Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Journal of Intellectual Disability Research doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2008.01105.

x
3
volume 53 part 1 pp 3–10 january 2009

Working memory functioning in children with learning


disabilities: does intelligence make a difference?
C. Maehler1 & K. Schuchardt2
1 University of Hildesheim, Hildesheim, Germany
2 University of Goettingen, Goettingen, Germany

Abstract However, unexpectedly, there were no differences


between the two groups of children with disabilities
Background Children with learning disabilities are
(normal vs. low IQ).
identified by their severe learning problems and
Conclusions These findings do not support the
their deficient school achievement. On the other
notion of different cognitive functioning because of
hand, children with sub-average school achievement
differences in intelligence of these two groups. In
and sub-average intellectual development are
the ongoing discussion about the role of intelligence
thought to suffer from a general intellectual delay
(especially as to the postulated discrepancy between
rather than from specific learning disabilities. The
intelligence and school achievement in diagnosis
open question is whether these two groups are char-
and special education), our findings might lead to
acterised by differences in their cognitive function-
rethinking the current practice of treating these two
ing. The present study explored several functions of
groups as fundamentally different.
working memory.
Method A working memory battery with tasks for Keywords working memory, intellectual disabili-
the phonological loop, the visual–spatial sketchpad ties, learning disabilities
and central executive skills was presented in indi-
vidual sessions to 27 children with learning disabili- Introduction
ties and normal IQ (ICD-10: mixed disorders of
scholastic skills), 27 children with learning disabili- Working memory deficits are being widely discussed
ties and low IQ (intellectual disabilities), and a and identified as possible causal factors underlying
control group of 27 typically developing children learning disabilities. Although various models of
with regular school achievement levels and normal working memory have been developed, the British
IQ. model by Baddeley (1986) has proved a particularly
Results The results reveal an overall deficit in useful theoretical tool in numerous studies on learn-
working memory of the two groups with learning ing disabilities. According to this model, working
disabilities compared with the control group. memory comprises three components: the modality-
free central executive, which is a kind of supervisory
Correspondence: Prof. Dr Claudia Maehler, Institute for Psychol-
system that serves to control and regulate the occur-
ogy, Marienburger Platz 22, D-31141 Hildesheim, Germany ing cognitive processes, and two slave systems, the
(e-mail: maehler@uni-hildesheim.de). phonological loop and the visual–spatial sketchpad.

© 2008 The Authors. Journal Compilation © 2008 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Journal of Intellectual Disability Research volume 53 part 1 january 2009
4
C. Maehler & K. Schuchardt • Working memory in children with learning disabilities

The functions of the central executive identified by to the fact that insufficient information is given
Baddeley (1996) include (1) coordinating the slave about the characteristics of the groups of children
systems; (2) focusing and switching attention; and with learning disabilities, hence it is not possible to
(3) retrieving representations from long-term precisely define the children as suffering from a
memory. The two slave systems perform modality- double deficit, or the existing studies were limited
specific operations. Verbal and auditory information to a small number of working memory tasks and
is temporarily stored and processed in the phono- therefore could not yield a comprehensive assess-
logical loop. Two components of the phonological ment of the working memory system. Van der Sluis
loop are distinguished: the phonological store and et al. (2005) found a deficit only in the central
the subvocal rehearsal process. The visual–spatial executive subsystem that could be interpreted as
sketchpad is concerned with remembering and combination of the minor deficits of either reading
processing visual and spatial information; it com- disabled or arithmetically disabled children. In a
prises a visual cache for static visual information and study performed by our own lab (Schuchardt et al.
an inner scribe for dynamic spatial information in press), we used a broad battery of working
(Logie 1995; Pickering et al. 2001). memory measures to assess phonological, visual–
Research has provided numerous indications that spatial, and central executive functioning in children
specific learning disabilities are associated with with specific disorders of arithmetical skills, specific
working memory impairments (Alloway & Gather- reading disorders, and mixed disorders of scholastic
cole 2006; Pickering 2006a). There is considerable skills, and in a control group of peers with normal
evidence that children with specific reading disabili- achievement levels. Altogether, results confirm the
ties have deficits in phonological processing and value of using a comprehensive battery of measures
storage (Vellutino et al. 2004; Pickering 2006b; to assess the cognitive memory deficits of children
Swanson 2006). Further evidence suggests that with clinically relevant learning disorders. While
these children also experience deficits in central previous findings have been mixed, a direct com-
executive functioning (Landerl et al. 2004; Pickering parison of different learning disorders within a
2006b). However, relatively few reports exist with single study design provides broad support to dis-
regard to impairments of the visual–spatial working tinct patterns of deficits. Children with impairments
memory of children with reading disabilities (Kibby in just one domain clearly outperformed children
et al. 2004; Pickering 2006b). with combined arithmetic and reading disorders on
Empirical findings on children with specific arith- almost all the tasks administered in the present
metic learning disabilities are also available for all study. The results indicate that these children
three domains of working memory (Passolunghi exhibit both deficits, i.e. those found for specific
2006). Here, the central executive seems to be par- disorders of arithmetic skills and for specific dis-
ticularly impaired (Geary et al. 1999, 2000; Passol- orders of reading/writing, and to a greater extent
unghi & Siegel 2001; Swanson & Sachse-Lee 2001), (see also Pickering & Gathercole 2004).
while findings on the phonological loop are incon- With regard to general intellectual disabilities
sistent (see Swanson & Sachse-Lee 2001 vs. Geary (ID, low intelligence) working memory performance
et al. 1999, 2000 or Landerl et al. 2004). Hence, seems to depend strongly on the severity of ID
deficits in the phonological loop may not be a (Henry 2001). While children at borderline of ID
defining characteristic for children with arithmetic only showed deficits in phonological working
learning disabilities. Recent studies (Van der Sluis memory, children with mild or moderate ID were
et al. 2005; Passolunghi 2006) have also reported characterised by overall deficits in the different sub-
visual–spatial deficits in children with specific arith- systems of working memory. Accordingly, working
metic disabilities (but see also Bull et al. 1999; memory functioning seems to be strongly related to
Geary et al. 2000). mental age and is consistent with a developmental
So far, little research has been performed regard- delay theory of mild ID (Henry 2002; Van der
ing children who show specific developmental dis- Molen et al. 2007). We have recently obtained
orders in both areas of scholastic skills (arithmetics similar results (overall deficit) from a study with
and reading and/or spelling skills). This is either due children of subnormal intelligence (IQ 55–85), the

© 2008 The Authors. Journal Compilation © 2008 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Journal of Intellectual Disability Research volume 53 part 1 january 2009
5
C. Maehler & K. Schuchardt • Working memory in children with learning disabilities

severest deficit being located in phonological All children were screened with standardised tests
working memory (Hasselhorn & Mähler 2007; of intellectual ability, spelling, reading and arithmet-
Mähler 2007). ics. We administered the full IQ scale from the
The diagnosis ‘mixed disorder of scholastic skills’ German version of the Kaufman Assessment
(ICD-10 F81.3) is given for a category of disorders Battery for Children (Melchers & Preuß 2001) to
where both arithmetical and reading or spelling skills assess general intelligence, and the score for non-
are significantly impaired, but the disorder cannot be verbal holistic thinking was used for matching the
explained in terms of general ID or inadequate groups. Spelling abilities were assessed by using the
schooling. The essential criterion is the discrepancy Weingartener spelling tests for children from second
between (normal) intelligence and (sub-average) and third grades ( WRT 2+, Birkel 1994a; WRT 3+,
performance in standardised school achievement Birkel 1994b) and the Westermann spelling test was
tests. Children showing a sub-average school applied to children in the fourth grade ( WRT 4/5,
achievement level and sub-average intellectual devel- Rathenow 1979). In both of these standardised
opment are thought to suffer from a general intellec- German tests, children insert dictated words into
tual delay rather than from learning disabilities and given sentences. Reading was tested by using the
therefore do not receive this diagnosis. The open Salzburg Reading and Spelling Test (Landerl et al.
question is whether these two groups are character- 1997). Mathematical skills were assessed by using
ised by different cognitive functioning, especially by standardised German mathematics tests for the
distinct patterns of working memory functioning. second, third and fourth graders (DEMAT 2+,
Therefore, we addressed two questions: Krajewski et al. 2004; DEMAT 3+, Roick et al.
1 Are there specific working memory deficits 2004; DEMAT 4, Gölitz et al. 2006). These multi-
underlying learning disabilities? component tests include computation problems,
2 Does intelligence make a difference, i.e. are there word problems and geometry problems.
differences in working memory between children Table 1 summarises the data describing the three
with learning disabilities showing normal vs. subgroups.
subnormal levels of intelligence? The control group showed an average level of
performance in all these measures. ID- and MDSS-
Methods groups displayed the typical pattern of deficits in
spelling, reading and mathematics, ID children
Participants
performing better but still on a sub-average level
Three groups of children participated in the study. (T < 40) in spelling and reading. The ID group is
Twenty-seven children received the diagnosis ‘mixed defined by the lower IQ-score. There is a significant
disorder of scholastic skills’ (MDSS-group, ICD-10 difference in intelligence between the two groups of
F81.3, discrepancy between normal intelligence and children with learning disabilities [t (44) = 10.17,
sub-average scholastic skills). Another 27 children P = 0.00] and between the ID- and the control
with comparable learning disabilities did not meet group [t (46) = 8.53, P = 0.00], whereas the MDSS-
the diagnostic criteria because of the lack of dis- group is not different from the control group
crepancy between scholastic skills and intelligence [t (44) = 0.33, n. s.).
(ID group, i.e. ID-group, IQ 55–85). These two
groups of children with learning disabilities were
Tasks
compared with a control group (C-group) of 27
children without any specific developmental disor- Working memory was assessed by a battery of 14
ders of scholastic skills. Children from grades two, tasks: five phonological tasks (memory span for
three or four with German as a native language digits, one-syllable and three-syllable words, one-
were included. The two groups of children with syllable non-words, non-word repetition), five
learning disabilities were recruited from our coun- visual–spatial tasks (memory span for locations,
selling centre; all of them attended regular primary matrix span simple and complex, corsi-block simple
schools but did not reach the required levels of and complex), four central executive tasks (double
achievement. span, backward spans for one-syllable words and

© 2008 The Authors. Journal Compilation © 2008 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Journal of Intellectual Disability Research volume 53 part 1 january 2009
6
C. Maehler & K. Schuchardt • Working memory in children with learning disabilities

Table 1 Sex distribution and means


ID (n = 27) MDSS (n = 27) C (n = 27) (Standard Deviations) of age, K-ABCIQ,
WRT spelling T-scores, SLT text reading
T-Scores, and DEMAT mathematics
Sex (m/f) 11/16 13/14 13/14 T-scores by subgroups
Age (months) 106.63 (12.45) 107.63 (12.35) 107.59 (10.58)
K-ABC IQ 75.44 (7.49) 100.08 (8.25) 101.04 (11.44)
Spelling 36.74 (8.22) 32.22 (5.54) 48.33 (6.91)
Text reading 37.64 (9.60) 33.80 (8.11) 49.15 (8.68)
Mathematics 34.00 (10.12) 32.33 (6.20) 50.11 (8.15)

ID, children with intellectual disabilities; MDSS, children with and mixed disorders of
scholastic skills; C, normally achieving control children matched for chronological age;
K-ABC, Assessment Battery for Children; WRT Weingarten Spelling Test; SLT, Salzburg
Reading and Spelling Test; DEMAT, German Mathematics Test.

digits, counting span). Tasks were presented in a correct order. Corsi-block tasks were used to assess
fixed order: location span, double span, one- and the dynamic spatial component of visual–spatial
three-syllable word span, corsi-block simple and working memory. The experimenter taps a sequence
complex, non-word repetition, backward word span, of red blocks on a grey board at the rate of one per
backward digit span, counting span, matrix span second. The child then attempts to reproduce the
simple and complex, digit span, one-syllable non- sequence of taps in the correct order. We used two
word span. A detailed description of all tasks variations of the Corsi-block task: simple sequences
follows below. involving short distances between blocks without
path crossings, and complex sequences involving
Phonological loop long distances between blocks with path crossings.
A matrix span task was used to measure the static
The digit span is the conventional measure used to
component of visual–spatial memory. Patterns of
assess the short-term phonological capacity. A series
white and black squares in a 4 ¥ 4 matrix were pre-
of digits (1–9) was presented acoustically at a rate
sented on the computer, beginning with two black
of one digit per second, starting with two and
squares and continuing up to a maximum of eight
continuing up to a maximum of eight digits per
black squares. Immediately after presentation, chil-
sequence. Participants had to repeat the digits
dren were asked to reproduce the pattern in an
immediately in the given order. The one-syllable
empty matrix. Two variations of this task were also
(e.g. Stern = star, Fisch = fish) and three-syllable
implemented: a simple matrix span with the black
word span tasks (e.g. Erdbeere = strawberry,
squares arranged in simple patterns, and a complex
Briefkasten = letterbox) and the one-syllable non-word
matrix span with the black squares located at some
span tasks (e.g. fen, sim) were presented in the same
distance from one another.
manner as in the digit span measure. In the non-
word repetition task children had to repeat 24 word-
like non-words of two, three or four syllables (e.g. Central executive
vorluch, karflumen, sulibritzen) immediately after
The same items and procedures were used for the
their presentation. Non-words of different lengths
backward digit and word span tasks as for the
were presented acoustically in random order. The
forward spans, the only difference being that par-
number of correctly repeated non-words was taken
ticipants were required to recall the sequences of
as the score for this task.
items in reverse order. Additionally, a double span
task was implemented to assess the children’s ability
Visual–spatial sketchpad
to coordinate the functioning of the phonological
In the location span task, children were shown a loop and the visual–spatial sketchpad (see also
series of green dots at different locations on a 3 ¥ 3 Towse & Houston-Price 2001 for justification of
matrix and asked to recall these locations in the such a task). Pictures of well-known objects were

© 2008 The Authors. Journal Compilation © 2008 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Journal of Intellectual Disability Research volume 53 part 1 january 2009
7
C. Maehler & K. Schuchardt • Working memory in children with learning disabilities

presented in different locations on a 3 ¥ 3 matrix. of items repeated in correct order. Children were
Children had to recall the pictures and their loca- credited an extra 0.25 point if they repeated a
tion in the order of presentation. The complex further sequence of the same length correctly (e.g.
counting span task (cf. Case et al. 1982) was admin- a score of 5.25 was awarded if two of four five-item
istered to assess storage and processing efficiency. sequences were recalled correctly, 5.5 if three of
Increasing series of maps (maximum eight maps) four sequences, and 5.75 if all four sequences were
with yellow circles (target items) and squares recalled correctly).
(distractor items) were presented in a random,
computer-generated pattern. Children were Results
instructed to always count the number of circles.
Performance on the different memory span tasks
Finally, the experimenter asked the child to recall
was measured for the three groups of participants
the number of circles counted on each map.
(ID, MDSS, C), and group comparisons were
carried out separately for the three subsystems of
Stop criterion
working memory. The significance level of all analy-
We used the same stop criterion for all span tasks. ses was set at alpha = 0.05.
The length of the sequences presented was gradu- Table 2 informs about the performance of the
ally increased, beginning with a minimum of two, children on the different tasks, sorted by subsystems
and increasing to a maximum of eight items. There of working memory (phonological loop, visual–
were four trials at each sequence length. If a child spatial sketchpad and central executive).
succeeded on two successive trials of the same The first question of the study was what kind of
length, the task continued with the next span deficits children with learning disabilities show
length. If a child failed on two successive trials of compared with unimpaired control peers. To answer
the same length, he or she was not presented with this question, we compared MDSS-children with
any further sequences of the same length, but with controls (both groups with normal IQ) for each
a sequence one item shorter. The dependent subsystem separately. The scores of the six tasks
measure for all span tasks was the longest sequence assessing phonological loop functioning were entered

Table 2 Means (Standard Deviations) for


ID (n = 27) MDSS (n = 27) C (n = 27) working memory measures by subgroups

Phonological loop
Digit span 4.23 (0.80) 4.20 (0.44) 5.08 (0.74)
One-syllable word span 3.95 (0.67) 3.86 (0.54) 4.51 (0.66)
Three-syllable word span 3.34 (0.41) 3.35 (0.48) 3.73 (0.49)
One-syllable non-word 3.54 (0.52) 3.38 (0.89) 4.14 (0.58)
span
Non-word repetition 18.67 (3.61) 18.26 (4.29) 20.81 (1.52)
Visual–spatial sketchpad
Location span 4.20 (0.70) 4.61 (0.79) 5.18 (0.84)
Corsi-block simple 4.57 (1.59) 5.30 (1.20) 5.85 (1.21)
Corsi-block complex 3.77 (1.16) 4.20 (1.24) 4.91 (0.84)
Matrix span simple 5.12 (1.73) 5.86 (1.34) 6.78 (1.33)
Matrix span complex 3.11 (0.91) 3.50 (1.39) 4.79 (1.56)
Central executive
Backward digit span 3.01 (0.71) 3.34 (0.44) 3.81 (0.64)
Backward word span 3.15 (0.46) 3.26 (0.37) 3.65 (0.54)
Double span 3.07 (0.80) 3.53 (0.66) 4.04 (0.69)
Counting span 3.31 (0.73) 3.23 (0.61) 4.29 (0.87)

ID, children with intellectual disabilities; MDSS, children with mixed disorders of scholastic
skills; C, normally achieving control children matched for chronological age.

© 2008 The Authors. Journal Compilation © 2008 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Journal of Intellectual Disability Research volume 53 part 1 january 2009
8
C. Maehler & K. Schuchardt • Working memory in children with learning disabilities

into a manova. The multivariate main effect, F5,49 < 1; central executive: F4,49 = 2.44, P > 0.05).
F6,47 = 5.83, P < 0.001, proved to be significant. The Thus, the different levels of intelligence held by the
univariate tests showed significant differences two groups of children with learning disabilities (ID
between groups for all phonological tasks (digit vs. MDSS) did not correspond with differences in
span: F1,52 = 27.85, MSE = 0.38, P < 0.001; one- working memory performance.
syllable word span: F1,52 = 15.75, MSE = 0.36,
P < 0.001; three-syllable word span: F1,52 = 8.24,
Discussion
MSE = 0.24, P < 0.01; one-syllable non-word span:
F1,52 = 13.85, MSE = 0.56, P < 0.00; non-word Children with general learning disabilities (impair-
repetition: F1,52 = 8.50, MSE = 10.37, P < 0.01). ment of arithmetic and reading/spelling scholastic
In the same way, the scores of the five tasks skills) show deficits in all measured aspects of
assessing visual–spatial sketchpad were entered into a working memory functions. This result is in line
second manova: again, the multivariate group effect, with other studies (Pickering & Gathercole 2004)
F5,48 = 2.48, P < 0.05, proved to be significant. The and may lead to the conclusion that these children
univariate tests of visual–spatial sketchpad revealed are more severely impaired with respect to their
significant differences between groups for all visual– working memory than children with either dyslexia
spatial memory tasks (location span: F1,52 = 6.44, or dyscalculia, a fact that might explain the broader
MSE = 0.67, P < 0.05; corsi-block complex: learning disorder.
F1,52 = 5.95, MSE = 1.12, P < 0.05; matrix span However, unexpectedly, there were no significant
simple: F1,52 = 6.38, MSE = 1.78, P < 0.05; matrix differences between the two groups of children with
span complex: F1,52 = 10.42, MSE = 2.18, P < 0.01), disabilities: their working memory functioning did
with the exception of the corsi-block simple task, not differ despite an IQ difference of 23 points,
F1,52 = 2.76, MSE = 1.46, P > 0.05. which is equivalent to more than 1.5 standard
Third, the scores of the four tasks assessing central deviations. This result corroborates the notion that
executive were entered into a manova: here, the mul- working memory is associated with learning dis-
tivariate group effect, F4,49 = 7.52, P < 0.001, proved abilities irrespective of the intelligence level.
to be significant. Univariate tests showed significant Support for this finding comes from other studies
differences between groups on all central executive which conclude that working memory skills (espe-
memory tasks (digit backward span: F1,52 = 9.96, cially the performance in complex memory tasks
MSE = 0.30, P < 0.01; word backward span: similar to our central executive tasks) place an
F1,52 = 9.68, MSE = 0.21, P < 0.01; double span: important constraint on the acquisition of skill and
F1,52 = 7.37, MSE = 0.46, P < 0.01; counting knowledge in reading and mathematics. The impact
span: F1,52 = 26.44, MSE = 0.57, P < 0.001). In of this constraint seems to be independent from
general, the results reveal an overall deficit in intelligence (Gathercole et al. 2006).
working memory of children with learning disabili- Following this argument we agree with Dyck et al.
ties (MDSS-group). (2004), who doubt the validity of the discrepancy
The second and more interesting question of the criterion for defining developmental disorders.
study was whether normal vs. subnormal levels of According to their study, the severity of under-
intelligence constitute a crucial factor to the achievement, as measured in a standardised test that
working memory performance shown by children defines the relation to normal development, is the
with learning difficulties. As can easily be figured most important criterion, possibly combined with
out from Table 2, unexpectedly, there were no concurrent deficits in functionally related abilities.
obvious differences between the two groups of Consequently, learning disorders can be understood
children with disabilities. The statistical analysis as a substantial scholastic underachievement that is
(comparison between ID- and MDSS-group) was associated with working memory deficits explaining
performed once again for each working memory the learning disorder. Currently there is an ongoing
subsystem separately by manova, but none of the discussion on the appropriateness and justification of
multivariate group effects were significant (phono- the criterion of discrepancy for diagnosing learning
logical loop: F6,47 < 1; visual–spatial sketchpad: disorders, particularly regarding ‘mixed disorders of

© 2008 The Authors. Journal Compilation © 2008 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Journal of Intellectual Disability Research volume 53 part 1 january 2009
9
C. Maehler & K. Schuchardt • Working memory in children with learning disabilities

scholastic skills’ (Fletcher et al. 2003; Kavale & Baddeley A. D. (1986) Working Memory. University Press,
Forness 2003; Francis et al. 2005). Performance pro- Oxford.
files of children with specific learning disabilities and Baddeley A. D. (1996) Exploring the central executive.
children with more general ID seem to differ in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 49, 5–28.
some ways whereas they are very similar in other Birkel P. (1994a) Weingartener Grundwortschatz
Rechtschreib-Test für zweite und dritte Klassen ( WRT 2+)
aspects (as, for example, working memory in this
[Weingarten Basic Vocabulary Spelling Test for Second and
study). Hence, they do not provide a reason for Third Grades ( WRT 2+)]. Hogrefe, Göttingen.
relying on the criterion of discrepancy. Furthermore, Birkel P. (1994b) Weingartener Grundwortschatz
intervention studies did not yield specific outcomes Rechtschreib-Test für dritte und vierte Klassen ( WRT 3+)
that could be attributed to differences in intelligence [Weingarten Basic Vocabulary Spelling Test for Third and
( Weber et al. 2002). Taken together, little support is Fourth Grades ( WRT 3+)]. Hogrefe, Göttingen.
rendered to the clinical practice of treating learning Bull R., Johnston R. S. & Roy J. A. (1999) Exploring the
disabled children with or without normal levels of roles of the visual–spatial sketch pad and central execu-
tive in children’s arithmetical skills: views from cogni-
intelligence as fundamentally different (i.e. by
tion and developmental neuropsychology. Developmental
sending them to different schools, offering more Neuropsychology 15, 421–42.
support to children with learning disabilities who Case R. D., Kurland M. & Goldberg J. (1982) Operational
show a higher intelligence level). efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span.
Nevertheless, our results cannot support the Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 33, 386–404.
notion that working memory and intelligence are Dyck M. J., Hay D., Anderson M., Smith L. M., Piek J. &
independent from one another. Comprehensive evi- Hallmayer J. (2004) Is the discrepancy criterion for
dence suggests a strong relation between the two defining developmental disorders valid? Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry 45, 979–95.
constructs, and even that working memory is a
Fletcher J. M., Morris R. D. & Lyon G. R. (2003) Classi-
strong predictor of general fluid intelligence
fication and definition of learning disabilities: an inte-
(Oberauer et al. 2005). Different levels of intelli- grative perspective. In: Handbook of Learning Disabilities
gence come along with different working memory (eds H. L. Swanson, K. R. Harris & S. Graham),
profiles (Henry 2001). But our current data are at pp. 30–56. Guilford, New York.
odds with these existing findings. However, the aim Francis D. J., Fletcher J. M., Stuebing K. K., Lyon G. R.,
of our study was not, to predict intelligence or to Shaywitz B. A. & Shaywitz S. E. (2005) Psychometric
approaches to the identification of LD: IQ and achieve-
predict working memory but to explain learning dis-
ment scores are not sufficient. Journal of Learning
abilities. Perhaps our data reveal a special link Disabilities 38, 98–108.
between working memory and intelligence in chil-
Gathercole S. E., Alloway T. P., Willis C. S. & Adams A.
dren with learning disabilities: Working memory M. (2006) Working memory in children with reading
deficits might be so dominant in causing learning disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 93,
disorders that intelligence does no longer make a 265–81.
difference when working memory functioning falls Geary D. C., Hoard M. K. & Hamson C. O. (1999) Num-
below a certain threshold. Nevertheless, we do not erical and arithmetical cognition: patterns of functions
and deficits in children at risk for a mathematical disabil-
intend to suggest that similar deficits in working
ity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 74, 213–39.
memory in our two groups of children with disabili-
Geary D. C., Hamson C. O. & Hoard M. K. (2000)
ties will predict similar development and similar Numerical and arithmetical cognition: a longitudinal
coping with the learning disorders. Instead, the study of process and concept deficits in children with
question concerning the predictive validity of learning disability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychol-
working memory deficits for scholastic achievement ogy 77, 236–63.
is still open to future research. Gölitz D., Roick T. & Hasselhorn M. (2006) Deutscher
Mathematiktest für vierte Klassen (DEMAT 4) [German
Mathematics Test for Fourth Grades (DEMAT 4). Beltz,
Göttingen.
References
Hasselhorn M. & Mähler C. (2007) Phonological working
Alloway T. P. & Gathercole S. E. (eds) (2006) Working memory of children in two German special schools.
Memory and Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Psychology International Journal of Disability Development and Educa-
Press, Hove. tion 54, 225–44.

© 2008 The Authors. Journal Compilation © 2008 Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Journal of Intellectual Disability Research volume 53 part 1 january 2009
10
C. Maehler & K. Schuchardt • Working memory in children with learning disabilities

Henry L. A. (2001) How does the severity of a learning Pickering S. J. & Gathercole S. E. (2004) Distinctive
disability affect working memory performance? Memory working memory profiles in children with special educa-
9, 233–47. tional needs. Educational Psychology 24, 393–408.
Henry L. A. (2002) Working memory performances in Pickering S. J., Gathercole S. E., Hall M. & Lloyd S. A.
children with and without intellectual disabilities. (2001) Development of memory for pattern and path:
American Journal on Mental Retardation 107, 421–32. further evidence for the fractionation of visuo–spatial
Kavale K. A. & Forness S. R. (2003) Learning Disability memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychol-
as a Discipline. In: Handbook of Learning Disabilities (eds ogy 54, 397–420.
H. L. Swanson, K. R. Harris & S. Graham), pp. 76–93. Rathenow P. (1979) Westermann Rechtschreibtest 4/5
Guilford, New York. ( WRT 4/5). Westermann, Braunschweig.
Kibby M., Marks W., Morgan S. & Long C. (2004) Spe- Roick T., Gölitz D. & Hasselhorn M. (2004) Deutscher
cific impairment in developmental reading disabilities: a Mathematiktest für dritte Klassen (DEMAT 3+) [German
working memory approach. Journal of Learning Disabili- Mathematics Test for Third Grades (DEMAT 3+)]. Beltz,
ties 37, 349–63. Göttingen.
Krajewski K., Liehm S. & Schneider W. (2004) Deutscher Schuchardt K., Mähler C. & Hasselhorn M. Working
Mathematiktest für zweite Klassen (DEMAT 2+) [German memory deficits in German children with different
Mathematics Test for Second Grades (DEMAT 2+)]. Beltz, learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities
Göttingen. (in press).
Landerl K., Wimmer H. & Moser E. (1997) Salzburger Swanson H. L. (2006) Working memory and reading dis-
Lese- und Rechtschreibtest (SLT) [Salzburg Reading and abilities: both phonological and executive processing
Spelling Test (SLT)]. Huber, Bern. deficits are important. In: Working Memory and Neurode-
Landerl K., Bevan A. & Butterworth B. (2004) Develop- velopmental Disorders (eds T. P. Alloway & S. E.
mental dyscalculia and basic numerical capacities: A Gathercole), pp. 59–88. Psychology Press, Hove.
study of 8–9-year old students. Cognition 93, 99–125. Swanson H. L. & Sachse-Lee C. (2001) A subgroup
Logie R. H. (1995) Visual–Spatial Working Memory. analysis of working memory in children with reading
Erlbaum, Hove. disabilities: domain-general or domain-specific defi-
Mähler C. (2007) Arbeitsgedächtnisfunktionen bei lernbe- ciency? Journal of Learning Disabilities 34, 249–63.
hinderten Kindern und Jugendlichen. Zeitschrift für Towse J. N. & Houston-Price C. M. T. (2001) Combining
Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie 39, representations in working memory: a brief report.
97–106. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 19, 319–24.
Melchers P. & Preuß U. (2001) Assessment Battery for Van der Molen M. J., Van Luit J. E. H., Jongmans M. J. &
Children (K-ABC), German version, 5th edn. Hogrefe, Van der Molen M. W. (2007) Verbal working memory in
Göttingen. children with mild intellectual disabilities. Journal of
Oberauer K., Schulze R., Wilhelm O. & Süß H.-M. Intellectual Disabilities Research 51, 162–9.
(2005) Working memory andintelligence – their correla- Van der Sluis S., van der Leij A. & de Jong P. F. (2005)
tion and their relation: comment on Ackerman, Beier, Working memory in Dutch children with reading- and
and Boyle (2005). Psychological Bulletin 131, 61–5. arithmetic-related LD. Journal of Learning Disabilities
Passolunghi M. C. (2006) Working memory and arith- 38, 207–21.
metic learning disability. In: Working Memory and Neu- Vellutino F. R., Fletcher J. M., Snowling M. J. & Scanlon
rodevelopmental Disorders (eds T. P. Alloway & S. E. D. M. (2004) Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what
Gathercole), pp. 113–38. Psychology Press, Hove. have we learned in the past four decades. Journal of
Passolunghi M. C. & Siegel L. S. (2001) Short-term Child Psychology and Psychiatry 45, 2–40.
memory, working memory, and inhibitory control in Weber J.-M., Marx P. & Schneider W. (2002) Profitieren
children with specific arithmetic learning disabilities. Legastheniker und allgemein lese-rechtschreibschwache
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 80, 44–57. Kinder in unterschiedlichem Ausmaß von einem
Pickering S. J. (ed.) (2006a) Working Memory and Educa- Rechtschreibtraining? [Are there different remedial
tion. Academic Press, San Diego, CA. potentials for dyslexics and garden-variety poor
Pickering S. J. (2006b) Working memory in dyslexia. In: readers?] Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht 49,
Working Memory and Neurodevelopmental Disorders (eds 56–70.
T. P. Alloway & S. E. Gathercole), pp. 7–40. Psychology
Press, Hove. Accepted 25 June 2008

© 2008 The Authors. Journal Compilation © 2008 Blackwell Publishing Ltd

You might also like