxx (2016) xxx–xxx

Thorndike, E.L., 1911. Animal Intelligence: Experimental Studies. Macmillan and Compa- Volpp, K.G., John, L.K., Troxel, A.B., Norton, L., Fassbender, J., Loewenstein, G., 2008. Finan-
ny, New York, NYhttps://archive.org/stream/animalintelligen00thor#page/n5/mode/ cial incentive-based approaches for weight loss: a randomized trial. JAMA 300 (22),
2up (Accessed February 17, 2016). 2631–2637.
Tucker, J.A., Foushee, H.R., Black, B.C., 2008. Behavioral economic analysis of natural reso- Weller, R.E., Cook, I.I.I., Edwin, W., Avsar, K.B., Cox, J.E., 2008. Obese women show greater
lution of drinking problems using IVR self-monitoring. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. delay discounting than healthy-weight women. Appetite 51 (3), 563–569.
16 (4), 332. White, T.J., Redner, R., Skelly, J.M., Higgins, S.T., 2014. Examining educational attainment,
Tucker, J.A., Roth, D.L., Huang, J., Crawford, M.S., Simpson, C.A., 2012. Effects of interactive prepregnancy smoking rate, and delay discounting as predictors of spontaneous quit-
voice response self-monitoring on natural resolution of drinking problems: utiliza- ting among pregnant smokers. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2014 Oct;22 (5),
tion and behavioral economic factors. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 73 (4), 686. 384–391. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0037492.
Tucker, J.A., Roth, D.L., Vignolo, M.J., Westfall, A.O., 2009. A behavioral economic reward Woolf, S.H., Aron, L., 2013. US Health in International Perspective: Shorter Lives, Poorer
index predicts drinking resolutions: moderation revisited and compared with other Health. National Academies Press.
outcomes. J. Consult. Clin. Psych. 77 (2), 219. World Health Organization, 2009. Global Health Risks: Mortality and Burden of Disease
Tversky, A., Kahneman, D., 1974. Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Sci- Attributable to Selected Major Risks. World Health Organization.
ence 185 (4157), 1124–1131. World Health Organization, 2011. Global Information System on Alcohol and Health.
Tversky, A., Kahneman, D., 1981. The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Yi, R., Mitchell, S.H., Bickel, W.K., 2010. Delay discounting and substance abuse-depen-
Science 211 (4481), 453–458. dence. In: Madden, G., Bickel, W. (Eds.), Impulsivity: The Behavioral and Neurological
Ugur, Z.B., 2015. Does presumed consent save lives? Evidence from Europe. Health Econ. Science of Discounting xvi. Washington, DC, US, American Psychological Association,
24 (12), 1560–1572. p. 453.
Verbeken, S., Braet, C., Goossens, L., der Oord, V., Saskia, 2013. Executive function training Yoon, J.H., Higgins, S.T., MP, B., GJ, B., CS, T., 2009. Changes in the relative reinforcing ef-
with game elements for obese children: A novel treatment to enhance self-regulatory fects of cigarette smoking as a function of initial abstinence. Psychopharmacology
abilities for weight-control. Behav. Res. Ther. 51 (6), 290–299. (Berl) 205 (2), 305–318. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-0091541-4.

Please cite this article as: Bickel, W.K., et al., Some current dimensions of the behavioral economics of health-related behavior change, Prev. Med.
(2016), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.06.002
View publication stats

You might also like