Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

Dissociation of duration-based and beat-based

auditory timing in cerebellar degeneration


Manon Grubea,1, Freya E. Coopera, Patrick F. Chinneryb,c, and Timothy D. Griffithsa,b
a
Newcastle Auditory Group, Medical School, Newcastle University, Newcastle-upon-Tyne NE2 4HH, United Kingdom; bDepartment of Neurology,
Medical School, Newcastle University, Newcastle-upon-Tyne NE2 4HH, United Kingdom; and cInstitute of Human Genetics, Newcastle University,
Newcastle-upon-Tyne NE1 3BZ, United Kingdom

Edited by William Thomas Thach, Washington University, St. Louis, MO, and accepted by the Editorial Board May 17, 2010 (received for review September
24, 2009)

This work tests the hypothesis that the cerebellum is critical to the Results
perception of the timing of sensory events. Auditory tasks were In all five tasks (Fig. 1), performance was measured by adaptive
used to assess two types of timing in a group of patients with a tracking of thresholds (Fig. S1). Group differences (Fig. 2A)
stereotyped specific degeneration of the cerebellum: the analysis of and occurrence and severity of individual impairments were
single time intervals requiring absolute measurements of time, and evaluated (Figs. 2B and S2), and correlation with motor im-
the holistic analysis of rhythmic patterns based on relative measures pairment (Table S1) was assessed.
of time using an underlying regular beat. The data support a specific Both single-interval timing tasks (Var, Fix; Fig. 1 A and B)
role for the cerebellum only in the absolute timing of single demonstrated significant deficits in the patients compared with
subsecond intervals but not in the relative timing of rhythmic the control group. The larger difference in thresholds was found
sequences with a regular beat. The findings support the existence for the reference interval having a variable duration (significant at
of a stopwatch-like cerebellar timing mechanism for absolute the level of P < 0.001, t test on log-transformed data; effect size

NEUROSCIENCE
intervals that is distinct from mechanisms for entrainment with 0.87; Fig. 2A). The difference for a reference of fixed duration
a regular beat. was also significant, with a smaller effect size (significant at the
level of P < 0.05, t test on log-transformed data; effect size 0.40;
human | perception | absolute | relative | subsecond Fig. 2A). In accordance with the group-level analysis, single-
subject inference based on z scores demonstrated a frequency of
T he relevance of the human cerebellum to the perception of time
intervals and rhythmic sequences is controversial. Involvement
occurrence of significant impairments with P < 0.05 in 44% of
the patients for the timing of single intervals with variable ref-
of the cerebellum in perceptual timing (the perception of the timing erence duration and 29% for fixed-reference duration (Fig. 2B).
of sensory events), in addition to its role in motor timing (the timed The relative timing of beat-based rhythmic patterns was sys-
execution of movements), has been suggested by a number of tematically assessed at three levels of perceptual analysis: the
studies (1–6). One distinction that we wish to address here, which detection of a regular beat (Fig. 1C, Reg), the detection of a de-
has not been made clear in previous work, is between the absolute, viation from an isochronous beat (Fig. 1D, Iso), and the detection
duration-based timing of single subsecond intervals and the relative of a distortion in a metrical beat pattern (Fig. 1E, Met). No group
timing of subsecond intervals based on a regular beat. Functional level deficit in performance was found for any of the three tasks
imaging studies suggest neural activity in the human cerebellum (not significant at the level of P < 0.05; Reg, Mann–Whitney
during the perception of the absolute duration of single time U test; Iso, Welch’s t test; Met, Mann–Whitney U test; Fig. 2A).
intervals (7, 8) as well as rhythmic patterns with a regular beat (9– The frequency of occurrence of individual deficits was low overall
13). However, previous lesion work to assess an obligatory cere- and comparable to that in controls: Significant impairments for
bellar role in the perception of single time intervals has not yielded the detection of a roughly regular beat, the deviation from an
consistent results (4, 14–17). Previous lesion work to assess any isochronous beat, and the distortion in a metrical beat were found
obligatory role of the cerebellum in the analysis of rhythmic in 9%, 6% and 12%, respectively (Fig. 2B).
sequences has assessed only deficits in related motor activity, such No correlations were found between performance in perceptual
as tapping out a beat (4, 14, 18), that do not allow clear inference timing tasks and the duration or severity of motor symptoms (Fig.
about perception. S2 and Table S1). Impairments tended not to occur in pre-
In this study, we test whether the cerebellum is a critical sub- symptomatic individuals, and were substantial in some severely
strate for perceptual tasks that require the absolute, duration- affected individuals, but there was no consistent relationship at
based analysis of single time intervals as well as those that require the group level.
the relative analysis of time intervals within rhythmic patterns
Discussion
based on a regular beat. Perceptual tests were conducted in the
auditory domain, where accurate temporal encoding of sensory This work tested the role of the human cerebellum as a critical
events is essential and entrainment with a beat is induced naturally. substrate for the perceptual timing of single intervals based on
Tasks were administered to a group of 34 patients with a stereo- their absolute duration and that of rhythmic sequences with
typed cerebellar degeneration and a matched control group of 40
healthy individuals. Two absolute timing tasks tested the percep-
Author contributions: M.G. and T.D.G. designed research; M.G., F.E.C., and P.F.C. per-
tion of single intervals for a variable and a fixed reference duration, formed research; M.G. and F.E.C. analyzed data; and M.G., F.E.C., P.F.C., and T.D.G. wrote
respectively (Fig. 1 A and B). Three relative timing tasks tested the the paper.
beat-based analysis of rhythmic sequences, including the detection The authors declare no conflict of interest.
of the presence of a roughly regular beat (19), a deviation from an This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. W.T.T. is a guest editor invited by the Editorial
isochronous beat (20) and a distortion of a rhythmic pattern with Board.
a metrical beat (21) (Fig. 1 C–E). The data support a cerebellar Freely available online through the PNAS open access option.
role in the absolute timing of single intervals, but not the relative 1
To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: manon.grube@ncl.ac.uk.
timing of beat-based sequences, and distinct brain mechanisms for This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.
these two types of perceptual timing. 1073/pnas.0910473107/-/DCSupplemental.

www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.0910473107 PNAS Early Edition | 1 of 5


Subjects were recruited on the basis of their diagnosis with SCA-6
and included irrespective of the severity of motor symptoms.
Substantial group effects were shown despite the inclusion of
asymptomatic subjects. The absence of correlations with motor
symptoms may be due to insufficient power or dissociation of
neural substrates for motor tasks relevant to ataxia and perception
within the cerebellum. The group-level and individual deficits for
the fixed-interval task, where the reference interval is presented
repeatedly throughout the test, compare well with the moderate
effects reported in previous studies (4, 14, 16). The larger effect
size and proportion of individual deficits in more than 40% of the
patients in the variable-interval task, in which the duration of
the reference interval changes from trial to trial, support the
specific involvement of the cerebellum in the perceptual analysis of
unrepeated intervals. In this task, the duration of each interval
must be timed anew with no possibility of creating an internal long-
Fig. 1. Stimuli for the five timing tasks depicting one example of one target term reference: The task is a more exacting test of the perception
stimulus (black) and one reference stimulus (gray) per task. Horizontal lines
of absolute time. Cerebellar integrity seems crucial to the normal
depict tones (200 Hz; 100 ms); ticks represent units corresponding to an
underlying beat; dotted lines mark interval changes in target stimuli.
functioning of this stopwatch-like timing mechanism (25). This
(A) (Var) Variable-single interval discrimination. (B) (Fix) Fixed-single interval notion is consistent with cerebellar activation in normal individuals
discrimination. (C) (Reg) Regularity detection. (D) (Iso) Isochrony-deviation being stronger for the perception of novel compared with familiar
detection. (E) (Met) Metrical pattern discrimination (roving of unit-duration sequences of intervals (11).
omitted for clarity; thick ticks marking the metrical beat of 4). One alternative explanation for the difference in performance
between patients and controls could be based on cognitive
demands and the recent implication of the cerebellum in cog-
a regular beat based on the relative durations of intervals. The nition (26–29). This appears unlikely, as the single-interval tim-
findings are based on reliable measures of individual perfor- ing tasks are less demanding than those of beat-based timing,
mance for both types of timing in a large and homogeneous and as the cognitive deficits demonstrated in patients with cer-
group of patients with a specific type of cerebellar degeneration ebellar damage were specific to aspects of executive function or
(SCA-6). A significant deficit in the patients compared with the attention (27–29) and would not lead to the present dissociation.
matched control group was demonstrated for the timing of single The cerebellar substrate for interval perception might have a
number of precise anatomical locations. Previous functional
intervals, but not for rhythmic sequences based on a regular beat.
imaging studies have not demonstrated clear differences between
The data suggest impairment in a mechanism for absolute timing
cerebellar activation patterns for absolute and relative percep-
and preservation of mechanisms of relative timing. tual timing (7, 8, 10, 11, 30). Repetitive transcranial magnetic
stimulation targeting the medial and right cerebellum can pro-
Deficit in Absolute Duration-Based Timing. The significant deficit in
duce similar acute deficits in absolute time-interval perception
the timing of single intervals in the patients compared with con-
(31), demonstrating consistent effects of both chronic and acute
trols is consistent with previous neuropsychological reports of cerebellar dysfunction. One previous neuropsychological report
deficits or trends to deficits in related tasks (16, 17, 22, 23). Some of a perceptual timing deficit implicated superior parts of the
inconsistency of the effect in previous studies may reflect the cerebellum, in particular, the right lateral hemisphere (14). The
heterogeneity of lesions in these populations of stroke patients (14, present study demonstrates a specific deficit in absolute time
15). The current study assessed 34 individuals with a genetically perception in a group with SCA-6 that experiences stereotyped
determined, isolated disorder of the cerebellum that advances in progression of the cerebellar degeneration from the superior to
a stereotyped way from the superior to the inferior part (24). the inferior parts, especially affecting the vermis (24). Our data
are consistent with previous suggestions of a particular role of
the superior cerebellum in absolute timing, and suggest a par-
ticular importance of the midline cerebellum.

Preservation of Relative, Beat-Based Timing Mechanisms. All three


tests of relative timing of rhythmic sequences based on a regular
beat were unaffected in the patients at the group level. The first
task, requiring the detection of regularity, was based on iso-
chronous sequences that become increasingly irregular (19) and
was unaffected with the exception of a few individuals. If the de-
tection of a regular beat depended on accurate single-interval
timing in a hierarchical fashion, one would expect to find a deficit
in the regularity task also. The lack of impairment suggests that this
task depends on a mechanism distinct from the stopwatch-type
device that we suggest for single intervals. The second task, the
detection of a deviation from an isochronous beat, was also un-
impaired at the group level. Thresholds were lower than those for
single intervals or within sequences with no regular beat, as
Fig. 2. Timing thresholds group data and frequency of occurrence of in-
dividual deficits for patients (n = 34; black) compared with controls (n = 40;
reported in previous studies (20, 32, 33), with the drop in thresh-
gray). (A) Mean timing thresholds and SEMs. Group differences were sig- olds being even larger in the patients than in the controls. The third
nificant for Var and Fix. (B) Proportion of significant impairment at the in- task, assessing the ability of subjects to use metrical sequences to
dividual level Note high values for Var and Fix, contrasted by low values for achieve greater precision in rhythmic timing tasks (21), was also
Reg, Iso, and Met. Significance level was P < 0.05. unimpaired at the group level.

2 of 5 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.0910473107 Grube et al.


All three tasks could be accomplished by one or more mech- present data further suggest that beat-based perceptual timing
anisms that are different from the timing of single intervals. can function independently of an impairment in this mechanism;
Unlike single-interval mechanisms being based on absolute du- they do not allow a direct inference on a common mechanism of
ration, those additional mechanisms would use the presence of entrainment for motor and perceptual timing.
a regular beat as an alternative reference frame for the timing of The occurrence of impairment may relate to views of the
intervals relative to the beat. Psychophysical data from previous cerebellum as a “universal” basic sensory acquisition controller
studies in normals support a dissociation between mechanisms for (1) and as a part of a network for processing magnitude (55), in
duration-based as opposed to beat-based timing of intervals and
this case time or duration. Both views are congruent with the
rhythmic sequences (20, 21, 32, 34–38). The preservation of rela-
present argument for a cerebellar role in the absolute, duration-
tive timing tasks is not consistent with the idea that the cerebellum
provides a single mechanism subserving both single-interval and based perceptual timing of single intervals but not to the relative,
beat-based timing; rather, the data suggest one or more mecha- beat-based timing within rhythmic sequences. The involvement
nisms of perceptual entrainment with a regular beat that are of the postulated cerebellar mechanism of absolute, subsecond
independent of single-interval timing and can occur despite cere- timing in other perceptual modalities is supported by previous
bellar damage. The preservation of beat-based perception here is reports of the effect of cerebellar TMS on duration-based so-
consistent with that of sensorimotor transduction elsewhere (39), matosensory timing (56) and cerebellar activations in visual
and with a perceptual mechanism of entrainment that depends on timing (7, 11).
a neural substrate that is unaffected in the present study. Studies of lesions of the other components, including the basal
We cannot rule out the possibility that focal lesions in parts of ganglia and prefrontal cortex, and in other modalities will allow
the cerebellum that were unaffected in the patients in the present further tests of the dissociation between duration-based and
study might impair beat-based rhythm perception. The present beat-based timing.
study demonstrates an obligatory role of the cerebellum in abso-
lute, subsecond timing but does not exclude any possible role in Materials and Methods
relative timing involving mechanisms that are relatively spared by

NEUROSCIENCE
Subjects. The patient group included 34 individuals (12 males) with geneti-
the degenerative process. We would point out, however, that the cally diagnosed spinocerebellar ataxia type 6 (SCA-6) (Fig. 3).The patients’
study includes subjects with advanced disease and widespread at- age range was 45–81 y (mean 64 ± 10 SD); estimated premorbid IQs (57) and
rophy who do not show individual deficits in beat-based rhythm verbal IQs (58) were in the normal range (premorbid, mean 101 ± 7 SD;
perception. The preservation of performance contrasts with verbal, mean 105 ±11 SD). Duration of symptoms was self-reported and
expectations based on cerebellar activation in fMRI studies of ranged from 0 to 20 y (mean 8.8 ± 5.8 SD), severity of motor impairment
normal controls, and suggests that such activity might not be an assessed on a locally developed scale from 1 to 5; patient details are listed in
obligatory aspect of relative time perception (9, 11, 13, 40). Table S2. SCA-6 is an autosomal-dominant disease caused by a loss-of-
function mutation in a voltage-gated calcium channel that specifically
Cerebellar Contribution to the Timing Network of the Brain. This affects the Purkinje cells of the cerebellum. Degeneration starts at the su-
work suggests a differential involvement of the cerebellum as perior–anterior end and progresses inferior–posteriorly in a stereotyped
a part of the perceptual timing network of the brain. A “cere- manner (24). The matched control group included 40 subjects (18 males) of
bellar clock” has previously been promoted as a device that both 45–81 y (mean 64 ± 9 y). There were no group differences in age, years of
measures absolute interval duration and entrains with a regular education, estimated premorbid IQ, or hearing sensitivity (Table 1).
beat of a sequence of intervals at the subsecond level (20, 41–43).
The present data support a role of the cerebellum in the former
but not the latter. We argue for a role for the cerebellum as
a stopwatch mechanism with specific and obligatory involvement
in absolute time-interval perception (25), in which the cerebellar
circuitry would provide the suitable neural machinery (44–46).
This is in accordance with the notion of “event timing” and the
multiple-timer model suggested by Ivry et al. (20, 42, 43).
The preserved beat-based perception suggests a distinct neural
substrate from the proposed cerebellar stopwatch mechanism for
single-interval timing. Possible substrates include a distinct cere-
bellar subregion. However, the preservation of beat-based per-
ception even in patients with extensive cerebellar damage would
argue against this. Alternatively, beat-based perception may de-
pend on distinct parts of the timing network beyond the cere-
bellum such as the basal ganglia or cerebral cortex (10, 30, 47, 48).
Anatomical connections between the cerebellum and the basal
ganglia and prefrontal cortex (via the thalamus) (46, 49–51) could
support efficient integrated network processing. Recent func-
tional data further suggest specialized loops or zones of cortico-
cerebellar connectivity, including one for sensori-motor functions
involving cerebellar lobules V–VII (52, 53).
This study addresses mechanisms of perceptual timing, and any
link between perceptual and motor timing (54) needs to be made
with caution. Previous patient studies suggested an obligatory
cerebellar role in the motor timing of individual movements in
isolation and in the context of a regular beat (4, 14, 18). Deficits in
perceptual timing were here only demonstrated for single inter- Fig. 3. Structural MRI scans of SCA-6–induced cerebellar degeneration in
vals in isolation and not within the context of a regular beat. four patients. (A) P27: male, 60 y, 3 y postonset, x 4. (B) P32: female, 62 y, 6 y
Overall the data could support a common cerebellar mechanism postonset, x +4. (C) P07: female, 69 y, 10 y postonset, x −7. (D) P13: male,
in the timing of individual motor or perceptual events. The 74 y, 20 y postonset, x −3. x coordinates according to MNI convention.

Grube et al. PNAS Early Edition | 3 of 5


Table 1. Control and patient group descriptive statistics
Age, y Education, y Estimated premorbid IQ Audiogram, dB hearing loss

Controls 64.1 ± 9.0 10.0 ± 3.0* 103.0 ± 9.7* 19.4 ± 11.0


Patients 64.1 ± 10.1 11.0 ± 1.4* 102.0 ± 4.9* 20.6 ± 7.9
Difference NS NS NS NS

Values are group mean ± SD in cases of normal distributions, and group medians ± mean deviations from the
median for non-normal distributions (*tested by Lilliefors modification of the Kolmogorov–Smirnoff test for
composite normality, significance level, P = 0.05). No significant group differences were found at the level of P <
0.05, tested by the independent two-sample t test or Mann–Whitney U test in cases of normal and non-normal
distributions, respectively. IQ, intelligence quotient; NS, not significant.

Setup and Stimuli. All stimuli were composed of 200-Hz pure tones, 100 ms in Isochrony deviation detection (Iso). Subjects were required to detect a length-
duration with 20 ms gating times. Stimuli were created using Matlab 6.5 ening of the third interval in an otherwise isochronous, five-tone sequence
(Mathworks) with a 44.1-kHz sampling rate and 16-bit resolution, delivered at with a regular interonset-interval of 300 ms (20) (Fig. 1D). This task was
70 dB rms Sound Pressure Level via an external soundcard (Edirol Audio Capture physically identical to that using a fixed single interval (B, Fix), with the
UA-3FX) and closed headphones (Sennheiser HD265 linear). added context of an isochronous beat. The initial difference in interval
length was 20%; adaptive step sizes were 2% and 1%.
Procedure. The timing tasks included two interval timing tasks and three beat- Metrical pattern discrimination (Met). Subjects were required to detect a relative
based timing tasks. All tasks used an adaptive, two alternative forced-choice change in the timing of a rhythmic sequence of seven tones with a metrical
procedure following a two-down–one-up tracking algorithm (59) (Fig. S1). beat (Fig. 1E) based on two levels of periodicity (21). The duration of the
Each test consisted of 60 trials, preceded by a minimum of three practice lower level periodicity was 200 ms on average (roved from 180 to 220 ms in
trials to familiarize the subject with the task. Each trial contained one target 4-ms steps). The higher-level metrical beat of 4 was induced by a regular
and one reference stimulus in randomized order, and the task of the subject occurrence of temporally induced accents on every fourth unit (with a peri-
was to indicate the position of the target. Target positions were randomized odicity of 800 ms on average; marked by thick ticks). The target contained
at equal probabilities, with order fixed across subject. Interstimulus and in- a change in the relative timing of the intervals within the sequence that
tertrial intervals were 1,500 ms each. Subjects communicated their responses resulted in a distortion of the metrical beat pattern. The change in pattern
by pressing corresponding buttons on a response box (controls) or pointing was introduced by changes in duration in the four long intervals of 2x one
to corresponding circles on paper (patients) to avoid motor difficulties with and 2x two silent units, where one of each was lengthened and one short-
button presses. Response time was not limited, but subjects were encour- ened, all by the same percentage. The intervals containing silent units were
aged to make the decisions quickly. The difference between target and thus no longer integer multiples of the underlying unit and the pattern
reference was at suprathreshold levels initially and decreased after two would sound “wrong.” The change amounted to 65% initially, and was
adaptively adjusted in steps of 12% and 6%.
responses correct in a row and increased after each incorrect one. A larger
step size was used up to the fourth reversal (change between increase and
decrease) and after that a smaller one. Thresholds were calculated as the Statistical Data Analysis. Group-level comparison. Distribution of data samples
mean of the last six reversals, estimating the 70.7% correct point of the was tested by the Lilliefors modification of the Kolmogorov–Smirnoff test for
psychometric function (59). The total time needed was less than 2 h, in- composite normality. Between-group comparisons were carried out by Stu-
cluding a pure tone audiogram for 0.125, 0.25, 0.5, 1, 2, and 4 kHz. dent’s or Welch’s t test for normally distributed samples (original or logged)
with equal or unequal variance, or the Mann–Whitney U Test for non-
normally distributed samples of equal dispersion (no unequal dispersion
Tasks of Absolute Timing of Single Intervals. Variable interval timing (Var). In the
occurred). The significance level was P = 0.05. Effect sizes were calculated
variable interval task, subjects were required to discriminate a longer target
taking into account sample size.
interval against a shorter reference interval (Fig. 1A). Intervals were marked
Single-subject inference. Single-subject inference was based on z score trans-
by pairs of tones; the reference interval had a variable interonset-interval of
formation of patients’ thresholds in relation to the control group while taking
300, 360, 420, 480, 560, or 600 ms in duration (at equal probabilities in
into account the effect of age. A complete list of age-dependency correlation
pseudorandomized order fixed across subjects). The target interval had
coefficients based on the use of Pearson product-moment or Spearman rank
a silent gap between the flanking tones that was longer than the reference
correlation test for normal and nonnormal distributions, respectively, is given
by 90% of the silent reference interval initially and adaptively adjusted in
in Table S2.
steps of 12% and 6%.
Age-dependence of thresholds was modeled by linear regression of the
Fixed-interval timing (Fix). As in the variable-interval task, subjects were re-
control data:
quired to discriminate a longer target against a shorter reference interval,
with the only difference that the interonset-interval of the reference was
fixed at 300 ms in this task (Fig. 1B). The target interval was longer by 30%
x ¼ β1 · age þ β0 þ ε
initially and adaptively adjusted in steps of 4% and 1.33%.
where x is the individual threshold, β1 and β0 are the regression coefficients,
and ε is the error of the regression model between threshold and age.
Tasks of Relative Timing of Beat-Based Patterns. Regular beat detection (Reg). In Individual patients’ z scores were calculated as a function of age:
the beat detection task, subjects had to discriminate a more regular target
sequence against a less regular reference sequence based on an underlying _
beat and despite the introduction of an increasing amount of irregularity
zðageÞ ¼ ðθ − x ðageÞÞ=stdðεÞ
(Fig. 1C). Both sequences consisted of 11 tones and were based on a regular
where θ is the individual threshold, bx ðageÞ is the age-dependent expected
beat of a 400 ms interonset-interval. The beat could easily be detected in the
threshold, and std(ε) is the SD of the error of the regression model.
target sequence initially as it was perfectly regular (isochronous). The ref-
The significance level was P = 0.05, equivalent to a score of z > 1.65.
erence sequence was highly irregular as each time interval was shortened or
lengthened at random by 30% on average, making the underlying regular
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS. We thank Sukhbinder Kumar and Martin O’Gorman
beat imperceptible (19). The mean irregularity in the target, starting at 0% for advice with data analysis and Sundeep Teki for comments on the man-
initially, was adaptively increased in steps of 4% and 2.5% until the un- uscript. This work was supported by Wellcome Trust Grant WT061136MA (to
derlying beat could not be detected and the target could not be reliably T.D.G.). F.C. is funded by an Ataxia UK Ph.D. studentship. P.F.C. and T.D.G.
discriminated against the reference anymore. are both Wellcome Trust Senior Fellows in Clinical Science.

1. Gao JH, et al. (1996) Cerebellum implicated in sensory acquisition and discrimination 3. Hore J, Wild B, Diener HC (1991) Cerebellar dysmetria at the elbow, wrist, and fingers.
rather than motor control. Science 272:545–547. J Neurophysiol 65:563–571.
2. Glickstein M (1992) The cerebellum and motor learning. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2: 4. Ivry R, Keele SW (1989) Timing functions of the cerebellum. J Cogn Neurosci 1:
802–806. 136–152.

4 of 5 | www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.0910473107 Grube et al.


5. Ivry RB, Keele SW, Diener HC (1988) Dissociation of the lateral and medial cerebellum 33. Hirsh IJ, Monahan CB, Grant KW, Singh PG (1990) Studies in auditory timing: 1. Simple
in movement timing and movement execution. Exp Brain Res 73:167–180. patterns. Percept Psychophys 47:215–226.
6. Thach WT, Goodkin HP, Keating JG (1992) The cerebellum and the adaptive 34. Deutsch D (1986) Recognition of durations embedded in temporal patterns. Percept
coordination of movement. Annu Rev Neurosci 15:403–442. Psychophys 39:179–186.
7. Lewis PA, Miall RC (2003) Distinct systems for automatic and cognitively controlled 35. Handel S, Oshinsky JS (1981) The meter of syncopated auditory polyrhythms. Percept
time measurement: Evidence from neuroimaging. Curr Opin Neurobiol 13:250–255. Psychophys 30:1–9.
8. Lewis PA, Miall RC (2006) Remembering the time: A continuous clock. Trends Cogn Sci 36. Patel AD, Iversen JR, Chen Y, Repp BH (2005) The influence of metricality and
10:401–406. modality on synchronization with a beat. Exp Brain Res 163:226–238.
9. Chen JL, Penhune VB, Zatorre RJ (2005) Tapping in synchrony to auditory rhythms: 37. Povel DJ, Essens P (1985) Perception of temporal patterns. Music Percept 2:411–440.
Effect of temporal structure on behavior and neural activity. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1060: 38. Rammsayer TH, Brandler S (2004) Aspects of temporal information processing: A
400–403. dimensional analysis. Psychol Res 69:115–123.
10. Grahn JA, Brett M (2007) Rhythm and beat perception in motor areas of the brain. J 39. Molinari M, et al. (2005) Sensorimotor transduction of time information is preserved
Cogn Neurosci 19:893–906. in subjects with cerebellar damage. Brain Res Bull 67:448–458.
11. Penhune VB, Zattore RJ, Evans AC (1998) Cerebellar contributions to motor timing: A 40. Xu D, Liu T, Ashe J, Bushara KO (2006) Role of the olivo-cerebellar system in timing. J
PET study of auditory and visual rhythm reproduction. J Cogn Neurosci 10:752–765. Neurosci 26:5990–5995.
12. Rao SM, Mayer AR, Harrington DL (2001) The evolution of brain activation during 41. Buhusi CV, Meck WH (2005) What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms
temporal processing. Nat Neurosci 4:317–323. of interval timing. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:755–765.
13. Thaut MH (2003) Neural basis of rhythmic timing networks in the human brain. Ann N 42. Ivry RB, Richardson TC (2002) Temporal control and coordination: The multiple timer
Y Acad Sci 999:364–373. model. Brain Cogn 48:117–132.
14. Harrington DL, Lee RR, Boyd LA, Rapcsak SZ, Knight RT (2004) Does the representation 43. Ivry RB, Spencer RM, Zelaznik HN, Diedrichsen J (2002) The cerebellum and event
of time depend on the cerebellum? Effect of cerebellar stroke. Brain 127:561–574. timing. Ann N Y Acad Sci 978:302–317.
15. Ivry RB, Spencer RM (2004) Evaluating the role of the cerebellum in temporal 44. Medina JF, Garcia KS, Nores WL, Taylor NM, Mauk MD (2000) Timing mechanisms in
processing: Beware of the null hypothesis. Brain 127:E13.
the cerebellum: Testing predictions of a large-scale computer simulation. J Neurosci
16. Mangels JA, Ivry RB, Shimizu N (1998) Dissociable contributions of the prefrontal and
20:5516–5525.
neocerebellar cortex to time perception. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 7:15–39.
45. Middleton SJ, et al. (2008) High-frequency network oscillations in cerebellar cortex.
17. Nichelli P, Alway D, Grafman J (1996) Perceptual timing in cerebellar degeneration.
Neuron 58:763–774.
Neuropsychologia 34:863–871.
46. Voogd J, Glickstein M (1998) The anatomy of the cerebellum. Trends Neurosci 21:
18. Spencer RM, Zelaznik HN, Diedrichsen J, Ivry RB (2003) Disrupted timing of discontinuous
370–375.
but not continuous movements by cerebellar lesions. Science 300:1437–1439.
47. Chen JL, Zatorre RJ, Penhune VB (2006) Interactions between auditory and dorsal

NEUROSCIENCE
19. Madison G, Merker B (2002) On the limits of anisochrony in pulse attribution. Psychol
premotor cortex during synchronization to musical rhythms. Neuroimage 32:1771–1781.
Res 66:201–207.
48. Grahn JA, Brett M (2009) Impairment of beat-based rhythm discrimination in
20. Foxton JM, Nandy RK, Griffiths TD (2006) Rhythm deficits in ‘tone deafness’. Brain
Parkinson’s disease. Cortex 45:54–61.
Cogn 62:24–29.
49. Hoshi E, Tremblay L, Féger J, Carras PL, Strick PL (2005) The cerebellum communicates
21. Grube M, Griffiths TD (2009) Metricality-enhanced temporal encoding and the
with the basal ganglia. Nat Neurosci 8:1491–1493.
subjective perception of rhythmic sequences. Cortex 45:72–79.
50. Middleton FA, Strick PL (1994) Anatomical evidence for cerebellar and basal ganglia
22. Ivry RB (1996) The representation of temporal information in perception and motor
involvement in higher cognitive function. Science 266:458–461.
control. Curr Opin Neurobiol 6:851–857.
51. Middleton FA, Strick PL (2001) Cerebellar projections to the prefrontal cortex of the
23. Malapani C, Dubois B, Rancurel G, Gibbon J (1998) Cerebellar dysfunctions of
temporal processing in the seconds range in humans. Neuroreport 9:3907–3912. primate. J Neurosci 21:700–712.
24. Butteriss D, Chinnery P, Birchall D (2005) Radiological characterization of spinocerebellar 52. Habas C, et al. (2009) Distinct cerebellar contributions to intrinsic connectivity
ataxia type 6. Br J Radiol 78:694–696. networks. J Neurosci 29:8586–8594.
25. Buhusi CV, Meck WH (2009) Relativity theory and time perception: Single or multiple 53. O’Reilly JX, Beckmann CF, Tomassini V, Ramnani N, Johansen-Berg H (2010) Distinct
clocks? PLoS ONE 4:e6268. and overlapping functional zones in the cerebellum defined by resting state
26. Schmahmann JD, Sherman JC (1998) The cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. functional connectivity. Cereb Cortex 20:953–965.
Brain 121:561–579. 54. Keele SW, Pokorny RA, Corcos DM, Ivry R (1985) Do perception and motor production
27. Cooper FE, et al. The contribution of the cerebellum to cognition in spinocerebellar share common timing mechanisms: A correctional analysis. Acta Psychol (Amst) 60:
ataxia type 6. Behav Neurol, in press. 173–191.
28. Gottwald B, Mihajlovic Z, Wilde B, Mehdorn HM (2003) Does the cerebellum 55. Walsh V (2003) A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and
contribute to specific aspects of attention? Neuropsychologia 41:1452–1460. quantity. Trends Cogn Sci 7:483–488.
29. Gottwald B, Wilde B, Mihajlovic Z, Mehdorn HM (2004) Evidence for distinct cognitive 56. Fierro B, et al. (2007) Role of the cerebellum in time perception: A TMS study in
deficits after focal cerebellar lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 75:1524–1531. normal subjects. J Neurol Sci 263:107–112.
30. Chen JL, Penhune VB, Zatorre RJ (2008) Listening to musical rhythms recruits motor 57. Crawford JR, et al. (1989) Estimating premorbid IQ from demographic variables:
regions of the brain. Cereb Cortex 18:2844–2854. Regression equations derived from a UK sample. Br J Clin Psychol 28:275–278.
31. Lee KH, et al. (2007) The role of the cerebellum in subsecond time perception: Evidence 58. Nelson HE, Willison JR (1991) National Adult Reading Test Manual (NFER-Nelson,
from repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. J Cogn Neurosci 19:147–157. Windsor, UK) 2 Ed.
32. Friberg A, Sundberg J (1995) Time discrimination in a monotonic, isochronous 59. Levitt H (1971) Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. J Acoust Soc Am 49
sequence. J Acoust Soc Am 98(5):2524–2531. (Suppl 2):467–477.

Grube et al. PNAS Early Edition | 5 of 5

You might also like