Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Developmental Psychology © 2013 American Psychological Association

2014, Vol. 50, No. 3, 914 –921 0012-1649/14/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0034169

The Development of Recipient-Dependent Sharing Behavior and Sharing


Expectations in Preschool Children

Markus Paulus Chris Moore


Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München Dalhousie University
and University of Erfurt

This study investigated the development of sharing expectations and sharing behavior in 3 groups of 3-,
4-, and 5-year-old children. We examined (a) whether preschool children expect a person to share more
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

with a friend than with a disliked peer and (b) whether their expectation about others’ sharing behavior
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

depends on whether there is a cost or not. Additionally, (c) we assessed children’s own sharing behavior
toward the different recipients in the same situation. The results show that expectations about sharing and
actual sharing follow similar developmental pathways and that recipient-dependent sharing and sharing
expectations emerge around 4 years of age. Moreover, individual-level analyses provide evidence that by
5 years of age, children’s own sharing and their expectations of others’ sharing are closely related to each
other.

Keywords: prosocial development, sharing, friendship, peer relations, cognitive development

Sharing is part of social life and plays a very important role in Kuhlmeier, 2010; Kärtner, Keller, & Chaudhary, 2010; Paulus,
the formation and maintenance of relationships. Being used to the Kühn-Popp, Licata, Sodian, & Meinhardt, 2013; Vaish, Carpenter,
fact that people share with each other, humans develop expecta- & Tomasello, 2009; Warneken & Tomasello, 2007; Zahn-Waxler,
tions about others’ prosocial behavior. For example, if someone is Radke-Yarrow, Wagner, & Chapman, 1992; for a recent review,
hungry and meets a friend who has cookies, he has reason to see Paulus & Moore, 2012), including sharing resources with
expect that his friend will share his cookies with him. As this others (e.g., Rheingold, Hay, & West, 1976; Svetlova, Nichols, &
example illustrates, expectations about others’ prosocial behavior Brownell, 2010; Thompson, Barresi, & Moore, 1997). Interest-
play an important role in the regulation of relationships (cf. Berndt, ingly, preschool children do not share in every situation to the
2002; Gifford-Smith & Brownell, 2003). Such knowledge is im- same extent, but their sharing behavior is affected by factors such
portant for behavioral planning and supports proper functioning in as the nature of the social cues provided by the helpee (Svetlova et
the social environment. However, little is known about the devel- al., 2010), the amount of resources available (Hay, Caplan, Castle,
opment of these expectations in early childhood. Moreover, it is & Stimson, 1991), the relative distribution of resources (Paulus,
largely unclear how preschool children’s expectations about oth- Gillis, Li, & Moore, 2013), or a shared history of previous collab-
ers’ sharing relate to their own sharing behavior, that is, whether or oration (Warneken, Lohse, Melis, & Tomasello, 2011). Moreover,
not they develop in parallel and whether or not they are related to it has been shown that children’s prosocial behavior increasingly
each other. depends on the other’s gender (e.g., Hay, Castle, Davies, Dem-
Research on the development of prosocial behavior has indi-
etriou, & Stimson, 1999) and their relationship with the other (e.g.,
cated that from early in life children behave prosocially toward
Birch & Billman, 1986; Fehr, Bernhard, & Rockenbach, 2008),
others (e.g., Brownell, Svetlova, & Nichols, 2009; Dunfield &
suggesting that early sharing behavior becomes more selective in
the course of the preschool years (Hay & Cook, 2007). Moore
(2009), for example, provided evidence that 4.5- to 6-year-old
preschool children share more with friends than with disliked
This article was published Online First August 26, 2013.
Markus Paulus, Department of Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-
peers. Additionally, research has shown that children’s sharing
Universität München, Munich, Germany, and Department of Psychology, behavior depends on whether there is a cost to themselves or not.
University of Erfurt, Erfurt, Germany; Chris Moore, Department of Psy- In particular, preschool- and school-aged children will choose an
chology, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada. equitable distribution of resources more often when there is no cost
This research was supported by a grant to Markus Paulus from the Social to themselves than when there is a cost to themselves (Fehr et al.,
Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada and by a European 2008; Thompson et al., 1997). These results provide clear evidence
Research Council advanced grant, Dividnorm Project 269616. Thanks are that from the preschool age onward people’s personal relations to
due to Monika Wörle for assistance with the project as well as to the St.
another person and the costs related to sharing affect their moti-
Martin and St. Josef kindergartens in Penzing and the St. Margareta
kindergarten in Schwifting, Germany. vation to share with another. However, as research has mainly
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Markus focused on children’s own inclination to share with others, less is
Paulus, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Leopoldstrasse 13, known about children’s developing expectations of other people’s
80802 Munich, Germany. E-mail: markus.paulus@lmu.de sharing behavior.
914
DEVELOPMENT OF SHARING BEHAVIOR AND SHARING EXPECTATIONS 915

The aim of the current study was to examine the development of showed that the infants who looked longer at the unequal than the
sharing expectations in the preschool years. Furthermore, we equal distributions were also more likely to share with another
aimed to investigate whether children’s own sharing behavior is person. Moreover, a recent study by Henderson and colleagues
related to their expectations of others’ sharing. Knowledge about (Henderson, Wang, Matz, & Woodward, 2012) indicates that after
children’s expectations of other’s sharing would be informative for active experiences in a collaboration paradigm, 10-month-old in-
a number of reasons. First, it would reveal the expectations that fants were able to understand other persons’ common collaborative
guide children’s interactions with other people—an important part goal. These findings open up the possibility that sharing behavior
of social information processing (cf. Crick & Dodge, 1994). More and sharing expectations are related to each other. Indeed, a
concretely, it would show how and when young children acquire number of studies in developmental and cognitive psychology
knowledge of which kind of sharing behavior to expect from have suggested that action production and action perception are
different people. Second, such knowledge would be informative closely related (e.g., Daum, Prinz, & Aschersleben, 2011; Grede-
for research on the development of concepts of social relation- bäck & Melinder, 2010; Paulus, Hunnius, Vissers, & Bekkering,
ships, in particular the development of an understanding of friend- 2011; Sommerville & Woodward, 2005; for reviews see Decety &
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ship (e.g., Laursen & Hartup, 2002). Finally, studying the relation Sommerville, 2003; Meltzoff, 2007). Findings about a close rela-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

between sharing expectations and sharing behavior would be in- tion between own sharing and sharing expectations on the level of
formative for research and theory on the relation between action the individual would suggest that children have acquired a schema
perception and production. Given that recent findings suggested a or conceptual representation on how to behave in sharing situa-
tight relation between own behavior and expectations of other tions that is likewise applicable to self and other (Barresi & Moore,
people’s actions (e.g., Gredebäck & Melinder, 2010; Sommerville 1996). In sum, the reviewed literature shows that young children
& Woodward, 2005), it would be interesting to examine whether have expectations that a person will share with another person and
this tight relation holds also in the realm of prosocial behavior. provides some evidence for a possible relation between sharing
Two lines of research are informative with respect to our knowl- behavior and sharing expectations. Yet, it remains unclear how
edge of children’s expectations of others’ sharing. One line of these initial expectations change and develop in the course of the
research assessed children’s behavior in resource distribution sce- preschool years, how they become more differentiated and, in
narios. Commonly, in these scenarios children are presented with particular, whether they are affected by the relationship between
a number of resources, which do not belong to them, and different sharer and recipient or the costs associated with sharing.
potential recipients. They are asked to distribute all the resources To this end, we presented preschool children with two tasks—a
between recipients or to judge different resource allocations (e.g., task in which they could share themselves and a task in which they
Damon, 1977; Kenward & Dahl, 2011; McGillicuddy-De Lisi, had to predict other people’s sharing behavior—and investigated
Watkins, & Vinchur, 1994; Olson & Spelke, 2008). For example, the relation between both tasks. As previous studies have provided
Olson and Spelke (2008) asked 3.5-year-old children to help a evidence for recipient-dependent sharing in 4.5- to 6-year-old
protagonist distribute resources between pairs of different potential children (Moore, 2009), we decided to investigate three different
recipients. The results showed that the children guided the protag- age groups, 3-, 4-, and 5- to 6-year-old children, in an active
onist to give more resources to friends and siblings than to strang- sharing task and in a sharing expectation task. We included friends
ers. These findings are informative for research on prosocial de- and disliked peers as potential recipients. The aim of this experi-
velopment. Yet, in this task children could never decide to share ment was threefold: First, given that previous research has shown
own resources. This task differs thus from studies on children’s that 4.5- to 6-year-old children’s sharing is recipient-dependent,
expectations of others’ prosocial and sharing behavior, as these we wanted to examine the development of this behavior by exam-
behaviors require that agents can act prosocially and share some of ining a broader age range of children. Second, we were interested
their own goods— or not. to see whether the development of children’s own sharing behavior
The second line of developmental research that directly exam- and their expectations of others’ sharing would show the same
ined children’s expectations of others’ prosocial behavior is re- developmental timeline. Third, we investigated individual-level
stricted to only a few studies (e.g., Berndt, 1981; Schmidt & associations between children’s own sharing behavior and their
Sommerville, 2011). Berndt (1981) paired 5- to 10-year-old chil- expectations of others’ sharing.
dren with either a friend or a close acquaintance. He interviewed
the children and asked for their intentions to behave prosocially Method
toward their partner in different hypothetical situations. Interest-
ingly, he also asked children about their partner’s expectation of Participants
whether the child would help him or share with him. He reported
no effect of partner type. Yet, it is possible that the interview The final sample included 18 3-year-old children (range 3 years
technique underestimated children’s expectations and that the 2 months to 3 years 11 months; M ⫽ 43.0, SD ⫽ 2.7; 4 boys), 17
presence of the other person affected children’s answers in terms 4-year-old children (range 4 years 1 month to 4 years 11 months;
of a social desirability bias. One result that confirms this view has M ⫽ 54.7, SD ⫽ 3.5; 10 boys), and 19 5- to 6-year-old children
recently been provided by Schmidt and Sommerville (2011). They (range 5 years 1 month to 6 years 4 months; M ⫽ 69.6, SD ⫽ 5.8;
presented 15-month-old children with a habituation-based proce- 9 boys). Two 3-year-old children were tested but not included in
dure in which fair (i.e., equal) and unfair (i.e., unequal) resource the final sample due to refusal to continue the study and due to
distributions were presented. The authors found that infants looked procedural errors. The participants attended one of three day care
longer at the unequal than at the equal distribution, indicating an centers located in the surroundings of a larger German city. Chil-
early expectation for fair distributions. Additionally, the results dren were native German speakers from heterogeneous socioeco-
916 PAULUS AND MOORE

nomic backgrounds. Informed consent for participation was given two of the 3-year-old children, one of the 4-year-old children, and
by the children’s caregivers. The participants received stickers for two of the 5-year-old children still opted for one item.1
their participation. Children were then presented with three blocks of trials. Each
block contained one trial of each of four trial types. The trial
types resulted out of the factorial combination of the factor
Materials and Procedures
Recipient (friend, disliked peer) and the factor Choice Type
Children were tested individually in a quiet room in one of three (prosocial, sharing). In each trial, the experimenter put the
day care centers. Experimental sessions were scored online by the respective number of stickers on the table, and children had to
experimenter and videotaped for later reliability coding. The child choose between two options (establishing a mini-dictator game;
was seated at a child table opposite the experimenter. In the cf. Gummerum, Keller, Takezawa, & Mata, 2008). In the proso-
following, we first introduce the Other task, before explaining the cial trials, the protagonist (e.g., yellow bear) could choose to
Self task. take one sticker for himself only (1,0 alternative) or to take one
Other task. sticker for himself and one sticker for the other bear (e.g., blue
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Materials. For the Other task, children were presented with bear; 1,1 alternative). More specifically, the experimenter said,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

three toy bears. The bears were of different colors and were “Yellow bear can have one sticker for himself and no sticker for
wearing differently colored T-shirts. For every participant, one of his friend blue bear or one sticker for himself and one sticker
the toy bears represented the protagonist, one bear represented the for his friend blue bear. What do you think yellow bear would
protagonist’s friend, and one bear a disliked peer. The roles of the do?” In the sharing trials, the protagonist (e.g., yellow bear)
bears were counterbalanced across participants. Each of the bears could choose to take two stickers for himself (2,0 alternative) or
had an envelope, in which his items could be put. Children were to take one sticker for himself and one sticker for the other bear
told that each bear would keep his stickers. As items for the Self (e.g., blue bear; 1,1 alternative). More concretely, the experi-
and the Other task, stickers and balloons were used (counterbal- menter said, “Yellow bear can have two stickers for himself and
anced between tasks). no sticker for his friend blue bear or one sticker for himself and
Procedure. Initially all bears were placed on the table by the one sticker for his friend blue bear. What do you think yellow
experimenter. The experimenter then introduced the bears to the bear would do?” Trial order as well as the order of the choices
child. The color of the bears’ shirts served as their names (e.g., red offered in each question (i.e., equitable or inequitable) were
bear, blue bear). First, the protagonist was introduced by his name. counterbalanced among blocks and participants.
Furthermore, the participants were told that the bear lives in a city At the beginning of the study, after introducing the protagonists,
where he goes to kindergarten or school every day and where he we assessed all children for their knowledge about the relation-
meets other bears. Subsequently, the other bears were introduced ships between the bears in a first memory check.2 All children
and their relationship to the protagonist was highlighted by stress- were able to correctly label the bears and their relationships.
ing their common/not common activities (cf. Bigelow, 1977). Furthermore, at the end of the study, we assessed approximately
After the introductions, the child was asked to identify each of the one randomly chosen third of the 3- and 4-year-old children for
bears and their relation to the protagonist. Children were corrected their knowledge about the relationships between the bears in an
if they misremembered any of the bears, and the procedure was additional memory check. More concretely speaking, they were
repeated until the child remembered the relationship between the first asked to name the bears. Then, for every recipient, they were
bears. To assure that children’s performance was not affected by asked whether or not he is a friend of the protagonist bear. All six
memory problems, the relationships between protagonist and re- of the 3-year-old children who were asked remembered the rela-
cipient were repeatedly described in each trial. tionships correctly. Out of eight 4-year-old children who were
After the presentation of the three bears, the experimenter in- asked, one committed one mistake. This shows that children were
troduced the task. First, he took some of the items (i.e., stickers or able to correctly represent the relations between the agents in-
balloons) and presented them to the child. Then, he explained that volved in the tasks.
they were going to play a game. In this game, the protagonist was Self task.
told that sometimes he could choose items just for himself and Materials. Materials of the Self task included a number of toy
sometimes for both himself and another bear. Every bear was figures (Playmobile), which differed in hair color, skin color, and
supplied with an envelope, and children were informed that every clothing. Children were asked to think about a good friend (i.e.,
bear’s items would be put in his envelope. Furthermore, it was they were asked to think about someone they like to play with) and
emphasized that the protagonist really likes the items and tries to a disliked peer (i.e., someone they do not like to play with) and to
collect them. Then, children were told that in each trial they had to name these specific individuals. Then, they were asked to choose
indicate what they think that the protagonist was going to do. two toy figures that best represented these two persons. Every toy
After receiving the instructions, children performed a practice figure had an envelope, in which her items could be put. Children
trial. The experimenter put a single sticker and two stickers on the were told that the envelopes would go to the respective person
table. Participants were asked if the protagonist would choose the after the study had ended. During the entire experimental sessions,
single sticker or two stickers. If children choose one sticker, they
were told that having two stickers is more fun than having one
1
sticker and another practice trial was administered. In these prac- Excluding the children who insisted in opting for one sticker in the
Other task or the Self task did not change the pattern of results across all
tice trials, three of the 3-year-old children, three of the 4-year-old age groups.
children, and four of the 5-year-old children opted for one item. 2
Owing to an experimenter error, this part of the protocol was not
When these children were presented with a second practice trial, performed for two 5-year-old children.
DEVELOPMENT OF SHARING BEHAVIOR AND SHARING EXPECTATIONS 917

the experimenter referred to the two toy figures by using the


individuals’ names.
Procedure. The procedure for the Self task was modified after
Moore (2009) and was closely related to the procedure of the Other
task with the crucial difference that the children were themselves
the protagonists who were paired with either one of two toy figures
who represented their friend or their disliked peer. That is, they
were presented with the same blocks and trials as in the Other task
and could decide whether they wanted to share with their friend
and disliked peer, respectively.
In the practice trial, children could decide to take one or two
stickers. One 3-year-old child, three 4-year-old children, and four
5-year-old children opted for one item. When these children were
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

presented with a second practice trial, the 3-year-old child, two


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

4-year-old children, and no 5-year-old child opted still for one


item.
In the memory check, all children were able to correctly label
the figures and the relationships. At the end of the study, all
assessed children remembered the relationships.
Design. Taken together, children received both tasks (Self and
Other task). In each task, children were presented with three blocks
of four trials each. Thus, overall, children received 24 trials. The
four trial types resulted out of the factorial combination of the
factor Recipient (friend, disliked peer) and the factor Choice Type
(prosocial, sharing). Trial order as well as the order of the choices
offered in each question (i.e., equitable or inequitable) were coun-
terbalanced among blocks and participants. The order of task
presentation (Self- or Other-task first) was counterbalanced be-
tween participants. Furthermore, we decided to use different re-
source types for each task: stickers and balloons. The assignment
of resource types to task (i.e., Self or Other) was counterbalanced
between participants.

Data Analysis
Data were coded online by the experimenter. For each trial,
participants received a score of 0 if they chose the inequitable (1,0)
or (2,0) option and a score of 1 if they chose the equitable (1,1)
option. All children completed all trials. Scores were recorded as
proportional measures of equitable choices for each trial type. To
assess interrater reliability, 11 randomly chosen children were
recoded by a second person blind to the purpose of the study.
Pearson’s correlations were calculated for each trial type sepa-
rately. The analysis shows that the correlation coefficients ranged
from r ⫽ .96 to r ⫽ 1.

Results
Figure 1. Mean proportion of trials on which participants choose the
Figure 1 shows the descriptive statistics for each age group and equitable (1,1) option as a function of trial type (prosocial or sharing),
task. Data were analyzed using a 2 ⫻ 2 ⫻ 2 ⫻ 3 mixed-model recipient (i.e., friend or disliked peer), and task (Self, Other). A: Shows the
repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the within- results for the 3-year-old children. B: Shows the results for the 4-year-old
children. C: Shows the results for the 5- to 6-year-old children. Error bars
subjects factors Recipient (friend, disliked peer), Trial Type
indicate standard errors of the means.
(prosocial, sharing), and Task (Self sharing, Other sharing) as well
as the between-subjects factors Age Group (3 years, 4 years, 5
years). A preliminary analysis including the factors gender, order
of presentation, and items yielded no significant effects of these interaction between recipient and age, F(2, 51) ⫽ 5.120, p ⬍ .01,
factors so that they were dropped from the main analysis. ␩2 ⫽ .17. Furthermore, the analysis revealed a main effect of trial
The analysis yielded a main effect of recipient, F(1, 51) ⫽ type, F(1, 51) ⫽ 18.497, p ⬍ .001, ␩2 ⫽ .27 (all other ps ⬎ .06),
30.596, p ⬍ .001, ␩2 ⫽ .38, which was further qualified by an showing that children took the equitable option more often in the
918 PAULUS AND MOORE

prosocial trials (M ⫽ .79, SE ⫽ .03) than in the sharing trials (M ⫽ .25, respectively). For the 5-year-olds, children’s own generosity
.70, SE ⫽ .03). and their expectations of others’ generosity was significantly cor-
To explore the interaction effect, we averaged the data across related with each other (r ⫽ .68, p ⫽ .001), suggesting that the
the self and other task and conducted separate 2 ⫻ 2 ANOVAs more the 5-year-old children were themselves inclined to act
with the within-subjects factors Recipient (friend, disliked peer) generously the more they expected another agent to act generously.
and Trial Type (prosocial, sharing) for every age group. The
ANOVA for the 3-year-olds revealed only a significant effect of
Discussion
trial type, F(1, 17) ⫽ 9.812, p ⬍ .01, ␩2 ⫽ .37 (all other ps ⬎ .11),
showing that the 3-year-old children chose the equitable option This study aimed at investigating developmental changes in 3-,
more often in the prosocial (M ⫽ .84, SE ⫽ .04) than in the sharing 4-, and 5-year-old children’s sharing behavior and their expecta-
trials (M ⫽ .71, SE ⫽ .05). The ANOVA for the 4-year-old tions of others’ sharing: in the Self task, they could distribute
children revealed a main effect of Recipient, F(1, 16) ⫽ 8.774, p ⬍ valuable items between themselves and a friend or a disliked peer;
.01, ␩2 ⫽ 0.35, indicating that the 4-year-old children opted more in the Other task, they were asked to predict how another agent
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

often for the equitable option when the friend was involved (M ⫽ would distribute valuable items between himself and a friend or a
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

.91, SE ⫽ .03) than when the disliked peer was involved (M ⫽ .63, disliked peer. Additionally, whether sharing was costly for the
SE ⫽ .08). There was also a marginal effect of trial type, F(1, agents or not was manipulated. The results showed similar age-
16) ⫽ 4.171, p ⫽ .058, ␩2 ⫽ .21 (all other ps ⬎ .09), suggesting related changes for both tasks. Children of all age groups behaved
a trend that children chose the equitable option more often in the more prosocially and expected more prosocial behavior from an-
prosocial (M ⫽ .82, SE ⫽ .04) than in the sharing trials (M ⫽ .73, other protagonist when the choice bore no cost. Moreover, the 4-
SE ⫽ .05). The ANOVA for the 5-year-olds revealed a main effect and 5-year-old children, but not the 3-year-old children, differen-
of Recipient, F(1, 18) ⫽ 20.344, p ⬍ .001, ␩2 ⫽ .53, showing that tiated between a friend and a disliked peer as potential recipients
children chose the equitable option more often when the friend was in the sharing and the sharing expectation task, suggesting devel-
the recipient (M ⫽ .91, SE ⫽ .03) than when the disliked peer was opmental changes in preschool children’s sharing behavior and
the recipient (M ⫽ .46, SE ⫽ .09), and an effect of trial type, F(1, their expectations of others’ sharing.
18) ⫽ 4.904, p ⬍ .05, ␩2 ⫽ .21 (all other ps ⬎ .39), indicating that Furthermore, we found a clear relation between 5-year-old chil-
the 5-year-old children chose the equitable option more often in dren’s own sharing behavior and their sharing expectations. Al-
the prosocial trials (M ⫽ .72, SE ⫽ .05) than in the sharing trials though the 3- and 4-year-old children showed similar patterns in
(M ⫽ .66, SE ⫽ .05). In sum, the separate ANOVAs revealed the Self and the Other task on a group level, no corresponding
effects of Recipient for the 4- and 5-year-olds, but not the 3-year- results emerged on a personal level. This suggests that the relation
old children. Moreover, the analyses revealed effects of Trial Type between sharing behavior and sharing expectations is itself subject
for every age group. to developmental changes as it only emerged strongly at 5 years of
To further investigate direct interrelations between children’s age. In sum, our study suggests that early prosocial development
sharing behavior and their sharing expectations, we followed the undergoes an increasing differentiation and hierarchical integra-
group-level analyses (i.e., ANOVAs) up by conducting correla- tion in the course of the preschool years.
tional analyses between children’s performances in both tasks. As Overall, three findings of the present study are noteworthy.
trial type (i.e., prosocial trials, sharing trials) had a constant effect First, extending previous work on recipient-dependent sharing
across both tasks and across all age groups (i.e., was orthogonal (e.g., Fehr et al., 2008; Moore, 2009), the current study points to
with respect to the other variables), we averaged across the two developmental changes with 3-year-old children not differentiating
trial types to generate an overall sharing measure for each recipient between different recipients and with 4- and 5-year-old children
(friend, disliked peer) in each task (Self, Other). To build differ- showing a clear differentiation. Nevertheless, the 3-year-old chil-
ence scores for the Self and Other task, we subtracted the propor- dren decided to act prosocially in the majority of trials. The lack of
tion of equitable decisions with the disliked peer from the propor- recipient-dependency in 3-year-olds’ sharing suggests that they
tion of equitable decisions with the friend (indicating how much have a general tendency to behave prosocially toward others. This
better the friend is treated than the disliked peer). For the 3-year- might be based on their developing understanding of others’ pref-
old children, the difference scores for the Self and Other tasks were erences (cf. Fawcett & Markson, 2010), for example, knowledge
not significantly related (r ⫽ .27, p ⫽ .29). For the 4-year-old that stickers or balloons are desired items, and on a general
children, the relation approached significance (r ⫽ .42, p ⫽ .09). prosocial motivation (Warneken & Tomasello, 2009).
For the 5-year-old children, the difference scores were highly Second, only little is known about children’s developing expec-
correlated with each other (r ⫽ .66, p ⬍ .01), indicating that the tations of others’ sharing behavior. Our results indicate that chil-
more the 5-year-old children differentiated in their own sharing dren of all three age groups expected someone to share less when
behavior between friends and disliked peers the more they ex- sharing was costly. Moreover, our findings show that by 3 years
pected the same behavior from the others. children showed a general disposition to expect that someone will
To investigate age-related changes in the interrelation between share with others. This indicates that at this age children possess an
children’s own generosity and their expectations of others’ gener- undifferentiated expectation that humans behave prosocially to-
osity, we calculated for every participant an average score for ward each other. This finding relates to studies showing that
generosity (i.e., proportion of equitable options chosen) shown by children act prosocially from early on (Brownell et al., 2009;
the self and expected from the other (across trial type and recipi- Thompson et al., 1997; Warneken & Tomasello, 2009) and dis-
ent). For the 3- and 4-year-old children there was a positive, tribute resources equally between others (Paulus, Gillis, et al.,
though not significant relation (r ⫽ .20, p ⫽ .42, and r ⫽ .29, p ⫽ 2013; Shaw & Olson, 2012), and it is in line with recent infant
DEVELOPMENT OF SHARING BEHAVIOR AND SHARING EXPECTATIONS 919

findings that suggested that even 2-year-olds show a sensitivity for Then, at 5 years of age, sharing behavior and sharing expecta-
equal distributions in a looking-time task (e.g., Schmidt & Som- tions become integrated as evident in the strong correlation be-
merville, 2011). It extends theoretical proposals that children have tween children’s own sharing and their expectations of others’
from early on a prosocial motivation that guides their interactions sharing. This suggests that at 5 years of age an integrated schema
with others (e.g., Eisenberg & Fabes, 1998; Warneken & Toma- is acquired of how to behave in sharing situations with different
sello, 2009) by showing that children also have a corresponding recipients. This schema is then applied to self and other (Barresi &
expectation that others will behave prosocially. Moore, 1996), forming the basis of children’s own sharing behav-
Where does this expectation come from? It is possible that it is ior and their expectations of others’ sharing. This developmental
based on an early tendency to trust in the prosocial motivation of change might be driven by children’s growing cognitive abilities
others. If this were true, our results would extend recent findings (e.g., Perner, 1991) and their growing grip of norms (e.g., Rakoczy
on preschool children’s trust in others in epistemic situations (e.g., & Schmidt, 2012), which guide a person’s own behavior as well as
Koenig, Clement, & Harris, 2004; Sobel & Corriveau, 2010) to the a person’s expectations about others’ behavior.
realm of prosocial development. Although further research is In this respect it is interesting to discuss differences between our
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

needed to substantiate this speculation, it is possible that this findings and the findings of Olson and Spelke (2008), who em-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

expectation forms the basis of a prosocial trust in the goodwill of ployed a resource-distribution paradigm to examine whether chil-
other people–a tendency that could help children to seek help when dren distribute extraneous resources rather to a friend or to another
needed and to facilitate the establishment of relationships. person. They reported that 3.5-year-olds helped a protagonist to
Moreover, it is possible that 3-year-old children’s expectations differentially distribute resources to friends than to strangers, while
of others’ sharing is the result of their learning about the norms of in our study, 3-year-old children did not share differently with
the social world. More concretely, research has provided evidence friends and disliked peers. Note that in our study children could
that young children understand that social behaviors are guided by share their own resources (or not) between themselves and a
conventional norms and expect others to behave according to these recipient, and children were alternately paired with one of the
norms (for a review see Rakoczy & Schmidt, 2012). It is possible recipients. In contrast, in Olson and Spelke’s (2008) study children
that their expectation that others will share is based on such a had to distribute a predefined number of resources between the two
recipients—sometimes an uneven number, urging the children to
normative understanding of sharing behavior, which could be the
prefer one over the other. That is, Olson and Spelke (2008) showed
result of socialization processes in their day care centers. Even
that, when urged by situational constraints, children think that
though further research is necessary to examine this hypothesis
someone rather gives items to a friend than to a stranger. Yet, our
directly, findings that 4-year-old children tend to punish moral
study shows that in situations without situational constraints
transgressions against third parties (Kenward & Östh, 2012) lend
3-year-old children share with friends and disliked peers to the
some support to this interpretation.
same extent.
In contrast, the 4- and 5-year-old children expected someone to
The findings are also informative for research on the develop-
share more with a friend than with a disliked peer, indicating
ment of concepts of friendship and other relationships (e.g.,
specific expectations of how the relationship between an agent and
Gifford-Smith & Brownell, 2003; Laursen & Hartup, 2002). They
another person affects the probability of showing prosocial behav-
show that by 5 years of age children have clear expectations of the
ior. This shows that the undifferentiated expectation that people behaviors associated with friendship. This indicates that they have
generally share with others becomes differentiated in the course of acquired a model of how friends or disliked peers behave toward
the preschool period (see also Hay & Cook, 2007). each other and thus a differentiated understanding of the various
Third, the same developmental trend was found for children’s interpersonal relations of their social world. This knowledge en-
own sharing and their expectations of other people’s sharing ables them to successfully guide their actions in their social envi-
behavior, suggesting that both show a parallel developmental ronment and might be the basis for their ability to form more
progression on a group level. Moreover, at 5 years of age, but not enduring friendship relations in the school-age period (cf. New-
at 3 or 4 years, sharing behavior and sharing expectations were on comb & Bagwell, 1995).
a personal level closely related to each other. Notwithstanding the insights gained by the current results, the
How might we explain the similar developmental patterns found present study has also some limitations and leads to a number of
in the younger preschool children? Children’s own sharing behav- open research questions. One limitation concerns the use of animal
ior may change for a number of reasons. For example, preschool characters to represent the different social roles involved in the
children may develop more empathy for friends and less empathy study. Although several studies have successfully employed ani-
(or more antipathy) for their disliked peers, leading them to be mal characters or toy figures to display agents in developmental
more generous to their friends. Alternatively, they might adopt a studies (e.g., Fawcett & Markson, 2010; Olson & Spelke, 2008;
strategy of tit-for-tat, expecting that sharing with a friend pays off Paulus & Moore, 2011), it would be informative to investigate in
in the future (cf. Axelrod, 1984). At the same time, when their own greater detail whether the insights derived from these studies also
sharing behavior changes, children may observe similar changes in hold in situations in which children have to interact with real peers.
their peers’ behavior, noticing that they also start to treat friends Additionally, the current findings open avenues for further re-
and disliked others differently. This representation of a regularity search. For example, given that social-cognitive skills contribute to
of the social world (cf. Nelson & Gruendel, 1981), in turn, forms friendship quality (Dunn & Cutting, 1999) and given that friend-
the basis of their expectations about others’ future sharing. Ini- ship quality in sharing tasks accounts for aggressive behavior
tially, these two processes develop in parallel but appear not to be (McElwain & Volling, 2005) in preschoolers, future research
related to each other. could examine whether individual differences in children’s expec-
920 PAULUS AND MOORE

tations of others’ sharing relates to their friendship quality and Fawcett, C. A., & Markson, L. (2010). Children reason about shared
their aggressive behavior. preferences. Developmental Psychology, 46, 299 –309. doi:10.1037/
Taken together, the present study reveals developmental a0018539
changes in preschool children’s sharing behavior and their expec- Fehr, E., Bernhard, H., & Rockenbach, B. (2008). Egalitarianism in chil-
dren. Nature, 454, 1079 –1083. doi:10.1038/nature07155
tations of others’ sharing. It shows that 3-year-old children have a
Gifford-Smith, M. E., & Brownell, C. A. (2003). Childhood peer relation-
general expectation that someone will share with others (irrespec- ships: Social acceptance, friendships, and peer networks. Journal of
tive of the relationship between protagonist and recipient) and School Psychology, 41, 235–284. doi:10.1016/S0022-4405(03)00048-7
show a corresponding inclination to share with others. In contrast, Gredebäck, G., & Melinder, A. M. D. (2010). Infants’ understanding of
4- and 5-year-old children have acquired a specific understanding everyday social interactions: A dual process account. Cognition, 114,
that someone is more likely to share with a friend than with a 197–206. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.004
disliked other and also display this recipient-dependency in their Gummerum, M., Keller, M., Takezawa, M., & Mata, J. (2008). To give or
own sharing behavior. At 5 years of age, sharing expectations and not to give: Children’s and adolescents’ sharing and moral negotiations
sharing behavior were found to be closely related to each other. in economic decision situations. Child Development, 79, 562–576. doi:
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01143.x
This pattern of developmental changes in preschool children’s
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Hay, D. F., Caplan, M., Castle, J., & Stimson, C. A. (1991). Does sharing
prosocial behavior corresponds to the idea that development fol- become increasingly “rational” in the second year of life? Developmen-
lows the orthogenetic principle, that is, an increasing differentia- tal Psychology, 27, 987–993. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.27.6.987
tion and hierarchical integration (cf. Werner, 1957). Hay, D. F., Castle, J., Davies, L., Demetriou, H., & Stimson, C. (1999).
Prosocial action in very early childhood. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 40, 905–916. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00508
References Hay, D. F., & Cook, K. V. (2007). The transformation of prosocial
behavior from infancy to childhood. In C. A. Brownell & C. B. Kopp
Axelrod, R. (1984). The evolution of cooperation. New York, NY: Basic
(Eds.), Transitions in early socioemotional development: The toddler
Books.
years (pp. 100 –131). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Barresi, J., & Moore, C. (1996). Intentional relations and social under-
Hay, D. F., Payne, A., & Chadwick, A. (2004). Peer relations in childhood.
standing. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19, 107–122. doi:10.1017/
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 84 –108. doi:10.1046/
S0140525X00041790
j.0021-9630.2003.00308.x
Berndt, T. J. (1981). Effects of friendship on prosocial intentions and
Henderson, A. M. E., Wang, Y., Matz, L. E., & Woodward, A. L. (2012).
behavior. Child Development, 52, 636 – 643. doi:10.2307/1129184
Active experience shapes 10-month-old infants’ understanding of col-
Berndt, T. J. (2002). Friendship quality and social development. Current
laborative goals. Infancy. Advance online publication. doi:10.1111/j
Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 7–10. doi:10.1111/1467-8721
.1532-7078.2012.00126.x
.00157 Kärtner, J., Keller, H., & Chaudhary, N. (2010). Cognitive and social
Bigelow, B. J. (1977). Children’s friendship expectations: A cognitive- influences on early prosocial behavior in two sociocultural contexts.
developmental study. Child Development, 48, 246 –253. doi:10.2307/ Developmental Psychology, 46, 905–914. doi:10.1037/a0019718
1128905 Kenward, B., & Dahl, M. (2011). Preschoolers distribute scarce resources
Birch, L. L., & Billman, J. (1986). Preschool children’s food sharing with according to the moral valence of recipients’ previous actions. Devel-
friends and acquaintances. Child Development, 57, 387–395. doi: opmental Psychology, 47, 1054 –1064. doi:10.1037/a0023869
10.2307/1130594 Kenward, B., & Östh, T. (2012). Enactment of third-party punishment by
Brownell, C. A., Svetlova, M., & Nichols, S. (2009). To share or not to 4-year-olds. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 373. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012
share: When do toddlers respond to another’s needs? Infancy, 14, 117– .00373
130. doi:10.1080/15250000802569868 Koenig, M. A., Clement, F., & Harris, P. L. (2004). Trust in testimony:
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social Children’s use of true and false statements. Psychological Science, 15,
information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. 694 – 698. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00742.x
Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74 –101. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74 Laursen, B., & Hartup, W. W. (2002). The origins of reciprocity and social
Damon, W. (1977). The social world of the child. San Francisco, CA: exchange in friendships. New Directions for Child and Adolescent
Jossey-Bass. Development, 95, 27– 40. doi:10.1002/cd.35
Daum, M. M., Prinz, W., & Aschersleben, G. & (2011). Perception and Longo, M. R., & Berthental, B. I. (2006). Common coding of observation
production of object-related grasping movements in 6-month-old in- and execution of action. Infancy, 10, 43–59. doi:10.1207/
fants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 810 – 818. doi: s15327078in1001_3
10.1016/j.jecp.2010.10.003 McElwain, N. L., & Volling, B. L. (2005). Preschool children’s interac-
Decety, J., & Sommerville, J. A. (2003). Shared representations between tions with friends and older siblings: Relationship specificity and joint
self and others: A social cognitive neuroscience view. Trends in Cog- contributions to problem behavior. Journal of Family Psychology, 19,
nitive Sciences, 7, 527–533. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.004 486 – 496. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.19.4.486
Dunfield, K. A., & Kuhlmeier, V. A. (2010). Intention-mediated selective McGillicuddy-De Lisi, A. V., Watkins, C., & Vinchur, A. J. (1994). The
helping in infancy. Psychological Science, 21, 523–527. doi:10.1177/ effect of relationship on children’s distributive justice reasoning. Child
0956797610364119 Development, 65, 1694 –1700. doi:10.2307/1131288
Dunn, J., & Cutting, A. L. (1999). Understanding others, and individual Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). Like me: A foundation for social cognition.
differences in friendship interactions in young children. Social Devel- Developmental Science, 10, 126 –134. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007
opment, 8, 201–219. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00091 .00574.x
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1998). Prosocial development. In W. Moore, C. (2009). Fairness in children’s resource allocation depends on the
Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, recipient. Psychological Science, 20, 944 –948. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280
emotional, and personality development (pp. 701–778). Berlin, Ger- .2009.02378.x
many: Springer. Nelson, K., & Gruendel, J. (1981). Generalized event representations:
DEVELOPMENT OF SHARING BEHAVIOR AND SHARING EXPECTATIONS 921

Basic building blocks of cognitive development. In M. E. Lamb & A. L. Sobel, D. M., & Corriveau, K. H. (2010). Children monitor individuals’
Brown (Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology (pp. 131–158). expertise for word learning. Child Development, 81, 669 – 679. doi:
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01422.x
Newcomb, A. F., & Bagwell, C. L. (1995). Children’s friendship relations: Sommerville, J. A., & Woodward, A. L. (2005). Pulling out the intentional
A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 306 –347. doi: structure of human action: The relation between action production and
10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.306 processing in infancy. Cognition, 95, 1–30. doi:10.1016/j.cognition
Olson, K. R., & Spelke, E. S. (2008). Foundations of cooperation in young .2003.12.004
children. Cognition, 108, 222–231. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.003 Svetlova, M., Nichols, S., & Brownell, C. (2010). Toddlers’ prosocial
Paulus, M., Gillis, S., Li, J., & Moore, C. (2013). Preschool children behavior: From instrumental to empathic to altruistic helping. Child
involve a third party in a dyadic sharing situation based on fairness. Development, 81, 1814 –1827. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01512.x
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 78 – 85. doi:10.1016/j Thompson, C., Barresi, J., & Moore, C. (1997). The development of
.jecp.2012.12.014 future-oriented prudence and altruism in preschoolers. Cognitive Devel-
Paulus, M., Hunnius, S., Vissers, M., & Bekkering, H. (2011). Imitation in opment, 12, 199 –212. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(97)90013-7
infancy: Rational or motor resonance. Child Development, 82, 1047– Vaish, A., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Sympathy through
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

1057. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01610.x affective perspective-taking and its relation to prosocial behavior in


Paulus, M., Kühn-Popp, N., Licata, M., Sodian, B., & Meinhardt, J. (2013).
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 45, 534 –543. doi:10.1037/


Neural correlates of prosocial behavior in infancy: Different neurophys-
a0014322
iological mechanisms support the emergence of helping and comforting.
Warneken, F., Lohse, K., Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Young
NeuroImage, 66, 522–530. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.041
children share the spoils after collaboration. Psychological Science, 22,
Paulus, M., & Moore, C. (2011). Whom to ask for help? Children’s
267–273. doi:10.1177/0956797610395392
developing understanding of other people’s action capabilities. Experi-
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Helping and cooperation at 14
mental Brain Research, 211, 593– 600. doi:10.1007/s00221-011-2676-1
months of age. Infancy, 11, 271–294. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007
Paulus, M., & Moore, C. (2012). Producing and understanding prosocial
actions in early childhood. Advances in Child Development and Behav- .tb00227.x
ior, 42, 271–305. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-394388-0.00008-3 Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2009). The roots of human altruism.
Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, British Journal of Psychology, 100, 455– 471. doi:10.1348/
MA: MIT Press. 000712608X379061
Rakoczy, H., & Schmidt, M. F. H. (2012). The early ontogeny of social Werner, H. (1957). The concept of development from a comparative and
norms. Child Development Perspectives. Advance online publication. organismic point of view. In D. Harris (Ed.), The concept of development
doi:10.1111/cdep.12010 (pp. 125–148). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
Rheingold, H., Hay, D., & West, M. (1976). Sharing in the second year of Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., & Chapman, M. (1992).
life. Child Development, 47, 1148 –1158. doi:10.2307/1128454 Development of concern for others. Developmental Psychology, 28,
Schmidt, M. F. H., & Sommerville, J. A. (2011). Fairness expectations and 126 –136. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.28.1.126
altruistic sharing in 15-month-old human infants. PLoS ONE, 6, e23223.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023223
Shaw, A., & Olson, R. (2012). Children discard a resource to avoid Received August 8, 2012
inequity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 382–395. Revision received April 15, 2013
doi:10.1037/a0025907 Accepted May 6, 2013 䡲

You might also like