Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2003, 4 , 105 - 114 NUMBER 1 (SUMMER 2003) 105

Choice between individual and shared social


contingencies in children: An experimental
replication in a natural setting

Emilio Ribes-Iñesta, Nora Rangel, Guadalupe Carbajal and Erandeni Peña


University of Guadalajara

A study evaluated in a natural setting the preference for individual versus social contingencies under
different social exchange conditions. Twelve children were distributed in six dyads. Children had to solve a
puzzle placed on the same table for each child in a dyad. In the experimental conditions each subject in every
dyad could also place pieces in the peer’s puzzle. Dyads were exposed to two sequences of experimental
conditions comprising competition, partial altruism, and total altruism. In all these conditions subjects could
choose to solve their puzzle individually instead of working additionally on the peer’s puzzle. Results show
that most subjects chose to solve the puzzle individually, even when they obtained less earnings.
Keywords: children, dyads, social interactions, competition, altruism

Competition, cooperation and altruism are (children and young adults) seem to prefer indi-
social phenomena that have been extensively stud- vidual contingencies (Ribes & Rangel, 2002).
ied using operant conditioning procedures (Azrin Ribes and Rangel (2002) used an experimen-
& Lindsley, 1956; Cohen & Lindsley, 1964; Hake tal situation with two synchronized computers,
& Vukelich, 1972; Lindsley, 1966; Marwell & one for each experimental subject. On each
Schmitt, 1975; Skinner, 1962; Weiner, 1977). Most computer’s screen, two identical puzzles were
experimental situations have consisted of rein- presented, one corresponding to each subject.
forcing the temporal coordination or sequencing Subjects could solve only their own puzzle or
of repetitive, arbitrary responses such as press- they could place pieces into the peer’s puzzle.
ing a button or a key or putting a stylus into a Under competition, unavoidable cooperation, and
hole. The literature shows that responding first partial altruism contingencies, subjects could earn
(competition), responding simultaneously or al- more points by placing pieces in the peer’s puzzle
most simultaneously with the peer (cooperation), than by responding individually in their own
and “give” responses (altruism) can be increased puzzle. Under the total altruism contingency they
in frequency through reinforcement procedures obtained the same amount of points by respond-
using points and actual earnings and consumables. ing either way. The results of this study showed
However, when a choice procedure that allows that children and young adults systematically chose
to respond on an individual basis or under a to solve their own puzzle instead of the peer’s
shared contingency is used, experimental subjects puzzle, in spite of the fact that they obtained less
earnings by doing so. These results were observed
when subjects worked in separate rooms and
Correspondence should be addressed to the first author: when they worked in the same room, being al-
Centro de Estudios e Investigaciones en Comportamiento, 12
de diciembre 204 (Col. Chapalita), 45040 Zapopan, México. lowed to continuously audit or track each other’s
Email: ribes@cencar.udg.mx performance and earnings (Schmitt, 2000).
This research was supported by grant #32375-H from the
National Council of Science and Technology of Mexico.
105
106 Emilio Ribes-Iñesta, Nora Rangel, Guadalupe Carbajal and Erandeni Peña

These initial findings suggest that when sub- received edibles they like (chosen from a menu
jects can choose between individual and shared available before each session) for their participa-
contingencies, they prefer individual contingen- tion. Each dyad comprised one girl and one boy
cies even when their earnings are lower than those chosen at random, and the dyads were randomly
that could be obtained under a shared contin- allocated to two groups.
gency. Thus, reinforcing the behavior of two in-
dividuals to cooperate, compete or be altruistic Material
might be insufficient to promote social behav- Twenty 50-pieces puzzles were used, as well
ior; social interaction is not necessarily an auto- as a Sony Handicam videocamera (model CCD-
matic result of contingencies that provide higher TR413), compact VHS videotapes, recording
earnings than those of independent individual sheets, colored labels, printed instructions and
performance. To evaluate the independence of copies of the puzzles drawings.
individual and shared contingencies it is neces-
sary to separately identify individual and social Experimental situation
responses as well as individual and social earn- Experimental sessions took place twice a day
ings; but traditional operant procedures do not in four successive days, in one room (6 x 3 m),
discriminate between individual and social re- relatively isolated from noise and other distractors.
sponses and consequences. Hence, the findings In the center of the room was a rectangular table
reported in the literature (Schmitt, 1998) might with two chairs located at each end. The video
well be an artifact of the type of responses be- camera was placed 1.5 m from the table facing
ing used and the functional restrictions imposed both chairs. At each end of the table a heap was
by a non-choice situation that prevents individu- located with all the pieces of a same puzzle, along
ally-based contingency responding (Ribes, 2001). with a copy of the puzzle’s drawing, colored la-
However, one could argue, along the same bels to identify the pieces of each child, and a
lines, that the results from the study by Ribes and sheet with instructions for each particular session.
Rangel (2002) are an outcome of the environ- During the first baseline session, each child solved
ment provided by the computer-based interac- his/her puzzle in a separate room.
tion, even in the conditions where subjects could
interact directly which each other. To test the gen- Procedure
erality of our previous finding, we designed an Except in Phase A (see below), children in
experiment with children that replicated the ba- each dyad sat facing each other at each end of
sic features of the study by Ribes and Rangel the table, a situation which allowed visual, physi-
(2002). The experiment involved a similar task cal and verbal contact. A different puzzle was
(puzzle solving) in a natural setting, and evaluated used in each session in order to prevent a learn-
choice between individual and shared contingen- ing and/or boredom effect, but in any session
cies (competition, partial altruism, and total al- the same puzzle was used for both children.
truism). If the children again chose the individual Two different sequences of exchange contin-
contingency over the shared contingency, this gencies were analyzed. The experiment com-
would support our assumption that social con- prised five phases, one conducted as an individual
tingencies per se are insufficient to produce social condition and the rest as social interaction condi-
interactions when alternative individual contingen- tions (Table 1). Phases A and B served as baseline
cies are available. or control conditions to assess the subjects’
puzzle-solving performance under individual,
Method non-shared contingencies.
In Phase A, each subject solved the puzzle in
Subjects a separate room and without receiving points.
Twelve 10 to 12 year old children enrolled in Phase B was identical to Phase A, with the ex-
a primary school volunteered to participate in this ception that both children had to solve their
experiment. The children, six girls and six boys, puzzle at the same time in the same room. They
Shared Social Contingencies 107

were told that they could either leave the room Phase A. “You must try to solve a puzzle. Try
when they completed the puzzle or wait until the to place all the pieces. You have to watch care-
peer to concluded his/her task. fully the drawing to your right, it is a picture of
The next three phases (C, D, E) involved in- the puzzle you have to solve. Also, you must stick
dividual and shared contingencies in which both a label at the back of each piece you place. Good
children could place pieces in any of the puzzles. luck!”
In Phase C, any correct response provided points Phase B. “Two puzzles are placed on the table:
for the child placing the piece correctly in any of yours, in front of you, and your peer’s puzzle, in
the two puzzles. In Phase D, when a child placed front of him/her. You should try to solve only
a piece correctly in his/her own puzzle, points your puzzle. You have to place all the pieces. As
were awarded only to this child. But when a piece in the previous game, at your right you have the
was correctly placed in the peer’s puzzle points drawing of the puzzle you have to solve. You
were awarded to both children. In Phase E, when must stick a label at the back of each piece you
a child placed a piece correctly in his/her own place. When you complete your puzzle, you may
puzzle, points were awarded only to this child, end the game for you and your peer, by calling
but when a piece was correctly placed in the peer’s me and leaving the room. You may also wait for
puzzle points were awarded to the peer. Thus, your peer to end his/her own puzzle. Good
Phase C can be seen as involving choices between luck!”
individual behavior and competition, Phase D Phase C. “Two puzzles are placed on the table:
between individual behavior and partial altruism, yours, in front of you, and your peer’s puzzle, in
and Phase E between individual behavior and front of him/her. You and your peer may place
total altruism. In these three experimental phases, pieces in ANY of the two puzzles, yours and
the child who first completed his/her puzzle could his/hers. As in previous games, at your right you
end the session by leaving the room or calling the have the drawing of the puzzles that you have to
experimenter. Children were explicitly instructed solve. You must stick a label at the back of each
at the beginning of every session about the vari- piece you place. I need this to identify which pieces
ous contingencies and about how many points you placed in each puzzle and to compute how
they could obtain by placing pieces in their puzzle many points you earned. For any piece you place
or in the peer’s puzzle. When the session ended correctly in any puzzle, you’ll obtain 10 points.
the experimenter counted how many pieces each The same will happen to your peer. When you
child had correctly placed in each of the two complete one of the two puzzles, you may end
puzzles, and exchanged the points earned by each the game for you and your peer by calling me
child for edibles. Different kinds and amounts and leaving the room. You may also wait for
of edibles were provided proportionally to the your peer to complete his/her puzzle, or you may
number of points earned (the exchange values place pieces in his/her puzzle. The highest num-
of different candies were set up at 10, 15, 20, 30, ber of points that you may obtain in this session
40 and 50 points). Children were informed about is 1000: 500 if you place pieces only in your
the exchange ratios before the session began. puzzle, plus 500 if you place all the pieces in your
Sessions ended when both children completed peer’s puzzle. When the game ends you can ex-
their puzzle or when the child who first com- change your points for candies. You will get a
pleted his/her puzzle ended the session. Dyads 1 better prize with more points.”
to 3 were exposed to the phases A,B,C,D,E, in Phase D. Instructions were similar to those in
this order. Dyads 4 to 6 were exposed to the Phase C, but changes were introduced regarding
inverse sequence of experimental contingencies: how to earn points: “For any piece that you place
A,B,E,D,C. Since two sessions were run every correctly in your puzzle, you and only you will
day, a five-minute rest period was scheduled be- obtain 10 points. For any piece you place cor-
tween sessions. rectly in your peer’s puzzle, you and your peer
The following instructions were given in the will obtain 10 points. Your peer will be able to
different baseline and experimental phases: do the same. The highest number of points you
108 Emilio Ribes-Iñesta, Nora Rangel, Guadalupe Carbajal and Erandeni Peña

can obtain is 1000, if you place pieces in your and transcription of the data was done by two
peer’s puzzle and if he/she places pieces in your trained observers. Reliability of observational data
puzzle.” was calculated by dividing the number of agree-
Phase E. Instructions were similar to those in ments by the total number of observations and
Phase D, but changes were introduced regarding multiplying by 100. The obtained reliability coef-
how to earn points: “For any piece that you place ficient was 87.5%.
correctly in your puzzle, you will obtain 10 points.
For any piece that you place correctly in your peer’s Results
puzzle, your peer will obtain 10 points. The same
holds for your peer. The highest number of Figure 1 shows the performance of the six
points you can obtain in this session is 500". dyads. The panels labelled “local” indicate the
All sessions were videotaped in order to number of correct placements in one’s own
record direct physical and verbal interactions be- puzzle. The panels labelled “remote” indicate the
tween the children. Behavioral categories were number of correct placements made in the peer’s
formulated from the inspection of the videos, puzzle. The children showed few correct place-

Figure 1. Number of correct placements for each child in his/her own puzzle (local) or in the peer’s puzzle (remote). The
asterisks indicate that the child ended the session before his/her peer complement the corresponding puzzle. The subjects in
each dyad are denoted by a letter (a,b) after the dyad number (1...).
Shared Social Contingencies 109

ments in the peer´s puzzle, except for two dyads (usually a boy, except in Dyad 5) always ended
(1 and 6). In Dyad 1 both children showed more the session.
placements in the remote puzzle than in their own Figure 2 shows the number of pieces each
puzzle during the competition contingency. In child placed in each puzzle during or after solv-
Dyad 6, the highest number of placements was ing his/her own puzzle. The children in Dyad 1
done only by one child (S6a), also under condi- placed pieces in the peer´s puzzle both during
tions of competition. During baseline, 9 of the and after completing their own puzzle. In con-
12 children completed their puzzles; those chil- trast, one child in Dyad 6 placed almost all the
dren who did not complete it failed to do so in pieces after completing her puzzle.
almost all sessions. Starting from the second Figure 3 shows how many points each dyad
baseline conditions, children in Dyads 4, 5 and 6 obtained. The dotted line indicates the number
ended the session before the other peer com- of points that could be obtained if the children
pleted the puzzle. In each dyad, the same child responded optimally under shared contingencies.
During the total-altruism condition, five of the

Figure 2. Number of correct placements in the peer’s puzzle during and after the completion of one’s own puzzle (local).
110 Emilio Ribes-Iñesta, Nora Rangel, Guadalupe Carbajal and Erandeni Peña

six dyads obtained as many points as possible. In Discussion


the other phases, under competition and partial
altruism conditions, children always obtained less The results of this experiment confirm pre-
points than what was possible. Most of the time vious findings by Ribes and Rangel (2002) that
children worked on their own puzzle. when offered choices between individual and
Figure 4 shows the average session duration shared contingencies, children and young adults
(in minutes) across successive sessions. Excepting often prefer individual, non-social contingencies,
one dyad (2), whose session time tended to de- even when the payoff for doing so is lower than
crease, the session duration varied unsystematically for responding under shared contingencies. The
within a range of 15 to 45 minutes. replication of this effect suggests that when indi-
Figure 5 shows the total number of direct vidual responding (in the local puzzle) and social
physical and verbal interactions for each group responding (in the remote puzzle) are indepen-
of children in Phases 2 to 5. Explaining the task dent, subjects tend to respond on an individual
(ET) and other non-relevant (O) behaviors were basis even when they obtain lesser benefits or
the most frequent categories. Other categories that earnings. This effect was described as interper-
also occurred but at a lower frequency were sonal risk-avoidance by Marwell and Schmitt
complaining (C), helping (H), talking softly (TS) (1975), who attributed it to the anticipation of
and in occasions yelling (Y), verbal aggression negative effects from taking earnings from the
(VA), asking for help (AH), intrusion (I), refer- peer. In our experiments, however, individual and
ring to rules (RR) and defending the game (DG). social responses were clearly distinguished both
Physical aggression (PA), threatening (T) and ac- in location and by contingency prescriptions. In
cusing (A) practically did not occur. contrast to customary experimental preparations
using buttons, knobs and similar operanda

Figure 3. Number of points obtained by each child. Dotted lines indicate the maximum number of points that could be
obtained individually under each contingency.
Shared Social Contingencies 111

(Schmitt, 1998), we used a meaningful task, a In our experiments, preferring individual con-
puzzle that changed every session and that allowed tingencies while shared contingencies are avail-
for the “intrusion” of each subject into the other’s able has been shown to occur under unavoid-
task to compete, cooperate or be altruistic able cooperation, partial altruism, total altruism,
(among other possibilities). Perhaps the typical and competition, both with children and young
findings on competition, cooperation and altru- adults. Furthermore, the exposure of experimen-
ism with operant preparations result from the tal subjects to sequences of conditions that might
simplicity and repetitiveness of the response be- have facilitated responding in the remote puzzle
ing used, and its confounding with coordination did not produce any increase in shared respond-
and sequencing effects that differ from actual ing. In the study by Ribes and Rangel (2002), sub-
social interactions (as Marwell and Schmitt ,1975, jects first exposed to an unavoidable coopera-
themselves documented in relation to Cohen’s tive condition responded most of the time in
[1962] and Lindsley’s [1966] experimental settings). their own puzzle in later conditions that included
altruistic and competitive contingencies. In the
present experiment, children did not change their
preference for the individual contingencies after
being exposed to a partial-altruism condition in
which, by switching to the remote puzzle, they
could obtain twice as many points as by placing
the same number of pieces in their own puzzle.
Actually, when responding in the peer’s puzzle
was observed, it occurred under a competition
contingency prior to the exposure to partial altru-
ism (Dyad 1 in Phase 3).
This experiment was designed to evaluate if
the computer-based situation used by Ribes and
Rangel (2002) had interfered with the occurrence
of the social interactions that usually take place in
a natural setting (in which individuals deal with
actual instead of virtual objects). The results of
this experiment suggest the opposite. Children
did not prefer to work on the peer’s puzzle any
more than in the previous experiment in which
they solved puzzles on the screens of two syn-
chronized computers. The consistency of this
effect is confirmed by the fact that in the experi-
ment by Ribes and Rangel (2002), no differences
were observed as to whether the children solved
their puzzles in the same room or in separate
rooms.
In a related, natural-setting study by Camacho
(2000), each child of a dyad was given 50% of
the pieces of his/her own puzzle and 50% of
the pieces of the peer puzzle, so that the children
had to ask or take from their the pieces needed
to complete their own puzzle. In the coopera-
tion condition, both children had to complete
their puzzles to obtain a reward. In the competi-
Figure 4. Average session length (in minutes). tion condition, the first child completing his or
112 Emilio Ribes-Iñesta, Nora Rangel, Guadalupe Carbajal and Erandeni Peña

her own puzzle obtained the reward. In this study, tions may occur only when subjects have no other
children showed physical violence and snatching choice than to interact for shared consequences.
of pieces during competition, and donation of Summing up, the results of this study sup-
pieces during cooperation, especially when the port previous findings showing that when chil-
children were not exposed to both conditions. dren have a choice between individual and shared
In our experiment, physical aggression and other contingencies, they prefer individual contingen-
coercive behaviors practically did not occur. Two cies. This choice also reduces direct physical and
factors may account for these differences. First, verbal contacts between the children, especially
earnings in our experiment were not shared and coercive and reciprocity interactions. These find-
children could choose to solve their puzzle on an ings may be the systematic outcome of two fea-
individual basis. Second, children were not forced tures of our experimental preparation: a) it in-
or required to exchange pieces with their peers in volves actual choices between individual and
order to solve their puzzle. On the contrary, they shared contingencies, and b) it does not prescribe
were allowed and instructed to intrude their peer's shared responses or shared consequences, since
puzzle without having to exchange pieces. These even in the shared contingency situation responses
results suggest that the occurrence of coercive and earnings are individually specified. Further
and reciprocity behaviors during social interac- research should explore the social interactions
promoted by the choice between individual and

Figure 5. Number of accumulated verbal and physical responses during each baseline and experimental condition for both
groups of dyads.
Shared Social Contingencies 113

shared contingencies when responses and/or 470-501). New York: Appleton Century
consequences are prescribed as individual or Crofts.
shared requirements. Marwell, G., & Schmitt, D.R. (1975). Cooperation:
An experimental analysis. New York: Academic
References Press.
Ribes, E. (2001). Functional dimensions of so-
Azrin, N.H., & Lindsley, O.R. (1956). The rein- cial behavior: Theoretical considerations and
forcement of cooperation between children. some preliminary data. Mexican Journal of Be-
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 100- havior Analysis, 27, 285-306.
102. Ribes, E., & Rangel, N. (2002). A comparison of
Camacho, E. (2000). Análisis experimental del choice between individual and shared social
intercambio social de diadas de niños en contingencies in children and young adults. Eu-
condiciones de cooperación y competencia. ropean Journal of Behavior Analysis, 3, 61-73.
Master in Science Thesis. University of Schmitt, D.R. (1998). Social behavior. In K.A.
Guadalajara. Lattal & M. Perone (Eds.), Handbook of re-
Cohen, D.J. (1962). Justin and his peers: An ex- search methods in human operant behavior (pp. 471-
perimental analysis of a child’s social world. 505). New York: Plenum Press.
Child Development, 33, 697-717. Schmitt, D.R. (2000). Effects of competitive re-
Cohen, D.J., & Lindsley, O.R. (1964). Catalysis ward distribution on auditing and competi-
of controlled leadership in cooperation by tive responding. Journal of the Experimental
human simulation. Journal of Child Psychological Analysis of Behavior, 74, 115-125.
Psychiatry, 5, 119-137. Skinner, B.F. (1962). Two “synthetic social rela-
Hake, D.F., & Vukelich, R. (1972). A classifica- tions”. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
tion and review of cooperation procedures. Behavior, 5, 531-533.
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, Weiner, H. (1977). An operant analysis of human
18, 333-343. altruistic responding. Journal of the Experimen-
Lindsley, O.R. (1966). Experimental analysis of tal Analysis of Behavior, 27, 515-528.
cooperation and competition. In T. Verhave
(Ed.), The Experimental Analysis of Behavior (pp.
114 Emilio Ribes-Iñesta, Nora Rangel, Guadalupe Carbajal and Erandeni Peña

You might also like