Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

RESEARCH REPORTS

Clinical

D.W. McNeil1,2*, A.J. Helfer1,3,


B.D. Weaver4, R.W. Graves4,
Memory of Pain and
B.N. Kyle1, and A.M. Davis1,5 Anxiety Associated
1
Department of Psychology, West Virginia University, 53
Campus Drive, PO Box 6040, Morgantown, WV 26506-6040,
with Tooth Extraction
USA; and 2Department of Dental Practice and Rural Health,
3
Division of Dental Hygiene, and 4Department of Oral and
Maxillofacial Surgery, West Virginia University School of
Dentistry; and 5Department of Psychology, University of
Alabama; *corresponding author, Daniel.McNeil@mail.wvu
.edu

J Dent Res 90(2):220-224, 2011

Abstract Introduction
Concerns regarding pain constitute a large compo-
nent of dental anxiety, and patients with high den-
tal anxiety are likely to have exaggerated memory
D espite advances in dental care, population-based dental fear and anxiety
have remained stable since the 1960s (Smith and Heaton, 2003). (Fear and
anxiety are different constructs, but for simplicity of expression, dental anxi-
and prediction of dental pain. It remains to be
ety will be used as the terminology in the present paper.) Approximately 6% of
investigated, however, if memory of anxiety is
the population reports severe dental anxiety, and high levels of dental anxiety
exaggerated in a manner similar to that of pain,
are associated with dental non-attendance (Gatchel et al., 1983; Maggirias and
and if anxiety and pain assimilate in memory over
Locker, 2002); if left untreated, dental anxiety can persist across decades of
time. A sample of 79 patients presenting for emer-
life (Thomson et al., 2009). Individuals avoiding dental care have more oral
gency extraction rated their anxiety and pain
disease and experience greater negative impact on their daily life from poor
before, during, and two weeks after the procedure.
oral health (Abrahamsson et al., 2001).
Measures of trait dental anxiety and fear of pain
Pain is often cited as both an etiological and a maintaining factor in
also were collected. All patients exaggerated their
patients’ dental anxieties (Lautch, 1971; Bernstein et al., 1979; Wardle, 1982;
recall of procedure pain, but only those high in
McNeil and Berryman, 1989). Yet, not all patients who experience pain dur-
trait dental anxiety exaggerated their recall of
ing dental procedures develop disabling dental anxiety (Davey, 1989), and not
anxiety. Highly anxious patients reported more
all patients whose over-estimation of dental pain is disconfirmed necessarily
pain prior to the procedure and expected more
decrease their recall of past pain or prediction of future pain (Arntz et al.,
pain; ratings of anxiety and pain for all participants
1990). In addition to the avoidance-promoting function of over-prediction and
assimilated over time.
recall of aversive stimuli intensity (Rachman and Bichard, 1988), one expla-
nation for inaccurate prediction and recall of pain in high-dental-anxiety
KEY WORDS: dental anxiety, pain, memory. patients is that anxiety biases memory of prior pain, subsequently affecting
predictions of future pain (Wardle, 1984).
Research with dental patients supports the concept of a cycle of anxiety
and exaggerated expectation and recall of pain. In a general dental practice,
patients remembered more pain after 3 mos than they reported immediately
after the procedure, but highly dentally anxious patients had a greater increase
in remembered pain (Kent, 1985). Another study of patients undergoing root
canal therapy revealed that only experienced pain intensity predicted pain
intensity recalled after 1 wk (Gedney et al., 2003). For pain intensity recalled
after 18 mos and pain unpleasantness recalled after both 1 wk and 18 mos,
state anxiety immediately prior to treatment was a significant predictor.
DOI: 10.1177/0022034510385689
Finally, the best predictor of pain expected before oral surgery, during sur-
gery, and recalled 4 wks later was state anxiety at the time of report (Eli et al.,
Received December 10, 2008; Last revision July 7, 2010; 2003).
Accepted July 8, 2010 Other questions regarding the effect of anxiety on recall of dental pain,
however, remain unanswered. It is not known if memory of experienced
A supplemental appendix to this article is published elec-
tronically only at http://jdr.sagepub.com/supplemental.
anxiety increases with time, as does memory of pain. An increase in recalled
anxiety could exacerbate recall of pain by increasing the recall of anxiety-
© International & American Associations for Dental Research provoking aspects of the dental visit (e.g., pain). Additionally, it would be

220
J Dent Res 90(2) 2011 Memory of Pain and Anxiety   221

expected that reporting of the two aversive experiences would Procedure


become more closely related over time, especially as memory
becomes less accurate and specific. Yet, no study has examined After entering the operatory and sitting in a dental chair prior to
such a temporally dynamic relation between anxiety and pain treatment, participants were approached by a research assistant,
with dental patients. who gained informed consent, completed a demographic inter-
This study examined reports of anxiety and pain over time view, and recorded the patient’s NRS rating of pre-procedure
for oral surgery patients high and low in dental anxiety. It was state anxiety and dental pain, as well as anxiety and pain
predicted that: (a) high-anxiety patients would expect and expected during extraction. Participants also completed the DFS
remember, but not experience, more pain than low-anxiety and SF-FPQ.
patients; (b) high-dental-anxiety patients would report greater For all patients, local anesthesia was used. Immediately fol-
anxiety at all times; (c) high dental anxiety would be related to lowing extraction, participants completed NRS to rate maximal
greater fear of pain; (d) the relation between anxiety and pain pain and anxiety during the procedure. Follow-up NRS, asking
would strengthen with time for all patients; and (e) recalled pain participants to rate maximal pain and anxiety remembered dur-
and anxiety would be significantly related, even after adjust- ing the extraction, were mailed to patients 2 wks later, and were
ment for other relevant variables. returned after an average of 25.2 (SD = 17.4) days.

Analysis Plan
Materials & Methods
DFS total scores were used to divide participants into high- and
Participants low-dental-anxiety groups. A 2 (dental anxiety group) x 4 (rat-
A final sample of 79 (n = 44 female) adult outpatients reporting ing time) multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) approach
to the West Virginia University (WVU) Oral and Maxillofacial to repeated measures was used to examine the effect of dental
Surgery Clinic for emergency extraction was included in this anxiety on ratings of pain and anxiety over time. Tests for sim-
study. Appendix A includes additional information about recruit- ple effects of dental anxiety on pain and anxiety at different
ment and participants, who received $10 USD for study comple- rating times were planned a priori. Pearson correlation coeffi-
tion. Data were collected with approval from the WVU cients were used to analyze the relation between dental anxiety
Institutional Review Board. and fear of pain. Finally, hierarchical linear regression equations
examined the contributions of dental anxiety, fear of pain, and
Measures ratings of pain and anxiety across time to the prediction of
recalled pain and anxiety. Age and gender, then fear of pain and
Numerical Rating Scale dental anxiety, were entered first and second to control for these
demographic and trait variables, respectively. Then anxiety and
We used 2 numerical rating scales (NRS) to measure self-report
pain ratings were entered in chronological order.
of anxiety and pain at the current moment on a continuum from
0 (no pain or anxiety) to 100 (extreme pain or anxiety). The
NRS has been used in previous research of pain and emotion Results
(Kyle et al., 2009), has demonstrated internal consistency mea-
Division of participants based on DFS total scores yielded a
suring pain (Price et al., 1994), is influenced by emotion in a
low-dental-anxiety group of 40 participants (n = 22 female) and
manner similar to other pain-rating scales (Kremer et al., 1981),
a high-dental-anxiety group of 39 participants (n = 22 female).
and can be administered in interview or questionnaire format.
Split half scores were calculated separately for males and
Dental Fear Survey (DFS) females to avoid confounding gender and dental anxiety, because
females tend to report more, and males less, dental anxiety
This scale is a 20-item measure of fears of a variety of situations
(Kleinknecht et al., 1973). Total DFS scores for the high- and
and stimuli associated with dentistry (Kleinknecht et al., 1973).
low-dental-anxiety groups matched well, respectively, to prior
The DFS has been used globally and has a wealth of support
scores for dental phobic individuals and an unselected student
regarding its psychometric properties (e.g., McGlynn et al.,
sample (McGlynn et al., 1987; Moore et al., 1991; Johansson
1987).
and Berggren, 1992).
Fear of Pain Questionnaire-III (SF-FPQ)
Trait Dental Anxiety and Pain
Developed by McNeil and Rainwater (1998), the Fear of Pain
Questionnaire-III (FPQ-III) assesses fear associated with differ- A 2 x 4 repeated-measures MANOVA revealed a significant
ent painful situations. The FPQ has demonstrated reliability and effect of time for ratings of pain, F(3, 75) = 4.06, p < 0.05, par-
validity in clinical and community samples internationally (e.g., tial η2 = 0.07 (Fig. 1). The main effect of the DFS group just
Osman et al., 2002), and has been used in oral health settings missed the standard level of statistical significance, F(1, 77) =
(e.g., McNeil et al., 2001). The current study used the 9-item 3.78, p = 0.06. The interaction of DFS group and time was not
FPQ short form (SF-FPQ). significant, F(3, 75) = 0.96, p = 0.42. Comparison of marginal
222  McNeil et al. J Dent Res 90(2) 2011

60 High DFS
a, A High DFS 75
55
NRS Pain Rating

Low DFS a, B
50 Low DFS
65
a, A

NRS Anxiety Rating


45 a, A, B a, A a, A
40 a, A 55
35 a, B a 45
30 b
a
25 a 35 b, D
a, D
20
state expected reported recalled 25 b, D
Rating Time
15 b, C
Figure 1.  Mean rating of pain across rating times for participants in 5
state expected reported recalled
both DFS groups. Means that do not share upper-case subscripts differ
significantly across rating times, and means that do not share lower- Rating Time
case subscripts differ significantly between groups. Each cell of data
Figure 2.  Mean rating of anxiety across rating times for participants in
for the low-dental-anxiety group represents 40 participants, and each
both DFS groups. Means that do not share lower-case subscripts differ
cell of data for the high-dental-anxiety group represents 39 participants.
significantly between groups, and means that do share upper-case
Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. **p < 0.001.
subscripts differ significantly within groups across rating times. Each cell of
data for the low-dental-anxiety group represents 40 participants, and each
cell of data for the high-dental-anxiety group represents 39 participants.
Error bars represent the standard error of the mean.**p < 0.001.
means for the main effect of time revealed that, for all partici-
pants, pain reported immediately following extraction (M =
28.2, SD = 3.7) was significantly lower than pain prior to extrac-
tion (M = 43.7, SD = 4.0) and pain recalled several weeks later greater anxiety during tooth extraction than they reported imme-
(M = 35.2, SD = 3.9). Expected pain during extraction (M = diately after the procedure.
35.4, SD = 3.6) did not significantly differ from other rating
times. Relation of Anxiety and Pain
A priori simple effects tests of the effects of dental anxiety at
each rating time were calculated as planned. The only time- The DFS and SF-FPQ correlated significantly (r = 0.51, p <
point yielding a significant difference was state pain before 0.001), consistent with McNeil and Berryman (1989). Pain and
extraction, F(1, 77) = 5.95, p < 0.05; participants in the high- anxiety related significantly at the p < 0.05 level at all time-
dental-anxiety group reported greater pain (M = 53.4, SD = points, and the correlation increased over time (Appendix B).
35.3) than participants in the low-dental-anxiety group (M = Further, correlation between recalled state anxiety and dental
33.9, SD = 35.8). Participants in the two groups did not signifi- pain (r = 0.56) was higher than the correlation prior to the proce-
cantly differ on expected pain, F(1, 77) = 2.33, p = 0.13; pain dure (r = 0.24, z = 2.39, p < 0.05) or the correlation of expected
reported immediately following extraction, F(1, 77) = 0.06, p = anxiety and pain (r = 0.30, z = 1.99, p < 0.05). Correlation
0.81; or recalled pain, F(1, 77) = 0.03, p = 0.87. between anxiety and pain reported about the extraction (r = 0.44)
did not significantly differ from other correlations.
Further, two significant predictors of recalled pain emerged
Trait Dental Anxiety and State Anxiety
from a hierarchical regression equation: pain reported following
A 2 x 4 repeated-measures MANOVA produced a significant extraction, and recalled anxiety (Table 1). The three strongest
interaction between DFS group and time for ratings of anxiety, predictors of recalled anxiety were trait dental anxiety, anxiety
F(3, 75) = 3.57, p < 0.05, partial η2 = 0.06 (Fig. 2). There also during the extraction, and recalled pain (Table 2). Additional
were significant main effects for both DFS group, F(1, 77) = analyses are in Appendix C.
25.21, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.25, and time, F(3, 75) = 5.70,
p < 0.01, partial η2 = 0.10.
Discussion
Simple effects tests of DFS group at rating time revealed that
patients in the high-dental-anxiety group, compared with Regarding the first hypothesis, trait dental anxiety had a signifi-
patients in the low-dental-anxiety group, reported greater pre- cant effect on state pain prior to extraction. Results also partially
extraction state anxiety (M = 55.3, SD = 4.6 vs. M = 14.8, SD = replicated those from Kent (1985), in that recall of pain for all
4.6), expected anxiety (M = 56.0, SD = 5.2 vs. M = 28.9, SD = participants was exaggerated even after 2 wks. Perhaps differ-
5.1), and recalled anxiety (M = 65.0, SD = 5.7 vs. M = 34.9, ences in recall of pain between high- and low-dental-anxiety
SD = 5.6). Although patients in the high-dental-fear group also patients take longer to manifest, as in Kent (1985), in which dif-
reported more anxiety during the extraction (M = 52.1, SD = 5.9 vs. ferences were shown at 3 mos.
M = 35.9, SD = 5.8), the difference was not statistically significant. The second hypothesis also was supported, since the high-
Simple comparisons of anxiety at different rating times for dental-anxiety group had greater ratings of anxiety than the
both DFS groups further revealed that participants low in dental low-dental-anxiety group at all times except immediately after
anxiety reported less state anxiety prior to extraction compared tooth extraction. Additionally, patients high in dental anxiety
with expected, reported, and recalled anxiety. Participants with recalled more anxiety weeks later than immediately after sur-
a higher level of dental anxiety, however, recalled significantly gery; patients low in dental anxiety reported less state anxiety
J Dent Res 90(2) 2011 Memory of Pain and Anxiety   223

Table 1. Hierarchical Regression Analysis Summary for Patient Variables Predicting Recalled Pain

Variable B SE B b R2 ΔR2

Step 1 0.03 0.03


  Gender 5.08 7.79 0.07  
  Age –0.44 0.32 –0.16  
Step 2 0.04 0.01
  DFS total score 0.17 0.23 0.10  
  SF-FPQ total score 0.05 0.61 0.01  
Step 3 0.09 0.05
  NRS rating of state pain –0.19 0.11 –0.20  
  NRS rating of state anxiety –0.15 0.17 –0.16  
Step 4 0.12 0.03
  NRS rating of expected pain –0.19 0.13 –0.18  
  NRS rating of expected anxiety 0.11 0.17 0.12  
Step 5 0.45** 0.33**
  NRS rating of pain during extraction 0.63 0.12 0.60**  
  NRS rating of anxiety during extraction 0.03 0.11 0.03  
Step 6 0.62** 0.17**
  NRS rating of recalled anxiety 0.64 0.12 0.71**  

Notes. DFS = Dental Fear Survey, SF-FPQ = Short Form-Fear of Pain Questionnaire, NRS = Numerical Rating Scale. Each regression equation is
based on 79 participants. Gender was coded as male = 0 and female = 1, so being female was (non-significantly) associated with higher
recalled pain. Also, age was entered in years; being younger was (non-significantly) associated with more recalled pain.
**p < 0.001.

Table 2. Hierarchical Regression Analysis Summary for Patient Variables Predicting Recalled Anxiety

Variable B SE B β R2 ΔR2

Step 1 0.17* 0.17*


  Gender 22.58 8.03 0.30*  
  Age –0.81 0.34 –0.25*  
Step 2 0.43** 0.25**
  DFS total score 0.90 0.20 0.46**  
  SF-FPQ total score 0.49 0.53 0.10  
Step 3 0.45** 0.02
  NRS rating of state pain –0.07 0.10 –0.07  
  NRS rating of state anxiety 0.22 0.15 0.21  
Step 4 0.45** 0.01
  NRS rating of expected pain –0.12 0.12 –0.10  
  NRS rating of expected anxiety 0.05 0.15 0.05  
Step 5 0.67** 0.21**
  NRS rating of pain during extraction 0.15 0.10 0.12  
  NRS rating of anxiety during extraction 0.48 0.10 0.46**  
Step 6 0.77** 0.10**
  NRS rating of recalled pain 0.48 0.09 0.43**  

Notes. DFS = Dental Fear Survey, SF-FPQ = Short Form-Fear of Pain Questionnaire, NRS = Numerical Rating Scale. Each regression equation is
based on 79 participants. Gender was coded as male = 0 and female = 1, so being female was associated with higher recalled pain (or
anxiety). Also, age was entered in years; being older was associated with less recalled anxiety.
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.001.

immediately before surgery. Several processes may help explain procedure. Third, perhaps patients high in dental anxiety under-
this pattern. First, differences in state anxiety before tooth reported their procedural anxiety immediately after the extrac-
extraction might have influenced later recall of the procedure, as tion, with a sense of relief from being “finished”, and their
it did with recall of pain and unpleasantness in Gedney et al. recall of anxiety was closer to their actual experience during
(2003). Second, patients high in dental anxiety might have extraction.
experienced more anxiety in recalling a dental event and, Strong support was provided for the third and fourth hypoth-
subsequently, recalled more anxiety-provoking aspects of the eses. Dental anxiety and fear of pain were significantly related,
224  McNeil et al. J Dent Res 90(2) 2011

and the correlation between anxiety and pain ratings converged Arntz A, Van Eck M, Heijmans M (1990). Predictions of dental pain: the
with time. Finally, in terms of the fifth hypothesis, recalled fear of any expected evil is worse than the evil itself. Behav Res Ther
28:29-41.
anxiety was one of only two significant predictors of recalled Bernstein DA, Kleinknecht RA, Alexander LD (1979). Antecedents of den-
dental pain; perhaps state anxiety was less of a predictor than in tal fear. J Public Health Dent 39:113-124.
other studies (Eli et al., 2003; Gedney et al., 2003) because of Davey G (1989). Dental phobias and anxieties: evidence for conditioning
the shorter duration before recall. Similarly, recalled pain was a processes in the acquisition and modulation of a learned fear. Behav Res
significant predictor of recalled anxiety, as was trait dental Ther 27:51-58.
Eli I, Schwartz-Arad D, Baht R, Ben Tuvim H (2003). Effect of anxiety on
anxiety. Although neither trait dental anxiety nor anxiety during the experience of pain in implant insertion. Clin Oral Implants Res
extraction significantly predicted recalled pain, these variables 14:115-118.
could have indirectly influenced recalled pain through their Gatchel RJ, Ingersoll BD, Bowman L, Robertson MC, Walker C (1983). The
effects on recalled anxiety. Thus, the aversive states of fear and prevalence of dental fear and avoidance: a recent survey study. J Am
Dent Assoc 107:609-610.
pain became assimilated in memory over time, a finding congru-
Gedney J, Logan H, Baron R (2003). Predictors of short-term and long-term
ent with both Gedney et al. (2003) and laboratory research by memory of sensory and affective dimensions of pain. J Pain 4:47-55.
Rainville et al. (2004) suggesting that memory of pain quickly Heaton LJ, McNeil DW, Milgrom P (2010). Propranolol and D-cycloserine
becomes less precise and more categorical. as adjunctive medications in reducing dental fear in sedation practice.
A limitation is that oral surgery is a variable experience; the SAAD Dig 26:27-35.
Johansson P, Berggren U (1992). Assessment of dental fear: a comparison of
tooth extraction experience varied across participants. Also, this
two psychometric instruments. Acta Odontol Scand 50:43-49.
sample received emergency oral surgery at a dental school, and so Kent G (1985). Memory of dental pain. Pain 21:187-194.
may be somewhat unique. NRS data were collected by different Kleinknecht RA, Klepac RK, Alexander LD (1973). Origins and character-
formats (i.e., interview vs. questionnaire) depending on the time- istics of fear of dentistry. J Am Dent Assoc 86:842-848.
point in the study. Differences likely are minimal, however, since Kremer E, Atkinson J, Ignelzi R (1981). Measurement of pain: patient pref-
erence does not confound pain measurement. Pain 10:241-248.
the questionnaire version was handed immediately to the researcher. Kyle BN, McNeil DW, Weinstein BJ, Mark JD (2009). Interaction of inten-
Overall, findings lend support to the idea that memory of sity and order regarding painful events. J Behav Med 32:360-370.
dental pain becomes exaggerated with time, and memory of Lautch H (1971). Dental phobia. Br J Psychiatry 119:151-158.
anxiety becomes exaggerated for high-dental-anxiety patients. Maggirias J, Locker D (2002). Five-year incidence of dental anxiety in an
Furthermore, the relation between anxiety and pain increases adult population. Community Dent Health 19:173-179.
McGlynn F, McNeil D, Gallagher S, Vrana S (1987). Factor structure, stabil-
with time. Results highlight the important role both trait dental ity, and internal consistency of the Dental Fear Survey. Behav Assess
anxiety and state anxiety play in exaggerating recall and subse- 9:57-66.
quent prediction of dental pain, especially for highly dentally McNeil DW, Berryman M (1989). Components of dental fear in adults?
anxious patients. Additionally, analysis of these data emphasizes Behav Res Ther 27:233-236.
McNeil DW, Rainwater AJ (1998). Development of the Fear of Pain
the importance of reducing both trait dental anxiety and state
Questionnaire – III. J Behav Med 21:389-410.
anxiety, potentially through behavioral and/or pharmacological McNeil DW, Au AR, Zvolensky MJ, McKee DR, Klineberg IJ, Ho CC
methods, to reduce the potential aversiveness of the dental expe- (2001). Fear of pain in orofacial pain patients. Pain 89:245-252.
rience (Heaton et al., 2010). Moore R, Berggren U, Carlsson S (1991). Reliability and clinical usefulness
of psychometric measures in a self-referred population of odontopho-
bics. Community Dent Oral Epidemiol 19:347-351.
Acknowledgments Osman A, Breitenstein JL, Barrios FX, Gutierrez PM, Kopper BA (2002).
The fear of pain questionnaire-III: Further reliability and validity with
Thanks to the participating patients as well as the WVU Anxiety, nonclinical samples. J Behav Med 25:155-173.
Psychophysiology, and Pain Research Laboratory, and staff of Price DD, Bush FM, Long S, Harkin SW (1994). A comparison of pain
the WVU Oral Surgery Clinic. Funding was provided by the measurement characteristics of mechanical visual analogue and simple
numerical rating scales. Pain 56:217-226.
WVU Department of Psychology, Eberly College of Arts and Rachman S, Bichard S (1988). The overprediction of fear. Clin Psychol Rev
Sciences, School of Dentistry, and McNair Scholars Program, as 8:303-312.
well as Psi Chi, the International Honor Society in Psychology. Rainville P, Doucet J, Fortin M, Duncan G (2004). Rapid deterioration of
A preliminary report was presented at the 84th General Session pain sensory-discriminative information in short-term memory. Pain
of the International Association for Dental Research, July, 2006, 110:605-615.
Smith T, Heaton L (2003). Fear of dental care: are we making any progress?
in Brisbane, Queensland, Australia. J Am Dent Assoc 134:1101-1108.
Thomson WM, Broadbent JM, Locker D, Poulton R (2009). Trajectories of
References dental anxiety in a birth cohort. Community Dent Oral Epidemiol
37:209-219.
Abrahamsson K, Berggren U, Håkeberg M, Carlsson S (2001). Phobic Wardle J (1982). Fear of dentistry. Br J Med Psychol 55:119-126.
avoidance and regular dental care in fearful dental patients: a compara- Wardle J (1984). Dental pessimism: negative cognitions in fearful dental
tive study. Acta Odontol Scand 59:273-279. patients. Behav Res Ther 22:553-556.

You might also like