Structural Language, Pragmatic Communication, Behavior, and Social Competence in Children Adopted Internationally: A Pilot Study

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: CHILD

http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2016.1182433

Structural language, pragmatic communication, behavior, and social


competence in children adopted internationally: A pilot study
Christine L. Petranovicha, Nicolay Chertkoff Walzb, Mary Allen Staatc, Chung-Yiu Peter Chiud, and Shari L. Wadee
a
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, The University of New Mexico Health Sciences Center, Albuquerque, New Mexico, USA;
b
Division of Behavioral Medicine and Clinical Psychology, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, Cincinnati, Ohio, USA; cDivision of
Infectious Disease, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, The University of Cincinnati, College of Medicine, Cincinnati, Ohio, USA;
d
Department of Psychology, The University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio, USA; eDivision of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Cincinnati
Children’s Hospital Medical Center, The University of Cincinnati, College of Medicine, Cincinnati, Ohio, USA

ABSTRACT KEYWORDS
The objectives of this study were to examine the association of structural language and pragmatic Communicative
communication with behavior problems and social competence in girls adopted internationally. disorders-speech;
Participants included girls between 6–12 years of age who were internationally adopted from international adoption;
social competence
Downloaded by [University of Cambridge] at 23:38 13 June 2016

China (n ¼ 32) and Eastern-Europe (n ¼ 25) and a control group of never-adopted girls (n ¼ 25).
Children completed the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. Parents completed the Child
Communication Checklist- second edition, the Child Behavior Checklist, and the Home and
Community Social Behavior Scales. Compared to the controls, parents in the Eastern European
group reported more problems with social competence, externalizing behaviors, structural
language, and pragmatic communication. The Chinese group evidenced more internalizing
problems. Using generalized linear regression, interaction terms were examined to determine if the
associations of pragmatic communication and structural language with behavior problems and
social competence varied across groups. Controlling for general intellectual functioning, poorer
pragmatic communication was associated with more externalizing problems and poorer social
competence. In the Chinese group, poorer pragmatic communication was associated with more
internalizing problems. Post-adoption weaknesses in pragmatic communication are associated
with behavior problems and social competence. Internationally adopted children may benefit from
interventions that target pragmatic communication.

Introduction Poorer pragmatic language has also been was associated


with more externalizing behavior problems in typically
Recent theories from the field of social neuroscience
developing children (Ketelaars, Cuperus, Jansonius, &
have posited that language and communication skills
Verhoeven, 2010) and greater impulsivity in children
may be central to social and behavioral adjustment in
with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Geurts &
children. Pragmatic communication, or the ability to
Embrechts, 2008). Beauchamp and Anderson (2010)
understand social elements of language and to adjust
more broadly considered the role of language and pos-
communication strategies in response to situational
ited that pragmatic communication and expressive and
demands (Yeates et al., 2007), has been primarily asso-
receptive abilities are relevant to social functioning in
ciated with right frontal lobe functioning (Ferstl,
children. According to the authors, language is the basis
Neumann, Bogler, & von Cramon, 2008; Martin &
for processing thoughts, intentions, and social infor-
McDonald, 2003) with limited evidence suggesting
mation. Expressive and receptive language skills allow
involvement of the right posterior and temporal areas
for efficient expression and comprehension of infor-
(Johns, Tooley, & Traxler, 2008). In typically developing
mation that is exchanged in social interactions. In typi-
children (Leonard, Milich, & Lorch, 2011) and those
cally developing children, weaknesses in structural
with clinical disorders, including specific language
language may result in an impaired ability to effectively
impairment (Brinton & Fujiki, 1999) and autism spec-
communicate with others and to implement effective
trum disorder (Volden, Coolican, Garon, White, & Bry-
behavioral solutions (Ayduk, Rodriguez, Mischel,
son, 2009), poorer pragmatic communication has been
Shoda, & Wright, 2007). In children with speech/
associated with more problems with social competence.
language impairment, poorer expressive and receptive

CONTACT Christine L. Petranovich, Ph.D cpetranovich@salud.unm.edu The University of New Mexico Health Sciences Center MSC 09 5030,
1 University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USA.
© 2016 Taylor & Francis
2 C. L. PETRANOVICH ET AL.

language has been associated with problems with social negative outcomes, including internalizing and externa-
assertiveness, more internalizing problems, and poorer lizing behavior problems (Pomerleau et al., 2005).
self-control (McCabe & Meller, 2004). Greater risk in Eastern European adoptees is thought
Internationally adopted children with a history of to be related to numerous factors, including increased
early life institutionalization often have had experiences risk of in-utero substance exposure (Gunnar et al.,
that place them at risk for negative cognitive, beha- 2007), genetic factors that may place the child at elev-
vioral, and social outcomes. These risk factors may ated risk for mental health disorders (Stevens et al.,
include in-utero substance exposure, poor prenatal 2009), and the potential for early life trauma, including
and perinatal healthcare services, malnutrition, neglect, exposure to war, a lack of social welfare in their country
abuse (McGuinness & Pallansch, 2000; O’Connor et al., of origin, and particularly poor conditions in the insti-
2003), lack of individualized care and attention (Gunnar tutionalized setting, including malnutrition, neglect,
et al., 2000), and few opportunities to engage in and lack of consistent caregivers (Gunnar, Morrison,
activities that facilitate cognitive and social development Chisholm, & Schuder, 2001). Older age at adoption,
(Johnson, 2002). Potentially related to these negative which is thought to represent longer duration of institu-
early experiences, many internationally adopted chil- tionalization and less time in the adopted family, has
dren evidence a variety of neurobiological consequences been associated with more problems with behavior
compared to never- institutionalized children. This (Gunnar et al., 2001; Merz & McCall, 2010), social com-
Downloaded by [University of Cambridge] at 23:38 13 June 2016

includes abnormal patterns of connectivity in the fron- petence (Merz & McCall, 2010), and language (Glennen
tostriatal projections (Behen et al., 2009) and decreased & Masters, 2002; Jacobs, Miller, & Tirella, 2010). Finally,
metabolism in the amygdala, hippocampus, orbital gender differences have been reported. Compared to
frontal gyrus, and infralimbic prefrontal cortex girls, internationally adopted boys may have more prob-
(Chugani et al., 2001). Atypical neurodevelopment lems with behavior, including hyperactivity (Stevens
may have long-term consequences, including a general et al., 2008) and aggression (Groza, Ryan, & Cash,
lag in development compared to children without a 2003). Other lines of research have not found gender
history of institutionalization (Pollak et al., 2010) and differences (Abrines et al., 2012; Hanson et al., 2013;
problems with behavioral and emotional regulation Jacobs et al., 2010; Merz & McCall, 2011).
(Anderson, Damasio, Tranel, & Damasio, 2000) and For many children adopted internationally, they are
social functioning (Stuss & Alexander, 2000). Consistent exposed to one culture and language in early life and,
with this, results of a meta-analysis that included upon adoption, are expected to enter a new culture
adoptees from multiple areas, including Eastern Europe and acquire a second language. This atypical develop-
and Asia, indicated significantly more internalizing and mental course may make language and communication
externalizing problems compared to never-adopted particularly relevant to post-adoption psychosocial
peers in the destination country (Juffer & van functioning (Glennen & Masters, 2002). The results of
IJzendoorn, 2005). In a sample of children inter- a meta-analysis indicated that internationally adopted
nationally adopted from China, most were in the children evidence weaker overall language compared
nonclinical range on a parent-report measure of to peers in the destination country and normative data,
behavior. There was, however, substantial variability, although there was notable variability (Scott, Roberts, &
particularly with regard to hyperactivity, aggression, Glennen, 2011). Findings from other lines of research
and withdrawal (Rojewki, Shapiro, & Shapiro, 2000). indicated weaknesses in receptive and expressive language
Problems with social competence, including difficulties compared to never-adopted peers in the destination
forming relationships with peers, have also been docu- country (Helder, Behen, Wilson, Muzik, & Chugani,
mented in school-aged children adopted internationally 2014; Loman, Wiik, Frenn, Pollak, & Gunnar, 2009)
from Eastern Europe (Colvert et al., 2008; Gunnar, Van and children internationally adopted from foster care
Dulmen, & The International Adoption Project Team, (Loman et al., 2009). Using the Child Communication
2007), Asia, and South America (Gunnar et al., 2007). Checklist- second edition, parent-reported pragmatic
Using parent-report and observational measures in a communication was significantly weaker than structural
sample of children that were internationally adopted language in a sample of children that were internationally
from Eastern Europe and Asia, Bruce, Tarullo, and adopted from Eastern Europe (Glennen & Bright, 2005).
Gunnar (2009) reported disinhibited social behavior Although prior research has documented problems
and atypical lack of social reserve. with language, behavior, and social competence in chil-
Prior research has identified factors that influence dren adopted internationally, few studies have examined
post-adoption functioning. Children adopted from associations among these areas of functioning. In a sam-
Eastern Europe may be particularly vulnerable to ple of internationally adopted children with and without
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: CHILD 3

language impairment, Helder et al. (2014) found more from Eastern Europe (Glennen & Bright, 2005; Gunnar
parent-reported problems with hyperactivity and impul- et al., 2007; Loman et al., 2013) and China (Loman et al.,
sivity compared to never-adopted controls. These 2013; Rojewki et al., 2000; Tarullo, Gavin, & Gunnar,
problems were not associated with performance on 2011) and a comparison group of demographically-
measures of expressive or receptive language. It is matched, never-adopted controls in the country
notable that this study did not include an assessment of destination (Bruce et al., 2009; Tarullo, Gavin, &
of pragmatic communication. Additional research is Gunnar, 2011; Tottenham et al., 2010). The children
needed to examine the relationships of pragmatic com- that participated in this study were selected to represent
munication and structural language with behavior and higher functioning international adoptees. Therefore,
social competence. exclusionary criteria for both groups included a history
This study aimed to investigate the associations of of significant developmental disability, such as intellec-
structural language and pragmatic communication with tual disability. As previously noted, prior studies have
behavior problems and social competence in post-insti- found gender differences in post-adoption behavior
tutionalized children who were internationally adopted (Groza et al., 2003; Stevens et al., 2008). During the
from China or Eastern Europe. We further aimed to study period (December 2007–April 2014), female
determine if these relationships varied across groups. children from Eastern Europe and China accounted
It was hypothesized that the internationally adopted for the majority of children adopted internationally
Downloaded by [University of Cambridge] at 23:38 13 June 2016

groups would evidence poorer structural language and locally, nationally, and worldwide (Juffer & van
pragmatic communication and more problems with Ijzendoorn, 2005). Therefore, only girls between 6–12
behavior and social competence compared to a group years of age were included in an effort to reduce
of never-adopted controls. It was also expected that heterogeneity within the sample. For internationally
pragmatic communication would be more strongly adopted children, additional eligibility criteria included
associated with behavior problems and social com- being between 7–48 months of age at adoption and
petence than would structural language. Finally, it was having spent at least 6 months in pre-adoption institu-
hypothesized that older age at adoption and Eastern tionalize care (e.g., orphanage, baby home, or hospital).
European area of origin would be associated with more These criteria were included to assure that the inter-
problems with behavior and social competence. nationally adopted children had a history of early life
institutionalization.
Participant demographic information is reported in
Methods Table 1. Groups were well-matched for age, family
Procedure income, and primary caregiver education. The Eastern
European group was older (in months) at the time of
This study was approved by the Institutional Review adoption compared to the Chinese group (p ¼ .005).
Board at the study site. Potential participants were Information about history of in-utero substance
identified from records maintained by an international exposure was inconsistently available. Five parents
adoption center at a medical center in the Midwestern (20.0%) in the Eastern European group reported a
United States. Never-adopted, never-institutionalized known or suspected history of in-utero exposure to sub-
control participants were recruited by word of mouth, stances. No parents in the Chinese and control groups
including a hospital wide email and recommendations referenced a history of in-utero substance exposure.
from enrolled participants. This recruitment strategy With respect to language development, the mean age
was utilized in effort to recruit a group of never-adopted (in months) at which the child spoke her first words was
peers that were raised in similarly enriched family envir- 17.33 (5.85) in the Chinese group and 24.93 (11.12) in
onments. At a study visit that was at the medical center, the Eastern European group. The Chinese (p ¼ .02)
informed consent was obtained. Parents provided and the Eastern European (p < .001) groups were sig-
demographic information and completed standardized nificantly delayed compared to controls (12.59 [3.30]
rating forms. Children were administered a measure months). Sixteen children (50.0%) in the Chinese
of general intellectual functioning. Families were com- group, 14 (56.0%) in the Eastern European group,
pensated for time and travel. and zero controls had a history of speech/ language
therapy. Proportions were higher in the Chinese
(χ2 ¼ 17.378, p < .001) and the Eastern European
Participants
(χ2 ¼ 19.44, p < .001) groups relative to the controls.
Consistent with other studies of international adoption, Six children (18.7%) in the Chinese group, seven
participants included children that were adopted (28.0%) in the Eastern European group, and zero
4 C. L. PETRANOVICH ET AL.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics and neurobehavioral test data.


Control Eastern European Chinese
Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
N 25 25 32
Age at testing in years 8.89 (1.64) 8.60 (1.17) 8.44 (1.23)
Age at adoption in monthsc – 23.70 (10.38) 16.64 (6.84)
Primary caregiver education High school or GED ¼ 4.0% High school or GED ¼ 8.0% High school or GED ¼ 6.2%
Some college ¼ 16.0% Some college ¼ 16.0% Some college ¼ 6.2%
4-year degree ¼ 16.0% 4-year degree ¼ 36.0% 4-year degree ¼ 46.9%
Graduate/professional Graduate/professional Graduate/professional
degree ¼ 64.0% degree ¼ 40.0% degree ¼ 40.7%
Family income $30–$59,000 ¼ 4.0% $30–$59,000 ¼ 4.0% $30–$59,000 ¼ 9.4%
$60–$79,000 ¼ 4.0% $60–$79,000 ¼ 8.0% $60–$79,000 ¼ 18.7%
$80–$99,000 ¼ 24.0% $80–$99,000 ¼ 20.0% $80–$99,000 ¼ 12.5%
>$100,000 ¼ 68.0% >$100,000 ¼ 68.0% >$100,000 ¼ 59.4%
HCSBS Social competencea 58.52 (6.26) 50.48 (8.80) 55.68 (8.45)
CBCL Externalizinga,c 45.44 (8.20) 54.40 (10.72) 47.43 (10.28)
CBCL Internalizingb 43.96 (7.72) 48.68 (9.22) 51.53 (11.24)
CCC-2 Structural languagea,b,c 11.35 (1.31) 8.19 (3.02) 9.67 (2.24)
CCC-2 Pragmatic communicationa,b 11.98 (1.45) 9.63 (2.40) 10.50 (1.91)
WASI IQa,b,c 119.20 (13.44) 94.08 (13.34) 106.87 (13.28)
Note. Group comparisons are Bonferroni corrected and control for family income logarithm. CBCL and HCSBS subscales are reported as age-adjusted T scores;
CCC-2 Composites are reported as age-adjusted scaled scores. asignificant difference between control and Eastern European; bsignificant difference between
Downloaded by [University of Cambridge] at 23:38 13 June 2016

control and Chinese; csignificant difference between Eastern European and Chinese.

controls were receiving speech/ language therapy at the language impairment (Geurts & Embrechts, 2008). This
time of study participation. This was greater in the measure includes six subscales assessing pragmatic
Chinese (χ2 ¼ 18.06, p < .001) and the Eastern European communication in the areas of initiation, stereotyped
(χ2 ¼ 18.43, p < .001) groups compared to the controls. language, use of context, non-verbal communication,
communication in social relations, and interests. Con-
sistent with prior research (Bishop, 1998), a Pragmatic
Measures
Communication composite was calculated by averaging
Consistent with other studies of international adoption the scores on these subscales. The CCC-2 also includes
(Olsavsky et al., 2013; Tottenham et al., 2010), children four subscales that assess structural language in the
completed the two subtest estimate of intelligence areas of speech, syntax, semantics, and coherence. We
that is obtained from the Vocabulary and Matrix calculated an average of these subscales to create a
Reasoning subtests from the Wechsler Abbreviated Structural Language composite. For both composite
Scale of Intelligence (WASI; Wechsler, 1999). measures, lower scores indicate more poorly developed
Validation studies have reported that the WASI pro- abilities.
vides a reliable estimate of the full scale intelligence Parents also completed the Child Behavior Checklist
quotient from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for (CBCL), a standardized rating of child behavior that is
Children-III (Weschler, 1999). widely used in both clinical and research settings
Parents completed the Children’s Communication (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). This study used the
Checklist- second edition (CCC-2; Norbury, Nash, Internalizing and Externalizing Problems subscales to
Baird, & Bishop, 2004), a parent-report measure of assess functioning in these domains. The CBCL has high
structural language and pragmatic communication. By test-retest reliability and criterion validity and has been
providing an assessment of pragmatic communication shown to be sensitive to childhood behavior problems
in everyday occurring behavior, standardized question- (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). Higher scores represent
naires may be more ecologically valid than perfor- more behavior problems.
mance-based assessments. In support, it has been Finally, parents completed the Home and Com-
reported that pragmatic communication in children munity Social Behavior Scale (HCSBS; Merrell &
with autism spectrum disorder is often better when Caldarella, 2002). This study utilized the Social Com-
assessed using performance-based measures than it is petence subscale as a measure of social functioning in
in real-world social situations (Volden et al., 2009). the areas of peer relations, self-management, and com-
The CCC-2 has high levels of inter-rater reliability pliance. The HCSBS has been normed for children ages
and consistency (Norbury et al., 2004). It has been used 5 to 18 years old. It has demonstrated satisfactory
to in studies of international adoption (Glennen & reliability and validity (Lund & Merrell, 2001; Merrell
Bright, 2005) and in pediatric clinical populations, & Boelter, 2001; Merrell, Streeter, & Boelter, 2001).
including autism spectrum disorder and specific Lower scores represent poorer social competence.
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: CHILD 5

Statistical approach Results


Univariate statistics were used to examine the distri- Distributions and bivariate relationships
bution of the data and to test for normality. Means
The following variables were normally distributed:
and standard deviations for demographic variables
CBCL Externalizing and Internalizing Problems,
and measures of structural language, pragmatic com-
HCSBS Social Competence, WASI IQ, age at adoption,
munication, externalizing and internalizing behavior,
and the CCC-2 Pragmatic Communication and Struc-
and social competence were reported. Pearson’s
tural Language. Family income (skew ¼ 1.04) was
correlations were used to test the bivariate relation-
slightly negatively skewed. For family income, the logar-
ships between predictors and outcome variables.
ithm was used in the generalized linear regression
Analysis of co-variance (ANCOVA), Bonferroni
models.
corrected, was used for group comparisons (Eastern
Correlations among outcome measures ranged from
European, Chinese, or controls). Because socioeco-
.30 (CBCL Internalizing and HCSBS Social Com-
nomic status influences access to healthcare and
petence) to .56 (CBCL Externalizing and HCSBS
mental health services (Snells-Johns, Mendez, &
Social Competence). The correlation of CCC-2 Prag-
Smith, 2004), family income was used as a covariate.
matic Communication and Structural Language was
For mean comparisons, Cohen’s d was reported as a
.80. Relationships of outcomes measures with the
Downloaded by [University of Cambridge] at 23:38 13 June 2016

measure of effect size (d ¼ .20 indicating small effect


CCC-2 composites ranged from .30 (CBCL Internaliz-
size, .50 indicating medium, .80 indicating large;
ing and CCC-2 Structural Language) to .67 (HCSBS
Cohen, 1988).
Social Competence and CCC-2 Pragmatic
Using contrast terms for group (Chinese, Eastern
Communication).
European, or controls), generalized linear regression
was used to examine the main effects of CCC-2
Structural Language and Pragmatic Communication
Mean comparisons
on CBCL Internalizing and Externalizing Problems
and HCSBS Social Competence. Group and family Means, standard deviations, and ranges are reported in
income were included as covariates. WASI was included Table 1. ANOVA, Bonferroni corrected and controlling
as a control variable in order to determine if structural for family income, indicated significant differences on
language and pragmatic communication were associa- CBCL Externalizing, F(82), ¼ 5.91, p ¼ .004. Post-hoc
ted with behavior problems and social competence comparisons revealed more problems in the Eastern
beyond the effects of estimated general intellectual European group relative to the Chinese group (p ¼ .02,
functioning. Group � CCC-2 interaction terms were d ¼ .66) and the controls (p ¼ .007, d ¼ .93). With
included to determine if these associations differed by respect to CBCL Internalizing, F(82) ¼ 3.81, p ¼ .02,
group. For each model, Variance Inflation Factor the Chinese group evidenced more problems than the
(VIF) and tolerance were calculated to assess for controls (p ¼ .02, d ¼ .78). On HCSBS Social Com-
multicollinearity among main effects. Using manual petence, F(82) ¼ 6.82, p ¼ .002, parents in the Eastern
backwards elimination, non-significant interactions European group reported more problems compared to
were eliminated. The models were re-estimated and the controls (p ¼ .001, d ¼ 1.52). The difference between
non-significant main effects were eliminated (p > .05). the Eastern European and the Chinese groups trended
Group, family income, and WASI were retained even toward significant (p ¼ .06; d ¼ .60)
if non-significant. For significant interactions, we Groups also significantly differed on the WASI,
reported estimated marginal means, co-varying for the F(82) ¼ 22.83, p < .001. The Eastern European (p < .001,
factors that were included in the final estimations of d ¼ 1.87) and the Chinese (p ¼ .005, d ¼ 0.92) groups
the models. For the final estimations, R2 was reported had lower scores than the controls. The Eastern
as a measure of effect size (R2 ¼ .10 indicating small European also underperformed the Chinese group
effect size, .30 indicating medium, .50 indicating large; (p ¼ .001, d ¼ 0.96).
Cohen, 1992). On the CCC-2, groups significantly differed on
We also conducted an analysis that included only the Pragmatic Communication composite, F ¼ 8.92,
the internationally adopted groups in which age at p < .001. Compared to the controls, scores were lower
adoption was introduced to the final estimations. This in the Eastern European (p < .001, d ¼ 1.18) and the
allowed for examination of the specific effects of area Chinese groups (p ¼ .02, d ¼ .87). On the Structural
of origin (Eastern European or Chinese) and age at Language composite, F(82) ¼ 11.57, p < .001, the
adoption. Eastern European group (p � .001, d ¼ 1.35) and the
6 C. L. PETRANOVICH ET AL.

Table 2. Correlation coefficients among factors.


Income WASI IQ CCC-2 structural CCC-2 pragmatic CBCL external CBCL internal HCSBS social
Income – .24 .12 .15 .16 .18 .14
WASI IQ – – .55 .53 .37 .22 .38
CCC-2 Structural – – – .80 .49 .30 .53
CCC-2 Pragmatic – – – – .66 .44 .67
CBCL External – – – – – .45 .56
CBCL Internal – – – – – – .30

Chinese group (p ¼ .03, d ¼ 0.91) had lower scores com- of origin, and older age at adoption were associated with
pared to the controls. The Eastern European group was more externalizing problems.
also lower than the Chinese group (p ¼ .04, d ¼ 0.55). With CBCL Internalizing Problems as the dependent
measure, there was a significant group � CCC-2
Pragmatic Communication interaction (R2 ¼ .30). See
Association of CCC-2 with behavior problems and Table 2. Post-hoc analysis of the estimated marginal
social competence means indicated that within the Chinese group, lower
For all models, VIF and tolerance values were within the CCC-2 Pragmatic Communication was associated
acceptable range (i.e., tolerance > 0.1 and VIF � 10; see with higher ratings on CBCL Internalizing Problems
Downloaded by [University of Cambridge] at 23:38 13 June 2016

Freund & Littell, 2000). (p ¼ .01). See Figure 1. In the analysis that included only
With CBCL Externalizing Problems as the dependent the internationally adopted groups, the group � CCC-2
measure, there was a significant main effect of CCC-2 Pragmatic Communication interaction was non-signifi-
Pragmatic Communication (R2 ¼ .46). See Table 2. cant (p ¼ .13). There was a significant main effect of
Lower CCC-2 Pragmatic Communication (poorer CCC-2 Pragmatic Communication (R2 ¼ .30). Lower
abilities) was associated with higher scores on CBCL CCC-2 Pragmatic Communication was associated with
Externalizing Problems (more problems). See Table 3. higher CBCL Internalizing Problems.
In the analysis that included only the internationally Considering HCSBS Social Competence as the
adopted groups, there were significant main effects of dependent measure, there were significant main effects
CCC-2 Pragmatic Communication, area of origin, and of family income and CCC-2 Pragmatic Communi-
age at adoption (R2 ¼ .52). See Table 4. Lower CCC-2 cation (R2 ¼ .53). See Table 2. Lower CCC-2 Pragmatic
Pragmatic Communication, Eastern European country Communication and higher family income was associa-
ted with lower HCSBS Social Competence scores
(poorer competence). In the analysis within the
Table 3. Regression analysis examining the association of area
of origin and age at adoption.
Table 4. Trimmed regression models examining the association
Variable b SE Std b t p
of CCC-2 composites with behavior problems and social
CBCL Externalizing behaviors competence.
Area of origin 7.26 2.61 .33 2.78 .008*
Family income 7.12 3.67 .20 1.93 .058 Variable b SE Std b t p
WASI IQ .01 .09 .02 .20 .830 CBCL Externalizing behaviors
CCC-2 Pragmatic 3.10 .55 .61 5.59 <.001** Group 1.75 1.09 .14 1.60 .11
communication Family income 2.36 2.94 .06 .80 .42
Age at adoption .33 .13 .28 2.47 .010* WASI IQ .02 .06 .04 .39 .69
2
R .52** CCC-2 Pragmatic 3.26 .48 .67 6.75 <.001**
CBCL Internalizing behaviors communication
Area of origin 20.88 12.63 1.00 1.65 .100 R2 .46**
Family income 4.03 4.28 .12 .94 .350 CBCL Internalizing behaviors
WASI IQ .12 .10 .18 1.22 .220 Group 19.66 7.66 1.63 2.56 .01*
CCC-2 Pragmatic 5.43 1.96 1.12 2.76 .008* Family income 3.80 3.28 .11 1.16 .25
communication WASI IQ .03 .07 .05 .45 .64
Group � Pragmatic 1.81 1.19 .97 1.52 .130 CCC-2 Pragmatic 4.76 1.34 1.01 3.53 .001*
communication communication
Age at adoption .09 .16 .08 .61 .540 Group � Pragmatic 1.52 .67 1.70 2.26 .02*
R2 .30* communication
HCSBS Social competence R2 .30**
Area of origin 3.40 2.07 .19 1.64 .100 HCSBS Social competence
Family income 4.09 2.92 .14 1.40 .160 Group .59 .83 .05 .71 .470
WASI IQ .02 .07 .03 .31 .750 Family income 7.65 2.24 .27 3.40 .001*
CCC-2 Pragmatic 2.72 .44 .66 6.17 <.001** WASI IQ .05 .04 .10 1.08 .280
communication CCC-2 Pragmatic 2.69 .36 .68 7.32 <.001**
Age at adoption .15 .10 .16 1.44 .150 communication
R2 .54** R2 .53**
Note. To correct for skew, the logarithm for family income was used. Note. To correct for skew, the logarithm for family income was used.
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: CHILD 7

Figure 1. Estimated marginal means for significant group � CCC-2 pragmatic communication interaction with CBCL Internalizing as
the dependent measure.

internationally adopted groups, there was a significant risk of in-utero substance exposure (Gunnar et al.,
Downloaded by [University of Cambridge] at 23:38 13 June 2016

main effect of CCC-2 Pragmatic Communication 2007), genetic factors that may place the child at
(R2 ¼ .54). See Table 4. Lower CCC-2 Pragmatic Com- elevated risk for mental health disorders (Stevens
munication remained associated with lower ratings of et al., 2009), possible exposure to war and a lack of
social competence. social welfare in their country of origin, and the
potential for particularly poor conditions in the
institutionalized setting (Gunnar et al., 2001). These
Discussion pre-adoption factors may lead to alterations in neural
This study examined the relationships of structural lan- development that are particularly prominent in the
guage and pragmatic communication with behavior frontal areas (Behen et al., 2009; Chugani et al., 2001).
problems and social competence in children adopted Atypical neural development may contribute to
internationally from China and Eastern Europe relative problems with pragmatic communication (Martin &
to a group of never-adopted controls. Consistent with McDonald, 2003), behavioral and emotional regulation
study hypotheses, the internationally adopted groups (Anderson et al., 2000), and social functioning (Stuss
evidenced more problems relative to the controls. Par- & Alexander, 2000). Finally, internationally adopted
ents in the Chinese group reported more internalizing children are often exposed to one culture and language
problems and poorer structural language and pragmatic in early life and, upon adoption, are expected to enter a
communication compared to the controls. Parents in new culture and acquire a second language. This atypi-
Eastern European group reported more externalizing cal developmental course may further contribute to the
problems compared to both the Chinese and the control weaknesses in pragmatic communication, behavior, and
groups and poor social competence and pragmatic com- social competence that were documented by this study.
munication relative to the controls. This is in line with Overall, there are a multitude of pre-adoption factors
previous research suggesting that children adopted that contribute to post-adoption outcomes. However,
internationally often experience difficulties with prag- given the nature of international adoption, knowledge
matic communication (Glennen & Bright, 2005; Loman of pre-adoption adversity and early history is often
et al., 2009; Scott, Roberts, & Glennen, 2011) and that unavailable. This lack of information limits our ability
Eastern European adoptees may be particularly vulner- to identify the specific range of factors that influence
able to negative post-adoption outcomes (Gunnar et al., post-adoption outcomes.
2001). Weaknesses in pragmatic communication, exter- Consistent with theories from the social neuroscience
nalizing behaviors, and social competence, which were literature (Beauchamp & Anderson, 2010; Yeates et al.,
most prominent in the Eastern European group, may 2007), our findings support the hypothesized relation-
be related to numerous factors. Many children adopted ship of pragmatic communication with behavior prob-
internationally evidence lags in cognitive and social lems and social competence. Controlling for general
development relative to never-adopted peers that were intellectual functioning, poorer pragmatic language
raised in similar environments (Pollak et al., 2010). In was associated with more externalizing problems and
children adopted from Eastern Europe, developmental poorer social competence. These associations did not
lags may be related to multiple factors, including higher vary by group, suggesting that these relationships
8 C. L. PETRANOVICH ET AL.

existed in both the control and the internationally (Abrines et al., 2012; Hanson et al., 2013; Jacobs et al.,
adopted groups. Consistent with studies that have 2010; Merz & McCall, 2011).
reported more internalizing problems in children inter- In children adopted internationally, it is often diffi-
nationally adopted from China (Rojewki et al., 2000), cult to distinguish problems that are associated with
parents the Chinese group reported more internalizing English as a second language from more fundamental
behaviors than the controls. Within this group, poorer weaknesses in structural language and pragmatic com-
pragmatic communication was associated with more munication (Welsh, Viana, Petrill, & Mathias, 2007).
internalizing problems. This is in line with other studies This study is therefore limited by having not assessed
that have reported associations of pragmatic language English language proficiency. However, there is avail-
and internalizing problems in typically developing chil- able information that may speak to English language
dren (Ketelaars et al., 2010) and children with speech- experience. All parents reported that English is the
language impairment (Gallagher, 1999). In explaining child’s primary language. The average number of
this relationship, pragmatic communication weaknesses months since adoption was 84.76 in the Chinese group
may result in emotional problems that result from an (range 55–128 months) and 78.74 months in the Eastern
inability to use communication to form social bonds European group (range 43–136 months). In a study of
(Hyter, Rogers-Atkinson, Self, Simmons, & Jantz, bilingual English-learning children, it was reported that
2001). Alternatively, it may be that emotional problems it takes an average of 3.31 years to achieve performance
Downloaded by [University of Cambridge] at 23:38 13 June 2016

result in fewer social opportunities, which may contrib- on English language tests that is similar to that of native
ute to weaknesses in pragmatic communication. English speakers (McSwan & Pray, 2005). In a separate
The internationally adopted groups reported more study, children that were internationally adopted as
problems with structural language compared to the infants and toddlers demonstrated language abilities
controls. However, the findings of this study failed to that were similar to English based norms by 36–40
identify an association of structural language with months of age (Glennen & Masters, 2002). In view of
behavior problems or social competence. This is con- these findings, participants in this study may be
sistent with research in typically developing children expected to have English language proficiency that is
that suggested that pragmatic communication is more similar to that of native English speakers and
salient than structural language when considering English-based norms.
behavior problems (Ketelaars et al., 2010). As suggested As noted above, the population of children adopted
by Ketelaars et al., structural language problems internationally is heterogeneous because of the numer-
may often be identified and remediated in early child- ous factors that often vary, including genetic risks, poor
hood. Weaknesses in pragmatic communication may maternal health, in-utero exposure to teratogens, quality
be less likely to be identified and may therefore go of pre-adoptive medical care, pre-adoptive abuse,
unaddressed, potentially resulting in a greater impact neglect, and duration of institutionalization (Colvert
on long-term behavior and social functioning. et al., 2008; Tottenham et al., 2010). From a research
Also consistent with study hypotheses, Eastern perspective, heterogeneity poses difficulty when making
Europe as the area of origin and older age at adoption comparisons between groups of internationally adopted
were associated with more externalizing problems. As children and when selecting a control group (Colvert
previously noted, other studies have reported delays in et al., 2008). The advantage of the never-adopted con-
frontal lobe development in internationally adopted trol group is that it offers a comparison relative to peers
children, which may be expected to negatively impact that were raised in similarly enriched family environ-
executive functioning (Behen et al., 2009; Hanson ments. Its use is limited because it is not possible to
et al., 2013). Externalizing problems may be closely ascertain the role of institutionalization itself and the
related to executive functioning, particularly behavioral proportion of observable differences that may be
regulation (Anderson et al., 2000). Delays in frontal lobe attributable to cultural and family-level factors.
development, potentially resulting in behavioral dysre- Additionally, as also noted above, we are unable to
gulation and externalizing behaviors, may be parti- account for adoption-related risk factors, including
cularly prominent in Eastern Europe adoptees and trauma, degree of deprivation and lack of social welfare
those with longer duration of institutionalization. In in the country of origin, which would be expected to
contrast to study hypotheses, area of origin and age at impact the severity of post-adoption problems. We are
adoption were not significantly associated with interna- also unable to account for pre-existing developmental
lizing problems or social competence. This is in line and/or medical problems, which may have contributed
with other lines of research that similarly reported to the biological family’s decision to place the child up
non-significant findings with respect to age at adoption for adoption. This study is therefore limited by its
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: CHILD 9

inability to identify the specific pre-adoption factors measures, which may be related to rater bias
that contribute to post-adoption problems with beha- (Holmbeck, Li, Schurman, Friedman, & Coakley,
vior and social competence. Inconsistent knowledge of 2002). A more comprehensive assessment of behavior
pre-adoption adversity and early history is a limitation problems and social functioning would be obtained
that is inherent to this population. by using observational measures and standardized
There may also be characteristics of the adoptive measures from multiple raters, including teacher-
family that impact post-adoption child outcomes. For and self-report. This study assessed structural language
example, Hellerstedt et al. (2008) reported that the and pragmatic communication by parent report. As
majority of parents that adopted internationally ident- noted above, this method may have the advantage of
ified as white/ Caucasian. Depending on the race of offering information about abilities as they present in
the child and the adopting family, the child may or everyday occurring situations, which may be parti-
may not appear to be of the same race as his or her cularly important in view of findings indicating that
adopted family. The desire to have a child that appears pragmatic communication may be better on
racially similar or dissimilar may be a relevant consider- performance-based measures than in real-world social
ation when families are deciding from where to adopt a situations (Volden et al., 2009). However, a more
child. In a separate study, Lee et al. (2006) described comprehensive assessment of structural language
attitudes about cultural socialization and race in parents and pragmatic communication would be obtained by
Downloaded by [University of Cambridge] at 23:38 13 June 2016

of internationally adopted children, including those using multiple methods, including standardized ques-
adopted from Eastern Europe and Asia. Across coun- tionnaires from multiple raters and observational
tries of origin, higher value on cultural socialization to and performance-based measures. As an additional
the child’s country of origin was associated with greater consideration, this study used a cross-sectional
help seeking behavior, such as speaking with a teacher design. Therefore, we can only document functioning
about issues that their child is experiencing. Taken at a single time point and cannot make conclusions
together, these findings highlight family characteristics about development and long-term maintenance of
that may influence post-adoption outcomes. Additional problems. Finally, only girls were included in this
research is needed that more comprehensively examines sample in an effort to reduce heterogeneity. Other
the impact of family-related factors, such as parenting authors have found that internationally adopted boys
style, cultural values, and quality of life, on behavior have more behavior problems than girls (Groza
and social competence. et al., 2003; Stevens et al., 2008). Patterns of findings
When considering these findings, sample size is a may have been different if this study had included boys
key limitation. With a larger sample, this study may and girls.
have been able to detect patterns that were not observ- These findings further our knowledge about factors
able in this sample. Consistent with other lines of that are associated with behavior problems and social
research (e.g., Glennen & Bright, 2005; Gunnar et al., functioning in internationally adopted children with a
2007; Loman et al., 2013), this study grouped together history of institutionalization. This study has the
children that were adopted from Eastern Europe. This advantage of using a comparison group of peers without
was done because there are pre-adoption risk factors a history of institutionalization that was well-matched
that are common to children that were adopted from on key post-adoption demographic variables. As an
institutions in this region (Gunnar et al., 2001; additional strength, this study controlled for general
Pomerleau et al., 2005). However, Eastern Europe is intellectual functioning, thereby demonstrating that
a large geographical area that includes many diverse pragmatic communication is associated with behavior
countries and cultures. There may be variability problems and social competence above that accounted
among Eastern European adoptees that was not cap- for by intellectual ability. In view of these findings, there
tured by this study. As an additional limitation, the is a need for longitudinal research that uses multiple
children that participated in this study were selected assessment methods to better understand the inter-
to represent higher functioning international adoptees relationship of pragmatic communication, behavior
(e.g., those without a history of intellectual disability problems, and social functioning over time. Future
or significant developmental delays). As a result, this investigations may also seek to develop interventions
sample may not be representative of the larger popu- that are tailored to meet the specific needs of inter-
lation of internationally adopted children. This study nationally adopted children and their families. This
is further limited by having assessed behavior prob- may include programs that target weaknesses in
lems and social competence using only parent-report. pragmatic communication and support post-adoption
Reliance on a single rater may inflate variance among behavioral and social adjustment.
10 C. L. PETRANOVICH ET AL.

Acknowledgments Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral


sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
The authors wish to acknowledge the International Adoption Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112,
Center at Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center for 155–159. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155
assistance with participant recruitment and financial support. Colvert, E., Rutter, M., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Groothues, C.,
We also wish to acknowledge Holly MacPherson, Karen Hawkins, A., … Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2008). Emotional
Oberjohn, Jennifer Taylor, and Julia Smith for assistance with difficulties in early adolescence following severe early
participant recruitment, data collection, and data entry. deprivation: Findings from the English and Romanian
adoptees study. Development and Psychopathology, 20,
547–567. doi:10.1017/S0954579408000278
Funding Ferstl, E. C., Neumann, J., Bogler, C., & von Cramon, D. Y.
Funding was provided by the University of Cincinnati, Uni- (2008). The extended language network: A meta-analysis
versity Research Council Interdisciplinary Grant. The authors of neuroimaging studies on text comprehension. Human
have no conflicts of interest to disclose. Brain Mapping, 29, 581–593. doi:10.1002/hbm.20422
Freund, R., & Littell, R. (2000). SAS system for regression
(3rd ed.). New York, NY: Wiley.
References Gallagher, T. M. (1999). Interrelationships among children’s
language, behavior, and emotional problems. Topics in
Abrines, N., Barcons, N., Marre, D., Brun, C., Fornieles, A., & Language Disorders, 19(2), 1–15.
Fumado, V. (2012). ADHD-like symptoms and attachment Geurts, H. M., & Embrechts, M. (2008). Language profiles
Downloaded by [University of Cambridge] at 23:38 13 June 2016

in internationally adopted children. Attachment & Human in ASD, SLI, and ADHD. Journal of Autism and Develop-
Development, 14(4), 405–423. doi:10.1080/14616734.2012. mental Disorders, 38, 1931–1943. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-
691656 0587-1
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for Glennen, S., & Bright, B. J. (2005). Five years later: Language in
ASEBA school- aged forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: school-aged internationally adopted children. Seminars in
University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Speech and Language, 26, 86–101. doi:10.1055/s-2005-864219
Youth, & Families. Glennen, S., & Masters, M. G. (2002). Typical and atypical
Anderson, S. W., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. language development in infants and toddlers adopted
(2000). Long-term sequelae of prefrontal cortex damage from Eastern Europe. American Journal of Speech-Language
acquired in early childhood. Developmental Neuropsychol- Pathology, 11, 417–433. doi:10.1044/1058-0360
ogy, 18(3), 281–296. doi:10.1207/s1532694202anderson Groza, V., Ryan, S. D., & Cash, S. J. (2003). Institutionaliza-
Ayduk, O., Rodriguez, M. L., Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & tion, behavior and international adoption: Predictors of
Wright, J. (2007). Verbal intelligence and self-regulatory behavior problems. Journal of Immigrant Health, 5, 5–17.
competencies: Joint predictors of boys’ aggression, Journal doi:10.1023/A:1021096028003
of Research in Personality, 41, 374–388. Gunnar, M. R., Bruce, J., & Grotevant, H. D. (2000). Inter-
Beauchamp, M. H., & Anderson, V. (2010). Social: An national adoption of institutionally reared children:
integrative framework for the development of social Research and policy. Development and Psychopathology, 4,
skills. Psychological Bulletin, 36(1), 39–64. doi:10.1037/ 677–693.
a0017768 Gunnar, M. R., Morrison, S. J., Chisholm, K., & Schuder, M.
Behen, M. E., Muzik, O., Saporta, A. S. D., Wilson, B. J., Pai, (2001). Salivary cortisol levels in children adopted from
D., Hua, J., & Chugani, H. T. (2009). Abnormal fronto- Romanian orphanages. Development and Psychopathology,
striatal connectivity in children with histories of early 3, 611–628. doi:10.1017/S095457940100311X
environmental deprivation: A diffusion tensor imaging Gunnar, M. R., Van Dulmen, M. H. M., & The International
study. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 3(3), 292–297. Adoption Project Team. (2007). Behavior problems in
doi:10.1007/s11682-009-9071-6 postinstitutionalized, internationally adopted children.
Bishop, D. V. M. (1998). Development of the children’s com- Development and Psychopathology, 19, 129–148.
munication checklist: A method for assessing qualitative doi:10.1017/S0954579407070071
aspects of communicative impairment in children. Journal Hanson, J. L., Adluru, N., Chung, M. K., Alexander, A. L.,
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(6), 879–891. Davidson, R. J., & Pollak, S. D. (2013). Early neglect is
doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00388 associated with alterations in white matter integrity and
Brinton, B., & Fujiki, M. (1999). Social interaction behaviors cognitive functioning. Child Development, 84(5), 1566–
of children with speech language impairment. Topics in 1578. doi:10.1111/cdev.12069
Language Disorders, 19(2), 183–195. Helder, E. J., Behen, M. E., Wilson, B., Muzik, O., & Chugani,
Bruce, J., Tarullo, A. R., & Gunnar, M. R. (2009). Disinhibited H. T. (2014). Language difficulties in children adopted
social behavior among internationally adopted children. internationally: Neuropsychological and functional corre-
Development and Psychopathology, 21, 157–171. lates. Child Neuropsychology, 20(4), 470–492. doi:10.1080/
doi:10.1017/S0954579409000108 09297049.2013.819846
Chugani, H. T., Behen, M. E., Muzik, O., Juhsaz, C., Nagy, F., Hellerstedt, W. L., Madsen, N. J., Gunnar, M. R., Grotevant,
& Chugani, D. C. (2001). Local brain functional activity fol- H. D., Lee, R. M., & Johnson, D. E. (2008). The inter-
lowing early deprivation: A study of postinstitutionalized national adoption project: Population-based surveillance
Romanian orphans. Neuroimage, 14(6), 1290–1301. of Minnesota parents who adopted internationally.
doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0917 Maternal and Child Health Journal, 12(2), 162–171.
APPLIED NEUROPSYCHOLOGY: CHILD 11

Holmbeck, G. N., Li, S. T., Schurman, J. V., Friedman, D., & McCabe, P. C., & Meller, P. J. (2004). The relationship
Coakley, R. M. (2002). Collecting and managing multi- between language and social competence: How language
source and multimethod data in studies of pediatric popu- impairment effects social growth. Psychology in the Schools,
lations. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 27, 5–18. 41(3), 313–321. doi:10.1002/pits.10161
doi:10.1093/jpepsy/27.1.5 McGuiness, T., & Pallansch, L. (2000). Competence of
Hyter, Y. D., Rogers-Atkinson, D. L., Self, T. L., Simmons, B. children adopted from the former Soviet Union. Family
F., & Jantz, J. (2001). Pragmatic language intervention for Relations, 49(4), 457–464.
children with language and emotional/behavioral disorders. Merrell, K. W., & Boelter, E. (2001). An investigation of the
Communication Disorders Quarterly, 23, 4–16. doi:10.1177/ relationships between social behavior and ADHD in
152574010102300103 children and youth: Construct validity of the Home and
Jacobs, E., Miller, L. C., & Tirella, L. G. (2010). Develop- Community Behavior Scales, Journal of Emotional and
mental and behavioral perfomrance of internationally Behavioral Disorders, 9, 260–269.
adopted preschoolers: A pilot study. Child Psychiatry Merrell, K. W., Streeter, A. L., & Boelter, E. W. (2001).
and Human Development, 41, 15–29. doi:10.1007/s10578- Validity of the home and community social behavior scales:
009-0149-6 Comparisons with five behavior-rating scales, Psychology in
Johns, C. L., Tooley, K. M., & Traxler, M. J. (2008). Discourse the Schools, 38, 313–325.
impairments following right hemisphere brain damage: A Merz, E. C., & McCall, R. B. (2010). Behavior problems in
critical review. Language and Linguistics Compass, 2(6), children adopted from psychosocially deprived institutions.
1038–1062. doi:10.1111/j.1749-818X.2008.00094.x Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38(4), 459–470.
Johnson, D. E. (2002). Adoption and the effect on children’s doi:10.1007/s10802-009-9383-4
Downloaded by [University of Cambridge] at 23:38 13 June 2016

development. Early Human Development, 68, 39–54. Merz, E. C., & McCall, R. B. (2011). Parent ratings of execu-
doi:10.1016/S0378-3782(02)00017-8 tive functioning in children adopted from psychosocially
Juffer, F., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2005). Behavior problems depriving institutions. The Journal of Child and Adolescent
and mental health referrals of international adoptees: A Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(5), 537–546.
meta-analysis. Journal of the American Medical Association, Norbury, C. F., Nash, M., Baird, G., & Bishop, D. V. M.
293, 2501–2515. doi:10.1001/jama.293.20.2501 (2004). Using a parental checklist to identify diagnostic
Ketelaars, M. P., Cuperus, J., Jansonius, K., & Verhoeven, L. groups of children with communication impairment: A
(2010). Pragmatic language impairment and associated validation of the Children’s Communication Checklist-2.
behavior problems. International Journal of Language and International Journal of Language and Communication Dis-
Communication Disorders, 45(2), 204–214. doi:10.3109/ order, 39(3), 345–364. doi:10.1080/13682820410001654883
13682820902863090 O’Connor, T. G., Marvin, R. S., Rutter, M., Olrick, J. T.,
Leonard, M. A., Milich, R., & Lorch, E. P. (2011). The role of Britner, P. A., & the English, and Romanian Adoptees
pragmatic language use in mediating the relationship Study Team. (2003). Child-parent attachment following
between hyperactivity and inattention and social skills early institutional deprivation, Development and Psycho-
problems, Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing pathology, 15, 19–38.
Research, 54, 567–579. Olsavsky, A. K., Telzer, E. H., Shapiro, M., Humphreys, K. L.,
Lee, R. M., et al. (2006). Cultural socialization in families with Flannery, J., Goff, B., & Tottenham, N. (2013). Indiscrimi-
internationally adopted children. Journal of Family nate amygdala response to mothers and strangers after
Psychology, 20(4), 571–580. early maternal deprivation. Biological Psychiatry, 74(11),
Loman, M. M., Johnson, A. E., Westerlund, A., Pollak, S. D., 853–860. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.05.025
Nelson, C. A., & Gunnar, M. R. (2013). The effect of early Pollak, S. D. et al. (2010). Neurodevelopmental effects of early
deprivation on executive attention in middle childhood, deprivation in postinstitutionalized children, Child Devel-
Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychology and Psychiatry, opment, 81, 224–236.
54, 37–45. Pomerleau, A., Malcuit, G., Chicoine, J., Seguin, R.,
Loman, M. M., Wiik, K. L., Frenn, K. A., Pollak, S. D., & Belhumeur, C., Germain, P., … Jeliu, G. (2005). Health sta-
Gunnar, M. R. (2009). Post-institutionalized children’s tus, cognitive and motor development of young children
development: Growth, cognitive, and language outcomes. adopted from China, East Asia, and Russia across the
Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 30(5), first 6 months after adoption. International Journal of
426–434. doi:10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181b1fd08 Behavioral Development, 29(5), 445–457. doi:10.1080/
Lund, J., & Merrell, K. W. (2001). Social and antisocial beha- 01650250500206257
vior of children with learning and behavioral disorders: Rojewski, J. W., Shapiro, M. S., & Shapiro, M. (2000). Parental
Construct validity of the Home and Community Social assessment of behavior in Chinese adoptees during early
Behavior Scales, Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, childhood, Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 31,
19, 112–122. 79–96.
MacSwan, J., & Pray, L. (2005). Learning English bilingually: Scott, K. A., Roberts, J. A., & Glennen, S. (2011). How well do
Age of onset of exposure and rate of acquisition among children who are internationally adopted acquire language?
English language learners in a bilingual education program. A meta-analysis, Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
Bilingual Research Journal, 29(3), 653–678. Research, 54, 1153–1169.
Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2003). Weak coherence, no theory Scott, K. A., Roberts, J. A., & Glennen, S. (2011). How well do
of mind, or executive dysfunction? Solving the puzzle of children adopted internationally acquire language? A meta-
pragmatic language disorders. Brain and Language, 85, analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
451–466. doi:10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00070-1 54, 1153–1169. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2010/10-0075)
12 C. L. PETRANOVICH ET AL.

Snells-Johns, J., Mendez, J. L., & Smith, B. H. (2004). in internationally adopted children. Developmental
Evidence-based solutions for overcoming access barriers, Psychology, 47(2), 417–431.
decreasing attrition, and promoting change with under- Tottenham, N., Hare, T. A., Quinn, B. T., McCarry, T. W., Nurse,
served families. Journal of Family Psychology, 18, 19–35. M., Gilhooly, T., … Casey, B. J. (2010). Prolonged institutional
doi:10.1037/0893-3200.18.1.19 rearing is associated with atypically larger amygdala volume
Stevens, S., Kumsta, R., Kreppner, J., Brookes, K., Rutter, M., & and difficulties in emotion regulation. Developmental Science,
Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2009). Dopamine transporter gene 13, 46–61. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00852.x
polymorphism moderates the effects of severe deprivation Volden, J., Coolican, J., Garon, N., White, J., & Bryson, S.
on ADHD symptoms: Developmental continuities in (2009). Brief report: Pragmatic language in autism spec-
gene-environment inter-play. American Journal of trum disorder: Relationships to measures of ability and
Medical Genetics, Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 150B, disability. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
753–761. 39(2), 388–393. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0618
Stevens, S., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Kreppner, J. M., Beckett, C., Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of
Castle, J., Colvert, E., … Rutter, M. (2008). Inattention/ Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
overactivity following early severe institutional deprivation: Welsh, J. A., Viana, A. G., Petrill, S. A., & Mathias, M. D.
Presentation and associations in early adolescence. Journal (2007). Interventions for internationally adopted children
of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 385–398. doi:10.1007/ and families: A review of the literature, Journal of Child
s10802-007-9185-5 and Adolescent Social Work, 243, 285–311.
Stuss, D. T., & Alexander, M. P. (2000). Executive functions Yeates, K. O., Bigler, E. D., Dennis, M., Gerhardt, C. A., Rubin,
and the frontal lobes: A conceptual view. Psychological K. H., Stancin, T., … Vanatta, K. (2007). Social outcomes in
Downloaded by [University of Cambridge] at 23:38 13 June 2016

Research, 63, 289–298. doi:10.1007/s004269900007 childhood brain disorder: A heuristic integration of social
Tarullo, A. R., Gavin, M. C., & Gunnar, M. R. (2011). Atypical neuroscience and developmental pathology. Psychological
EEG power correlates with indiscriminate friendly behavior Bulletin, 133(3), 535–556. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.3.535

You might also like