Prenatal Stress Increases The Axis Response in Young and Adult Rats

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 1994, Vol.

6, 341-345

Prenatal Stress Increases the Hypothalamo-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis


Response in Young and Adult Rats

Chantal Henry, Moharned Kabbaj, Herve Simon, Michel Le Moal and Stefania Maccari
Psychobiologie des Cornportements Adaptatifs, lnstitut National de la Sante et de la Recherche Medicale U. 259,
Universite de Bordeaux II, Domaine de Carreire-Rue Camille St Saens, 33077 Bordeaux Cedex, France.

Key words: hyporesponsive period, hippocampus, plasma corticosterone, type I and type II corticosteroid receptors.

Abstract
Prenatal stress is considered as an early epigenetic factor able to induce long-lasting alterations in brain structures and functions. It is
still unclear whether prenatal stress can induce long-lasting modifications in the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis. To test this possibility
the effects of restraint stress in pregnant rats during the third week of gestation were investigated in the functional proper'ties of the
hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis and hippocampal type I and type II corticosteroid receptors in the male offspring at 3, 21 and 90 days
of age. Plasma corticosterone was significantly elevated in prenatally-stressed rats-at 3 and 21 days after exposure to novelty. At 90
days of age, prenatally-stressed rats showed a longer duration of corticosterone secretion after exposure to novelty. No change was
observed for type I and type II receptor densities 3 days after birth, but both receptor subtypes were decreased in the hippocampus of
prenatally-stressed offspring at 21 and 90 days of life. These findings suggest that prenatal stress produces long term changes in the
hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis in the offspring.

The development of the central nervous system and of behavior Thus, the development of the HPA axis of the fetus could be
is determined by genetic factors and by the postnatal environment, influenced by in-utero exposure to abnormal levels of maternal
but also by the maternal environment during gestation (1). glucocorticoids during stress which are able to cross the placental
Clinical studies have shown a highly significant correlation and blood-brain barriers (21) and could result in long Iasting
between infant morbidity and maternal stress (2-4). However, perturbations in the offspring. However, literature data show that
these studies present methodological and interpretative difficulties. a prenatal stress is able to increase HPA axis reactivity in the
Thompson (S), conducted the first experiment to explore the early stage of life (15, 22) but it is not clear if this manipulation
manipulation of the prenatal environment in animals. Further modifies glucocorticoid feedback in the adult and, if so, by which
animal studies have shown behavioral abnormalities in the off- mechanism (23, 24). Hippocampal type I and type 11 corticos-
spring of stressed dams, lasting until adulthood (6-7). Stress teroid receptors regulate at least in part the negative feedback of
during pregnancy induces alterations in early motor development the HPA axis in adult animals (25, 26). Long-lasting alterations
(S), increases emotionality in adulthood and disrupts the normal in these receptors are responsible for impaired stress responses,
course of sexual differentiation (8-1 1 ). We have recently shown and aged rats which have less corticosteroid receptors in the
that prenatally-stressed animals exhibit a higher locomotion hippocampus have a delayed stress-response (27).
response to novelty and a higher level of amphetamine self- Given this evidence, we investigated the influence of restraint
administration (12). stress, during the third- week of pregnancy, on the development
It has also been found that prenatal stress reduces dopamine of the HPA axis and corticosteroid receptors of the offspring.
turnover rates in the left corpora striata (13-14) and induces Thus, we have measured basal and stress corticosterone levels in
changes in brain 5 hydroxytryptamine especially in the hypothal- 3, 21, and 90 day-old male rats and hippocampal type I and
amus at 60 days of age (15). The activity of the hypothalamo- type I1 corticosteroid receptors at the same age.
pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis may also play a critical role in the
behavioral modifications observed in prenatally-stressed rats
Results
since, in the adult, the HPA axis is central for the control of the
homeostatic disturbances induced by stress ( 16) and the hyperac- Data concerning circulating corticosterone levels in 3, 21, 90 day-
tivity of the HPA axis has already been associated with behavioral old rats under basal conditions, at the end of a 30 min exposure
disorders (17-19). It has been shown that a maternal corticos- to novelty and, for the adult subjects, also after 120 min of
terone manipulation modifies the development of this axis (20). exposure to novelty, are shown in Figure 1.

Correspondence 20: D r Stefania Maccari, INSERM U259-Universitt de Bordeaux 11, Rue Camille St Saens, 33077 Bordeaux Cedex, France.
342 Prenatal stress increases HPA axis response

3D 21 D ** 90 D

2.5 -- *
25 25
T
- 20
E 20
0
z -
p 2.0
Q)
C 15 15
E
c
v)
0
.-
5 1.5- 10 10
V

5 5

l.1
I
- 0 a I I I I 0 I I I I I
0 10 20 30 0 10 20 30 0 30 60 90 120
lime (min)

FIG.1. Study of corticosterone secretion after exposure to novelty in 3- 21- and 90-day-old rats. Basal levels (T 0) of corticosterone were not affected
by prenatal stress in 3-, 21- and 90-day-old male rats. Conversely, 3 and 21 day-old offsprings of stressed mothers exhibited a significant rise in
corticosterone after exposure to stress (T 30), whereas control 3 day-old pups remained unresponsive. At 90 days of age the prenatally-stressed animals
showed no difference 30 min after stress (T 30) however, after 120 min (T 120), corticosterone levels remained elevated in prenatally-stressed rats with
respect to controls. Eight to 5 animals were used for each group. *=P<O.O5; * * = P t 0 .0 1 . 0 Control; 0 Prenatal Stress.

At 3 days of age prenatal stress modified stress-induced corticos- not differ between the two groups. At 21 days the number of the
terone secretion (ANOVA Prenatal Stress x stress novelty inter- two types of receptors was significantly lower in prenatally-
action F( 1.28) = 3.84; P ~ 0 . 0 5 ) No
. difference was observed in stressed rats (ANOVA Prenatal Stress effect F ( 1.28)= 13.614;
the basal corticosterone levels of the two groups, however, the P=O.OOl). Thus, type I receptors were reduced of 31% and
corticosterone levels in response to stress (T30) were higher for type I1 of 18%. The same results were found at 90 days of age.
the prenatally-stressed group ( F ( 1.14)= 5.342; P < 0.05). Type I and type I1 receptors were reduced by prenatal stress
Furthermore, control rats at 3 days of age displayed no response (ANOVA Prenatal Stress effect F(1.12)=6.35; P=0.02). Type I
to novelty-stress ( F ( 1.14) =0.246; P = 0.6277), whereas at the receptors were reduced by 70% and type I1 by 30%.
same age prenatally-stressed rats were able to respond to stress The affinities of both receptors for these compounds remained
(F(1.14)= 15.323; P<O.OOl). unaffected (Table 1) .
At 21 days of age both groups of animals were able to respond
to novelty (ANOVA novelty stress effect F ( 1.28) = 67.94,
Discussion
P <0.0001). However, prenatally-stressed animals showed a
higher corticosterone secretion (ANOVA Prenatal Stress effect The data indicate that maternal stress during pregnancy has
F( 1.28) = 7.97; P < 0.01 ). Thus, prenatally-stressed animals short- and long-term effecfs on HPA axis reactivity of the
showed a higher response to stress (T30) than controls (ANOVA offspring. In 3 and 21 day-old male rats, the rise in plasma
F(1.14)= 12.39; P<O.Ol). corticosterone in response to stress was significantly higher in
At 90 days of age, prenatal stress also increased plasma prenatally-stressed pups. Furthermore, control 3 day-old rats
corticosterone secretion (ANOVA Prenatal Stress effect F ( 1.9) = showed no response to novelty-stress. h control adult rats
8.531; P=O.O17) and this effect depended on the time of exposure corticosterone levels declined at 120 min, whereas prenatally-
to stress (ANOVA Prenatal Stress x Time interaction F(2.18) = stressed subjects still displayed elevated levels after the same
5.041; P=0.018). Thus, the prenatally-stressed rats showed a delay. Prenatal stress decreased type I and type I1 corticosterone
higher corticosterone secretion only after 120 min of novelty receptors in the hippocampus at 21 and 90 days of age, whereas
exposure ( F ( 1.9)= 12.087; P=0.007). the same group studied at 3 days after birth did not display any
These changes in corticosterone responsiveness to novelty were change in receptor number. No change in receptor affinity was
concomitant to a decrease in cytosolic corticosterone receptors in encountered in animals examined at 21 and 90 days of age. This
the hippocampus of prenatally-stressed offspring at different study shows that both basal and stress corticosterone levels
ages (Fig. 2). increase during development. Type I1 hippocampal glucocorticoid
At 3 days of age type I and type I1 corticosteroid receptors did receptor density appeared to be greater than type I density at
Prenatal stress increases HPA axis response 343
125 TABLE 1. Affinity Values of Type I and Type I1 Hippocampal
Corticosteroid Receptors in 21- and 90-Day-Old Rats.
21 Days 90 Days
100

I
~~~ ~

Kd type I Kd type I1 Kd type I Kd type I1

Control 1.39k0.24 1.51k0.15 2.1350.28 1.42k0.22


Prenatal stress 1.15 k0.21 1.13k0.13 1.63k0.23 1.47 k0.35
75

I
The apparent affinities (Kd) of both receptors did not change in pre-
natally-stressed rats with age. The kd is expressed innM. The values
indicate the mean +SEM.
50

** corticosterone secretion after exposure to various stresses (3 1-34),


T

25 as we found in control 3 day-old rats. This hyporesponsive period


-.-
C
was recently discussed, and Walker et al. (35) reported that under
particularity severe forms of stress 1 and 5 day-old pups are able
al
c
to respond. These pups show an increase in corticosterone levels
g o
r
with a peak 10 min after the exposure to stress. However, in the
0 D 90 D mentioned study, the pregnant mothers were obtained on day 18
E"
2- of gestation, we could thus hypothesize that the attenuation of
E
'C 250
the SHRP in the pups could have been due to this maternal stress
(36). In fact, in our experiment we show that 3 day-old animals
X
born by mothers stressed during days 14 to 21 of pregnancy
;
rn - respond to the stress procedure. Furthermore, the prenatally-
200 stressed 3 day-old rats had the same density of hippocampal
corticosteroid receptors than controls, so the changes in corticos-
terone secretion appear earlier than the changes in corticosteroid
receptors. This disappearance of the SHRP could account for
150 * the decrease in hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor densities

I
T observed in prenatally-stressed 21 and 90 day-old rats. In fact,
rats treated with glucocorticoids during the first week of life have
100
permanently reduced brain weights and DNA contents (37, 38)
and the effects are most profound in those brain regions where
there is extensive postnatal mitosis, such as the hippocampus (39).
Our study shows that maternal stress alters in the long run the

L
50 reactivity of the HPA axis in response to stress and decreases the
number of type I and II corticosterone receptors in the hippocam-
pus of adult rats. The efficacy of the negative-feedback is deter-
mined by the ability of glucocorticoids to inhibit subsequent
0 3
3D ACTH release and is mediated by corticosteroid receptors. In
Type It addition to target sites such as the pituitary and the hypothalamus,
it seems, as previously mentioned, that hippocampal receptors
FIG.2. Hippocampal type I and type I1 corticosteroid receptor numbers play a role in terminating the HPA response to stress (25). The
in 3-, 21- and 90-day-old male rats. The maximal binding capacity (BmaX)
of type I receptors is indicated in the top of the figure. The B,,, of type I1
decrease of hippocampal type I and type I1 corticosteroid recep-
receptors in indicated in the bottom of the figure. Type I and type I1 tors observed in prenatally-stressed rats could be responsible, at
corticosterone receptor numbers were no different at 3 days of age, but least in part, for the liigher and longer corticosterone secretion.
were significantly lower in prenatally-stressed animals compared to con- The major interest of this model lies in the demonstration of
trols at 21 and 90 days. At 21 days, type I was -31% and type I1 - 18%. long lasting modifications in the central nervous system induced
At 90 days of age type I was -70% and type I1 -30%. For the study
at 3 and 21 days of age the number of animals was 14-16 for group; by environmental modifications that have occurred in early life,
for the study at 90 days of age 7 rats for group were used. *=P<0.05; but the mechanisms remain unclear. Maternal corticosterone,
**=P<O.Ol; ***=P<O.OOl. 0 Control; Prenatal stress. secreted during the stress, could play a decisive role in this
phenomenon. In fact, this hormone crosses the placental and
hematoencephalic barriers (21) and can thus interact with the
every age and type I and type I1 receptors reached the adult's central nervous system of the developing fetus. Furthermore, we
density at 21 days. These observations are in agreement with know that in adult animals high levels of corticosterone, obtained
previous works (28, 29). by stressing the subjects or by repeated corticosterone injections,
One of the salient points in our observation actually lies in the causes a decrease of glucocorticoid receptors (40-42).
plasticity of the stress hyporesponsive response period (SHRP) In conclusion, it would be interesting to link these short and
proposed by Schapiro (30), which is mainly based on the lack of long-term changes on HPA axis activity induced by prenatal
344 Prenatal stress increases HPA axis response
stress with behavioral impairment observed in these animals ( 12, Type I and type I1 corticosteroidreceptors in male offspring
23, 43). Interestingly, higher corticosterone secretion has been Three day-oldpups ( n = I4--15per group)
related t o various psychiatric conditions such as depression (18)
Each hippocampus was homogenized in 700 ml of ice-cold TEGDM
and t o impaired performance in memory tasks (44). Furthermore, buffer and individual maximal binding was determinated with tritiated
the increase in vulnerability to addictive drugs m a y depend on corticosterone (3H-B) and tritiated RU 28362 at a concentration of
changes in t h e activity o f the H P A axis (19). 20 nM. Non-specific binding (NSB) for the 3H-B binding was determined
in the presence of a 500-fold excess of unlabeled corticosterone, and for
3H-RU 28362, was assessed in the presence of a 500-fold excess of
unlabeled RU 28362. Binding equilibrium was reached after 22 h at 4 "C.
Materials and Methods
Twenty-one day-old rats ( n = 14-16per group) and 90 day-old rats (n = 7
Animals and housing conditions
per group)
Virgin female Wistar rats (Iffa-Credo, Lyon) were obtained at 15 weeks
In order to eliminate endogenous corticosterone, type 1 and type I1
of age. Sexually experienced male Wistar rats were used for mating.
corticosteroid receptors were determined using an exchange assay as
Animals were supplied with ad libitum food and water and maintained
previously shown (46).
on a constant light/dark cycle (light on from 06.00-20.00), with constant
One hippocampus per animal was used. The tissue was homogenized
temperature (23 "C).
in 2 ml of ice-cold TEGDM buffer for the 90 day-old rats, whereas for
Breeding the 21 day-old rats 1.7 ml were used (30 mM Tris, pH adjusted to 7.4
with HC1 6N, 1 mM EDTA, 10 mM sodium molybdate, 1 mM dithio-
Breeding was carried out in our laboratory in order to avoid the stress threitol and 10% glycerol). The tissue was centrifuged (105,OOOg, 15 min
due to shipping of pregnant females (36). For reproduction, 1 male was in a Beckman TL 100 ultracentrifuge) at 2 "C. Endogenous, unbound
housed with 4 females overnight during a whole estrous cycle. Vaginal steroids were removed from the soluble fraction by passing the sam-
smear was examined every morning for the presence of sperm (indicating ples through LH-20 columns equilibrated with TEGM buffer (10 mM
day 1 of pregnancy). Pregnant rats were then individually housed in Tris, 2 m M EDTA, 10mM sodium molybdate and 2.3 mM
plastic breeding cages, and divided into 2 groups: stress (n= 10) and p-mercaptoethanol). For the type I receptor assay, aliquots of cytosol
controls (n = 10). (140 pl) were incubated with tritiated corticosterone (specific' activity
88 Ci/mmol, New England Nuclear, Paris) over a concentration range of
Prenutal stress procedure 0.625-20nM (6 points for each Scatchard plot), and with a 100-fold
Stress began on day 14 and ended on day 21 of pregnancy (11). The excess of unlabeled'RU 28362. Unlabeled RU 28362 was used to displace
stress sessions consisted of introducing females in Plexiglas holders 'H-B from type I1 receptors (47). Type I1 receptor binding was evaluated
(20 x 6 x 8 cm) exposed to bright light for 45 min. Animals were submitted directly using pure glucocorticoid 3H-RU 28362 (specific activity
to three daily stress sessions starting at 09.00, 12.00 and 17.00. Control 74.3 Ci/mmol, Dositek) over a concentration range of 1.25-40 nM (6
animals were left undisturbed in their home cages. The litters used for points for each Scatchard plot). This has been shown to be sufficient for
both groups contained between 8 and 14 pups. Litters with different maximal exchange, and binding remains stable over this period (48, 49).
numbers of pups were eliminated. Offspring were weaned 21 days after Bound and unbound 3H-B or 3H-RU 28362 were separated on Sephadex
birth and housed in isosexual groups of 3 animals until 90 days of age. LH-20 columns equilibrated with TEDGM buffer at 2 "C, using 60 pl of
A maximum of 2 male pups was taken from each litter. the incubates and eluding with 940 pl of TEDGM buffer. A milliliter of
elute containing the bound form was added to 3 mi of scintillation fluid
(Aqua Luma Plus, Lumac, Paris), and radioactivity was counted. Protein
Corticosterone secretion in the male offsprings concentration was determined according to Lowry et al. (SO) using
Three day-old and 21 day-old pups before weaning albumin as standard. The apparent maximum binding capacity (Bmax)
of 3H-B or 3H-RU 28362 and dissociation constants (Kd) for both
In order to determine baseline secretion, blood was collected from 8 pups receptor types were evaluated from Scatchard plots (51).
per group in an adjacent room immediately after removal from the litter.
For determination of stress secretion, 2 pups of a same litter were Statistical analysis
separated from their mother and placed for 30 min in a clean cage. The Analysis of variance (ANOVA) for repeated measures was employed for
blood was collected immediately at the end of this stress period. All tests type I and type I1 corticosteroid receptors and plasma corticosterone
were performed in the morning between 09.00 and 11.00. Trunk blood levels in 90 day-old rats. For the analysis of plasma corticosterone levels
was collected in heparinized tubes. Tubes were centrifuged at 4 "C for at 3 and 21 days of age, time of sampling was introduced as between
20 min at 3000 rpm. Plasma was aliquoted and stored at -20 "C until factor. The Crunch statistical package (Crunch Software, Oakland CA,
assay. USA) was used throughout. A logarithmic transformation was applied
to normalize the distribution. Results are expressed as the mean & SEM.
Ninety day-old rats
At this age both groups (n=5-6 per group) were implanted with
intracardiac catheters. Animals were anaesthetized with chloral hydrate
(3.50 mg/kg, i.p.), and a catheter (Silastic, Sigma Medical, Nanterre) was
Acknowledgements
inserted into the right auricle through the external jugular vein, passed This study was supported by Institut National de la Sante et de la
under the skin, and exited in the mid scapular region. After 6 days of Recherche Medicale (INSERM), University de Bordeaux 11, Fondation
recovery, the rats were tested for their response to novelty. The animals pour le Recherche Medicale and Conseil Rtgional d'Aquitaine. We thank
were placed in the room of the test the day before the stress session. The Roussel-UCLAF for providing RU 28362.
morning after, the animals were placed for 120 min in the novel environ-
ment, which consisted of a circular corridor (10 cm wide and 70 cm in
diameter). Corticosterone was assayed in three blood samples (500 pl Accepted 24 February 1994
each) collected immediately before the exposure to novelty and at 30 and
120 min. Samples were collected through the catheter.
Plasma corticosterone was measured by radiocompetitive binding after References
extraction with dichloromethane (45). Corticosteroid-binding globulin
was obtained from pregnant women, and dextran-coated charcoal was 1. Joffe JM. (1965). Genotype and prenatal and premating stress interact
used to remove the unbound steroid. The sensitivity of the assay was to affect adult behaviour in rats. Science. 150: 1844-1845.
0.4 ng per tube, and the inter-and intra-assay variations were, respectively, 2. Cohen S, Evans GW, Krantz DS, Stokols D. (1980). Physiological,
7% and 3.8% at a mean value of 1.2 ng per tube and 9.8% and 5% at a motivational and cognitive effects of aircraft noise on children. Am
mean value of 5 ng per tube. Psychologist. 35: 231-243.
Prenatal stress increases HPA axis response 345
3. Jones FN, Tauscher J. (1978). Residence under an airport landing of type I and type I1 corticosteroid receptors in the rat hippocampus.
pattern as a factor in teratism. Arch Environ Health. 33: 10-12. Dev Brain Res 42: 113-1 18.
4. Stott DN. (1973). Follow-up study from birth of the effects of prenatal 30. Schapiro S. (1962). Pituitary ACTH and compensatory adrenal
stress. Dev Med Child Neurol. 15: 770-787. hypertrophy in stress-non-responsive infant rats. Endocrinology. 7 1:
5. Thompson WR. (1957). Influence of prenatal maternal anxiety on 986-989.
emotionality in young rats. Science. 125: 698-699. 31. Butte C, Kakihana R, Farnham ML, Noble EP. (1973). The relation-
6. Archer JE, Blackman DE. (1971). Prenatal psychological stress and ship between brain and plasma corticosterone stress in developing rats.
offspring behavior in rats and mice. Dev Psychohiol. 3: 193-248. Endocrinology. 92: 1775-1779.
7. Barlow SM, Knight AF, Sullivan FM. (1978). Delay in postnatal 32. Haltmeyer GC, Denenberg VT, Thatcher J, Zarrow MX. (1966).
growth and development of offspring produced by maternal restraint Response of the adrenal cortex of the neonatal rat after subjection to
stress during pregnancy in the rat. Teratology. 18: 211-218. stress. Nature. 212: 1371-1372.
8. Ader R, Conklin PM. (1963). Handling of pregnant rats: effects of 33. Walker CD, Perrin M, Vale W. (1986). Ontogeny of the stress
emotionality of their offspring. Science. 143: 41 1-412. response in the rat: role of the pituitary and the hypothalamus.
9. Masterpasqua F, Chapman RH, Lore RK. (1976). The effects of Endocrinology. 118: 1445-1451.
prenatal psychological stress on the sexual behavior and reactivity of 34. D’Amato FR, Cabib S, Puglisi-Allegra S, Patacchioli FR. Cigliana G,
male rats. Dev Psychohiol. 9: 403-41 1. Maccari S, Angelucci L. (1992). Effects of acute and repeated exposure
10. Ward IL. (1972). Prenatal stress feminises and demasculinises the to stress on the hypothalomo-pituitary-adrenocortical activity in mice
behavior of males. Science. 175: 82-84. during postnatal development. Hormones Behav. 26: 474-485.
1 1 . Ward IL. (1984). The prenatal stress syndrome: current status. 35. Walker CD, Scribner KA, Cascio CS, Dallman MF. (1991). The
Psychoneuroendocrinology.9: 3-1 1. pituitary-adrenocortical system of neonatal rats is responsive to stress
throughout development in a time-dependent and stressor-specific
12. Derniniire JM, Piazza PV, Guegan G, Abrous N, Maccari S, Le
fashion, Endocrinology. 128(3): 1385-1395.
Moal M, Simon H. (1992). Increased locomotor response to novelty
36. Wiley J. (1981). Is stress due to shipment of animals a confounding
and propensity to intravenous amphetamine self-administration in adult
variable in developmental research? Devel Psychohiol. 14(2): 169-171.
offspring of stressed mothers. Brain Res. 586: 135-139.
37. Ardeleanu A, Sterescu. (1978). RNA and DNA synthesis in develop-
13. Fride E, Weinstock M. (1987). Increased interhemispheric coupling ing rat brain: hormonal influences. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 3:
of the dopamine systems induced by prenatal stress. Brain Res Bull. 93-101,
18: 457-461. 38. Balazs R, Cotterrell M. (1972). Effect of hormonal siate on cell
14. Fride E, Weinstock M. (1989). Alterations in behavioral and striatal number and functional maturation of the brain. Nature. (London)
dopamine asymmetries induced by prenatal stress. Pharmacol Biochem 236: 348-350 ...
Behav. 32: 425-430. 39. Bohn MC. (1980). Glucocorticoid induced teratologies of the nervous
15. Peters DAV. (1982). Prenatal stress: effects on brain biogenic amine system. In: Yauci J. ed. Neurohehavioral teratologies of the nervous
and plasma corticosterone levels. Pharmac Biochem Behav. 17: 721-725. system. 365-387. Elsevier, Amsterdam.
16. Selye H. (1956). The stress of life. McGraw Hill Book Co., New York. 40. Sapolsky RM, Krey LC, McEwen BS. (1984). Stress down-regulates
17. Anisman H, Lapierre Y. (1982). In: Neufeld RW, ed. Psychological corticosterone receptors in a site-specific manner. Endocrinology. I 1 4
stress and psychopathology. 179-21 7. McGraw-Hill, New-York. 287-292.
18. Pepper GM, Krieger DT. (1984). Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal 41. Sapolsky RM, McEwen BS. (1985). Down-regulation of neural
abnormalities in depression: their possible relation to central mechan- corticosterone receptors by corticosterone and dexamethasone. Brain
isms regulating ACTH. In: Post RM, Ballenger JC, eds, Neurohiology Res. 339: 161-165.
of mood disorders. 245-254. Williams & Wilkins, Baltimore. 42. Maccari S, Piazza PV, Deminitre JM, Lemaire V, Mormede P,
19. Piazza PV, Maccari S, Deminiere JM, Le Moal M, Mormede P, Simon H, Angelucci L, Le Moal M. (1991). Life events-induced
Simon H. ( 1991). Corticosterone levels determine individual vulner- decrease of type I corticosteroid receptors is associated with a decrease
ability to amphetamine self-administration. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 88: of corticosterone feedback and increase the vulnerability to amphet-
2088-2092. amine self-administration. Brain Res. 547: 7-12.
20. Angelucci L, Patacchioli FR, Scaccianoce S, Di Sciullo A, Cardillo A, 43. Takahashi LK, Kalin NH, Barksdale CM, Van Der Burgt JA. (1988).
Maccari S. (1985). A model for later-life effects of perinatan drug Stressor controllability during pregnancy influences pituitary-adrenal
exposure: maternal hormone mediation. Neurohehav Toxic01 Teratol. hormone concentrations and analgesic responsiveness in offspring.
7: 51 1-517. Physiol Behav. 42: 323-329.
21. Zarrow MX, Philpott JE, Denenberg VH. (1970). Passage of I4C-4 44. Meaney MJ, Mitchell JB, Aitken DH, Bhatnagar S, Bodnoff SR, Iny
Corticosterone from the rat mother to the foetus and neonate. Nature. LJ and Sarrieau A. (1991). The effects of neonatal handling on the
226: 1058-1059. development of the adrenocortical response to stress: implications for
22. Takahashi LK, Haglin C, Kalin NH. (1992). Prenatal stress potenti- neuropathology and cognitive deficits in later life.
ates stress-induced behavior and reduces the propensity to play in Psychoneuroendocrinology. 16: 8.5-103.
juvenile rats. Physiology & Behavior. 51: 319-323. 45. Murphy BE. (1967). Some studies of the protein-binding steroids
23. Fride E, Dan Y, Feldon J, Halevy G, Weinstock M. (1986). Effects and their application to the routine micro measurement of various
of prenatal stress on vulnerability to stress in prepubertal and adult steroids in body fluids by repetitive protein-binding radioassay. J Clin
rats. Physiol Behav. 37: 681-687. Endoc Metahol. 27: 973-990.
46. Casolini P, Piazza P x Kabbaj M, Leprat F, Angelucci L, Simon H,
24. Weinstock M, Matlina E, Maor GL, Rosen H, McEwen B. (1992).
Le Moal M, Maccari S. (1993). The mesolimbic dopaminergic system
Prenatal stress selectively alters the reactivity of the hypothalamo- exerts an inhibitory influence on brain corticosteroid receptor affinities.
pituitary adrenal system in the female rats. Brain Res. 595: 195-198. Neuroscience. 55: 429-434.
25. McEwen BS, De Kloet ER, Rostene W. (1986). Adrenal steroid 47. Moguilevski M, Raynaud JP. (1980) ...Evidence for a specific mineral-
receptors and actions in the nervous system. Physiol Rev. 66: 1121-1 188. ocorticoid receptor in rat pituitary and brain. J Steroid Biochem.
26. De Kloet ER, Reul JMHM. (1987). Feedback action and tonic 12: 309-315.
influence of corticosteroids on brain function: a concept arising from 48. Kalimi M, Hubbard JR. (1983). Development of an exchange assay
the heterogeneity of brain receptor systems. Psychoneuroendocrinol. for cytosolic glucocorticoid receptors using the synergistic effects of
12: 83-105. molybdate plus dithiothreitol. Endocrinology. 113: 1161-1 163.
27. Meaney MJ, Aitken DH, Van Berkel C, Bhatnagar S, Sapolsky M. 49. Meaney MJ, Viau V, Aitken DH, Bhatnagar S. (1988). Stress-induced
( 1988). Effect of neonatal handling on age-related impairments associ- occupancy and translocation of hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors.
ated with the hippocampus. Science. 239: 766-768. Brain Res. 445: 198-203.
28. Sarrieau A, Sharma S, Meaney MJ. (1988). Postnatal development 50. Lowry OH, Rosenbough N, Farr AL, Randall RJ. (1951). Protein
and environmental regution of hippocampal glucocorticoid and measurements with the Folin phenol reagent. J Biol Chem. 193:265-267.
mineralocorticoid receptors. Dev Brain Res. 43: 158-162. 51. Scatchard G. (1949). The attraction of proteins for small molecules
29. Rosenfeld P, Sutanto W, Levine S, De Kloet ER. (1988). Ontogeny and ions. Ann NY Acad Sci. 51: 660.

You might also like