Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Neuropsychology Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association

2007, Vol. 21, No. 5, 549 –558 0894-4105/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.5.549

Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment: Difference of Memory Profile in


Subjects Who Converted or Did Not Convert to Alzheimer’s Disease
Roberta Perri and Laura Serra Giovanni Augusto Carlesimo and Carlo Caltagirone
Foundation IRCCS Santa Lucia Foundation IRCCS Santa Lucia and
University of Rome “Tor Vergata”

The Early Diagnosis Group of the Italian Interdisciplinary Network on Alzheimer’s Disease

Episodic long-term, short-term, and implicit memory were investigated in 79 elderly subjects who fulfilled criteria
for the amnestic form of mild cognitive impairment (a-MCI; i.e., by having an idiopathic amnestic disorder with
absence of impairment in cognitive areas other than memory and without confounding medical or psychiatric
conditions) and who developed Alzheimer’s disease (AD) after 2 years as well as in 111 subjects affected by a-MCI
who did not develop dementia. Results document a memory profile in a-MCI subjects characterized by preserved
short-term and implicit memory and extensive impairment of episodic long-term memory. In virtually all episodic
memory indexes examined (learning, forgetting, recognition abilities), a-MCI subjects who converted to AD were
more severely impaired than were subjects who did not become demented. This memory profile, which closely
resembles that exhibited by amnestic patients with bilateral mesial–temporal lobe lesions, confirms a precocious
phase in preclinical AD characterized by selective involvement of mesial–temporal areas and worsening of the
memory impairment as atrophic changes progress in hippocampal structures. In this context of pervasive episodic
memory impairment, tests assessing the free recall of verbal material following a delay interval demonstrated the
greater sensitivity to memory deficits of a-MCI subjects who developed AD.

Keywords: episodic memory, implicit memory, mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer’s disease

Much experimental work investigating the qualitative profile of explicit form of memory that permits the conscious recall or
memory dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) documents the recognition of previously experienced facts or events. Poor encod-
pervasive impairment of declarative long-term memory, that is, the ing of incoming information during the learning phase, accelerated
forgetting rates, and increased sensitivity to interference during
retrieval have all been documented in AD (see Spaan, Raaijmak-
Roberta Perri and Laura Serra, Foundation Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a ers, & Jonker, 2003, for a review). These deficits are consistent
Carattere Scientifico (IRCCS) Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy; Giovanni Augusto with the neuropathology of the disease, which extensively and
Carlesimo and Carlo Caltagirone, Foundation IRCCS Santa Lucia and Uni- precociously affects the mesial–temporal lobe (MTL) structures
versity of Rome “Tor Vergata,” Rome, Italy; the Early Diagnosis Group of the (Braak, Braak, & Bohl, 1993) known to be critically involved in
Italian Interdisciplinary Network on Alzheimer’s Disease. Group members and
declarative long-term memory functioning (Squire, 1992). In ad-
centers participating in the study are as follows: Fondazione Don Gnocchi,
Milano: M. Alberoni; Ospedale S. Gerardo, Monza: I. Appollonio; Instituto
dition to declarative memory, there is evidence that repetition
Nazionale Riposo e Cura Anziani (INRCA) – C. da Mossa, Ascoli Piceno: S. priming, an implicit form of memory, and short-term memory are
Bonaiuto; Ospedale Niguarda, Milano: G. Bottini; Università di Parma, Parma: also impaired in AD patients (Fleischman et al., 1999; Carlesimo,
P. Caffarra; Università Tor Vergata and Fondazione Santa Lucia, Roma: C. Fadda, Sabbadini, & Caltagirone, 1996). Because short-term mem-
Caltagirone; II Università di Napoli, Napoli, S. Carlomagno; Ospedale S. ory and repetition priming are supported by different neocortical
Salvatore, L’Aquila: A. Carolei and P. Sucapane; Ospedale Sant’Anna, Fer- regions than is explicit long-term memory, their impairment is
rara: P. De Bastiani; Università di Milano, Milano: M. Di Luca; Clinica Santa
generally considered a sign that the neuropathology from the MTL
Maria HSR, Castellanza: M. Franceschi; Ospedale Ca’ Foncello, Treviso: M.
Gallucci; Azienda Ospedaliera di Verona, Verona: G. Gambina; Arcispedale structures has progressed to the associative neocortical areas in the
Santa Maria Nuova, Reggio Emilia: E. Ghidoni; Ospedale C. Besta, Milano: F. temporal, parietal, and frontal lobes (Desgranges et al., 1998;
Girotti; Università La Sapienza, Roma, F. Giubilei; Ospedale degli Infermi di Fleischman & Gabrieli, 1999; Perry & Hodges, 1996).
Rimini, Rimini: S. Lorusso; IRCCS Fondazione Maugeri, Veruno: C. Mar- Subjects suffering from the amnestic type of mild cognitive
chetti; Università di Palermo, Palermo: R. Monastero; Università La Sapienza, impairment (a-MCI) have a high risk of developing AD (Petersen
Roma, C. Mina; Università di Brescia, Brescia: A. Padovani; Azienda Osped- et al., 2001). A recent study carried out by the Italian Interdisci-
aliera Sant’ Antonio Abate, Gallarate: M. Perini; Centro Regionale Alzheimer plinary Network on Alzheimer’s Disease (Perri, Carlesimo, Serra,
– Passirana di Rho, Milano: C. Pettenati; Università di Sassari, Sassari: M.R.
Caltagirone, & the Early Diagnosis Group, 2005) documented
Piras; Università degli Studi, Ancona: L. Provinciali; Policlinico Universitario,
impairment of several indexes of explicit long-term memory func-
Messina, A. Quartarone and A. Graceffa; Università di Perugia, Perugia: U.
Senin; Ospedale Santa Chiara, Pisa: G. Tognoni; Azienda Sanitaria Locale tioning in a relatively large group of a-MCI subjects. Indeed,
(ASL) 15, Cuneo: P. Zagnoni; Bracco - Spa, Milano: E. Grossi and R. Savarè. compared with a group of healthy control subjects, these patients
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Roberta showed poor learning and increased forgetting rates of verbal and
Perri, Fondazione IRCCS Santa Lucia, Via Ardeatina, 306, 00179, ROMA, visual material and reduced effectiveness of semantic encoding to
Italy. E-mail: r.perri@hsantalucia.it improve subsequent verbal recall. In contrast, a-MCI subjects’

549
550 PERRI ET AL.

performance was substantially normal on short-term and implicit girone, & Gainotti, 1996) and general cognitive efficiency (Mini-
memory tasks, thus supporting the view that the preclinical phase Mental State Examination [MMSE], Measso et al., 1993). For each
of AD is characterized by precocious involvement of the hip- test in the battery, Italian normative data were available for score
pocampal structures and substantial sparing of extra-MTL asso- adjustment on the basis of sex, age, and education as well as for
ciative neocortical areas. However, these results were obtained on normality cutoff scores, which were determined as the lower limit
the whole a-MCI sample, irrespective of whether individual sub- of the 95% tolerance interval for a confidence level of 95% (see
jects eventually developed dementia. Thus, although an impair- Spinnler & Tognoni, 1987, and Carlesimo, Caltagirone, & Gain-
ment of short-term and/or implicit memory function characterized otti, 1996).
the subjects in the incipient phase of dementia, perhaps this deficit The clinical assessment included clinical and cognitive history,
could not be detected when the entire a-MCI population was standard neurological examination, standard blood and chemistry
investigated. examinations, thyroid hormone levels, syphilis serologic testing,
The present study was aimed at investigating both quantitative vitamin B12 and folic acids levels, chest x-ray, electrocardiogram,
and qualitative differences in the memory deficit that characterizes and neuroradiological examination (CT or MR). The behavioral
a-MCI subjects who have an absence of impairment in cognitive battery consisted of Beck’s Depression Inventory (Beck & Steer,
areas other than memory, who do not have confounding medical or 1987) and Hamilton’s Anxiety (Hamilton, 1960) scales. The sub-
psychiatric conditions, and who will develop AD dementia jects’ autonomy in social and daily living activities was investi-
within 2 years in comparison with those of subjects who will not gated by means of the Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR; Hughes,
develop dementia. For this purpose, we compared performances on Berg, Danziger, Coben, & Martin, 1982) and Instrumental Activ-
declarative long-term, short-term, and implicit memory tasks ob- ities of Daily Living (Lawton & Brody, 1969) scales.
tained at the beginning of the study by examining 111 a-MCI Inclusion criteria for subjects in the a-MCI group were the follow-
subjects who did not develop dementia during a 2-year follow-up ing: (a) subjective memory impairment corroborated by an assistant
period with those obtained by examining 79 a-MCI subjects who and confirmed by a score below the normality cutoff on at least one
did develop AD during that period. of the three episodic memory tests of the neuropsychological screen-
ing battery; (b) nonfulfillment of the Diagnostic and Statistical Man-
Method ual of Mental Disorders (DSM–IV) criteria for dementia (American
Psychiatric Association, 1994); (c) preserved general cognitive func-
Subjects tions and absence of impairment in cognitive areas other than mem-
ory, as confirmed by normal scores on the MMSE (normality cutoff
a-MCI Subjects
score: 24; Measso et al., 1993) and on all tests of the neuropsycho-
The experimental sample of the present study was composed of logical screening battery that assessed cognitive functions other than
190 a-MCI subjects (82 male and 108 female; mean age ⫽ 70.1 episodic memory; (d) no or very mild impact of the memory deficit on
years, SD ⫽ 7.2; mean years of education ⫽ 7.6, SD ⫽ 3.5) who the subject’s activities, as confirmed by a normal score on instrumen-
were followed for 2 years. The subjects were taken from a sample tal activities of daily living and by a total CDR score of 0.5, consistent
of 269 subjects (115 male and 154 female; mean age ⫽ 70.3 years, with a minimal change in the patient’s habits; (e) lack of any evidence
SD ⫽ 6.9; mean years of education ⫽ 7.9, SD ⫽ 3.8) recruited of neurological or systemic pathology able to induce memory disor-
in 29 neurological and geriatric centers that are part of the Italian ders, as confirmed by normal thyroid functioning, vitamin B12 and
Interdisciplinary Network on Alzheimer’s Disease (see list in the folic acid levels, syphilis serologic results, neurological examination
author note). Enrollment was initiated with 50 – 80-year-old out- and CT or MR brain imaging negative for focal lesions (minimal
patients who had at least 5 years of formal education and had been diffuse changes or minimal lacunar lesions of white matter were
referred to a neurologist or a geriatrist for the first time for a admitted); (f) absence of moderate to severe depression and/or anxi-
memory disturbance. All subjects whose memory disorders were ety, as confirmed by scores on Beck’s Depression Inventory and
confirmed by an informant assistant, who had no history of focal Hamilton’s Anxiety that were consistent, at most, with mild depres-
brain pathology, and who did not meet the diagnostic criteria for sion or anxiety (14 was the highest acceptable for both scales).
dementia were submitted to a broad neuropsychological, clinical, All a-MCI subjects enrolled in the original study were admin-
behavioral, and functional screening evaluation to determine istered the experimental neuropsychological memory battery
whether they fulfilled the criteria for a-MCI. within 30 days from completion of the screening phase. Then they
The neuropsychological screening evaluation included the fol- were invited to follow-up evaluations after 12 (12 f-up) and 24 (24
lowing: episodic long-term memory (immediate and 15-min de- f-up) months during which they were again submitted to the
layed recall of a 15-word list, Carlesimo, Caltagirone, & Gainotti, clinical examination and the neuropsychological, behavioral, and
1996; short story recall, Novelli et al., 1986), short-term memory functional assessments administered in the screening phase. This
(Digit Span and Corsi Block Tapping task, Orsini et al., 1987), permitted classifying them as still having MCI or as developing
executive functions (phonological and categorical word fluency, dementia. Criteria for MCI classification at follow-ups included (a)
Carlesimo, Caltagirone, & Gainotti, 1996; Spinnler & Tognoni, nonfulfillment of the DSM–IV criteria for dementia (American
1987), language (Token Test and Naming subtest of Aachener Psychiatric Association, 1994); (b) preserved general cognitive
Aphasia test, Spinnler & Tognoni, 1987; De Blese et al., 1986), functions as confirmed by normal scores on the MMSE; and (c) no
problem-solving (Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices, Car- or very mild impact of the cognitive impairment on the subject’s
lesimo, Caltagirone, & Gainotti, 1996), attention (attentive matri- activities, as confirmed by a total CDR score ⱕ0.5. Patients were
ces, Spinnler & Tognoni, 1987), constructional praxis (copy of diagnosed as having dementia when they fulfilled the DSM–IV
drawings and copy of drawings with landmarks, Carlesimo, Calta- criteria for dementia. Subjects who did not fulfill the clinical
MEMORY PROFILE IN AMNESTIC MCI SUBJECTS 551

criteria for dementia but who obtained an MMSE score less ing significantly worse than did nonconverters who, in turn, per-
than 24 or a total CDR score greater than 0.5 were classified as formed significantly worse than did NCs.
having an uncertain diagnosis. Subjects who did not undergo the
entire neuropsychological, behavioral, and functional assessment
Experimental Memory Battery
at one of the two follow-ups were considered dropouts.
In this study, we present data collected from the 111 a-MCI The experimental memory battery, which was described in a
subjects who still received the diagnosis of MCI after the 2-year previous study (Perri et al., 2005), explored episodic long-term and
observation period (nonconverters) and the 79 a-MCI subjects who implicit memory. The contribution of short-term memory to the
converted to AD in the same period (converters). Diagnosis of immediate free recall of a word list was also investigated.
probable AD was established by using the National Institute of
Neurological and Communicative Diseases and Stroke/Alzhei-
mer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association criteria (Mc- Episodic Memory
Khann et al., 1984). Of the 79 patients who were not included in Word-list recall (Carlesimo et al., 1998). Two lists of 16
the present study, 2 died, 4 developed a form of dementia different words (semantically related and unrelated) were used. The first list
from AD, 7 received an uncertain diagnosis, and 66 were dropouts. was composed of 16 unrelated nouns of concrete objects, the
The 66 subjects who dropped out did not differ from the sample of second of 16 words, 4 for each of four semantic categories (ani-
subjects who completed the follow-ups as to demographic charac- mals, fruit, tools, clothing). For each list, the test consisted of five
teristics and performance levels on the tests of the screening consecutive immediate free-recall trials, followed by a 15-min
neuropsychological battery (data not shown, F ⬍ 2.54 in all delayed recall trial (score range ⫽ 0 –75 and 0 –15 for immediate
comparisons, p consistently ns), thus suggesting they were ran- and delayed recall, respectively).
domly missing with respect to baseline memory impairment. Word-list recognition (Carlesimo et al., 1998). Two lists were
used, one for the unrelated condition and one for the related
condition. Each list included the 16 words from the studied list
Control Subjects
and 16 new words. The recognition lists were presented 15 min
Eighty-seven healthy subjects (36 male and 51 female; mean after delayed recall of the respective list. Two separate scores for
age ⫽ 68.04 years, SD ⫽ 10.8; mean years of education ⫽ 8.13 correct recognition of the studied items (hit rates) and for incorrect
years, SD ⫽ 4.2), recruited through the Foundation IRCCS Santa recognition of the unstudied items (false alarms) were computed
Lucia of Rome, volunteered to participate in the study as normal (range ⫽ 0 –16 in both cases).
control subjects (NCs). Inclusion criteria were (a) absence of Prose recall (Carlesimo et al., 2002). The subject was asked
active or previous neurological or psychiatric disorders, (b) no to recall a short story immediately after its oral presentation by the
history of alcohol or drug abuse, (c) absence of subjective memory examiner and following a 20-min interval without repetition. Sep-
disturbance, and (d) normal general cognitive functions as con- arate scores were given for immediate and delayed recall accord-
firmed by an MMSE score greater than 24. ing to the number of informative units reported (range ⫽ 0 – 8).
As shown in Table 1, the 111 a-MCI subjects who still received Rey’s Figure Form B reproduction. The test (Carlesimo et al.,
the diagnosis of MCI at the 24 f-up (nonconverters), the 79 a-MCI 2002) consisted of freehand copying, followed by an immediate
who were diagnosed as having converted to AD at this time and a delayed (15-min later) reproduction of the Rey’s Complex
(converters), and the 87 NCs were comparable for education and Figure from memory without re-presentation. Separate scores were
gender distribution. However, converters were older than were given for copying and for immediate and delayed tests according
both nonconverters and NCs who, in turn, did not differ from each to accuracy of the figure reproduction (range ⫽ 0 –36).
other. Mean scores obtained by nonconverters, converters, and Implicit memory. Verbal repetition priming was evaluated
NCs on the MMSE significantly differed, with converters perform- with a stem completion test (Graf, Squire, & Mandler, 1984). The

Table 1
Demographic Data and Average MMSE Scores (With Standard Deviations) of Nonconverter,
Converter, and NC Groups
Nonconverter Converter NC
Measure (n ⫽ 111) (n ⫽ 79) (n ⫽ 87) ␹2 F p df

Sex .149 ns 2
Men 47 35 36
Women 64 44 51
Age (years) 67.8 (7.5) 73.2 (5.5)a,b 68.0 (10.9) 12.0 ⬍.000 2, 274
Education (years) 7.7 (3.6) 7.5 (3.2) 8.1 (4.3) 0.7 ns 2, 274
MMSE 27.7 (1.7)c 26.3 (1.9)a,b 28.4 (1.8) 26.8 ⬍.000 2, 274

Note. NC ⫽ normal control; MMSE ⫽ Mini-Mental State Examination.


a
Converters vs. nonconverters significant difference ( p ⬍ .000).
b
Converters vs. NCs significant difference ( p ⬍ .000).
c
Nonconverters vs. NCs significant difference ( p ⬍ .01).
552 PERRI ET AL.

subjects read 20 words one at a time and then rated each word on to better average recall from the related (5.88) than from the
a 5-point pleasantness scale. Five min after, 40 three-letter word unrelated (5.54) list. The Group ⫻ Type of List interaction was
stems (20 belonging to words of the studied list, 20 to words of an significant, F(2, 186) ⫽ 6.14, p ⫽ .003. In fact, although on
unstudied list) were presented visually one at a time. The subjects average NCs recalled significantly more words from the related
were asked to complete each stem with the first word that came to than the unrelated list (7.78 vs. 7.01, p ⬍ .001), the nonconverter
mind. The level of priming was established as the difference and converter groups recalled a comparable number of words from
between stems completed with words from the studied list and the two lists (5.43 vs. 5.33 and 4.43 vs. 4.27, respectively; p ⫽ ns
stems completed with words from the unstudied list. in both cases). The significant trial effect, F(4, 744) ⫽ 338.56, p ⬍
Visual–perceptual repetition priming was investigated by a .001, was an expression of the learning process passing from the
fragmented picture identification test (Heindel, Salmon, & Butters,
first to the fifth recall trial. The Group ⫻ Trial interaction was
1990). The test material consists of 14 line drawings with seven
significant, F(8, 744) ⫽ 21.57, p ⬍ .001. Indeed, the learning rate,
different levels of fragmentation. In the first session, the subjects
based on the improvement in performance passing from the first to
were presented with the most fragmented version of a first set of 7
the fifth immediate recall trial, was significantly higher in the NC
drawings and were requested to try to identify them. Progressively
less degraded versions of drawings that had not been identified than in the nonconverter group ( p ⬍ .001) and in the nonconverter
were presented in the subsequent trials until the subject was able to than in the converter group ( p ⫽ .04; Table 2). Finally, the
identify all of the drawings. In the second session, the following Group ⫻ Type of List ⫻ Trial interaction was not significant
day, the same testing procedure was used for the same and for a (F ⫽ 0.46).
new set of 7 drawings presented in random order. Depending on Short-term memory component. It is generally held that
the degradation level at which the subjects were able to identify the whereas immediate recall of early and mid-list items in a word list
drawing, they received a score from 7 (maximum fragmentation) is an expression of pure long-term memory processes, the retrieval
to 1 (nonfragmented version) for each drawing. The improvement of the last few items in a list represents the overlapping of short-
in performance passing from identification of the new set of term and long-term memory processes (Carlesimo, Fadda, et al.,
drawings to identification of the old set of drawings in the second 1996; Koppenaal & Glanzer, 1990; Waugh & Norman, 1965). We
session was a measure of perceptual memory for specific items adopted Waugh and Norman’s method (1965) and calculated the
(repetition priming). Informed consent was obtained from all sub- short-term memory component implicated in recall of the terminal
jects prior to the study. items of the list (serial positions 15 and 16) by correcting perfor-
mance accuracy over the middle part of the list (serial positions 10
and 11), considered as the most reliable estimate of the long-term
Data Analysis
memory contribution to recall.1 A one-way ANCOVA showed that
The average scores obtained by converters, nonconverters, and nonconverter, converter, and NC groups did not differ with regard
NCs on the tests of the experimental memory battery were ana- to the cumulated short-term memory values corresponding to trial
lyzed by performing multiple-way analyses of covariance positions 15 and 16, F(2, 226) ⫽ 1.15 (Table 2).
(ANCOVAs) with group as a three-level between-subjects factor Semantic encoding. We analyzed the ability to take advantage
and test conditions as within-subjects factors. Because of a differ- of the semantic relatedness between words on a list to improve
ence in the mean age of the groups, age was inserted as a covariate recall by studying clustering behavior in the free recall of the
factor. When results of the principal analyses were significant, post categorized list (i.e., the tendency to recall words belonging to the
hoc comparisons were performed by the least significant difference same category in immediate succession). To this aim, we com-
test to reveal the crucial differences between groups. puted, for each immediate recall trial of the semantically related
list, the adjusted ratio of clustering score, developed by Roenker,
Results Thompson, and Brown (1971) and used in previous studies of
aging and memory (Delis et al., 1991; Carlesimo et al., 1998). A
A summary of the most important findings that emerged from a Group ⫻ Trial two-way ANCOVA revealed a significant differ-
comparison of the performances of nonconverter, converter, and ence between groups, F(2, 229) ⫽ 8.14, p ⬍ .001. Post hoc
NC groups on the tests of the experimental memory battery is comparisons showed significantly better average adjusted ratio of
reported in Table 2. Cohen’s d effect size measure for the main clustering scores in NCs than in nonconverters ( p ⫽ .02) and
comparisons between groups is also reported in this table and, in converters ( p ⬍ .001); the difference between the two a-MCI
particular, for the converters versus nonconverters comparisons. A subgroups approached significance ( p ⫽ .08; Table 2).
graphic of the main effect sizes is shown in Figure 1. Forgetting. The forgetting rate passing from the last immedi-
ate to the delayed recall trial of both related and unrelated lists was
Explicit Long-Term Memory investigated by a three-way Group ⫻ Type of List ⫻ Trial (5th
immediate vs. 15-min delayed) ANCOVA. The critical Group ⫻
Word-List Recall Trial interaction was highly significant, F(2, 186) ⫽ 14.4, p ⬍
Learning rate. The results of a three-way Group ⫻ Type of
List ⫻ Trial ANCOVA revealed a significant difference between 1
Waugh and Norman (1965) suggested the following formula: STMi ⫽
groups, F(2, 185) ⫽ 62.91, p ⬍ .001: The converter group recalled Ri – LTMi / 1 – LTMi where STMi and LTMi represent the probability of
less words than did the nonconverter group which, in turn, recalled item i being retained in STM and LTM, respectively; Ri is the probability
less words than did the NC group ( p ⬍ .001 in both cases; Table of recalling an item belonging to the recency part of the curve. STM ⫽
2). The type of list effect, F(1, 186) ⫽ 12.97, p ⬍ .001, was due short-term memory; LTM ⫽ long-term memory.
Table 2
Summary of Most Relevant Performance Scores Exhibited by the Nonconverter, Converter, and NC groups on the Tests of the Neuropsychological Experimental Battery
Effect size (Nonconverter Effect size (NC Effect size (NC vs.
Measure Nonconverter Converter NC vs. converter) vs. converter) nonconverter)

Explicit long-term memory

Word list recall


Learning Immediate recall 5.38e (1.4) 4.35a,c (1.1) 7.40 (1.9) 0.81 1.96 1.21
Learning rate (I vs. V trial) ⫹3.0e (1.8) ⫹2.2b,c (1.7) ⫹4.9 (1.7)
Semantic 0.3 0.7 0.4
Encoding (related list) ARC Score ⫹0.12f (0.3) ⫹0.03c (0.3) ⫹0.24 (0.3)
Forgetting Decline (V Imm. vs. Delayed ⫺2.42f (1.6) ⫺3.30b,c (1.6) ⫺1.86 (1.2)
recall)
Recognition d⬘ score 1.71e (1.3) 1.04a,c (1.1) 3.32 (1.4) 0.55 1.81 1.19
C score 0.69e (1.2) 0.38b,c (0.8) 1.91 (0.9) 0.39 1.79 1.15
Prose recall
Learning Immediate recall 3.97e (2.2) 3.05a,c (2.0) 5.68 (1.4) 0.43 1.52 0.92
Forgetting Decline (Imm. vs. Delayed recall) ⫺0.6 (2.4) ⫺1.08d (2.4) ⫺0.21 (1.2)
Rey’s Figure Form B
Learning Immediate recall 15.3f (6.8) 11.4a,c (7.2) 17.58 (5.6) 0.55 0.95 0.36
Forgetting Decline (Imm. vs. Delayed recall) ⫺1.25 (4.4) ⫺2.77b,d (5.9) ⫺0.69 (2.3)

Short-term memory

Word list recall


Serial position curve Positions 15–16 1.03 (0.5) 1.1 (0.5) 1.16 (0.5) ⫺0.14 0.11 0.25

Implicit memory

Stem completion Studied words 6.98 (4.1) 6.27 (3.9) 5.16 (2.5)
Unstudied words 3.26 (3.1) 2.93 (2.7) 2.10 (1.5)
MEMORY PROFILE IN AMNESTIC MCI SUBJECTS

Priming effect ⫹3.72 (3.7) ⫹3.34 (2.8) ⫹3.06 (2.5) 0.11 ⫺0.10 ⫺0.21
Fragmented picture identification test Old figures 4.36 (0.8) 3.78 (1.0) 4.51 (0.8)
New figures 3.68 (0.6) 3.66 (1.1) 3.63 (0.7)
Priming effect ⫹0.68 (0.7) ⫹0.12a,c (0.7) ⫹0.88 (0.6) 0.80 ⫺1.16 ⫺0.30

Note. Data are presented as M (SD). Effect sizes are Cohen’s d. NC ⫽ normal control; ARC ⫽ adjusted ratio of clustering.
a
Nonconverters vs. converters significant difference ( p ⬍ .001).
b
Nonconverters vs. converters significant difference ( p ⬍ .05).
c
Converters vs. NCs significant difference ( p ⬍ .001).
d
Converters vs. NCs significant different ( p ⬍ .05).
e
Nonconverters vs. NCs significant difference ( p ⬍ .001).
f
Nonconverters vs. NCs significant difference ( p ⬍ .05).
553
554 PERRI ET AL.

Word List Immediate Recall

Word List – ARC Score

Word List Recognition d´ score


Explicit long-term memory Word List Recognition C score

Prose Immediate Recall

Rey’s Figure Immediate Recall

Word List - Serial Position Curve


Short-term memory

Fragmented Identification Picture test - Priming


Implicit memory
Stem Completion - Priming
1

-0.2 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1

Figure 1. Effect sizes from the converters versus nonconverters comparisons on the explicit long-term,
short-term, and implicit memory tests. d⬘ was a measure of the memory effect, that is, of the subject’s ability to
discriminate between previously studied and unstudied words; C was a measure of the subject’s overinclusion
strategy. ARC ⫽ adjusted ratio of clustering.

.001. Planned comparisons revealed a smaller immediate-delayed vs. delayed) ANOVA. The group effect, F(2, 270) ⫽ 55.56, p ⬍
recall decline in the NC than in the nonconverter group ( p ⫽ .032), .001, revealed better recall in NCs (5.58) than in the nonconverter
which, in turn, forgot less than did the converter group ( p ⫽ .003; group (3.67), which, in turn, recalled better than did the converter
Table 2). The three-fold Group ⫻ Type of List ⫻ Trial interaction group (2.51; in all cases p ⬍ .001). The Group ⫻ Delay interac-
was also significant, F(2, 186) ⫽ 5.97, p ⫽ .003. Indeed, whereas tion, F(2, 270) ⫽ 3.20, p ⫽ .042, was due to the higher perfor-
NCs forgot significantly less from the related than the unrelated list mance decrement passing from immediate to delayed recall in the
(–1.55 vs. –2.19, p ⬍ .01), the nonconverter group forgot a converter group with respect to NCs ( p ⫽ .012); the forgetting rate
comparable number of words from the two lists (–2.63 vs. –2.22, in the nonconverter group fell between those of the NC and
p ⫽ ns), and the converter group forgot more from the related than converter groups and did not significantly differ from either group
the unrelated list (–3.64 vs. 2.95, p ⫽ .045). (Table 2).
Word-list recognition. Performance on the recognition tests
for both the related and unrelated word lists was scored according
Rey’s Figure Form B Reproduction
to the signal detection theory (Kamman, 1970). Therefore, two
indices were calculated: d⬘ was a measure of the memory effect, Performance scores obtained by the three groups on the imme-
that is, of the subject’s ability to discriminate between previously diate and delayed reproduction of Rey’s Figure were analyzed by
studied and unstudied words; C was a measure of the subject’s a two-way Group ⫻ Trial ANCOVA. To correct for the construc-
overinclusion strategy. The mean d⬘ scores obtained by the three tive ability of each individual subject, performance scores on the
groups were analyzed by a two-way Group ⫻ Type of List copy test were inserted in this analysis as a covariate. The signif-
ANCOVA. The significant group effect, F(2, 265) ⫽ 60.41, p ⬍ icant group effect, F(2, 263) ⫽ 19.10, p ⬍ .001, was due to the
.001, was due to NCs scoring significantly higher than did non- higher memory scores of NCs (17.18) than nonconverter subjects
converters ( p ⬍ .001) who, in turn, obtained a significantly higher (14.7; p ⫽ .003) who, in turn, remembered significantly more than
d⬘ score than did converters ( p ⬍ .001; Table 2). did converter subjects (10.1; p ⬍ .001). The Group ⫻ Trial
The mean C scores obtained by the three groups were also interaction, F(2, 264) ⫽ 4.52, p ⫽ .012, revealed that the perfor-
analyzed. The significant group effect, F(2, 264) ⫽ 47.83, p ⬍ mance decrement passing from the immediate to the delayed
.001, was due to the significantly higher NC group than noncon- reproduction test in the converter group was larger than that shown
verter group C score ( p ⬍ .001), which, in turn, was significantly by the nonconverter group ( p ⫽ .02) and NCs ( p ⫽ .004), which
higher than that of the converter group ( p ⫽ .046; Table 2). Also did not differ reciprocally (Table 2).
the Group ⫻ Type of List interaction was significant, F(2,
265) ⫽ 3.96, p ⫽ .02. In fact, NCs made fewer false alarms on the
unrelated than the related list (C ⫽ 2.07 vs. 1.78, p ⫽ .017),
Implicit Memory
whereas nonconverter and converter groups made a comparable Stem Completion
number of false alarms on the two lists (C ⫽ 0.61 vs. 0.77 and 0.42
vs. 0.34, respectively). Performances on the stem completion test were submitted to a
two-way Group ⫻ Study Condition (unstudied vs. studied words)
ANCOVA. The group effect, F(2, 271) ⫽ 7.08, p ⫽ .001, was due
Prose Recall
to the fact that NCs completed significantly less stems with words
Performance scores on the immediate and delayed recall of the from the experimental list (both studied and unstudied; 3.63) than
short story were analyzed by a two-way Group ⫻ Trial (immediate nonconverter (5.12) and converter (4.61) groups ( p ⬍ .001 and
MEMORY PROFILE IN AMNESTIC MCI SUBJECTS 555

p ⫽ .025, respectively), which did not differ one from each other studied and unstudied words. We wanted to obtain an index of
( p ⫽ ns). The study condition effect, F(1, 272) ⫽ 327.22, p ⬍ sensitivity and specificity from the subjects’ performances on the
.001, revealed a robust priming (6.14 vs. 2.77 for words on the four delayed recall tasks of the memory battery that demonstrated
studied and unstudied lists, respectively), which, as revealed by a higher sensitivity to the memory deficit in the converter group
nonsignificant Group ⫻ Study Condition interaction, F(2, (unrelated and related word lists, prose recall, and Rey’s Figure
272) ⫽ 1.08, did not differ among the groups (Table 2). recall), so we computed an average z score on the four tasks for
each subject and calculated the number of subjects in the converter
Fragmented Picture Identification Test and nonconverter groups who performed over 1.5 standard devi-
ations below the NC group mean. This cumulative index of de-
Performance scores of the second session of the fragmented layed recall reached a sensitivity of 75% and a specificity of 68.5%
picture identification test were submitted to a two-way ANCOVA in discriminating subjects in the converter and nonconverter
with group as between-factor and drawing set (old figures vs. new groups.
figures) as within-factor. The group effect was nonsignificant, F(2,
265) ⫽ 1.53. Instead, the drawing set effect was significant, F(1,
266) ⫽ 165.65, p ⬍ .001. Indeed, the average identification score Discussion
was significantly lower in the new (3.66) than in the old set of In the present research, we investigated episodic long-term,
figures (4.22), revealing a significant overall repetition priming short-term, and implicit memory abilities in a group of a-MCI
effect. The Group ⫻ Trial interaction was also significant, F(2, subjects with absence of impairment in cognitive areas other than
266) ⫽ 25.07, p ⬍ .001, because of a highly significant priming memory and without confounding medical or psychiatric condi-
effect in NCs and in the nonconverter group ( p ⬍ .001 in both tions at the time of clinical diagnosis. We attempted to define
cases) and a lack of significant priming in the converter group differences between subjects who showed substantial cognitive
(F ⫽ 2.27; Table 2). stability and subjects who progressed toward AD during a 2-year
period. Results showed that subjects who converted to AD in the
Sensitivity and Specificity of Individual Tests to a-MCI following 2 years were more severely impaired than were subjects
Subjects’ Episodic Memory Deficit who did not progress toward dementia during the same period of
time on virtually all of the episodic memory indexes examined.
To evaluate the sensitivity and specificity of the individual tests
Tests assessing the delayed free recall of verbal and visual material
of the experimental battery in detecting episodic memory deficits
demonstrated the greater sensitivity to the memory deficit in con-
of subjects with a-MCI, we calculated the number of subjects in
verter subjects.
the converter and nonconverter groups who obtained scores below
the normality cutoff on each test of the memory battery that we had
normative data for (Table 3). For the recognition tests, we calcu- Episodic Long-Term Memory
lated a single score as the sum of the correct recognition of both
We found that a-MCI subjects’ ability to learn verbal and visual
material was defective compared with those of NCs, in agreement
with previous reports (Perri et al., 2005; Petersen et al., 1999).
Table 3
Poor learning abilities were particularly evident in a multitrial
Percentage of Subjects in the Nonconverter and Converter
word-list recall task in which a-MCI subjects showed less perfor-
Groups Who Scored Below the Normality Cutoff on the
mance improvement in performance passing from the first to the
Experimental Memory Battery and the Delayed Recall
fifth recall trial. In all tasks, the learning impairment was more
Cumulative Index
severe in converter than in nonconverter subjects.
Test Nonconverter Converter Analysis of the forgetting rate quite consistently documented a
larger performance decrement passing from immediate to delayed
Unrelated word list recall in the converter than in the nonconverter group. It is possible
Immediate recall 21.6 35.4
Delayed recall 28.8 63.2
that short-term memory processes could have contributed to the
Recognition 27.0 38.0 immediate recall task. In fact, previous evidence has documented
Related word list comparable forgetting rates in early AD patients and NCs when the
Immediate recall 30.6 50.6 experimental paradigm removed the contribution of short-term
Delayed recall 41.4 77.2 memory processes to immediate recall (Kopelman, 1985; Car-
Recognition 31.5 54.4
Prose recall lesimo et al., 1998). As a result, the converter subjects’ accelerated
Immediate recall 24.3 35.4 forgetting rate (and, limited to the word-list recall tasks, that of the
Delayed recall 36.0 59.49 nonconverter subjects) can be interpreted as an expression of the
Rey’s Figure decay of short-term memory traces passing from immediate to
Immediate reproduction 8.1 17.7
Delayed reproduction 11.7 35.4
delayed recall that were not compensated by an adequate transfer
Delayed recall of information from short- to long-term memory stores.
Cumulative index 31.5 75 Our results confirmed the reduced ability of a-MCI subjects to
take advantage of the semantic structure of the memorandum to
Note. Normality cutoff was determined for each test in the battery as the
lower limit of the 95% tolerance interval for a confidence level of 95% and
improve their memory performance (Perri et al., 2005; Petersen et
for the index as scores over 1.5 standard deviations below the means of al., 1999). This deficit seemed comparably severe in both converter
normal control groups. and nonconverter subjects. Indeed, neither of the two groups
556 PERRI ET AL.

showed the better learning and reduced forgetting rate on the son’s r ranging from .15 to .29, p consistently ⬍ .02). Finally,
related with respect to the unrelated word list that was evident in priming scores on the visual and verbal tests were not correlated
the NC group performance. Nor did they show the increase in false with each other (Pearson’s r ⫽ –.36).
alarms in recognizing the related with respect to the unrelated To summarize, the present data confirm that the a-MCI subjects’
word list observable in the NC group (a confounding effect due to memory profile is characterized by preserved short-term and im-
the fact that the studied and unstudied words of the related list plicit memory and extensive impairment of episodic long-term
belonged to the same semantic categories). Moreover, both a-MCI memory (Perri et al., 2005). This memory profile, which closely
groups showed poor semantic clustering of words on the related resembles that exhibited by amnestic patients with bilateral MTL
list during recall. In this case, however, the deficit was particularly lesions (Gabrieli, 2001), supports a precocious phase in preclinical
evident in the patients with incipient AD. Indeed, the difference in AD characterized by selective involvement of the mesial–temporal
the semantic clustering score of converter and nonconverter groups areas (Braak et al., 1993) and worsening of the memory impair-
was near statistical significance. ment as atrophic changes progress in the hippocampal structures
The two word-list recognition tests documented reduced avail- (Fox et al., 1996). This is consistent with the view that when
ability of memory traces and increased sensitivity to interference patients meet the criteria for a-MCI, at least in the absence of
from irrelevant material in converter and, to a lesser extent, in comorbid conditions, it is highly probable that they have early AD
nonconverter subjects. Indeed, the two groups obtained lower d⬘ (Morris et al., 2001; Petersen et al., 2001). Increased sensitivity to
and C scores (i.e., they recognized fewer studied items and made interference, as revealed by reduced ability to discriminate be-
more false alarms) than did NCs. tween experimental and irrelevant material in word recognition
tasks, could also be related to mesial–temporal pathology (see
Kopelman, 2002). However, reduced ability to inhibit incorrect
Short-Term Memory
responses could also be due to a dysexecutive impairment related
Analysis of the short-term memory component in the immediate to a frontal lobe pathology (Tierney et al., 1996). Finally, the
recall of the unrelated word list supports the normality of this substantial integrity of implicit memory, as measured by word-
memory component in subjects affected by a-MCI. The integrity of stem completion priming in subjects with a-MCI, supports the
short-term memory even in the converter group further strengthens view that perceptual priming, especially as related to functioning
previous claims that although mild AD patients generally show a of posterior cortical regions, may not be involved in the early
short-term memory deficit (Orsini, Trojano, Chiacchio, & Grossi, phases of the AD pathology (Fleischman et al., 2005).
1988), this deficit may not appear in the preclinical phase of AD These findings are only in apparent contrast with the frequently
(Backman & Small, 1998; Perri et al., 2005). reported involvement of many cognitive functions besides memory
in the preclinical phase of AD (such as executive functioning and
attention, but also verbal abilities, perceptual speed, or implicit
Implicit Memory
memory; Backman, Jones, Berger, Small, & Laukka, 2005; Spaan,
Both nonconverter and converter subjects disclosed a normal Raaijmakers, & Jonker, 2005). Indeed, because the aim of the
priming effect in the stem completion test. Instead, on the frag- present study was to highlight the characteristics of the memory
mented picture identification test, converter subjects revealed re- deficit in the very early phases of AD, by selecting a-MCI subjects
duced visual priming compared with both nonconverters and NCs. we excluded those individuals whose pattern of cognitive impair-
These results seem to suggest the presence of intact priming for ment suggested more diffuse brain involvement, even in the ear-
verbal stimuli and reduced priming for visual data in MCI subjects liest phases.
who eventually converted to AD. However, these results could Although cognitive deficits have often been investigated in the
also reflect greater contamination caused by explicit memory pro- preclinical phase of AD (Petersen et al., 1999; Tierney et al.,
cesses during performance of the fragmented picture identification 1996), this is the first systematic comparison of episodic long-
with respect to the stem completion test. In particular, explicit term, short-term, and implicit memory features in subjects with an
memory could have favored the recognition of old figures in the isolated memory deficit who either developed AD or did not. In the
fragmented picture identification test, thus resulting in a larger present study, we document a qualitatively similar profile of mem-
facilitation rate in nonconverter and NC groups than in converters. ory impairment in the converter and nonconverter a-MCI groups
Indeed, the high effect size produced by the comparison of priming even though the amnestic syndrome of subjects with incipient AD
shown by converters with respect to nonconverters on this test is resulted systematically more severely. Indeed, for virtually all of
similar to the one resulting from the comparison of the two groups the episodic memory indexes examined (learning, forgetting, rec-
on the word-list immediate recall (i.e., on an explicit memory test) ognition abilities) a-MCI subjects who converted to AD in the
but very different from the effect size produced by the comparison following 2 years were more severely impaired than were subjects
of the priming shown by the two groups on the stem completion who did not progress toward dementia during the same period of
test. In support of this view, a correlation analysis conducted on time. In this context of pervasive episodic memory impairment,
the overall sample (converters, nonconverters, and NCs) revealed tests assessing the free recall of verbal and visual material follow-
that the priming scores on the stem completion test did not corre- ing a delay interval demonstrated greater sensitivity to the memory
late with any of the performance scores on the explicit memory deficit in converter subjects. However, because these memory
tasks of the experimental battery (Pearson’s r consistently ⬍ .10). indexes were also those most sensitive to the memory deficit in the
Indeed, in the same subjects the size of the priming in the frag- nonconverter group, individual tests showed relatively low speci-
mented picture identification test correlated with the performance ficity in identifying subjects in the preclinical phase of AD. In any
scores on each of the explicit tests of the memory battery (Pear- case, the use of a combined performance index on the four delayed
MEMORY PROFILE IN AMNESTIC MCI SUBJECTS 557

recall tasks of the memory battery permitted distinguishing con- Fleischman, D. A., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Gilley, D. W., Hauser, J. D., Lange,
verter from nonconverter subjects with 75% sensitivity and 68.5% K. L., Dwornik, L. M., et al. (1999). Word-stem completion priming in
specificity. This result supports the use of multiple tasks, all healthy aging and Alzheimer’s disease: The effects of age, cognitive
exploring delayed free recall, to increase the diagnostic power of status and encoding. Neuropsychology, 13, 22–30.
neuropsychological tools in identifying a-MCI subjects progress- Fleischman, D. A, Wilson, R. S., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Schneider, J. A.,
ing toward dementia. However, note that this finding was obtained Bienias, J. L., & Bennett, D. A. (2005). Implicit memory and Alzhei-
in a population affected by memory deficits and at high risk for mer’s disease neuropathology. Brain, 128, 2006 –2015.
developing dementia (Petersen et al., 2001). Therefore, some sub- Fox, N. C., Warrington, E. K., Freeborough, P. A., Hartikainen, P.,
Kennedy, A. M., Stevens, J. M., & Rossor, M. N. (1996). Presymptom-
jects in the nonconverter group could develop dementia over a
atic hippocampal atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease. A longitudinal MRI
longer period of time than the 2 years of the present observation.
study. Brain, 119, 2001–2007.
For this reason, only a longer follow-up period will permit us to
Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2001). Functional neuroimaging of episodic memory. In
better define the discriminative power of the individual episodic
R. Cabeza & A. Kingstone (Eds.), Handbook of functional neuroimaging
memory tasks and of the entire memory battery. of cognition (pp. 253–291). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Graf, P., Squire, L. R., & Mandler, G. (1984). The information that
References amnesic patients do not forget. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical man- Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 164 –178.
ual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Hamilton, M. (1960). A rating scale for anxiety. Journal of Neurology
Backman, L., Jones, S., Berger, A. K., Small, B. J., & Laukka, E. J. (2005). Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 23, 56 – 62.
Cognitive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: A meta-anal- Heindel, W. C., Salmon, D. P., & Butters, N. (1990). Pictorial priming and
ysis. Neuropsychology, 19, 520 –531. cued recall in Alzheimer’s and Huntington’s disease. Brain and Cogni-
Backman, L., & Small, B. J. (1998). Influences of cognitive support on tion, 13, 282–295.
episodic remembering: Tracing the process of loss from normal aging to Hughes, C. P., Berg, L., Danziger, W. L., Coben, L. A., & Martin, R.
Alzheimer’s disease. Psychology and Aging, 2, 267–276. (1982). A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. British Journal
Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1987). Beck Depression Inventory manual. San of Psychiatry, 140, 566 –572.
Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Kamman, R. (1970). Signal detection theory and recognition memory: Part
Braak, H., Braak, E., & Bohl, J. (1993). Staging of Alzheimer-related I (basic calculation). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
cortical destruction. European Neurology, 33, 403– 408. Kopelman, M. D. (1985). Rates of forgetting in Alzheimer-type dementia
Carlesimo, G. A., Buccione, I., Fadda, L., Graceffa, A., Mauri, M., and Korsakoff’s syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 23, 623– 638.
Lorusso, S., et al. (2002). Standardizzazione di due test di memoria per Kopelman, M. D. (2002). Disorders of memory. Brain, 125, 2152–2190.
uso clinico: Breve Racconto e Figura di Rey [Normative data of two Koppenaal, L., & Glanzer, M. (1990). An examination of the continuous
memory tasks: Short-story recall and Rey’s Figure]. Nuova Rivista di distract task and the “long-term recency effect”. Memory and Cogni-
Neurologia, 12, 1–13. tion, 18, 183–195.
Carlesimo, G. A., Caltagirone, C., & Gainotti, G. (1996). The Mental Lawton, M. P., & Brody, E. M. (1969). Assessment of older people;
Deterioration Battery: Normative data, diagnostic reliability and quali- self-maintaining and instrumental activity of daily living. Gerontolo-
tative analyses of cognitive impairment. The Group for the Standardiza- gist, 9, 179 –186.
tion of the Mental Deterioration Battery. European Neurology, 36, McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., &
378 –384. Stadlan, E. M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report
Carlesimo, G. A., Fadda, L., Sabbadini, M., & Caltagirone, C. (1996). of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of Department
Recency effect in Alzheimer’s disease: A reappraisal. Quarterly Journal of Health Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 34,
of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 49A, 939 –944.
315–325. Measso, G., Cavarzetan, F., Zappalà, G., Lebowitz, B. D., Crook, T. H.,
Carlesimo, G. A., Mauri, M., Graceffa, A. M. S., Fadda, L., Loasses, A., Pirozzolo, F. J., et al. (1993). The Mini-Mental State Examination:
Lorusso, S., & Caltagirone, C. (1998). Memory performances in young, Normative study of a random sample of Italian population. Develop-
elderly and very old healthy individuals versus patients with Alzhei- mental Neuropsychology, 9, 77– 85.
mer’s disease: Evidence for discontinuity between normal and patho- Morris, J. C., Storandt, M., Miller, J. P., McKeel, D. W., Price, J. L., Rubin,
logical aging. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychol- E. H., & Berg, L. (2001). Mild cognitive impairment represents early-
ogy, 20, 14 –29. stage Alzheimer disease. Archives of Neurology, 58, 397– 405.
De Blese, R., Denes, G., Luzzati, C., Mazzucchi, A., Poeck, K., Spinnler, Novelli, G., Papagno, C., Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., Cappa, S. F., &
H., & Willmes, K. (1986). L’Aachener Aphasie test I: Problemi e Vallar, G. (1986). Tre test clinici di memoria verbale a lungo termine.
soluzioni per una versione italiana del test e per uno studio crossling- Taratura su soggetti normali [Three clinical tests of verbal long-term
uistico dei disturbi afasici [Aachener Aphasie test: An Italian version of memory. Standardization on normal subjects]. Archivio di Psicologia,
the test for a cross-linguistic study of asphasic disorders]. Archivio di Neurologia e Psichiatria, 47, 278 –296.
Psicologia, Neurologia e Psichiatria, 47, 209 –236. Orsini, A., Grossi, D., Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., Papagno, C., & Vallar,
Delis, D. C., Massman, P. J., Butters, N., Salmon, D. P., Cermak, L. S., & G. (1987). Verbal and spatial immediate memory span: Normative data
Kramer, J. H. (1991). Profiles of demented and amnesic patients on from 1355 adults and 1112 children. Italian Journal of Neurological
California Verbal Learning Test: Implications for the assessment of Sciences, 8, 539 –548.
memory disorders. Psychological Assessment, 3, 19 –26. Orsini, A., Trojano, L., Chiacchio, L., & Grossi, D. (1988). Immediate
Desgranges, B., Baron, J. C., de la Sayette, V., Petit-Taboué, M. C., Benali, memory spans in dementia. Perceptual Motor Skills, 67, 267–272.
K., Landeau, B., et al. (1998). The neural substrates of memory systems Perri, R., Carlesimo, G. A., Serra, L., Caltagirone, C., & the Early Diag-
impairment in Alzheimer’s disease. A PET study of resting brain glucose nosis Group of the Italian Interdisciplinary Network on Alzheimer’s
utilization. Brain, 121, 611– 631. Disease. (2005). Characterisation of memory profile in subjects with
Fleischman, D. A., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1999). Long-term memory in mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neu-
Alzheimer’s disease. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 9, 240 –244. ropsychology, 27, 1033–1055.
558 PERRI ET AL.

Perry, J. R., & Hodges, J. R. (1996). Spectrum of memory dysfunction in Spinnler, H., & Tognoni, G. (1987). Standardizzazione e taratura italiana di
degenerative disease. Current Opinion in Neurology, 9, 281–285. test neuropsicologici [Italian standardization and validation of neuropsy-
Petersen, R. C., Doody, R., Kurz, A., Mohs, R. C., Morris, J. C., Rabins, chological tests]. Italian Journal of Neurological Sciences, Suppl 8, pp.
P. V., et al. (2001). Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. 47, 74, and 78.
Archives of Neurology, 58, 1985–1992. Squire, L. R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from
Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Waring, S. C., Ivnik, R. J., Tangalos, E. G., findings with rats, monkeys and humans. Psychological Review, 99,
& Kokmen, E. (1999). Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical character- 195–231.
ization and outcome. Archives of Neurology, 56, 303–308. Tierney, M. C., Szalai, J. P., Snow, W. G., Fisher, R. H., Nores, A., Nadon,
Roenker, D. L., Thompson, C. P., & Brown, S. C. (1971). Comparison of G., et al. (1996). Prediction of probable Alzheimer’s disease in memory-
measures for the estimation of clustering in free recall. Psychological impaired patients: A prospective longitudinal study. Neurology, 46,
Bulletin, 76, 45– 48. 661– 665.
Spaan, P. E. J., Raaijmakers, J. G. W., & Jonker, C. (2003). Alzheimer’s Waugh, N. C., & Norman, D. A. (1965). Primary memory. Psychological
disease versus normal aging: A review of the efficiency of clinical and Review, 72, 89 –104.
experimental measures. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuro-
psychology, 25, 216 –233.
Spaan, P. E. J., Raaijmakers, J. G. W., & Jonker, C. (2005). Early Received October 16, 2006
assessment of dementia: The contribution of different memory compo- Revision received April 11, 2007
nents. Neuropsychology, 19, 629 – 640. Accepted April 11, 2007 䡲

Members of Underrepresented Groups:


Reviewers for Journal Manuscripts Wanted
If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts for APA journals, the APA Publications and
Communications Board would like to invite your participation. Manuscript reviewers are vital to the
publications process. As a reviewer, you would gain valuable experience in publishing. The P&C
Board is particularly interested in encouraging members of underrepresented groups to participate
more in this process.

If you are interested in reviewing manuscripts, please write to the address below. Please note the
following important points:

• To be selected as a reviewer, you must have published articles in peer-reviewed journals. The
experience of publishing provides a reviewer with the basis for preparing a thorough, objective
review.

• To be selected, it is critical to be a regular reader of the five to six empirical journals that are most
central to the area or journal for which you would like to review. Current knowledge of recently
published research provides a reviewer with the knowledge base to evaluate a new submission
within the context of existing research.

• To select the appropriate reviewers for each manuscript, the editor needs detailed information.
Please include with your letter your vita. In the letter, please identify which APA journal(s) you
are interested in, and describe your area of expertise. Be as specific as possible. For example,
“social psychology” is not sufficient—you would need to specify “social cognition” or “attitude
change” as well.

• Reviewing a manuscript takes time (1– 4 hours per manuscript reviewed). If you are selected to
review a manuscript, be prepared to invest the necessary time to evaluate the manuscript
thoroughly.

Write to Journals Office, American Psychological Association, 750 First Street, NE, Washington,
DC 20002-4242.

You might also like