Ip Presentation 1

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 73

Academy of Management Journal

When Does Pay for Performance Motivate Employee Helping


Behavior? The Contextual Influence of Performance
Subjectivity

Journal: Academy of Management Journal

Manuscript ID AMJ-2018-1408.R3

Manuscript Type: Revision

Lab experiment < Quantitative Orientation < Research Methods, Reward


and incentive systems < Human Resource Management and Industrial
Relations < Topic Areas, Organizational citizenship behavior < Behavior
Keywords:
< Organizational Behavior < Topic Areas, Performance evaluation &
management < Human Resource Management and Industrial Relations <
Topic Areas

An extensive body of literature demonstrates the incentive effect by


which pay for performance (PFP) motivates employees’ in-role task
performance. Nonetheless, scholars have also posited that PFP is likely to
demotivate employees’ extra-role behaviors. Drawing upon expectancy
theory (Vroom, 1964) and the heuristic processing literature (Kahneman,
2011), we examine the relationship between PFP and employee helping
behavior. We perform this examination not only by considering the “pay”
component (e.g., PFP intensity) but also by scrutinizing the
“performance” component, namely, performance subjectivity, which
refers to the extent to which the criteria or indicators used to measure
employee performance in the performance appraisal system are
Abstract:
subjective. Specifically, we propose that PFP has a conditional positive
effect (i.e., in the context of high performance subjectivity) on employee
helping behavior and further theorize and test the underlying
psychological mechanism by which individual perceived helping-
performance expectancy accounts for the interactive effects between PFP
and performance subjectivity on employee helping behavior. The
empirical results of three studies employing distinctive methodologies
provide general support for our hypotheses. Taken together, our
research challenges the conventional wisdom that PFP undermines
employees’ extra-role behaviors by providing new insight into
understanding when and why PFP motivates employee helping behavior.
Page 1 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4
5 When Does Pay for Performance Motivate Employee Helping Behavior?
6 The Contextual Influence of Performance Subjectivity
7
8
9 Wei He
10 Nanjing University
11 whe@nju.edu.cn
12
13
14 Shao-Long Li
15 Wuhan University
16
tli@whu.edu.cn
17
18
19 Jie Feng
20 Rutgers University
21
22 jie.feng@rutgers.edu
23
24 Guanglei Zhang
25
26
Wuhan University of Technology
27 zhangguanglei@whut.edu.cn
28
29
Michael C. Sturman
30
31 Rutgers University
32 michael.sturman@rutgers.edu
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
Acknowledgements:
52
53 We gratefully thank Associate Editor Anthony Nyberg and three anonymous reviewers for their
54 insightful comments and guidance. We also thank Peter Bamberger and Elena Belogolovsky for their
55
generous sharing of the experimental materials used in the anagram-solving task. This research was
56
57 supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (71822203; 71772073; 71602147;
58 71772072). Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Shao-Long Li, Economics
59 and Management School, Wuhan University, China. E-mail: tli@whu.edu.cn.
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 2 of 72

1
2
3
WHEN DOES PAY FOR PERFORMANCE INCREASE EMPLOYEE
4
5 HELPING BEHAVIOR? THE CONTEXTUAL INFLUENCE OF
6 PERFORMANCE SUBJECTIVITY
7
8
9 ABSTRACT
10 An extensive body of literature demonstrates the incentive effect by which pay for
11 performance (PFP) motivates employees’ in-role task performance. Nonetheless, scholars
12
13
have also posited that PFP is likely to demotivate employees’ extra-role behaviors. Drawing
14 upon expectancy theory (Vroom, 1964) and the heuristic processing literature (Kahneman,
15 2011), we examine the relationship between PFP and employee helping behavior. We
16
perform this examination not only by considering the “pay” component (e.g., PFP intensity)
17
18 but also by scrutinizing the “performance” component, namely, performance subjectivity,
19 which refers to the extent to which the criteria or indicators used to measure employee
20 performance in the performance appraisal system are subjective. Specifically, we propose
21
22 that PFP has a conditional positive effect (i.e., in the context of high performance
23 subjectivity) on employee helping behavior and further theorize and test the underlying
24 psychological mechanism by which individual perceived helping-performance expectancy
25
26
accounts for the interactive effects between PFP and performance subjectivity on employee
27 helping behavior. The empirical results of three studies employing distinctive methodologies
28 provide general support for our hypotheses. Taken together, our research challenges the
29
conventional wisdom that PFP undermines employees’ extra-role behaviors by providing new
30
31 insight into understanding when and why PFP motivates employee helping behavior.
32
33 Keywords: pay for performance, helping behavior, performance subjectivity, heuristic
34
35 processing, performance management
36
37
38 Pay for performance (PFP), broadly defined as “pay that varies with some measure of
39
40
individual or organizational performance” (Gerhart & Newman, 2020: 715), is a vital
41
42
43 component of an organization’s compensation strategy (Gerhart & Rynes, 2003; Gerhart,
44
45
46 Rynes, & Fulmer, 2009). Recent estimates suggest that PFP is appealing for employers, as
47
48 more than 90% of employers have designed PFP systems that link employees’ pay levels with
49
50
51 their prior or future job performance (Gerhart & Fang, 2014), seeking to motivate employees’
52
53
performance improvement (Maltarich, Nyberg, Reilly, Abdulsalam, & Martin, 2017; Nyberg,
54
55
56 Pieper, & Trevor, 2016). This incentive effect of PFP can be explained by both economic and
57
58
59 psychological theories and principles, such as the incentive intensity principle proposed in
60
Page 3 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 agency theory (Jensen & Meckling, 1976) and the instrumentality principle proposed in
5
6
7 expectancy theory (Vroom, 1964). Accumulative empirical findings have supported this
8
9 incentive effect by revealing a positive relationship between PFP and employee in-role task
10
11
12 performance (e.g., Han, Bartol, & Kim, 2015; Maltarich et al., 2017; Nyberg et al., 2016;
13
14
Park & Sturman, 2016). Through both incentive and sorting mechanisms (Cadsby, Fei, &
15
16
17 Tapon, 2007), PFP increases individual and collective performance at the team, unit, and/or
18
19
20 firm levels (see Byron & Khazanchi, 2012; Garbers & Konradt, 2014; Jenkins, Mitra, Gupta,
21
22 & Shaw, 1998 for meta-analytic reviews).
23
24
25 Despite the general evidence of PFP’s beneficial effects (Gerhart, 2017; Gerhart &
26
27
Fang, 2014), researchers have also long raised concerns about the overall effectiveness of
28
29
30 PFP (individual performance-based PFP in particular) due to potential unintended
31
32
33 consequences of such plans (e.g., Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 1999; Frey & Osterloh, 2012;
34
35 Kohn, Stewart, & McAdams, 1993; Lawler, 2000; Pfeffer, 1998). These concerns include that
36
37
38 PFP plans may foster coworker envy (Bamberger & Belogolovsky, 2017), encourage
39
40
unethical behavior (John, Loewenstein, & Rick, 2014; Kouchaki, Smith-Crowe, Brief, &
41
42
43 Sousa, 2013), reduce intrinsic motivation and creativity (Eisenberger & Cameron, 1996), and
44
45
46 encourage short-term thinking (Deci & Ryan, 1985). In short, although there may be strong
47
48 evidence that PFP facilitates higher in-role performance and the metrics specifically rewarded
49
50
51 by a PFP plan (Gerhart, 2017; Shaw & Gupta, 2015), substantial concern remains that PFP
52
53
plans may ultimately harm behaviors that fall outside of the PFP plan’s domain. One
54
55
56 exemplar of these out-of-role behaviors or so-called organizational citizenship behaviors
57
58
59 (OCBs) is coworker helping, which is defined as a form of “promotive behavior that
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 4 of 72

1
2
3
4 emphasizes small acts of consideration” (Van Dyne & LePine, 1998: 109). Helping behaviors
5
6
7 are cooperative in nature and include both supporting coworkers and preventing the
8
9 occurrence of coworkers’ work-related problems (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Paine, & Bachrach,
10
11
12 2000). While PFP specifically encourages particular in-role behaviors, the concern is that
13
14
PFP may simultaneously cause increased competition, decreased collaboration, and
15
16
17 ultimately fewer coworker helping behaviors (Arvey & Murphy, 1998; Deming, 1986;
18
19
20 Larkin, Pierce, & Gino, 2012). Given that OCBs in general and helping behaviors in
21
22 particular are considered to play such critical roles in organizational success (cf., Podsakoff,
23
24
25 Whiting, Podsakoff, & Blume, 2009; Podsakoff, Ahearne, & MacKenzie, 1997), a pay
26
27
system that ultimately inhibits such behaviors could have devastating negative implications
28
29
30 on organizational effectiveness.
31
32
33 Upon first examination, the concern that PFP may undermine helping behavior seems
34
35 to be consistent with the extant theoretical underpinnings of PFP. For example, economic
36
37
38 theories assume that individuals are economically rational and tend to maximize their self-
39
40
interests (Milgrom & Roberts, 1992). As such, the best behavioral strategy for maximizing
41
42
43 one’s economic returns is to increase efforts in accomplishing in-role work assignments that
44
45
46 are formally rewarded in organizations’ compensation system, but such efforts may limit the
47
48 time, energy, and efforts devoted to extra-role behaviors, such as helping other individuals
49
50
51 (Organ, Podsakoff, & MacKenzie, 2006). From a psychological perspective, expectancy
52
53
theory (Lawler, 1971; Vroom, 1964) similarly posits that individuals are demotivated to
54
55
56 engage in behaviors that are not linked with desirable outcomes (i.e., low instrumentality
57
58
59 perception), particularly when other behaviors (i.e., in-role behaviors) are explicitly
60
Page 5 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 rewarded. This perspective is also consistent with social exchange (Blau, 1964) and
5
6
7 psychological contract (Rousseau, 1995) theories, which similarly suggest that organizations
8
9 employing PFP systems may signal an emphasis on short-term oriented, economic-exchange
10
11
12 relationships with their employees. Such signals may make employees feel obligated to
13
14
complete the prescribed work tasks and responsibilities explicitly required in their
15
16
17 employment contracts, which are rewarded by the incentive system, rather than engage in
18
19
20 behaviors extending beyond these in-role obligations (e.g., helping other individuals).
21
22 Yet despite the theoretical and logical arguments suggesting PFP may inhibit helping
23
24
25 behavior, empirical evidence to date has yielded mixed results. Certainly, some studies have
26
27
shown negative relationships between PFP and extra-role behaviors (Bamberger &
28
29
30 Belogolovsky, 2017; Bamberger & Levi, 2009); however, other research has shown non-
31
32
33 significant relationships (Deckop, Mangel, & Cirka, 1999), while other studies have shown
34
35 conditionally positive relationships (Deckop, Merriman, & Blau, 2004; Du & Choi, 2010).
36
37
38 Furthermore, evidence concerning performance measurement calls into question the
39
40
theoretical premise underlying the potential negative effect of PFP on helping behavior.
41
42
43 Specifically, research has suggested that managers may evaluate employees’ task
44
45
46 performance in a heuristic way, causing managers to evaluate employees’ complex
47
48 performance outputs by simply grouping or classifying employees into some meaningful
49
50
51 categories (Murphy & DeNisi, 2008; Roberson, Galvin, & Charles, 2007). In turn, employees
52
53
may engage in opportunistic behaviors to maximize their personal gains (Manthei & Sliwka,
54
55
56 2013) or bolster their leaders’ evaluations (Bolino, 1999; Murphy & Balzer, 1986). Indeed,
57
58
59 evidence suggests that helping or related affiliative behaviors are commonly considered by
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 6 of 72

1
2
3
4 evaluators as a part of the employee performance domain even though such behaviors are
5
6
7 distinct from in-role behaviors (Borman & Motowidlo, 1993; MacKenzie, Podsakoff, &
8
9 Fetter, 1993; Podsakoff et al., 2009; Podsakoff et al, 2000; Rotundo & Sackett, 2002). These
10
11
12 variable findings highlight the need to consider an in-depth theoretical understanding of the
13
14
effect of PFP on helping and specifically underscore the need to consider the relevant context
15
16
17 and contingencies when exploring the influences of PFP (Nyberg et al., 2016).
18
19
20 In this research, we challenge the conventional wisdom that PFP undermines
21
22 employee helping behavior by examining the important yet often-overlooked influence of
23
24
25 performance measurement, which ultimately influences the nature, type, and meaning of the
26
27
job performance being rewarded by a PFP plan (Bommer, Johnson, Rich, Podsakoff, &
28
29
30 MacKenzie, 1995; Muckler & Seven, 1992; Sturman, 2003; Sturman, Cheramie, & Cashen,
31
32
33 2005). We specifically consider the role of performance subjectivity, which we define as an
34
35 attribute of the performance appraisal method reflecting the extent to which the criteria and
36
37
38 indicators used in the evaluation of employee performance are based on (subjective) human
39
40
judgments (cf., Gerhart & Rynes, 2003). Using subjective measures in performance appraisal
41
42
43 systems is highly prevalent across occupations and organizations (Levin, 2003; Rynes,
44
45
46 Gerhart, & Parks, 2005). These subjective measures are usually behavior-based and process-
47
48 oriented, such as the quality of the work/service, work proactivity and innovativeness, and
49
50
51 coworker satisfaction; thus, “they can be used for any type of job” (Gerhart et al., 2009: 256).
52
53
Drawing upon expectancy theory (Vroom, 1964) and the heuristic processing literature
54
55
56 (Kahneman, 2011), we ultimately propose that PFP has a conditional, positive effect on
57
58
59 employee helping behavior in the context of performance subjectivity.
60
Page 7 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 Overall, our research makes several important contributions. First, we challenge the
5
6
7 view that PFP has a simple direct (and generally presumably a negative) effect on employees’
8
9 extra-role behaviors by expanding the theoretical horizon to incorporate the contextual
10
11
12 contingency effects of performance measurement. We thus contribute to the burgeoning body
13
14
of literature that explores PFP’s effect on individual extra-role behaviors (e.g., Bamberger &
15
16
17 Belogolovsky, 2017; Deckop et al., 2004; Du & Choi, 2010). Second, we advance the PFP
18
19
20 literature by emphasizing the importance of the broader context (Nyberg et al., 2016),
21
22 particularly the performance appraisal context (e.g., how performance is measured and
23
24
25 evaluated), in shaping PFP’s influences on employees’ behavioral outcomes (Gerhart et al.,
26
27
2009). More importantly, our research provides more nuanced insights into understanding the
28
29
30 psychological mechanism (i.e., helping-performance expectancy) that explains how the
31
32
33 context of performance measurement moderates PFP’s influences on employees’ helping
34
35 behaviors. Finally, by incorporating the heuristic processing literature and demonstrating the
36
37
38 helping-performance expectancy mechanism that explains when and why PFP motivates
39
40
employees’ helping behaviors, our research provides new insights by applying expectancy
41
42
43 theory to predict not only the amount but also the direction of individual effort.
44
45
46 THEORETICAL BACKGROUND AND HYPOTHESIS DEVELOPMENT
47
48
49 The Incentive Effect of PFP and Employee Extra-role Behavior
50
51 According to expectancy theory (Vroom, 1964), individual work performance is
52
53
positively influenced by one’s perception of the strength of linkage between work effort and
54
55
56 work performance (i.e., expectancy) and the perception of the extent to which work
57
58
59 performance is linked with other desirable outcomes (i.e., instrumentality), such as through
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 8 of 72

1
2
3
4 the PFP system. Despite the extensive empirical evidence regarding PFP’s incentive effect on
5
6
7 in-role task effort and performance (Gerhart, 2017), researchers have cast doubt on PFP’s
8
9 influence on employee extra-role behaviors.
10
11
12 The argument that PFP will negatively affect extra-role behaviors is largely based on
13
14
two perspectives. One key perspective is that engaging in extra-role behaviors may thwart the
15
16
17 completion of in-role tasks and work assignments (Bergeron, 2007), undermining
18
19
20 performance evaluation results, and consequently lowering economic returns under a PFP
21
22 system. This perspective is consistent with the original conceptualizations of OCBs, which
23
24
25 define OCBs as “individual behavior[s] that [are] discretionary, not directly or explicitly
26
27
recognized by the formal reward system, and that in the aggregate promotes the effective
28
29
30 functioning of the organization” (Organ, 1988: 4). If explicitly unrewarded, expectancy
31
32
33 theory would clearly predict that a PFP plan would not motivate OCBs.
34
35 The second key perspective is based on cognitive evaluation theory (Deci, 1975),
36
37
38 which suggests that extrinsic rewards (e.g., PFP) have a “crowding out” effect on intrinsic
39
40
motivation because external controls undermine one’s needs for autonomy or self-
41
42
43 determination (Ryan & Deci, 2000). Based on this crowding out effect, some researchers
44
45
46 have argued that PFP can effectively motivate employee performance only in a context in
47
48 which work is routine, repetitive, and tedious but not in a context in which work is non-
49
50
51 repetitive, creative, and more intrinsically motivated (Amabile, 1988).
52
53
Both perspectives provide a theoretical rationale for why PFP may harm helping
54
55
56 behaviors; however, research suggests that these perspectives are not universally valid. For
57
58
59 example, while the crowding out effect has been repeatedly demonstrated in early laboratory
60
Page 9 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 studies (Deci et al., 1999), it has received little empirical support in organizational contexts.
5
6
7 In fact, extrinsic reward (e.g., PFP) can coexist with or facilitate intrinsic motivation (Gerhart
8
9 & Fang, 2015). Relatedly, the definition of OCBs has evolved, suggesting that OCBs entail
10
11
12 “performance that supports the social and psychological environment in which task
13
14
performance takes place” (Organ, 1997: 95). The revised definition relaxes the condition that
15
16
17 OCBs cannot be rewarded (Podsakoff et al., 2009), calling into question the assumed lack of
18
19
20 expectancy and instrumentality associated with pay and OCBs.
21
22 These theoretical discrepancies underscore the importance of delving into the specific
23
24
25 work context when considering PFP’s incentive effect (Nyberg et al., 2016), as the basis for
26
27
the negative effects of PFP on helping behaviors seems context-sensitive. For example,
28
29
30 scholars posit that a performance-based pay allocation system can motivate employees to
31
32
33 help their coworkers in work contexts in which collaboration is emphasized and required
34
35 (Shaw, 2015; Shaw, Gupta, & Delery, 2002). Indeed, empirical findings indicate that
36
37
38 differentiating team members’ pay levels based on their individual performance increases
39
40
team performance when teams are highly interdependent and, thus, require frequent
41
42
43 collaboration (Trevor, Reilly, & Gerhart, 2012). In their research that focused on the context
44
45
46 of emerging markets as characterized by high collectivism and a socialism-based group
47
48 orientation, Du and Choi (2010) identified a conditionally positive relationship between PFP
49
50
51 and individual helping behavior when the organization had high levels of justice climate
52
53
and/or performance appraisal satisfaction as perceived by employees.
54
55
56 Moreover, the helping-performance conflict is more probable when the rewarded
57
58
59 performance measure is not associated with (or even negatively related to) OCBs. This
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 10 of 72

1
2
3
4 conflict should be most prevalent when performance is result-based and the criteria used to
5
6
7 evaluate the performance measurement is highly objective (Rynes et al., 2005). In contrast, in
8
9 a context in which the performance measurement is behavior-based and process-oriented and
10
11
12 the criteria used to evaluate performance are more subjective, helping other individuals may
13
14
not undermine (and in fact may augment) subjective individual performance assessments
15
16
17 (Rotundo & Sackett, 2002). Basing pay on subjective performance ratings may mitigate the
18
19
20 potentially dysfunctional effects of PFP by reducing employees’ perceptions of risk (Baker,
21
22 Gibbons, & Murphy, 1994) and lowering “the possibility that employees will focus only on
23
24
25 explicitly measured tasks or results at the expense of broader pro-social behaviors,
26
27
organizational citizenship behaviors, or contextual performance” (Gerhart et al., 2009: 256).
28
29
30 To summarize, the extant empirical findings suggest that the nature of performance
31
32
33 matters in understanding PFP’s incentive effect (Gerhart & Fang, 2015). In addition to
34
35 differentiating employees’ performance among in-role routine tasks, in-role creative tasks,
36
37
38 and extra-role behaviors, it is also important to delve into the performance appraisal context
39
40
to examine how performance is evaluated and rewarded (Gerhart et al., 2009; Giarratana,
41
42
43 Mariani, & Weller, 2018; Nyberg et al., 2016). To gain more nuanced insights for an
44
45
46 understanding of the direction and underlying mechanism of the relationship between PFP
47
48 and employee helping behavior in the context of high/low performance subjectivity, we
49
50
51 develop a motivational model (see Figure 1) by drawing upon expectancy theory (Vroom,
52
53
1964) and focusing on the expectancy component of motivation. Specifically, we argue that
54
55
56 the use of a system that offers pay for subjectively evaluated performance enhances the
57
58
59 employee perceived helping-performance expectancy, which we define as the perceived
60
Page 11 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 linkage between engaging in helping behaviors and high performance evaluation (cf., Vroom,
5
6
7 1964); this linkage, in turn, explains the interaction between PFP and performance
8
9 subjectivity on employee helping behavior.
10
11 --------------------------------
12
13
Insert Figure 1 about Here
14 ---------------------------------
15 Revisiting the “Performance” Component of PFP
16
17 The determination of how to define and measure performance is a central issue in
18
19
20 conceptualizing PFP. Two dimensions of the “performance” component of PFP should be
21
22 considered when examining the incentive effect of PFP: 1) the source of performance, i.e.,
23
24
25 whether pay is linked with individual contributions or collective (e.g., group, unit, and
26
27
organization) contributions, and 2) the measurability of performance, i.e., whether
28
29
30 performance measures are objective or subjective (Gerhart et al., 2009).
31
32
33 PFP can be differentiated between individual PFP (e.g., merit pay, sales commissions,
34
35 bonuses, and cash incentives for individualized contributions; Gerhart & Newman, 2020;
36
37
38 Shaw et al., 2002) and collective PFP (e.g., profit sharing, gainsharing, and stock-related PFP
39
40
plans; Gerhart & Newman, 2020; Nyberg, Maltarich, Abdulsalam, Essman, & Cragun, 2018)
41
42
43 based on the source or basis of performance. Individual PFP is advantageous for motivating
44
45
46 work effort and individual performance, whereas collective PFP is advantageous for
47
48 maintaining relational harmony and collaboration (Gerhart & Rynes, 2003) and provoking
49
50
51 team members’ pro-social motivation and collaboration (Super, Li, Ishqaidef, & Guthrie,
52
53
2016). Bamberger and Levi (2009) demonstrated a positive effect of team performance-based
54
55
56 (rather than individual performance-based) pay allocation on team members’ helping
57
58
59
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 12 of 72

1
2
3
4 behaviors. In the present research, we focus on individual PFP (hereafter, PFP) and
5
6
7 reexamine its effect on individual helping behavior.
8
9 Performance measurability is another vital attribute of a performance appraisal system
10
11
12 affecting PFP’s incentive effect (Nyberg et al., 2016), and scholars have long been aware of
13
14
the pros and cons associated with both objective and subjective performance measures
15
16
17 (Bommer et al., 1995; Murphy & Cleveland, 1995; Sturman et al., 2005). The traditional
18
19
20 economic view considers high performance measurability, such as using objective, result-
21
22 based measures that are in alignment with organizational goals (Milgrom & Roberts, 1992). It
23
24
25 is generally advised for a PFP plan using objective performance measures to provide specific
26
27
instructions regarding the behaviors and outcomes that lead to rewards, leading to stronger
28
29
30 instrumentality as perceived by employees (Gerhart et al., 2009; Murphy & Cleveland, 1995).
31
32
33 Fairness and/or legitimacy problems appear to be another major concern associated with
34
35 using subjective measures in a PFP system (Rynes et al., 2005) because in organizational
36
37
38 contexts, supervisors tend to have biases (e.g., favorability, severity, leniency, or centrality)
39
40
in subjectively evaluating employee performance (Marchegiani, Reggiani, & Rizzolli, 2016;
41
42
43 Murphy & Cleveland, 1995). Rewarding individual performance without a legitimate basis
44
45
46 for performance may have no incentive effect on individuals or even detrimental effects on
47
48 collective performance (Shaw, 2015; Shaw et al., 2002; Trevor et al., 2012).
49
50
51 However, evaluating individual performance purely based on a few objective
52
53
measures may also generate unintended and dysfunctional consequences (Gerhart et al.,
54
55
56 2009) due to the narrow focus on quantitative results and outputs. For example, in an R&D
57
58
59 context, Giarratana and colleagues (2018) recently found that rewarding patent inventors’
60
Page 13 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 low-quality inventions (inaccurate objective measures) negatively predicted inventors’ effort
5
6
7 in R&D activities and social interactions with peers. Employees may maximize personal
8
9 utility by “gaming” the objective performance measures (Baker, 1992), leading to the
10
11
12 problem of multitasking tradeoffs (Manthei & Sliwka, 2013) and the misalignment between
13
14
personal gains and collective interests (i.e., agency problem; Jensen & Meckling, 1976).
15
16
17 Subjective performance measures allow evaluators to consider factors outside the employee’s
18
19
20 control (Sturman et al., 2005). Thus, subjective measures may facilitate the effectiveness of a
21
22 PFP system by creating a means-to-ends control system (Baker et al., 1994; Gibbs, Merchant,
23
24
25 Van der Stede, & Vargus, 2004; Levin, 2003). As such, subjective measures, which
26
27
emphasize various factors that influence the final performance output, may have the potential
28
29
30 to encourage individuals’ extra-role behaviors (Gerhart & Rynes, 2003; Gerhart et al., 2009;
31
32
33 Rotundo & Sackett, 2002; Rynes et al., 2005), such as helping coworkers with work-related
34
35 problems. Next, we elaborate on how performance subjectivity can play a crucial role in
36
37
38 affecting the relationship between PFP and employee helping behavior.
39
40 Performance Subjectivity, PFP, and Employee Helping Behavior
41
42 As one exemplar of employees’ extra-role behaviors, coworker helping can be both
43
44 pro-socially and extrinsically motivated (Flynn, 2006; Mossholder, Richardson, & Settoon,
45
46
47 2011; Rioux & Penner, 2001). For example, employees help other colleagues as a means to
48
49
50 gain a personal reputation (Bolino, 1999) and accumulate social capital (Mossholder et al.,
51
52 2011), and/or because of the collaborative climate or norms in their organizations (Bolino,
53
54
55 Turnley, Gilstrap, & Suazo, 2010). Monetary rewards or PFP in particular can also influence
56
57 employees’ motivation toward helping. Specifically, due to the enhanced linkage between
58
59
60 employees’ actual pay level and individual performance, employees tend to regard any
Academy of Management Journal Page 14 of 72

1
2
3
4 resources within an organization, including good relationships with colleagues, as a means to
5
6
7 improve their individual performance and economic returns (Mossholder et al., 2011). Thus,
8
9 employees may be motivated to engage in helping exchanges with other individuals to benefit
10
11
12 their personal interests, such as improving their performance evaluations and seeking career
13
14
advancement (Deckop, Cirka, & Andersson, 2003; De Dreu, 2006; Perlow & Weeks, 2002;
15
16
17 Rotundo & Sackett, 2002).
18
19
20 Employees’ self-benefiting-motivated helping behaviors in a PFP system can be more
21
22 prevalent in the work context of high performance subjectivity. When employees’ individual
23
24
25 performance is less objectively calculated and more subjectively evaluated by their leaders,
26
27
the line between in-role tasks and extra-role behaviors becomes less obvious (Kamdar,
28
29
30 McAllister, & Turban, 2006; Van Dyne, Kamdar, & Joireman, 2008). In other words, there
31
32
33 are fewer conflicts or tradeoffs between engaging in in-role tasks and engaging in helping
34
35 behaviors (Bergeron, 2007). In this context, employees do not decrease their effort in helping
36
37
38 others because the instrumental cost is minimal (Eisenhardt, 1989; Jensen & Meckling,
39
40
1976). Instead, despite the absence of a key performance indicator (KPI) related to helping
41
42
43 behavior on the list, when using subjective measures in a PFP system, individuals may
44
45
46 internalize coworker helping as a part of the individual input valued by the organization and a
47
48 means to maximize economic returns (Baker et al., 1994; Gibbs et al., 2004; Levin, 2003).
49
50
51 Thus, the blurred differentiation between in-role and extra-role behaviors in the context of
52
53
PFP with subjective measures strengthens employees’ instrumental (extrinsic) motivations to
54
55
56 help. We thus propose the following:
57
58
59
60
Page 15 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
Hypothesis 1: Performance subjectivity interacts with PFP to predict employee
4
5 helping behavior such that the effect of PFP on employee helping will be more
6 positive when performance subjectivity is high than when it is low.
7
8
9 Psychological Mechanism toward Helping: Helping-Performance Expectancy
10
11 The conventional wisdom based on expectancy theory (Vroom, 1964) suggests that
12
13 PFP has a negative effect on employee helping behavior as a result of the negative
14
15
16 performance expectancy perception that engagement in coworker helping undermines in-role
17
18 task performance. However, this perspective is based on the assumption that helping
19
20
21 behaviors do not facilitate the acquisition of the rewards provided by the system. Given the
22
23
24 aforementioned direct and indirect benefits of helping behaviors on the performance
25
26 outcomes rewarded by a PFP plan, we hypothesized that PFP has a positive effect on helping
27
28
29 behavior in the context of high performance subjectivity. The key psychological mechanism
30
31 underlying this effect should be exhibited by an explicit belief that there is a link between
32
33
34 helping behavior and individual performance appraisal outcome, which we refer to as
35
36
37 helping-performance expectancy (cf., Vroom, 1964).
38
39 We argue that a context of high performance subjectivity is associated with
40
41
42 employees’ enhanced perceptions of helping-performance expectancy. The dual-processing
43
44 theory (Kahneman, 2011) posits that individuals are likely to engage in a rationality-based,
45
46
47 analytic decision-making process when the situation is certain. In contrast, individuals are
48
49
50 likely to engage in a heuristic decision-making process when the situation is uncertain. In
51
52 performance appraisal systems, the use of objective measures typically includes quantitative
53
54
55 financial outcomes (e.g., revenue and profits) and quantity of work outputs (e.g., products
56
57 and services), creating a situation in which employees are certain regarding the behaviors that
58
59
60 are expected and rewarded. In this situation, according to expectancy theory (Vroom, 1964),
Academy of Management Journal Page 16 of 72

1
2
3
4 employees may focus exclusively on the ends/results and not the process leading to these
5
6
7 ends (Eisenhardt, 1989; Lawler, 1971; Rynes et al., 2005), lowering their beliefs of the
8
9 linkage between coworker helping and the individual performance evaluated.
10
11
12 In contrast, the use of subjective measures in performance appraisal systems typically
13
14
involves various qualitative indicators, such as the satisfaction of stakeholders, the quality of
15
16
17 products and services, process-oriented work attitudes and behaviors, and adherence to
18
19
20 organizational rules, norms, and values. When evaluated by subjective measures, employees
21
22 may lack straightforward instructions regarding the behaviors or outcomes that are expected
23
24
25 and rewarded and ultimately engage in heuristic processing to interpret the performance
26
27
appraisal system. As such, individual behaviors are likely affected by two heuristic biases: 1)
28
29
30 representativeness that individual perceived probability of an event or the frequency of co-
31
32
33 occurrences is upwardly biased by the representativeness attributes (e.g., favorability) and 2)
34
35 availability that individual perceived probability of an event or the frequency of co-
36
37
38 occurrences is upwardly biased by the ease with which the relevant mental operation of recall
39
40
or association can be achieved (Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). We argue that due to these two
41
42
43 heuristic biases, employees are more likely to expect helping behavior to boost performance
44
45
46 evaluation results in the context of high performance subjectivity.
47
48 Specifically, coworker helping is a representative and exemplary OCB that has
49
50
51 indirect beneficial consequences for an organization and is thus highly desired by leaders
52
53
(Podsakoff et al., 2009). In the organizational context, individuals who exhibit frequent
54
55
56 helping behaviors are typically entitled “good soldiers” (Bolino, 1999; Bolino et al., 2010;
57
58
59 Organ, 1988) or “extra-milers” (Li, Zhao, Walter, Zhang, & Yu, 2015). These representative
60
Page 17 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 and desirable titles make individuals easily associate the occurrence of coworker helping with
5
6
7 one’s favorable performance evaluation outcome (Rotundo & Sackett, 2002; Whiting,
8
9 Podsakoff, & Pierce, 2008). In addition, although helping behavior is not formally rewarded
10
11
12 in the organization’s compensation system, its occurrence in the workplace is prevalent as a
13
14
result of employees’ various motives (Rioux & Penner, 2001). Employees are likely to regard
15
16
17 coworker helping as a convenient way to make them look good in front of other individuals,
18
19
20 including their leaders who are in charge of performance evaluation, especially when
21
22 “objective criteria for assessing in-role performance are lacking” (Bolino, 1999: 88). This
23
24
25 ease of association of helping behavior with desirable personal returns in the workplace also
26
27
strengthens individual perceived helping-performance expectancy. Taken together, we argue
28
29
30 that the individual helping-performance expectancy is stronger when subjective measures are
31
32
33 employed than when objective measures are employed in a PFP system.
34
35
36 Hypothesis 2: Performance subjectivity positively predicts employee perceived
37 helping-performance expectancy.
38
39
40
According to expectancy theory (Lawler, 1971; Vroom, 1964), this enhanced
41
42
43 perception of helping-performance expectancy interacts with the PFP intensity to motivate
44
45
46 individual helping behavior. Expectancy theory posits that individuals exert more efforts
47
48 while engaging in a particular behavior when they believe that increased effort leads to better
49
50
51 performance (i.e., expectancy), which, in turn, is associated with more returns (e.g., pay;
52
53
instrumentality) that they value (i.e., valence). In other words, individual motivation toward a
54
55
56 particular behavior is jointly determined by one’s perceptions of expectancy, instrumentality,
57
58
59 and valence concerning this behavior (Vroom, 1964). Although linking pay to individual
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 18 of 72

1
2
3
4 performance that is more subjectively and less objectively measured may generate a weaker
5
6
7 incentive effect on in-role task performance (Nyberg et al., 2016), a high PFP intensity
8
9 accompanied by subjective measures can increase employees’ extra-role helping behaviors
10
11
12 because of enhanced perceptions of helping-performance expectancy (resulting from the use
13
14
of subjective measures) and instrumentality (resulting from the high PFP intensity). Taken
15
16
17 together, we propose:
18
19
20 Hypothesis 3: Helping-performance expectancy interacts with PFP to predict
21
22 employee helping behavior such that the effect of PFP on employee helping will be
23 more positive when the helping-performance expectancy is high than when it is low.
24
25
26
RESEARCH OVERVIEW
27
We conducted three studies to test the hypotheses derived from our research model. In
28
29
30 Study 1, we collected multisource data from different work occupations in nine different
31
32
33 organizations. The objective of Study 1 was to test the interactive effects of PFP with
34
35 performance subjectivity on employee helping behavior (Hypothesis 1). In Study 2, we
36
37
38 replicated Study 1’s findings and further explored the psychological mechanism of helping-
39
40
performance expectancy for the moderation effect of performance subjectivity (Hypotheses 2
41
42
43 and 3). We also strengthened the robustness of the empirical results by controlling for more
44
45
46 individual, task, and organizational pay-related attributes in Study 2. In Study 3, we
47
48 conducted a laboratory experiment to establish causality for our hypothesized relationships.
49
50
51
52
STUDY 1
53 Participants and Procedure
54
55 We collected data from 372 full-time employees in nine Chinese firms. Our research
56
57 team implemented and collected paper-based surveys following three steps in each
58
59
60 organization. First, we obtained information on the organizational structure, pay and
Page 19 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 performance appraisal systems and the criteria (objective vs. subjective measures) used in
5
6
7 evaluating the individual performance of employees working in different departments and
8
9 work groups from the HR managers. Based on this information, we selected jobs in which
10
11
12 either objective measures (e.g., quantity of products) or subjective measures (e.g., managers’
13
14
assessment of work quality) were used in the performance appraisal system as our targeted
15
16
17 research sample. Note that none of these measures explicitly included employees’ helping
18
19
20 behaviors. Second, we invited employees working in these two job categories to participate in
21
22 our survey on a voluntary basis. The participants from these two job categories were
23
24
25 surveyed independently, i.e., at different locations or different time points. Finally, with the
26
27
help of the HR manager in each firm, we collected multisource data. Employees were asked
28
29
30 to report the PFP intensity and provide demographic information, and their direct leaders
31
32
33 were asked to evaluate the employees’ helping behaviors. Table 1 presents the major
34
35 attributes of our sample firms (i.e., industry and PFP practices) and jobs.
36
37
38 Data from 323 matched leader-employee dyads were collected, resulting in a response
39
40
rate of 86.83%. In this final sample, the average age was 31 years (SD = 7.76), and the
41
42
43 average tenure was 4.59 years (SD = 5.17); 38.39% of the final sample was female, and
44
45
46 87.00% had completed a college education.
47
48 --------------------------------
49 Insert Table 1 about Here
50 ---------------------------------
51
52
Measures
53
All scale items underwent a back-translation process (cf. Brislin, 1986) to ensure the
54
55
56 internal validity of our translated scales.
57
58
59
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 20 of 72

1
2
3
4 PFP. Due to the economic reforms in China, PFP systems have been prevalently used
5
6
7 in Chinese firms since 1990 (Du & Choi, 2010). Chinese firms that implement PFP systems
8
9 span nearly all work occupations with varying levels of incentive intensity (i.e., the
10
11
12 proportion of floating wage in one’s total pay package). To regulate employee work behavior
13
14
and enhance work motivation in a timely manner, a performance evaluation is typically
15
16
17 implemented monthly, and the results of the performance evaluation determine the floating
18
19
20 wage that an employee can earn every month.
21
22 Chinese firms typically design a PFP system following three steps. First, the amount
23
24
25 of the overall (monthly) pay package (level) is decided by a systematic, firm-level job
26
27
evaluation that assesses and compares the value (e.g., scope, significance, and importance of
28
29
30 work responsibilities, workloads, and work environment) of every job in the organization.
31
32
33 Second, according to the nature of the work, the jobs are categorized into different series
34
35 (e.g., administration, sales, R&D, and production), and each category is allocated a different
36
37
38 target of the total pay package to be composed of the floating wage, thus setting the incentive
39
40
intensity. For example, administration jobs might have a target of 0.2 (i.e., 20% percent of
41
42
43 floating wage in the total pay package), and sales jobs would have a target of 0.8. Thus, the
44
45
46 nominal floating wage of each job is determined using the total pay level multiplied by the
47
48 incentive intensity. Finally, every employee’s actual floating wage is calculated by
49
50
51 multiplying this nominal floating wage with his/her monthly performance evaluation index
52
53
resulting from the performance evaluation result. Thus, the actual floating wage could be
54
55
56 more or less than the nominal floating wage depending on one’s performance appraisal
57
58
59
60
Page 21 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 results. In addition to this widely used monthly floating wage, bonus, sales commission, and
5
6
7 piece-rate pay are also common PFP practices in Chinese firms (refer to Table 1).
8
9 We used one item to evaluate the intensity of PFP involved in each employee’s pay
10
11
12 package (operationalized as the objective proportion of one’s performance-based variable pay
13
14
in one’s total pay). This item was “Please indicate the proportion of your performance-based
15
16
17 variable pay (e.g., floating wage, incentives, and bonuses) in your total pay using the
18
19
20 following eight categories: (1) 0-5%, (2) 5-15%, (3) 15-30%, (4) 30-50%, (5) 50-70%, (6)
21
22 70-85%, (7) 85-95% and (8) 95-100%”. This one-item measure of PFP has been validated in
23
24
25 prior research (Du & Choi, 2010).
26
27
Performance subjectivity. Following Nyberg et al. (2016), we measured
28
29
30 performance subjectivity using a dummy variable. We assigned a value of 1 to this variable
31
32
33 for employees who worked in jobs that employed subjective measures in their monthly
34
35 performance evaluations. In contrast, we assigned a value of 0 for employees who worked in
36
37
38 jobs that employed objective measures in their monthly performance evaluations. As shown
39
40
in Table 1, the sampled employees from the performance subjectivity category were mainly
41
42
43 from administration jobs, and the sampled employees from the performance objectivity
44
45
46 category were mainly from sales and production jobs.
47
48 We sought to strengthen the robustness of our empirical findings by using direct
49
50
51 measures of performance subjectivity.1 A three-item scale was developed following three
52
53
steps. First, according to the definition of performance subjectivity employed in this research
54
55
56 (i.e., the extent to which the criteria and indicators used in evaluating employee performance
57
58
59 1 We thank the review team for this suggestion.
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 22 of 72

1
2
3
4 are subjective), the first author created three initial items measuring the nature (process-
5
6
7 oriented or result-oriented) of performance, the measurability (subjectivity or objectivity) of
8
9 the KPIs, and the overall perception of subjectivity toward the performance appraisal system.
10
11
12 All initial items were generated by adapting from relevant descriptions that exist in the
13
14
performance management and PFP literature (Aguinis, 2009; Gerhart & Rynes, 2003; Gerhart
15
16
17 et al., 2009; Murphy & Cleveland, 1995; Rynes et al., 2005). Second, two other authors
18
19
20 independently evaluated the content validity of these three items and made modifications.
21
22 Finally, a sample of 163 MBA students from a large Chinese business school was used to test
23
24
25 the reliability (α =.76; omega2 = .77) and validity of these three items; the exploratory factor
26
27
analysis (EFA) results indicated that the three items loaded on one single factor, with factor
28
29
30 loadings that ranged from .74 to .88. These three items were “The criteria or standards used
31
32
33 in evaluating my work performance are behavioral and process-oriented rather than results-
34
35 oriented,” “There isn’t any objective, KPI that is used in evaluating my work performance,”
36
37
38 and “In general, my work performance is subjectively evaluated”.
39
40
We contacted the nine sampled firms again and asked their HR managers to complete
41
42
43 a follow-up survey. In this additional data collection, the HR managers were first asked to
44
45
46 write down the typical job titles that belong to both subjective performance and objective
47
48 performance categories, as listed in Table 1. Then, the mangers were asked to rate each job
49
50
51 category’s performance subjectivity using the three-item scale after shifting the referent. One
52
53
54
55 2 Coefficient omega is a practical alternative measure of internal consistency to alpha that has a lower risk of the
56 overestimation or underestimation of reliability and, in many circumstances, outperforms alpha (e.g., the assumption of scale
57
constancy that commingling or invariant response formats would potentially violate when using alpha does not underpin
58
59 omega) (McNeish, 2018).
60
Page 23 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 sample item was “In general, work performance of employees from this job category is
5
6
7 subjectively evaluated” (1 = strongly disagree and 7 = strongly agree; α = .96 for the nine HR
8
9 managers’ responses on a total of 18 groups of occupation; omega = .97).
10
11
12 Helping behavior. Leaders rated the employees’ helping behavior using Podsakoff et
13
14
al.’s (1997) seven-item scale. Sample items included “This employee is willing to help each
15
16
17 other out if someone falls behind in his/her work” and “This employee is willing to share
18
19
20 his/her expertise with other members of the crew” (1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly
21
22 agree; α = .87; omega = .87).
23
24
25 Controls. At the individual level, we controlled for the employees’ age, gender,
26
27
tenure with the organization, and education, which have been found to influence individual
28
29
30 helping behavior in prior studies (e.g., Van der Vegt, Bunderson, & Oosterhof, 2006; Van der
31
32
33 Vegt, Van de Vliert, & Oosterhof, 2003). In addition, because collective PFP may facilitate
34
35 coworker helping and collaboration (Nyberg et al., 2018) and we did not ask the participants
36
37
38 to report the intensity of collective PFP at the individual level, we added a control for
39
40
collective PFP at the job level in the previously described follow-up data collection.
41
42
43 Specifically, the HR managers were asked to evaluate the incentive intensity of collective
44
45
46 PFP in each job category (subjective vs. objective) using a similar one-item measure of
47
48 individual PFP intensity. At the organizational level, we controlled for the temporal basis of
49
50
51 PFP (i.e., a dummy variable that was coded as 1 for firms employing only monthly PFP
52
53
practices; refer to Table 1). We also controlled for industry differences. Specifically,
54
55
56 according to the Industry Classification Benchmark (ICB), our sampled firms can be
57
58
59 classified into the following six broad industries: Industrials, Consumer Goods, Consumer
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 24 of 72

1
2
3
4 Services, Telecommunications, Health Care, and Financials. We created five dummy
5
6
7 variables to control for these six industry types.
8
9 Results
10
11 The means, standard deviations, reliabilities, and correlations are presented in Table
12
13 2. Given the nested structure of our data (employees nested in separate companies), we used
14
15
16 multilevel path-analytical modeling to test our hypotheses. Model estimation was conducted
17
18 using Mplus 8.4 (Muthén & Muthén, 2019). To facilitate interpretation of the findings, we
19
20
21 group-mean centered Level 1 predicting variables and control variables (except dummies) to
22
23
24 obtain an unbiased estimate of the individual-level relationship (Hofmann & Gavin, 1998;
25
26 Hofmann, Griffin, & Gavin, 2000).
27
28 --------------------------------
29
Insert Table 2 about Here
30
31 ---------------------------------
32
33 We conducted two path-analytical models, as shown in Table 3, to test our interaction
34
35 prediction (Hypothesis 1). As shown in Model 1 (i.e., the main effects model), PFP (B = .04,
36
37
38 SE = .03, n.s.) and performance subjectivity (B = –.07, SE = .07, n.s.) had no main effects on
39
40
employee helping behavior. The results of Model 2 (i.e., the interaction effect model) showed
41
42
43 that the interaction term between PFP and performance subjectivity was positively related to
44
45
46 employee helping behavior (B = .09, SE = .04, p < .05). To further explore this interaction
47
48 effect, we followed Preacher, Curran, and Bauer (2006) and plotted the interaction effect in
49
50
51 Figure 2. The simple slope analyses indicated that PFP was positively related to helping
52
53
behavior among employees working in jobs with subjective performance evaluations
54
55
56 (performance subjectivity = 1; simple slope = .10, SE = .04, p < .05) but not among
57
58
59 employees working in jobs with objective performance evaluations (performance subjectivity
60
Page 25 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 = 0; simple slope = .01, SE = .02, n.s.). These results suggest that in the context of
5
6
7 performance subjectivity, increasing the PFP intensity from 0-5% to 30-50% (e.g., an
8
9 increase from category 1 to category 4 in our PFP measure) is associated with an approximate
10
11
12 9% (= (3.80-3.50)/3.50) increase in helping behavior. Thus, Hypothesis 1 was supported.
13
14 ---------------------------
15 Insert Table 3 & Figure 2 about Here
16
---------------------------
17
18 We conducted additional analyses to test the robustness of our proposed interaction
19
20
21 prediction (i.e., H1). We used the three-item measure of performance subjectivity to replace
22
23
24 the performance subjectivity dummy and reanalyzed the regression. The results in Model 4
25
26 showed that the interaction term, which was created by multiplying the products of two
27
28
29 manifest variables (i.e., PFP and performance subjectivity) that were first centered (Aiken &
30
31 West, 1991), was positively related to employee helping behavior (B = .02, SE = .01, p < .05).
32
33
34 We plotted the interaction effect in Figure 3. The simple slope analyses showed that PFP was
35
36
37 positively related to helping in occupations with high performance subjectivity (performance
38
39 subjectivity = 2.18; simple slope = .10, SE = .03, p < .01) but not among employees working
40
41
42 in occupations with low performance subjectivity (performance subjectivity = –2.18; simple
43
44 slope = –.01, SE = .02, n.s.). These results provide additional support for Hypothesis 1.
45
46 ---------------------------
47
48 Insert Figure 3 about Here
49 ---------------------------
50 Discussion
51
52 Through a field study with multisource data from 323 full-time employees, Study 1
53
54
55 provides support for our hypothesized effect that PFP increases employees’ helping behaviors
56
57 in a work context of high performance subjectivity. The results showed that, without
58
59
60 considering context, PFP seemed to be unrelated to employee helping behaviors. However,
Academy of Management Journal Page 26 of 72

1
2
3
4 when PFP and the performance measurement were simultaneously considered (by examining
5
6
7 their interaction), PFP could lead to helping behaviors when performance is subjectively
8
9 rated. Interestingly, when performance was objectively rated, PFP had no effect on helping
10
11
12 behaviors, thus indicating that PFP may not necessarily harm helping behaviors in many
13
14
organizational contexts.
15
16
17 Clearly, the implications of this study must be tempered by the nature of its
18
19
20 limitations. One critical limitation of this study is that we did not collect data on employees’
21
22 direct perceptions of performance subjectivity. While the use of pay system information
23
24
25 (provided by the organization about the pay system) provides important insights into the
26
27
effects of the PFP system on employee outcomes, it does not allow us to directly test the
28
29
30 mechanisms by which we predict that employee perceptions of pay systems influence their
31
32
33 behaviors. Although our findings support our arguments regarding how PFP and the context
34
35 jointly influence employee helping behaviors, the observed effects of PFP and performance
36
37
38 subjectivity may be affected by unmeasured work-related factors, such as task
39
40
interdependence, indicating the possibility of alternative explanations for our findings.
41
42
43 Hence, we conducted Study 2.
44
45 STUDY 2
46 Participants and Procedure
47
48 We collected data from six Chinese firms (all different firms than used in Study 1).
49
50
51 All surveys were implemented online via an online survey platform operated by a survey
52
53
company. With the support of the chief executive officer (CEO) of each firm, we distributed
54
55
56 the survey introduction, which explained the purpose, content, and format of the survey, to
57
58
59 employees across all jobs. The recruited participants who agreed to enroll in two time-lagged
60
Page 27 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 surveys were rewarded 50 Chinese yuan (approximately 7 US dollars). At Time 1, 199
5
6
7 employees from these six firms completed the survey measuring PFP, perceived performance
8
9 subjectivity, helping-performance expectancy, and the controls. After two weeks, 176 of
10
11
12 these 199 employees completed the Time 2 survey measuring individual helping behavior
13
14
and social desirability measures, resulting in an 88% response rate. There were no significant
15
16
17 differences in age, gender, tenure, and education between the Time 1 sample and the Time 2
18
19
20 sample. All data were collected online and anonymously, and the two waves of data were
21
22 matched by participants’ IDs recognized by the online survey platform.
23
24
25 In this final sample, the average age was 35 years (SD = 5.63), and the average tenure
26
27
was 7.78 years (SD = 4.38); 44.9% of the final sample was female, and 97.7% had completed
28
29
30 a college education. The participants worked in different job categories, including R&D
31
32
33 (25.6%), sales (17.0%), production and operation (11.9%), and administration (45.5%).
34
35 Measures
36
37 PFP. As a precondition for our data collection, all six sampled firms’ PFP systems
38
39 involved individual monthly performance evaluation and incentives. Moreover, none of these
40
41
42 firms’ performance evaluation indicators explicitly included employees’ helping behaviors.
43
44 The measure of PFP was improved in several aspects compared with that in Study 1. First, we
45
46
47 introduced the definition of individual PFP and provided several examples of individual
48
49
50 incentives to participants. Then, we asked employees to recall their monthly pay packages
51
52 and identified the individual incentives that fit with the definition of individual PFP. Finally,
53
54
55 we used one item to evaluate the intensity of individual PFP involved in each employee’s
56
57 monthly pay package. This item was “Please indicate the proportion of your individual
58
59
60 performance-based variable pay (e.g., actual floating wage and monthly bonus) in your total
Academy of Management Journal Page 28 of 72

1
2
3
4 monthly pay using the following eight categories: (1) 0-5%, (2) 5-15%, (3) 15-30%, (4) 30-
5
6
7 50%, (5) 50-70%, (6) 70-85%, (7) 85-95% and (8) 95-100%”. In general, the improved
8
9 measure of PFP specified the basis (individual performance), components, and time frame
10
11
12 (monthly) of PFP.
13
14
Performance subjectivity. We used the same three-item scale of performance
15
16
17 subjectivity that was developed and validated in Study 1. These three items were measured
18
19
20 using a 7-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree) with acceptable
21
22 reliability (α = .77; omega = .78).
23
24
25 Helping-performance expectancy. Drawing upon prior research (e.g., Colquitt &
26
27
Simmering, 1998) measuring expectancy perception (the belief of a linkage between work
28
29
30 effort and performance; Vroom, 1964), we modified the two-item scale of expectancy
31
32
33 developed by Lawler and Suttle (1973) to measure the helping-performance expectancy.
34
35 Specifically, we asked employees to indicate how true it was that helping effort led to a
36
37
38 higher performance evaluation using the following two items: “Trying hard to help others
39
40
increases performance evaluation” and “More helping behavior is associated with better
41
42
43 performance evaluation” (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree; α = .83; omega = .83).
44
45
46 Helping behavior. The participants reported their helping behaviors using the same
47
48 seven-item scale (Podsakoff et al., 1997) employed in Study 1. These seven items were
49
50
51 measured using a 7-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree) and
52
53
showed good reliability in this study (α = .77; omega = .77).
54
55
56 Controls. At the individual level, we controlled for age, gender, tenure with the
57
58
59 organization, and education as indicated in Study 1. In addition, we added individual
60
Page 29 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 difference controls of job level (1 = bottom-level employee, 2 = frontline supervisor, 3 = mid-
5
6
7 level manager, and 4 = executive manager), job type (three dummies created for four
8
9 categorical occupations: R&D occupations, sales occupations, production and operation
10
11
12 occupations, and administrative occupations), internal pay level (one single item from Blau
13
14
(1994): “Compared to relevant employees in my own department/team, may pay level is …”;
15
16
17 1 = a lot less and 5 = a lot more), relative performance (one single item: “Compared to
18
19
20 relevant employees in my own department/team, my task performance level is …”; 1 = much
21
22 better and 5 = much worse), and risk aversion (four-item scale from Gupta and Govindarajan
23
24
25 (1984); 1 = strongly disagree and 7 = strongly agree; α = .84; omega = .84) because prior
26
27
research found that these individual differences affect how employees react to PFP (Deckop
28
29
30 et al., 2004; Fulmer & Shaw, 2018). We also controlled for individual social desirability
31
32
33 using the 10-item scale (1 = strongly disagree and 7 = strongly agree; α = .82; omega = .83)
34
35 developed by Kovacic, Galic, and Jerneic (2014) given that coworker helping was self-
36
37
38 reported by the employees.
39
40
Because the work context of task interdependence was found to affect employees’
41
42
43 reactions to performance-based pay differentials (Shaw, 2015; Shaw et al., 2002; Trevor et
44
45
46 al., 2012), we controlled for employees’ perceived task interdependence using seven items
47
48 adapted from Pearce and Gregersen (1991). A sample item was “I work closely with others in
49
50
51 doing my work” (1 = strongly disagree and 7 = strongly agree; α = .80; omega = .81). For the
52
53
pay-related information, we controlled for collective PFP (monthly based) using the same
54
55
56 one-item measure employed in the robustness analysis of Study 1. Moreover, we controlled
57
58
59 for employee perceived pay system communication using the four-item scale developed by
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 30 of 72

1
2
3
4 Shaw and Gupta (2007). A sample item was “the pay system is clearly communicated to me”
5
6
7 (1 = strongly disagree and 7 = strongly agree; α = .86; omega = .86). Finally, given that our
8
9 six sample firms belonged to three different industries (Industrials, Consumer Goods, and
10
11
12 Technology), we created two industry dummies to control for industry differences at the firm
13
14
level, as employed in Study 1.3
15
16
Results
17
18 The means, standard deviations, reliabilities, and correlations of Study 2 are presented
19
20
21 in Table 4. We conducted a set of single-level confirmatory factor analyses (CFAs) using
22
23
24 Mplus 8.4 (Muthén & Muthén, 2019) to test the discriminant validity among the three focal
25
26 latent variables in our research model (i.e., performance subjectivity, helping-performance
27
28
29 expectancy, and helping behavior). The results showed that our hypothesized three-factor
30
31 model demonstrated a satisfactory fit to the data (χ2 = 94.67, df = 51, p < .001, CFI = .92, TLI
32
33
34 = .90, RMSEA = .07, SRMR = .06). In addition, the decreased fit values and Chi-square
35
36
37 difference tests suggested that the three-factor model was superior to the other models. For
38
39 example, (a) in the two-factor model (grouping performance subjectivity and helping-
40
41
42 performance expectancy), CFI = .65, TLI = .56, RMSEA = .15, SRMR = .14; Δχ2 = 159.96,
43
44 Δdf = 2, p < .001, and (b) in the two-factor model (grouping helping-performance expectancy
45
46
47 and helping behavior), CFI = .79, TLI = .73, RMSEA = .11, SRMR = .08; Δχ2 = 80.43, Δdf =
48
49
50 2, p < .001. Overall, the results support the discriminant validity of our focal constructs.
51
52
53
54 3 For robustness, we reanalyzed our data by removing these controls. The results suggested that although the interaction term
55 of PFP with performance subjectivity on helping behavior was marginally significant (B = .04, SE = .03, p < .10), the simple
56 slope of PPF predicting helping in the condition of high performance subjectivity was significantly positive (simple slope
= .16, p < .01). The interaction term of PFP with helping-performance expectancy on helping behavior was significant (B
57 = .06, SE = .03, p < .05), and the simple slope of PFP predicting helping among employees who perceived a high level of
58 helping-performance expectancy was significantly positive (simple slope = .12, p < .01). These results suggest that our
59 hypotheses were largely supported without adding any control variable in this study.
60
Page 31 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4
5
6 --------------------------------
7 Insert Table 4 about Here
8
9 ---------------------------------
10
11 Hypothesis Testing. Given the nested data, we used multilevel path-analytical
12
13 modeling to test our hypotheses. We first conducted a path-analytical model to show the main
14
15
16 effects of PFP and performance subjectivity on individual helping behavior. As shown in
17
18 Model 1 of Table 5, PFP (B = .08, SE = .05, n.s.) and performance subjectivity (B = .00, SE
19
20
21 = .01, n.s.) were not significantly related to employee helping behavior. Second, we added the
22
23
24 interaction term of PFP with performance subjectivity into the path-analytical model to test
25
26 Hypothesis 1. As shown in Model 2, the interaction term, which was created by multiplying
27
28
29 the products of two manifest variables (i.e., PFP and performance subjectivity) that were first
30
31 centered (Aiken & West, 1991), was significantly and positively related to helping behavior
32
33
34 (B = .07, SE = .02, p < .001). We plotted the interactive effects in Figure 4. Simple slope
35
36
37 analyses showed that PFP was positively related to helping behavior among employees
38
39 working in occupations of high performance subjectivity (performance subjectivity = 1.34;
40
41
42 simple slope = .18, SE = .08, p < .05) but not among employees working in occupations of
43
44 low performance subjectivity (performance subjectivity = –1.34; simple slope = –.01, SE
45
46
47 = .04, n.s.). These results suggest that in the context of high performance subjectivity,
48
49
50 increasing the PFP intensity from 0-5% to 30-50% (e.g., an increase from category 1 to
51
52 category 4 in our PFP measure) is associated with an approximate 11% (= (5.60-5.06)/5.06)
53
54
55 increase in helping behavior. Thus, Hypothesis 1 was again supported.
56
57 ---------------------------
58 Insert Table 5 & Figure 4 about Here
59 --------------------------
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 32 of 72

1
2
3
4 Hypothesis 2 predicted a direct effect of performance subjectivity on helping-
5
6
7 performance expectancy. As shown in Model 4a of Table 5, performance subjectivity was
8
9 positively related to employee perceived helping-performance expectancy (B = .11, SE = .04,
10
11
12 p < .01). Thus, Hypothesis 2 was supported.
13
14
Hypothesis 3 suggested that PFP interacted with helping-performance expectancy to
15
16
17 affect employee helping behavior. Consistent with our prediction, the interaction term, which
18
19
20 was created by multiplying the products of two manifest variables (i.e., PFP and helping-
21
22 performance expectancy) that were first centered (Aiken & West, 1991), was significantly
23
24
25 and positively related to helping behavior (B = .07, SE = .03, p < .05; Model 4b). The simple
26
27
slope analyses (see Figure 5) showed that PFP was positively related to helping behavior
28
29
30 among employees who perceived higher levels of helping-performance expectancy (helping-
31
32
33 performance expectancy = 1.16; simple slope = .17, SE = .08, p < .05) but not among
34
35 employees who perceived lower levels of helping-performance expectancy (helping-
36
37
38 performance expectancy = –1.16; simple slope = –.00, SE = .06, n.s.). Thus, Hypothesis 3
39
40
was supported.
41
42
--------------------------
43
44 Insert Figure 5 about Here
45 --------------------------
46 Discussion
47
48 Through a field study collecting multisource data from 176 full-time employees,
49
50
51 Study 2 replicated Study 1’s findings. That is, PFP alone was not related to helping
52
53
behaviors, but when considered in moderation with performance measurement subjectivity,
54
55
56 PFP was positively related to employee helping when job performance was perceived as
57
58
59 subjectively rated. In addition, Study 2 demonstrated employee perceived helping-
60
Page 33 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 performance expectancy as an underlying psychological mechanism accounting for the
5
6
7 moderating effect of performance subjectivity. Although we consider the findings of Study 2
8
9 more robust than the findings of Study 1 because of our improved measures of the PFP
10
11
12 constructs and enhanced control of individual differences, task attributes, and pay-related
13
14
information, the nature of the data still does not permit the testing of causal conclusions. To
15
16
17 address this limitation, we subsequently conducted a laboratory experiment.
18
19 STUDY 3
20 Participants and Procedure
21
22 One hundred sixty-eight undergraduate students in a Chinese university were
23
24
25 recruited for an experiment via an online advertisement. The participants were informed that
26
27
they would participate in a simulated business work task through which they could earn
28
29
30 monetary rewards. On the experiment day, 160 participants attended, and the participants
31
32
33 were randomly grouped into 40 four-person teams to create a social interaction context in
34
35 which coworker helping is likely to occur. Each team was also randomly assigned a team
36
37
38 leader (confederate) to facilitate the process of the experiment and implement performance
39
40
evaluations and rewards. These 40 confederates, who were senior undergraduates enrolled in
41
42
43 the Compensation Management course taught by the first author, engaged in a pilot
44
45
46 experiment to learn the experimental task (as explained later). These confederates were also
47
48 trained by the first author regarding their responsibilities throughout the experiment.
49
50
51 Before the experiment started, the participants were first gathered in a large classroom
52
53
to learn the experimental task through an oral presentation provided by the first author along
54
55
56 with a paper-based introduction. The participants were informed that they would play the role
57
58
59 of a management consultant for a consulting company and would compete for a consulting
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 34 of 72

1
2
3
4 project by writing a creative English essay using as many words, resolved from an anagram-
5
6
7 solving exercise, as possible. The participants were also informed that the division of teams
8
9 was meant to facilitate the grading of individual performance in a timely manner, there was
10
11
12 no between-team competition, and they would receive no monetary reward based on their
13
14
teams’ collective performance. We finally informed the participants of the responsibility of
15
16
17 their team leaders, who would provide them with detailed information regarding performance
18
19
20 evaluation and monetary rewards and monitor their behaviors throughout the task following
21
22 the competition rules. After learning about the experimental task and procedure, eight
23
24
25 participants, including all four participants from one team, withdrew (e.g., emergent personal
26
27
issues). Three teams’ data were excluded from our analyses because the leaders
28
29
30 (confederates) of these teams reported significant violations of the competition rules (e.g.,
31
32
33 alliance between team members to resist the forced distribution of performance ratings)
34
35 among the team members. As a result, the remaining 140 participants (Mage = 20, 36.4% were
36
37
38 male) were from 32 four-person teams and four three-person teams. According to our prior
39
40
random grouping, participants under six different experiment manipulations (explained
41
42
43 below) were led to six different rooms to complete the experimental tasks independently.
44
45
46 We employed a 3 (PFP intensity: high/moderate/low) by 2 (performance evaluation
47
48 criteria: objective measures vs. subjective measures) cross-sectional design. The experimental
49
50
51 task employed was based on the anagram-solving exercise developed by Belogolovsky,
52
53
Bamberger, Alterman, and Wagner (2016) and was also used in Bamberger and
54
55
56 Belogolovsky’s (2017) experimental study on PFP. We combined this task with an essay
57
58
59 writing task to generate a new creative writing task to enable manipulations for both objective
60
Page 35 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 and subjective performance (refer to the manipulation section). This 40-minute task involved
5
6
7 anagram solving and essay writing. In each team, each participant was presented a unique
8
9 version of a 30-item anagram. Following Bamberger and Belogolovsky (2017), we allowed
10
11
12 participants to seek help from team members in solving their own anagrams and borrow other
13
14
individuals’ resolved words in writing their essays. To increase external validity (e.g., helping
15
16
17 others might temporally conflict with performing individual tasks), the participants were
18
19
20 forbidden from making alliances (e.g., exchanging anagrams with each other and solving all
21
22 team members’ anagrams together). Instead, we asked the participants to write on a “helping
23
24
25 card” to lend help to other individuals either to solve the other individuals’ anagrams or lend
26
27
their own solved words to other individuals. Each participant had only 10 helping cards, and
28
29
30 each card could be used to deliver only one word.
31
32 Manipulations and Measures
33
34 PFP. We manipulated three conditions of PFP with varying levels of incentive
35
36
37 intensity (20%, 50%, and 80%). To maximize the experiment’s external validity, the average
38
39 pay level for a 40-minute task (plus a 20-miniute survey) among four participants in a team
40
41
42 was set to 50 Chinese yuan (approximately 7 US dollars). In the high PFP condition, the
43
44 participants were informed that they would receive 10 yuan in fixed pay as a reward for their
45
46
47 participation. In addition, based on the outcome of individual performance rated by the
48
49
50 leader, they would additionally receive 64, 48, 32, and 16 yuan (for performance grades A, B,
51
52 C, and D, respectively) as a reward for their individual performance. In the moderate PFP
53
54
55 condition, the fixed pay was 25 yuan, and the additional performance-based pay was 40, 30,
56
57 20, and 10 yuan (for performance grades A, B, C, and D, respectively). Finally, in the low
58
59
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 36 of 72

1
2
3
4 PFP condition, the fixed pay was 40 yuan, and the additional performance-based pay was 16,
5
6
7 12, 8, and 4 yuan (for performance grades A, B, C, and D, respectively).
8
9 Performance subjectivity. Following Belogolovsky and Bamberger (2014), we
10
11
12 manipulated the performance subjectivity by randomly dividing the participants into two
13
14
performance evaluation conditions: objective measures vs. subjective measures. The
15
16
17 participants in the objective condition were told that the rating of the task performance would
18
19
20 be calculated objectively, entirely contingent on the number of solved words used in their
21
22 completed creative essays. The number of solved anagrams and the length of the essay, which
23
24
25 are also objective indicators, would be considered when two or more participants used the
26
27
same number of solved words in writing their essays. In contrast, the participants in the
28
29
30 subjective condition were told that the rating of the task performance would be assessed
31
32
33 subjectively based on an array of quantitative (e.g., number of used words solved from the
34
35 anagrams and length of essay) and qualitative (e.g., logical consistency and creativity)
36
37
38 indicators of the essay and the leader’s observation of the individual’s performance. Under
39
40
both conditions, individual performance received one of four performance grades (A-D), and
41
42
43 each performance grade should have been given to no more than one team member.
44
45
46 After completing the task, the participants were asked to complete a questionnaire that
47
48 included measures of helping-performance expectancy, team members’ helping behavior, and
49
50
51 manipulation checks. All of the following scales were measured using a seven-point Likert
52
53
scale (1 = “strongly disagree” to 7 = “strongly agree”).
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
Page 37 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 Helping-performance expectancy. Helping-performance expectancy was measured
5
6
7 using the same two-item scale employed in Study 2, and the reliability scores of these two
8
9 items was α = .89 and omega = .89 in this study.
10
11
12 Helping behavior. We used two approaches to measure the participants’ helping
13
14
behaviors during the task. First, we employed a social network approach and asked the
15
16
17 participants to rate the extent to which each of their teammates lent help to him/her during the
18
19
20 task. Considering the complexity in answering a round-robin questionnaire and the fitness of
21
22 measures with our experimental context, we used a three-item scale of receiving help
23
24
25 developed by Uy, Lin, and Ilies (2017). A sample item was “Team member X went out of
26
27
his/her way to help me in the task” (α = .74; omega =.76). We then calculated each
28
29
30 participant’s helping behavior by averaging all other team members’ scores on their
31
32
33 perceived help received from the focal person for these three items. Second, we counted the
34
35 number of helping cards each participant used in helping other individuals solve their
36
37
38 anagrams and lending their own solved words to other individuals during the task as an
39
40
objective measure of helping behavior. Subjectively measured helping was highly correlated
41
42
43 with objectively measured helping in this experiment (r = .50, p < .001).
44
45
46 Manipulation check. The two manipulations in this experiment were both objective
47
48 HR policies, and we utilized two methods to guarantee that the participants accurately
49
50
51 understood these policies. First, before the task started, we provided each participant a job
52
53
description specifying the performance evaluation criteria, rating method, and reward policy
54
55
56 (e.g., PFP intensity). Second, we asked the team leaders to explain these HR practices to each
57
58
59 team member directly and ensure that the participants’ understandings were correct.
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 38 of 72

1
2
3
4 The leaders asked every team member to answer the following question to check the
5
6
7 validity of the PFP manipulation: “On average, what is the proportion of the performance-
8
9 based bonus in the total monetary reward for this task among all team members?” The
10
11
12 participants answered correctly as follows: 80% under the high PFP condition, 50% under the
13
14
moderate PFP condition, and 20% under the low PFP condition.
15
16
17 Performance subjectivity was measured using the same three-item scale employed in
18
19
20 Study 2. The reliability of these three items was .72 (α) and .75 (omega) in this study. The
21
22 participants under the performance subjectivity condition (M = 4.20, SD = .98) gave higher
23
24
25 scores on these measures than the participants under the performance objectivity condition
26
27
(M = 3.07, SD = 1.04, t (136) = 6.57, p < .001). These results together indicate that all
28
29
30 manipulations were effective.
31
32 Results
33
34 Given that PFP was manipulated as an ordinal multicategorical variable that consisted
35
36
37 of three different experimental conditions, we utilized Hayes and Montoya’s (2017)
38
39 recommended regression analysis of testing moderation effects for multicategorical
40
41
42 independent variables. Specifically, according to Darlington and Hayes (2017), we adopted
43
44 two different approaches (i.e., the Helmert coding and the indicator coding) to create two sets
45
46
47 of dummies variables for the three PFP conditions manipulated in this study. The regression
48
49
50 coefficients generated by Helmert coding can test the difference in helping behavior between
51
52 the low PFP condition and the unweighted means for the moderate and high PFP conditions
53
54
55 (coefficient of Helmert-D1) and the difference in helping behavior between the means of the
56
57 moderate and high PFP conditions (coefficient of Helmert-D2). In addition, the regression
58
59
60 coefficients generated by indicator coding can test the difference in helping between the
Page 39 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 means of the low and moderate PFP conditions (coefficient of Indicator-D1) and the
5
6
7 difference between the means of the low and high PFP conditions (coefficient of Indicator-
8
9 D2). By employing these two coding approaches (cf., Schabram, Robinson, & Cruz, 2018),
10
11
12 our analyses can obtain more nuanced findings regarding the differences in helping behavior
13
14
across various comparison conditions. Table 6 shows the descriptive statistics and
15
16
17 correlations for all variables in Study 3.
18
19 ---------------------------
20 Insert Table 6 about Here
21
22 ---------------------------
23
24 Testing of Hypothesis 1 was conducted using the PROCESS macro for SPSS with
25
26 1,000 resamples (Hayes, 2012; Hayes & Montoya, 2017). Regression results are presented in
27
28
29 Table 7. When using objective helping behavior as the dependent variable, the results
30
31 suggested that in the condition of high performance subjectivity (PS = 1), objective helping
32
33
34 behavior among the participants in the high PFP condition (M = 6.38) was higher than that
35
36
37 among the participants in the low (M = 4.38: Indicator-D2 = 2.00, SE = .88, p < .05, 95% CI
38
39 = [.25, 3.75]) and moderate (M = 3.57: Helmert-D2 = 2.80, SE = .91, p < .01, 95% CI = [1.00,
40
41
42 4.61]) PFP conditions. However, the difference in objective helping between the moderate
43
44 and low PFP conditions was not significant (Indicator-D1 = –.80, SE = .91, n.s., 95% CI = [–
45
46
47 2.61, 1.00]). In the condition of objective performance measurement (PS = 0), the results
48
49
50 suggested that the differences in objective helping between the high (M = 4.96) and low (M =
51
52 4.83) PFP conditions (Indicator-D2 = .13, SE = .89, n.s., 95% CI = [–1.63, 1.90]), the high
53
54
55 and moderate (M = 5.50) PFP conditions (Helmert-D2 = –.54, SE = .88, n.s., 95% CI = [–
56
57 2.29, 1.20]), and the moderate and low PFP conditions (Indicator-D1 = .67, SE = .89, n.s.,
58
59
60 95% CI = [–1.09, 2.44]) were all insignificant. These differences were contrasted and
Academy of Management Journal Page 40 of 72

1
2
3
4 graphed in Figure 6. Thus, Hypothesis 1 was again supported. When using subjective helping
5
6
7 behavior as the dependent variable, however, these differences were no longer significant
8
9 (see Table 7 for detailed information on the significance level of the regression coefficients).
10
11
12 Altogether, these results suggest that performance subjectivity moderated the effect of PFP
13
14
on individuals’ objective helping behaviors but not subjective helping behaviors.
15
16
---------------------------
17
18 Insert Table 7 and Figure 6 about Here
19 ---------------------------
20
21 Hypothesis 2 proposed that performance subjectivity has a positive effect on helping-
22
23
24 performance expectancy. We conducted an analysis of variance (ANOVA) to test this effect.
25
26 The results suggested that individuals perceived helping-performance expectancy to be
27
28
29 significantly higher (F (1, 138) = 4.68, p < .05, η2 = .03) under the performance subjectivity
30
31 condition (M = 5.62) than the performance objectivity condition (M = 5.25). Thus,
32
33
34 Hypothesis 2 was supported.
35
36
37 Hypothesis 3, which suggested that PFP interacted with helping-performance
38
39 expectancy to affect employee helping behavior, was tested using the PROCESS macro for
40
41
42 SPSS with 1,000 resamples (Hayes, 2012; Hayes & Montoya, 2017). The results shown in
43
44 Table 7 indicate that when individuals perceived that helping-performance expectancy was
45
46
47 high (helping-performance expectancy = 6.45), objective helping behavior among the
48
49
50 participants under the high PFP condition (M = 6.89) was marginally higher (Indicator-D2 =
51
52 1.58, SE = .87, p < .10, 95% CI = [–.13, 3.30]) than that among the participants under the low
53
54
55 PFP condition (M = 5.31) and significantly higher (Helmert-D2 = 2.25, SE = .93, p < .05,
56
57 95% CI = [.41, 4.10]) than that among the participants under the moderate PFP condition (M
58
59
60 = 4.64); however, the difference in objective helping between the moderate and low PFP
Page 41 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 conditions was not significant (Indicator-D1 = –.67, SE = .89, n.s., 95% CI = [–2.44, 1.10]).
5
6
7 When individuals perceived that helping-performance expectancy was low (helping-
8
9 performance expectancy = 4.40), the differences in objective helping between the high (M =
10
11
12 4.86) and low (M = 3.47) PFP conditions (Indicator-D2 = 1.39, SE = .92, n.s., 95% CI =
13
14
[–.43, 3.21]), the high and moderate (M = 4.56) PFP conditions (Helmert-D2 = .29, SE = .85,
15
16
17 n.s., 95% CI = [–1.39, 1.98]), and the moderate and low PFP conditions (Indicator-D1 =
18
19
20 1.10, SE = 1.00, n.s., 95% CI = [–.88, 3.07]) were all insignificant.
21
22 When using subjective helping behavior as the dependent variable, however, the
23
24
25 differences across the PFP conditions were no longer significant when the individual
26
27
perceived helping-performance expectancy was high (see Table 7 for detailed information on
28
29
30 the significance level of the regression coefficients). When individuals perceived that
31
32
33 helping-performance expectancy was low, none of these differences were significant, except
34
35 for the difference in subjective helping behavior between the high (M = 4.66) and moderate
36
37
38 (M = 5.43) PFP conditions, indicating that PFP has a significant negative effect on subjective
39
40
helping behavior (Helmert-D2 = –.77, SE = .27, p < .01, 95% CI = [–1.30, –.23]). Taken
41
42
43 together, these results suggest that Hypothesis 3 was supported when using objective helping
44
45
46 behavior as the dependent variable.4
47
48 ---------------------------
49 Insert Figure 7 about Here
50 ---------------------------
51
52
Discussion
53
54
55
56
57 4 For robustness, we considered the review team’s suggestion and reanalyzed our data by controlling each participant’s
58 performance grade as rated by the leader. The empirical results, which are available from the corresponding author upon
59 request, suggested that our findings remained unaffected.
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 42 of 72

1
2
3
4 Through a laboratory experiment, Study 3 helps establish causality regarding the
5
6
7 positive effect of performance subjectivity on perceived helping-performance expectancy and
8
9 the interactive effects between PFP and performance subjectivity and between PFP and
10
11
12 helping-performance expectancy on individual objective helping behavior. Moreover, the
13
14
results suggest that the positive effect of PFP on helping behavior under the condition of high
15
16
17 performance subjectivity is more likely significant at moderate to high levels of PFP
18
19
20 intensity. Altogether, these findings suggest that the effect of PFP on individual helping
21
22 behavior depends on both the degree of PFP intensity and how helping behavior is measured.
23
24
25 Despite the above causal effects demonstrated in Study 3, these experimental findings
26
27
should be interpreted with caution due to the limitations of the experimental design. For
28
29
30 instance, to effectively manipulate objective and subjective measures, additional performance
31
32
33 requirements (e.g., logical consistency and creativity of the essay) were added under the
34
35 subjective evaluation condition, leading to the possibility that the moderating effect of
36
37
38 performance subjectivity was confounded by the influences of additional performance
39
40
requirements. Although these additional performance requirements are theoretically unrelated
41
42
43 to individual helping behavior, future research is encouraged to design different experimental
44
45
46 tasks and different manipulations of performance measurement (subjectivity vs objectivity) to
47
48 replicate our findings. In addition, our measure of objective helping behavior may be biased
49
50
51 by the issue of truncation, i.e., each participant had a maximum of 10 helping cards.
52
53
Nonetheless, given that the average number of helping card the participants used was only
54
55
56 half of the total number of cards (M = 4.96; SD = 3.12), we are confident regarding the
57
58
59
60
Page 43 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 validity. Considering the pros and cons of each study, next, we discuss the general theoretical
5
6
7 and practical implications of all three studies’ findings.
8
9 GENERAL DISCUSSION
10
11 The main purpose of our research was to challenge the conventional wisdom that PFP
12
13
14 motivates employees’ in-role task performance at the expense of undermining their
15
16
motivations to engage in extra-role citizenship behaviors (e.g., coworker helping). Drawing
17
18
19 upon expectancy theory, we highlighted the role of performance measurement and propose a
20
21
22 motivation model that predicts the positive effect of PFP on employees’ helping behaviors
23
24 when employee performance is subjectively evaluated. The empirical results of the two field
25
26
27 studies and an experimental study provided convergent support for the interactive effects
28
29
between PFP and performance subjectivity on individual helping behavior. The results further
30
31
32 indicated that the individual perceived helping-performance expectancy was the underlying
33
34
35 psychological mechanism that accounted for why a pay-for-subjectively-evaluated-
36
37 performance system can increase coworker helping. We next discuss the theoretical and
38
39
40 practical implications of these findings.
41
42 Theoretical Implications
43
44 The first theoretical contribution of our research is that we demonstrate a positive
45
46 effect of PFP on employee helping behavior in a context where individual performance is
47
48
49 subjectively (as opposed to objectively) measured, evaluated, and rewarded. This finding
50
51
52
provides nuanced insights into understanding the inconclusive empirical results regarding the
53
54 effect of PFP on individual helping behavior, as documented in the extant literature
55
56
57 (Bamberger & Belogolovsky, 2017; Deckop et al., 1999, 2004; Du & Choi, 2010).
58
59 Specifically, in a context in which individual performance is subjectively measured, the
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 44 of 72

1
2
3
4 utilization of a PFP system may not cause a conflict between engaging in in-role tasks and
5
6
7 engaging in helping behaviors by employees; instead, PFP increases helping among
8
9 colleagues (Deckop et al., 2004; Du & Choi, 2010). This conditional positive effect of PFP
10
11
12 on helping behavior also enriches the current understanding of the relational consequences of
13
14
a PFP system in organizations (Belogolovsky & Bamberger, 2014; Nyberg et al., 2016).
15
16
17 Relatedly, we address past calls to pay more attention to the pivotal role of work
18
19
20 context in advancing the theoretical development and empirical findings regarding PFP’s
21
22 effectiveness (Gupta & Shaw, 2014; Nyberg et al., 2016). Specifically, departing from prior
23
24
25 studies that exclusively focused on the “pay” component of PFP (Gerhart et al., 2009; Rynes
26
27
et al., 2005), we focus on the “performance” component of PFP and highlight the crucial
28
29
30 context of performance appraisal (i.e., performance measurement) in shaping PFP’s effects
31
32
33 on individual work behaviors. The extant empirical evidence in the PFP literature “comes
34
35 primarily from studies that use results-based (objective) measures of performance and from
36
37
38 settings where work is simple, easy to measure, and not interdependent” (Gerhart & Fang,
39
40
2014: 42), ignoring the fact that employees’ work performance and rewards may be highly
41
42
43 dependent on their leaders’ subjective evaluations. The economic approach in the PFP
44
45
46 literature has increasingly emphasized the use of subjective measures in designing a more
47
48 effective PFP system by preventing employees from “gaming” the objective performance
49
50
51 indicators (Baker et al., 1994; Gibbs et al., 2004; Levin, 2003). Our research findings
52
53
complement these economic studies by directly demonstrating the advantages (i.e., increased
54
55
56 coworker helping) of using subjective measures in a PFP system, highlighting the importance
57
58
59
60
Page 45 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 of scrutinizing employees’ enactment of PFP’s signaling information in the workplace
5
6
7 (Belogolovsky & Bamberger, 2014; Maltarich et al., 2017).
8
9 Notably, our findings do not suggest that subjective performance evaluations are
10
11
12 superior to objective performance evaluations. In fact, the use of subjective performance
13
14
evaluations may cause fairness problems and additional management costs due to managers’
15
16
17 intentional and unintentional (e.g., heuristic) biases in evaluating employee performance as
18
19
20 reported in prior studies (Gerhart et al., 2009). Future research can contribute to the
21
22 performance management literature by exploring the boundary conditions of the effectiveness
23
24
25 of different performance measures to understand when subjective or objective measures
26
27
appear to be a better choice. Notably, although the results of Study 3 suggest that the
28
29
30 beneficial outcomes associated with the use of subjective performance measures may
31
32
33 outweigh their inferior results only when the PFP intensity is at moderate to high levels, the
34
35 companies surveyed in Studies 1 and 2, on average, still employed a low to moderate level of
36
37
38 PFP intensity. Therefore, more field data from various occupational, industrial, and cultural
39
40
samples are needed to replicate and extend our empirical findings.
41
42
43 In addition, our research provides nuanced insight enhancing our understanding of the
44
45
46 psychological mechanism (i.e., helping-performance expectancy) that transmits the
47
48 interactive effects between the PFP intensity and performance subjectivity on employee
49
50
51 helping behavior. Our research responds to recent calls for explorations of employees’
52
53
psychological reactions under different PFP systems (Maltarich et al., 2017) and integrating
54
55
56 interdisciplinary (e.g., economic and psychological) perspectives in examining PFP’s
57
58
59 contextualized influences (Larkin et al., 2012; Maltarich et al., 2017; Nyberg et al., 2016).
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 46 of 72

1
2
3
4 The psychological mechanism of helping-performance expectancy as demonstrated in our
5
6
7 research has notable implications for future empirical explorations in the PFP literature. Prior
8
9 PFP research drawing upon expectancy theory to examine PFP’s incentive effect exclusively
10
11
12 focused on the motivational component of instrumentality, i.e., the perceived strength of the
13
14
linkage between individual performance and pay, and sought to identify the context in which
15
16
17 PFP’s incentive effect could be strengthened by enhancing employees’ instrumentality
18
19
20 perceptions (Maltarich et al., 2017; Nyberg et al., 2016). Our model extends this stream of
21
22 research by emphasizing and expanding the motivational component of expectancy, that is,
23
24
25 the belief of a linkage between work effort and performance (Vroom, 1964).
26
27
Relatedly, our conceptualization and examination of helping-performance expectancy
28
29
30 contribute to the advancement of expectancy theory. Given that the PFP intensity positively
31
32
33 affects individual instrumentality perception, this finding provides strong support to the
34
35 central tenet of expectancy theory—individual behavior is motivated by the interaction but
36
37
38 not the mere summation of one’s instrumentality and expectancy perceptions (Vroom, 1964).
39
40
Moreover, relying on the assumption that human behavior is subjectively rational, expectancy
41
42
43 theory has been questioned for its ignorance of individual cognitive limits on rationality and
44
45
46 being “fixated on the amount rather than the direction of effort” (Vroom, 1995: 20). We
47
48 attribute the formation of helping-performance expectancy partially to the individual heuristic
49
50
51 biases activated in the context of high performance subjectivity. Our integration of the
52
53
heuristic processing literature (Kahneman, 2011) provides new insights into applying
54
55
56 expectancy theory to predict the direction rather than only the amount of individual behavior.
57
58
59
60
Page 47 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 Finally, we suggest that expectancy perceptions are not restricted to only in-role work
5
6
7 effort; instead, these perceptions originate from various behavioral domains (e.g., helping
8
9 behavior) that are assumed to boost one’s performance. By explicitly or implicitly
10
11
12 incorporating extra-role behavioral domains into the performance appraisal system,
13
14
organizations employing related PFP systems can motivate desired behaviors by enhancing
15
16
17 domain-specific expectancy perceptions. Given that a large stream of PFP research has
18
19
20 examined PFP effectiveness through an intrinsic/extrinsic motivation lens (Gerhart & Fang,
21
22 2015), our research redirects scholarly attention to underscore the positive implications of
23
24
25 employees’ domain-specific motivations related to various workplace behaviors.
26
27 Practical Implications
28
29 Beyond our theoretical implications, two key practical implications should be
30
31 highlighted. First, although prior research suggested that subjective measures suffered from
32
33
34 “idiosyncratic rater effects” (Buckingham & Goodall, 2015), undermined the incentive effect
35
36
37 of a PFP system (Belogolovsky & Bamberger, 2014; Murphy & Cleveland, 1995), and
38
39 introduced additional financial and relational costs to the organization due to the so-called
40
41
42 “administrative problems” (Gerhart & Milkovich, 1992), our research found one possible
43
44 benefit of using subjective measures in the PFP system—motivating employee helping
45
46
47 behavior. By weighing the pros and cons, we suggest that a performance appraisal system
48
49
50 that employs hybrid indicators, including both objective and subjective measures, is likely to
51
52 motivate overall outcomes. For example, a balanced scorecard (Kaplan & Norton, 1992), if
53
54
55 used appropriately, has a better potential than the KPI technique, which is purely based on
56
57 objective and financial measures, to promote employees’ comprehensive work outcomes
58
59
60 (Ittner, Larcker, & Meyer, 2003). However, our results should be interpreted with caution in
Academy of Management Journal Page 48 of 72

1
2
3
4 practice; when launching a behavior-oriented performance appraisal method (e.g.,
5
6
7 behaviorally anchored rating scales; Bernardin & Smith, 1981) among employees whose
8
9 performance outcomes are not result-based or objectively measured, leaders should be
10
11
12 systematically trained to avoid personal biases and evaluate employees’ performances as
13
14
objectively as possible.
15
16
17 Second, our motivational model of PFP and helping suggests that individual perceived
18
19
20 helping-performance expectancy is the core psychological impetus for helping behavior.
21
22 Thus, organizations can motivate employees to perform above and beyond their prescribed
23
24
25 in-role task obligations by activating their beliefs that “being good soldiers” is associated
26
27
with desirable individual outcomes and monetary rewards. For example, organizations can
28
29
30 incorporate extra-role behaviors, such as coworker helping, into their formal performance
31
32
33 appraisal system to strengthen the employee perceived helping-performance expectancy. In
34
35 addition, organizations can design more incentive practices that directly reward employees’
36
37
38 extra-role behaviors, such as a reward-for-voice plan, and bonuses for extraordinary extra-
39
40
role contributions.
41
42
Limitations and Directions for Future Research
43
44 Despite these theoretical and practical contributions, several limitations of this
45
46
47 research should be noted. First, although we operationalized PFP and performance
48
49
50 subjectivity differently across the three studies in our research, the PFP system examined was
51
52 short-term rather than long-term oriented (e.g., deferred compensation or stock option plans).
53
54
55 Additionally, we consider coworker helping a within-team phenomenon in our research, and
56
57 we did not consider team-level influences on employees’ helping behaviors in our conceptual
58
59
60 model. Our data were also unable to differentiate employees’ within-team helping behaviors
Page 49 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 from their cross-team helping behaviors. Given that team leaders are typically the rater of
5
6
7 employee performance, it is likely that our motivational model can be applied only to
8
9 predicting employees’ within-team helping behaviors. Notably, the motivational model of
10
11
12 PFP and helping behavior in Study 3, which is the only study conducted within a team
13
14
setting, was not supported when helping behavior was subjectively evaluated by teammates.
15
16
17 This finding is intriguing because teammates, as the help-recipients, should have accurate
18
19
20 information regarding all team members’ helping behaviors. One explanation for these
21
22 insignificant results is that the experiment participants may be short-term oriented, pay less
23
24
25 attention to relationship building and social interactions, and, hence, are less likely to value
26
27
and recognize the helping behavior of other individuals. Thus, compared with leaders,
28
29
30 coworkers in certain settings provide a downward biased evaluation of their colleagues’
31
32
33 instrumentally motivated helping behaviors. We encourage future research to incorporate a
34
35 real team design to unveil the role of teams (and teammates) on the relationship between PFP
36
37
38 and helping.
39
40
Although we controlled for the influences of collective PFP and perceived task
41
42
43 interdependence, future research is encouraged to examine the influence of more contextual
44
45
46 factors at the team level, such as leadership. Leaders’ direct modeling and enactment of an
47
48 organization’s HR practices can enhance the effectiveness of HR practices because leaders
49
50
51 have immediate and frequent social interactions with employees and, thus, are regarded as
52
53
representatives of the organization (Purcell & Hutchinson, 2007). Positive leadership
54
55
56 behaviors, such as transformational behaviors (Weller, Süß, Evanschitzky, & von
57
58
59 Wangenheim, 2019) and visionary behaviors (Ates, Tarakci, Porck, van Knippenberg, &
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 50 of 72

1
2
3
4 Groenen, 2018), are found to strengthen employee consensus regarding HR practices, which,
5
6
7 in turn, facilitates the execution and effectiveness of these HR practices. In contrast, frontline
8
9 managers’ leadership behaviors can also lead employees to hold negative attitudes toward HR
10
11
12 practices when these managers are not strategically aligned with the CEO (Ates et al., 2018).
13
14
The PFP efficacy is also likely affected by leadership because leaders can affect employee
15
16
17 work motivation by enacting different leadership behaviors (Han et al., 2015). One central
18
19
20 premise of our model is the effect by which performance subjectivity enhances individual
21
22 helping-performance expectancy. However, this premise may be challenged if the leader’s
23
24
25 implicit beliefs in evaluating employee performance are strictly against extra-role behaviors.
26
27
We encourage future studies to advance our model by adding the role of leadership.
28
29
30 We also encourage future research to perform more nuanced examinations of the
31
32
33 “performance” component of PFP, particularly the performance appraisal criteria and
34
35 accuracy in designing objective and subjective indicators, when exploring PFP’s influences
36
37
38 on employees’ workplace behaviors. For example, although our research demonstrates the
39
40
advantage of using subjective performance measures in facilitating PFP’s positive effect on
41
42
43 employee helping behavior, it is unknown whether, why, and when the use of objective
44
45
46 performance measures may cause other problems. Recent work suggests that the legitimacy
47
48 and accuracy of objective performance indicators play a vital role in affecting the
49
50
51 effectiveness of the performance appraisal and reward system (Giarratana et al., 2018;
52
53
Manthei & Sliwka, 2013). Thus, we urge future studies to examine how different dimensions
54
55
56 of performance evaluation (e.g., performance basis, measurability, and indicator accuracy or
57
58
59 legitimacy) work separately and jointly to shape PFP’s effectiveness.
60
Page 51 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
4 Another theoretical limitation of our research is that we did not consider alternative
5
6
7 mediating mechanisms in our conceptual model, such as justice perceptions associated with
8
9 the implementation of PFP in organizations (Park, Kim, & Sung, 2017; Trevor & Wazeter,
10
11
12 2006). Moreover, recent research has underscored the affect-based mechanism in explaining
13
14
PFP’s influences on individual behaviors (Belogolovsky & Bamberger, 2014; Maltarich et
15
16
17 al., 2017). Thus, further research should integrate cognitive and affective mechanisms to
18
19
20 explore employees’ reactions to a PFP system. We also encourage future research to extend
21
22 our conceptual model by considering more nuanced helping motives, such as pro-social,
23
24
25 organizational concern and impression management motives (Rioux & Penner, 2001).
26
27
A final limitation of our research is the lack of a direct comparison between one’s in-
28
29
30 role task performance and extra-role helping behavior as two parallel behavioral outcomes in
31
32
33 a PFP system. However, our empirical findings may still be robust because we ruled out the
34
35 confounding influences of the participants’ subjective, relative performance on helping
36
37
38 behavior in Study 2 and Study 3. We urge future researchers to measure individuals’
39
40
objective task performance and helping behavior simultaneously and explore whether the use
41
42
43 of a system that offers pay for subjectively evaluated performance decreases one’s objective
44
45
46 task performance (as claimed in prior research) while increasing one’s helping behavior (as
47
48 found in our research). Methodologically, future research could benefit from adopting a
49
50
51 longitudinal perspective and tracking changes in the PFP intensity and helping behaviors over
52
53
time. Future research might consider conducting field experiments, randomly assigning
54
55
56 organizations to implement varying levels of PFP intensity with both objective and subjective
57
58
59 performance measures and tracking how these systems impact employees’ outcomes.
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 52 of 72

1
2
3
4 CONCLUSION
5
6 Compensation scholars have recently called for nuanced explorations of PFP’s
7
8
9 impacts on individual extra-role behaviors. Our research contributes to this burgeoning
10
11 stream of research by theorizing and testing a motivational model of PFP and helping
12
13
14 behavior. By challenging the conventional view that PFP undermines employee extra-role
15
16
behavior, our findings indicate that PFP has a conditional positive effect on individual
17
18
19 helping behavior in the context of high performance subjectivity. Moreover, our findings
20
21
22 emphasize the importance of examining the psychological mechanisms (i.e., helping-
23
24 performance expectancy) that underlie employees’ motivation to help in response to the PFP
25
26
27 and performance appraisal systems that organizations enact.
28
29
30 REFERENCES
31 Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. 1991. Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting
32
33 interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
34 Aguinis, H. 2009. Performance management. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Prentice
35 Hall.
36
37 Amabile, T. M. 1988. A model of creativity and innovation in organizations. Research in
38 Organizational Behavior, 10: 123–167.
39 Arvey, R. D., & Murphy, K. R. 1998. Performance evaluation in work settings. Annual
40
Review of Psychology, 49: 141–168.
41
42 Ateş, N. Y., Tarakci, M., Porck, J. P., van Knippenberg, D., & Groenen, P. J. F. 2018. The
43 dark side of visionary leadership in strategy implementation: Strategic alignment,
44 strategic consensus, and commitment. Journal of Management.
45
46 https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206318811567
47 Baker, G. 1992. Incentive contracts and performance measurement. Journal of Political
48 Economy, 100: 598–614.
49
50 Baker, G., Gibbons, R., & Murphy, K. J. 1994. Subjective performance measures in optimal
51 incentive contracts. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 109: 1125–1156.
52 Bamberger, P., & Belogolovsky, E. 2017. The dark side of transparency: How and when pay
53
administration practices affect employee helping. Journal of Applied Psychology, 102:
54
55 658–671.
56 Bamberger, P. A., & Levi, R. 2009. Team-based reward allocation structures and the helping
57 behaviors of outcome-interdependent team members. Journal of Managerial
58
59 Psychology, 24: 300–327.
60
Page 53 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
Belogolovsky, E., & Bamberger, P. A. 2014. Signaling in secret: Pay for performance and the
4
5 incentive and sorting effects of pay secrecy. Academy of Management Journal, 57:
6 1706–1733.
7 Belogolovsky, E., Bamberger, P., Alterman, V., & Wagner, D. T. 2016. Looking for
8
9 assistance in the dark: Pay secrecy, expertise perceptions, and efficacious help seeking
10 among members of newly formed virtual work groups. Journal of Business and
11 Psychology, 31: 459–477.
12
13
Bergeron, D. M. 2007. The potential paradox of organizational citizenship behavior: Good
14 citizens at what cost? Academy of Management Review, 32: 1078–1095.
15 Bernardin, H. J., & Smith, P. C. 1981. Clarification of some issues regarding the development
16
and use of behaviorally anchored rating scales (BARS). Journal of Applied Psychology,
17
18 66: 458–463.
19 Blau, G. 1994. Testing the effect of level and importance of pay referents on pay level
20 satisfaction. Human Relations, 47: 1251–1268.
21
22 Blau, P. M. 1964. Exchange and power in social life. New York: Wiley.
23 Bolino, M. C. 1999. Citizenship and impression management: Good soldiers or good actors?
24 Academy of Management Review, 24: 82–98.
25
26
Bolino, M. C., Turnley, W. H., Gilstrap, J. B., & Suazo, M. M. 2010. Citizenship under
27 pressure: What’s a “good soldier” to do? Journal of Organizational Behavior, 31: 835–
28 855.
29
Bommer, W. H., Johnson, J. L., Rich, G. A., Podsakoff, P. M., & MacKenzie, S. B. 1995. On
30
31 the interchangeability of objective and subjective measures of employee performance: A
32 meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 48: 587–605.
33 Borman, W. C., & Motowidlo, S. J. 1993. Expanding the criterion domain to include
34
35 elements of contextual performance. In N. Schmitt & W. C. Borman (Eds.), Personnel
36 selection in organizations: 77–98. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
37 Brislin, R. W. 1986. The wording and translation of research instruments. In W. J. Lonner &
38
39
J. W. Berry (Eds.), Field methods in cross-cultural research: 137–164. Beverly Hills,
40 CA: Sage.
41 Buckingham, M., & Goodall, A. 2015. Reinventing performance management. Harvard
42
Business Review, 93: 40–50.
43
44 Byron, K., & Khazanchi, S. 2012. Rewards and creative performance: A meta-analytic test of
45 theoretically derived hypotheses. Psychological Bulletin, 138: 809–830.
46 Cadsby, C. B., Fei, S., & Tapon, F. 2007. Sorting and incentive effects of pay for
47
48 performance: An experimental investigation. Academy of Management Journal, 50:
49 387–405.
50 Colquitt, J. A., & Simmering, M. J. 1998. Conscientiousness, goal orientation, and
51
52
motivation to learn during the learning process: A longitudinal study. Journal of Applied
53 Psychology, 83: 654–665.
54 Darlington, R. B., & Hayes, A. F. 2017. Regression analysis and linear models: Concepts,
55
applications, and implementation. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
56
57 Deci, E. L. 1975. Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum.
58
59
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 54 of 72

1
2
3
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. 1999. A meta-analytic review of experiments
4
5 examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological
6 Bulletin, 25: 627–668.
7 Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. 1985. Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human
8
9 behavior. New York: Plenum.
10 Deckop, J. R., Cirka, C. C., & Andersson, L. M. 2003. Doing unto others: The reciprocity of
11 helping behavior in organizations. Journal of Business Ethics, 47: 101–113.
12
13
Deckop, J. R., Mangel, R., & Cirka, C. 1999. Getting more than you pay for: Organizational
14 citizenship behaviour and pay-for-performance plans. Academy of Management
15 Journal, 42, 420–428.
16
Deckop, J. R., Merriman, K. K., & Blau, G. 2004. Impact of variable risk preferences on the
17
18 effectiveness of control by pay. Journal of Occupational and Organizational
19 Psychology, 77: 63–80.
20 De Dreu, C. K. W. 2006. Rational self-interest and other orientation in organizational
21
22 behavior: A critical appraisal and extension of Meglino and Korsgaard (2004). Journal
23 of Applied Psychology, 92: 1625–1637.
24 Deming, W. E. 1986. Out of the crisis. Cambridge, MA: Center for Advanced Engineering
25
26
Study, MIT.
27 Du, J., & Choi, J. N. 2010. Pay for performance in emerging markets: Insights from China.
28 Journal of International Business Studies, 41: 671–689.
29
Eisenberger, R., & Cameron, J. 1996. Detrimental effects of reward: Reality or myth?.
30
31 American Psychologist, 51: 1153–1166.
32 Eisenhardt, K. M. 1989. Agency theory: An assessment and review. Academy of
33 Management Review, 14: 57–74.
34
35 Flynn, F. J. 2006. How much is it worth to you? Subjective evaluations of help in
36 organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior, 27: 133–174.
37 Frey, B. S., & Osterloh, M. 2012. Stop tying pay to performance. Harvard Business Review,
38
39
90: 51–52.
40 Fulmer, I. S., & Shaw, J. D. 2018. Person-based differences in pay reactions: A
41 compensation-activation theory and integrative conceptual view. Journal of Applied
42
Psychology, 103: 939–958.
43
44 Garbers, Y., & Konradt, U. 2014. The effect of financial incentives on performance: A
45 quantitative review of individual and team-based financial incentives. Journal of
46 Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 87: 102–137.
47
48 Gerhart, B. 2017. Incentives and pay for performance in the workplace. Advances in
49 Motivation Science, 4: 91–140.
50 Gerhart, B., & Fang, M. 2014. Pay for (individual) performance: Issues, claims, evidence and
51
52
the role of sorting effects. Human Resource Management Review, 24: 41–52.
53 Gerhart, B., & Fang, M. 2015. Pay, intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, performance,
54 and creativity in the workplace: Revisiting long-held beliefs. Annual Review of
55
Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior: 489–521.
56
57 Gerhart, B., & Milkovich, G. T. 1992. Employee compensation: Research and practice. In M.
58 D. Dunnette, & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial & organizational
59 psychology (2nd ed.): 481–569. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc.
60
Page 55 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
Gerhart, B., & Newman, J. M. 2020. Compensation (13th ed.). New York: McGraw Hill.
4
5 Gerhart, B., & Rynes, S. 2003. Compensation: Theory, evidence, and strategic implications.
6 Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
7 Gerhart, B., Rynes, S. L., & Fulmer, I. S. 2009. Pay and performance: Individuals, groups,
8
9 and executives. Academy of Management Annuals, 3: 251–315.
10 Giarratana, M. S., Mariani, M., & Weller, I. 2018. Rewards for patents and inventor
11 behaviors in industrial research. Academy of Management Journal, 61: 264–292.
12
13
Gibbs, M., Merchant, K. A., Van der Stede, W. A., & Vargus, M. E. 2004. Determinants and
14 effects of subjectivity in incentives. Accounting Review, 79: 409–436.
15 Gupta, A. K., & Govindarajan, V. 1984. Business unit strategy, managerial characteristics,
16
and business unit effectiveness at strategy implementation. Academy of Management
17
18 Journal, 27: 25–41.
19 Gupta, N., & Shaw, J. D. 2014. Employee compensation: the neglected area of HRM
20 research. Human Resource Management Review, 24:1–4.
21
22 Han, J. H., Bartol, K. M., & Kim, S. 2015. Tightening up the performance-pay linkage: Roles
23 of contingent reward leadership and profit-sharing in the cross-level influence of
24 individual pay-for-performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100: 417–430.
25
26
Hayes, A. F. 2012. PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for observed variable
27 mediation, moderation, and conditional process modeling [Computer software].
28 Retrieved from http://www.afhayes.com/public/process2012.pdf
29
Hayes, A. F., & Montoya, A. K. 2017. A tutorial on testing, visualizing, and probing an
30
31 interaction involving a multicategorical variable in linear regression analysis.
32 Communication Methods and Measures, 11: 1–30.
33 Hofmann, D. A., & Gavin, M. B. 1998. Centering decisions in hierarchical linear models:
34
35 Implications for research in organizations. Journal of Management, 24: 623–641.
36 Hofmann, D. A., Griffin, M. A., & Gavin, M. B. 2000. The application of hierarchical linear
37 modeling to organizational research. In K. J. Klein & S. W. J. Kozlowski (Eds.),
38
39
Multilevel theory, research, and methods in organizations: Foundations, extensions,
40 and new directions: 467–511. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
41 Ittner, C. D., Larcker, D. F., & Meyer, M. W. 2003. Subjectivity and the weighting of
42
performance measures: Evidence from a balanced scorecard. Accounting Review, 78:
43
44 725–758.
45 Jenkins, D. G., Jr., Mitra, A., Gupta, N., & Shaw, J. D. 1998. Are financial incentives related
46 to performance? A meta-analytic review of empirical research. Journal of Applied
47
48 Psychology, 83: 777–787.
49 Jensen, M. C., & Meckling, W. H. 1976. Theory of the firm: Managerial behavior, agency
50 costs and ownership structure. Journal of Financial Economics, 3: 305–360.
51
52
John, L. S. K., Loewenstein, G., & Rick, S. I. 2014. Cheating more for less: Upward social
53 comparisons motivate the poorly compensated to cheat. Organizational Behavior and
54 Human Decision Processes, 123: 101–109.
55
Kahneman, D. 2011. Thinking fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
56
57 Kamdar, D., McAllister, D. J., & Turban, D. B. 2006. “All in a day’s work”: How follower
58 individual differences and justice perceptions predict OCB role definitions and behavior.
59 Journal of Applied Psychology, 91: 841–855.
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 56 of 72

1
2
3
Kaplan, R. S., & Norton, D. P. 1992. The balanced scorecard—Measures that drive
4
5 performance. Harvard Business Review, 70: 71–79.
6 Kohn, A., Stewart, G. B., & McAdams, J. 1993. Rethinking rewards. Harvard Business
7 Review, 71: 48–49.
8
9 Kouchaki, M., Smith-Crowe, K., Brief, A. P., & Sousa, C. 2013. Seeing green: Mere
10 exposure to money triggers a business decision frame and unethical outcomes.
11 Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 121: 53–61.
12
13
Kovacic, M. P., Galic, Z., & Jerneic, Z. 2014. Social desirability scales as indicators of self-
14 enhancement and impression management. Journal of Personality Assessment, 96: 532–
15 543.
16
Larkin, I., Pierce, L., & Gino, F. 2012. The psychological costs of pay-for-performance:
17
18 Implications for the strategic compensation of employees. Strategic Management
19 Journal, 33: 1194–1214.
20 Lawler, E. E. III. 1971. Pay and organizational effectiveness: A psychological view. New
21
22 York: McGraw-Hill.
23 Lawler, E. E. III. 2000. Rewarding excellence: Pay strategies for the new economy. San
24 Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
25
26
Lawler, E. E., III., & Suttle, J. L. 1973. Expectancy theory and job behavior. Organizational
27 Behavior and Human Performance, 9: 482–503.
28 Levin, J. 2003. Relational incentive contracts. American Economic Review, 93: 835–857.
29
Li, N., Zhao, H. H., Walter, S. L., Zhang, X. A., & Yu, J. 2015. Achieving more with less:
30
31 Extra milers’ behavioral influences in teams. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100:
32 1025–1039.
33 MacKenzie, S. B., Podsakoff, P. M., & Fetter, R. 1993. The impact of organizational
34
35 citizenship behavior on evaluations of salesperson performance. Journal of Marketing,
36 57: 70–80.
37 MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., & Williams, J. 2004. Confidence limits for the indirect
38
39
effect: Distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivariate Behavioral
40 Research, 39: 99-128.
41 Maltarich, M. A., Nyberg, A. J., Reilly, G., Abdulsalam, D. D., & Martin, M. 2017. Pay-for-
42
performance, sometimes: An interdisciplinary approach to integrating economic
43
44 rationality with psychological emotion to predict individual performance. Academy of
45 Management Journal, 60: 2155–2174.
46 Manthei, K., & Sliwka, D. 2013. Multitasking and the benefits of objective performance
47
48 measurement—Evidence from a field experiment. Working Paper, University of
49 Cologne/Germany. Available at: https://www.econstor.eu/handle/10419/79968
50 Marchegiani, L., Reggiani, T., & Rizzolli, M. 2016. Loss averse agents and lenient
51
52
supervisors in performance appraisal. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization,
53 131: 183–197.
54 McNeish, D. 2018. Thanks coefficient alpha, we’ll take it from here. Psychological Methods,
55
23: 412–433.
56
57 Milgrom, P., & Roberts, J. 1992. Economics, organization and management. Englewood
58 Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
59
60
Page 57 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
Mossholder, K. W., Richardson, H. A., & Settoon, R. P. 2011. Human resource systems and
4
5 helping in organizations: A relational perspective. Academy of Management Review, 36:
6 33–52.
7 Muckler, F. A., & Seven, S. A. 1992. Selecting performance measures: “Objective” versus
8
9 “subjective” measurement. Human Factors, 34: 441–455.
10 Murphy, K. R., & Balzer, W. K. 1986. Systematic distortions in memory based behavior
11 ratings and performance evaluations: Consequences for rating accuracy. Journal of
12
13
Applied Psychology, 71: 39–44.
14 Murphy, K. R., & DeNisi, A. 2008. A model of the performance appraisal process. In A.
15 Varma, P. Budhwar, & A. DeNisi (Eds.), Performance management systems around the
16
globe: 81–96. London, UK: Routledge.
17
18 Murphy, K. R., & Cleveland, J. 1995. Understanding performance appraisal: Social,
19 organizational, and goal-based perspectives. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
20 Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. 2019. Mplus user’s guide (1998-2019). Muthén & Muthén:
21
22 Los Angeles, CA.
23 Nyberg, A. J., Maltarich, M. A., Abdulsalam, D. D., Essman, S. M., & Cragun, O. 2018.
24 Collective pay for performance: A cross-disciplinary review and meta-analysis. Journal
25
26
of Management, 44: 2433–2472.
27 Nyberg, A. J., Pieper, J. R., & Trevor, C. O. 2016. Pay-for-performance’s effect on future
28 employee performance: Integrating psychological and economic principles toward a
29
contingency perspective. Journal of Management, 42: 1753–1783.
30
31 Organ, D. W. 1988. Organizational citizenship behavior: The good soldier syndrome.
32 Lexington Books/DC Heath and Com.
33 Organ, D. W. 1997. Organizational citizenship behavior: It’s construct clean-up time. Human
34
35 Performance, 10: 85–97.
36 Organ, D. W., Podsakoff, P. M., & MacKenzie, S. B. 2006. Organizational citizenship
37 behavior: Its nature, antecedents, and consequences. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
38
39
Park, S., & Sturman, M. C. 2016. Evaluating form and functionality of pay-for-performance
40 plans: The relative incentive and sorting effects of merit pay, bonuses, and long-term
41 incentives. Human Resource Management, 55: 697–719.
42
Park, T-Y., Kim, S., & Sung, L-K. 2017. Fair pay dispersion: A regulatory focus theory view.
43
44 Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 142: 1–11.
45 Pearce, J. L., & Gregersen, H. B. 1991. Task interdependence and extrarole behavior: A test
46 of the mediating effects of felt responsibility. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76: 838–
47
48 844.
49 Perlow, L., & Weeks, J. 2002. Who’s helping whom? Layers of culture and workplace
50 behavior. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 23: 345–361.
51
52
Pfeffer, J. 1998. The human equation: Building profits by putting people first. Boston, MA:
53 Harvard Business School Press.
54 Podsakoff, P. M., Ahearne, M., & MacKenzie, S. B. 1997. Organizational citizenship
55
behavior and the quantity and quality of work group performance. Academy of
56
57 Management Journal, 82: 262–270.
58
59
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 58 of 72

1
2
3
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Paine, J. B., & Bachrach, D. G. 2000. Organizational
4
5 citizenship behaviors: A critical review of the theoretical and empirical literature and
6 suggestions for future research. Journal of Management, 26: 513–563.
7 Podsakoff, N. P., Whiting, S. W., Podsakoff, P. M., & Blume, B. D. 2009. Individual- and
8
9 organizational-level consequences of organizational citizenship behaviors: A meta-
10 analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 122–141.
11 Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. 2006. Computational tools for probing
12
13
interaction effects in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve
14 analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 31: 437–448.
15 Purcell, J., & Hutchinson, S. 2007. Front-line managers as agents in the HRM-performance
16
causal chain: Theory, analysis and evidence. Human Resource Management Journal,
17
18 17: 3–20.
19 Rioux, S. M., & Penner, L. A. 2001. The causes of organizational citizenship behavior: A
20 motivational analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86: 1306–1314.
21
22 Roberson, L., Galvin, B. M., & Charles, A. C. 2007. When group identities matter: Bias in
23 performance appraisal. Academy of Management Annals, 1: 617–650.
24 Rotundo, M., & Sackett, P. R. 2002. The relative importance of task, citizenship, and
25
26
counterproductive performance to global ratings of job performance: A policy-capturing
27 approach. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87: 66–80.
28 Rousseau, D. M. 1995. Psychological contracts in organizations: Understanding written
29
and unwritten agreements. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
30
31 Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. 2000. Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic
32 motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55: 68–78.
33 Rynes, S. L., Gerhart, B., & Parks, L. 2005. Personnel psychology: Performance evaluation
34
35 and pay for performance. Annual Review of Psychology, 56: 571–600.
36 Schabram, K., Robinson, S. L., & Cruz, K. S. 2018. Honor among thieves: The interaction of
37 team and member deviance on trust in the team. Journal of Applied Psychology, 103:
38
39
1057–1066.
40 Shaw, J. D. 2015. Pay dispersion, sorting, and organizational performance. Academy of
41 Management Discoveries, 1: 165–179.
42
Shaw, J. D., & Gupta, N. 2007. Pay system characteristics and quit patterns of good, average,
43
44 and poor performers. Personnel Psychology, 60: 903–928.
45 Shaw, J. D., & Gupta, N. 2015. Let the evidence speak again! Financial incentives are more
46 effective than we thought. Human Resource Management Journal, 25: 281–293.
47
48 Shaw, J. D., Gupta, N., & Delery, J. E. 2002. Pay dispersion and workforce performance:
49 Moderating effects of incentives and interdependence. Strategic Management Journal,
50 23: 491–512.
51
52
Sturman, M. C. 2003. Searching for the inverted U-shaped relationship between time and
53 performance: Meta-analyses of the experience/performance, tenure/performance, and
54 age/performance relationships. Journal of Management, 29: 609–640.
55
Sturman, M. C., Cheramie, R. A., & Cashen, L. H. 2005. The impact of job complexity and
56
57 performance measurement on the temporal consistency, stability, and test-retest
58 reliability of employee job performance ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90:
59 269–283.
60
Page 59 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
Super, J., Li, P., Ishqaidef, G., & Guthrie, J. 2016. Group rewards, group composition and
4
5 information sharing: A motivated information processing perspective. Organizational
6 Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 134: 31–44.
7 Trevor, C. O., & Wazeter, D. L. 2006. A contingent view of reactions to objective pay
8
9 conditions: Interdependence among pay structure characteristics and pay relative to
10 internal and external referents. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91: 1260–1275.
11 Trevor, C. O., Reilly, G., & Gerhart, B. 2012. Reconsidering pay dispersion’s effect on the
12
13
performance of interdependent work: Reconciling sorting and pay inequality. Academy
14 of Management Journal, 55: 585–610.
15 Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. 1974. Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases.
16
Science, 185: 1124–1131.
17
18 Uy, M. A., Lin, K. J., & Ilies, R. 2017. Is it better to give or receive? The role of help in
19 buffering the depleting effects of surface acting. Academy of Management Journal, 60:
20 1442–1461.
21
22 Van der Vegt, G. S., Bunderson, J. S., & Oosterhof, A. 2006. Expertness diversity and
23 interpersonal helping in teams: Why those who need the most help end up getting the
24 least. Academy of Management Journal, 49: 877–893.
25
26
Van der Vegt, G. S., Van de Vliert, E., & Oosterhof, A. 2003. Informational dissimilarity and
27 OCB: The role of intrateam interdependence and team identification. Academy of
28 Management Journal, 46: 715–727.
29
Van Dyne, L., Kamdar, D., & Joireman, J. 2008. In-role perceptions buffer the negative
30
31 impact of low LMX on helping and enhance the positive impact of high LMX on voice.
32 Journal of Applied Psychology, 93: 1195–1207.
33 Van Dyne, L., & LePine, J. A. 1998. Helping and voice extra-role behaviors: Evidence of
34
35 construct and predictive validity. Academy of Management Journal, 41: 108–119.
36 Vroom, V. 1964. Work and motivation. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
37 Vroom, V. 1995. Work and motivation. Wiley, New York, NY.
38
39
Weller, I., Süß, J., Evanschitzky, H., & von Wangenheim, F. 2019. Transformational
40 leadership, high-performance work system consensus, and customer satisfaction.
41 Journal of Management. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206318817605
42
Whiting, S. W., Podsakoff, P. M., & Pierce, J. R. 2008. Effects of task performance, helping,
43
44 voice, and organizational loyalty on performance appraisal ratings. Journal of Applied
45 Psychology, 93: 125–139.
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 60 of 72

1
2 TABLE 1 Sample Description in Study 1
3
4 High Performance Subjectivity Category Low Performance Subjectivity Category
Sample
5 Firm Industry Sample PS Sample PS Typical PFP Practices
6 Size Major Occupations Major Occupations
Size Score Size Score
7
8 1 Financials 11 4 Bank teller, Bookkeeper 4.33 7 Retail client service officer 1.00 Monthly floating wage
9
10 2 Consumer 22 7 Accountant, HR officer 6.33 15 Sales representative 1.67 Monthly floating wage,
11
Services Commission
12
13 3 Consumer 26 22 Accountant, 6.33 4 Process worker 1.67 Monthly floating wage,
14
15 Goods Administrative assistant Piece-rate pay, Bonus
16
17
4 Industrials 23 17 Administrative officer, 5.33 6 Marketing manager, 2.67 Monthly floating wage,
18 Financial staff Process worker Merit pay
19
20 5 Industrials 15 2 Accountant, Clerk 7.00 13 Sales representative, Worker 1.00 Monthly bonus, Annual bonus
21
22 6 Consumer 82 5 HR officer, 6.33 77 Business specialist 1.33 Monthly floating wage, Annual
23 Services Accountant bonus
24
25 7 Consumer 46 3 Administrative assistant 7.00 43 Purchasing specialist, 1.00 Commission, Annual bonus,
26
27 Services Sales representative Monthly floating wage
28 8 Telecommuni 55 43 Administrative officer, 5.67 12 Sales representative, 2.33 Monthly floating wage,
29
30 cations HR officer, Accountant Assembly operator Annual bonus
31
32 9 Health Care 43 11 Accountant, 5.00 32 Sales representative, 1.00 Monthly bonus, Annual bonus,
33
Administrative assistant Purchasing representative Commission
34
35 Note. We classified the industry type based on the Industry Classification Benchmark (ICB), which was launched by Dow Jones and FTSE in 2005; PS score = performance subjectivity score, as rated by each
36 firm’s HR manager.
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
Page 61 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2 TABLE 2 Descriptive Statistics and Correlations of Variables in Study 1
3
4 Correlations
5 Variables Mean SD
6 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15
7
1 Helping behavior 3.64 .59 (.87)
8
9 2 Age 30.58 7.76 .05 —
10
11 3 Gender .38 .49 .10 .03 —
12
4 Tenure 4.59 5.17 –.03 .54 .21 —
13
14 5 Education 2.09 .73 .11 .13 –.09 –.09 —
15
16 6 Collective PFP 2.43 1.23 .10 –.03 –.27 –.36 .05 —
17
7 Monthly PFP .18 .39 .01 .09 –.13 .13 –.04 .11 —
18
19 8 Industry-industrials .12 .32 .21 –.10 .36 .18 –.06 –.43 –.17 —
20
21 9 Industry-consumer goods .08 .27 –.03 –.01 –.12 .12 –.04 –.03 .63 –.11 —
22
10 Industry-health care .13 .34 .00 .01 .07 .07 –.07 –.22 –.19 –.14 –.12 —
23
24 11 Industry-consumer services .46 .50 .14 .07 –.25 –.37 .12 .75 –.09 –.34 –.28 –.37 —
25
26 12 Industry-telecommunications .17 .38 –.35 –.04 .07 .19 –.07 –.45 –.21 –.17 –.13 –.18 –.42 —
27
13 PS dummy .35 .48 –.19 –.09 .00 .21 –.00 –.70 .20 .11 .31 –.08 –.49 .41 —
28
29 14 PS score 2.88 2.18 –.19 –.09 –.01 .24 –.03 –.69 .26 .11 .37 –.15 –.47 .43 .98 (.96)
30
31 15 PFP 3.03 1.55 .22 .08 .09 .04 .00 .19 .00 .16 –.03 –.03 .17 –.31 –.22 –.22 —
32 Note: N = 323 at the individual level; N = 9 at the firm level. Correlations greater than ± .11 are statistically significant at p < .05. Coefficient alpha estimates of reliability are in parentheses on the diagonal.
33
PFP indicates pay for performance. PS indicates performance subjectivity.
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
Academy of Management Journal Page 62 of 72

1
2
3
TABLE 3 Multilevel Path Analysis Results in Study 1
4
5 Helping Behavior
6 Variables
7 Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
8
9 Intercept 3.76*** (.13) 3.76*** (.13) 3.75*** (.14) 3.75*** (.14)
10 Age –.01* (.00) –.01† (.01) –.01† (.01) –.01 (.01)
11
12 Gender .07 (.04) .07 (.05) .08† (.04) .08† (.05)
13
14 Tenure .01* (.00) .01† (.00) .01* (.00) .00 (.00)
15 Education .07† (.03) .07† (.04) .06† (.03) .07† (.04)
16
17 Collective PFP .12*** (.03) .14** (.05)
18
19 Monthly PFP –.05 (.16) –.06 (.16) –.07 (.15) –.07 (.15)
20 Industry-industrials .20 (.19) .19 (.19) .16 (.17) .16 (.17)
21
22 Industry-consumer goods –.08** (.03) –.09*** (.03) –.11*** (.00) –.11*** (.00)
23
24 Industry-health care –.13 (.16) –.14 (.16) –.15 (.15) –.15 (.15)
25 Industry-consumer services –.00† (.00) –.00 (.00) .00 (.00) –.00 (.00)
26
27 Industry-telecommunications –.55** (.19) –.57** (.19) –.61*** (.15) –.61*** (.15)
28
29 PS dummy –.07 (.07) –.06 (.06)
30 PS score .03 (.02) .05† (.02)
31
32 PFP .04 (.03) .01 (.02) .04 (.03) .04* (.02)
33
34 PFP × PS dummy .09* (.04)
35 PFP × PS score .02* (.01)
36
37 Residual variance (within-firms) .281*** (.03) .277*** (.03) .279*** (.03) .274*** (.03)
38
39 Residual variance (between-firms) .008 (.01) .008 (.01) .008 (.01) .008 (.01)
40 Note. N = 323 at the individual level; N = 9 at the firm level. We used the mean imputation method for 10 missing data
41 points in the analyses. To ensure robustness, we reanalyzed our data by removing these 10 cases and found that these results
42
remained unaffected. In Models 1-2, we did not control for collective PFP and used job dummy as the proxy of performance
43
44 subjectivity (named PS dummy in these models). In Models 3-4, performance subjectivity was measured by the firm’s HR
45 manager in the additional data collection (named PS score in these models). †p < .10. * p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001. All
46
coefficients displayed are unstandardized. Standard errors are in parentheses.
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
Page 63 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2 TABLE 4 Descriptive Statistics and Correlations of Variables in Study 2
3
Correlations
4 Variable Mean SD
5 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19
6 1 Helping behavior 5.47 .67 (.77)
7
2 Age 34.19 5.63 .01 —
8
9 3 Gender .55 .50 .09 .12 —
10 4 Tenure 7.78 4.38 .02 .68 .11 —
11 5 Education 2.09 .37 –.03 –.09 .02 –.16 —
12
13 6 Job-R&D .26 .44 .07 –.05 .01 .00 .08 —
14 7 Job-sales .17 .38 –.02 .20 .14 .19 –.02 –.27 —
15 8 Job-production .12 .33 .01 –.02 .12 –.07 –.13 –.22 –.17 —
16
9 Job level 1.84 .85 .19 .21 .30 .11 .14 –.16 .09 .03 —
17
18 10 Pay system communication 5.52 .93 .33 –.02 –.04 –.06 .05 –.00 –.05 –.03 .17 (.86)
19 11 Internal pay level 3.61 .63 .18 –.05 .01 –.06 .02 –.03 .06 .00 .28 .39 —
20
12 Relative performance 3.91 .56 .23 .11 .04 .05 .07 –.02 .05 .03 .28 .37 .45 —
21
22 13 Risk aversion 4.14 1.25 –.10 .03 .04 –.01 –.08 –.06 –.13 –.06 –.09 .02 –.00 –.11 (.84)
23 14 Task interdependence 5.06 .82 .43 .07 .05 .08 .05 .02 .02 –.10 .23 .47 .36 .39 .22 (.80)
24 15 Social desirability 3.10 1.91 .19 –.05 .06 –.04 –.04 .08 .00 .03 .04 –.01 –.01 –.09 –.08 –.08 (.82)
25
26 16 Collective PFP 2.48 1.12 .08 .14 .07 .16 .11 .01 .27 –.02 .25 .01 .09 .16 .08 .19 .04 —
27 17 Performance subjectivity 3.98 1.34 .01 .08 –.06 .01 .02 –.10 –.20 –.17 .01 .01 .05 –.04 .23 .15 –.09 .01 (.77)
28 18 HPE 5.06 1.16 .41 .11 –.00 –.05 .06 –.05 .02 .10 .22 .40 .28 .46 –.01 .47 –.03 .19 .13 (.83)
29
19 PFP 2.73 1.09 .16 .05 .05 .06 .00 –.08 .32 .11 .19 .08 .25 .19 –.08 .10 .13 .62 –.00 .19 —
30
31 Note. N = 176 at the individual level; N = 6 at the firm level. Correlations at or greater than ± .15 are statistically significant at p < .05. HPE indicates helping-performance expectancy. Reliability
32 coefficients are displayed within parentheses on the diagonal. Correlations for two industry dummy variables with the other variables are not presented due to the space considerations but are available from
33
the corresponding author.
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
Academy of Management Journal Page 64 of 72

1
2
3
TABLE 5 Multilevel Path Analysis Results in Study 2
4
5 Testing Hypothesis 1 Testing Hypotheses 2-3
6 Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4a Model 4b
7 Variables
8 Helping DV = Helping
Helping Behavior DV = HPE
9 Behavior Behavior
10
Intercept 5.36*** (.03) 5.34*** (.02) 5.37*** (.03) 4.83*** (.08) 5.35*** (.03)
11
12 Age .00 (.01) .00 (.01) –.01 (.01) .04* (.02) –.01 (.01)
13 Gender .07 (.05) .08 (.05) .09** (.03) –.11 (.11) .12*** (.04)
14
Tenure –.00 (.02) –.00 (.02) .01 (.02) –.07** (.02) .01 (.02)
15
16 Education –.14** (.05) –.13*** (.04) –.14*** (.04) –.02 (.07) –.15*** (.03)
17 Job-R&D .05 (.04) .03 (.04) .04 (.06) .05 (.12) .05 (.06)
18
19 Job-sales –.11 (.10) –.07 (.10) –.14 (.09) .13 (.21) –.15† (.08)
20 Job-production .05 (.11) .09 (.11) –.06 (.13) .55*** (.10) –.07 (.16)
21 Job level .05 (.04) .05† (.03) .04 (.03) .05 (.07) .04 (.03)
22
23 Pay system communication .10** (.03) .10*** (.03) .06* (.03) .22† (.13) .05 (.04)
24 Internal pay level –.09 (.06) –.09† (.06) –.07 (.05) –.09 (.06) –.07 (.06)
25 Relative performance .02 (.08) .01 (.08) –.07 (.08) .53*** (.13) –.07 (.09)
26
27 Risk aversion –.09** (.03) –.09** (.03) –.08* (.04) –.06† (.04) –.08* (.04)
28 Task interdependence .34*** (.06) .36*** (.06) .27*** (.05) .44*** (.11) .27*** (.04)
29
Social desirability .06*** (.02) .06*** (.02) .06*** (.01) .01 (.03) .06*** (.02)
30
31 Collective PFP –.04 (.05) –.05 (.05) –.06 (.03) .09 (.10) –.06 (.04)
32 PS .00 (.01) .02 (.01) –.02 (.01) .11** (.04) –.02 (.01)
33
HPE .17*** (.05) .19** (.07)
34
35 PFP .08 (.05) .08 (.05) .08 (.05) .02 (.07) .08 (.06)
36 PFP × PS .07*** (.02)
37
38 PFP × HPE .07* (.03)
39 Residual variance (within-firms) .323*** (.04) .316*** (.04) .297*** (.03) .800*** (.05) .290*** (.02)
40 Residual variance (between-firms) .000 (.01) .000 (.01) .006 (.03) .011 (.05) .005 (.03)
41
42 Note. N = 176 at the individual level; N = 6 at the firm level. Coefficients for two firm dummy variables included in each
43 calculation are not presented due to space considerations but are available from the corresponding author. All coefficients
44
displayed are unstandardized. Standard errors are in parentheses. Model 4a and 4b were estimated simultaneously in the
45
46 same calculation. †p < .10. * p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001.
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
Page 65 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
TABLE 6 Descriptive Statistics and Correlations of Variables in Study 3
4
5 Correlations
Variables Mean SD
6 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
7
1 Objective helping behavior 4.96 3.12 —
8
9 2 Subjective helping behavior 5.03 .99 .50*** (.74)
10 3 Performance subjectivity .49 .50 –.04 .16† —
11 4 HPE 5.43 1.02 .20* .10 .18* (.89)
12
13 5 Helmert-D1 –.00 .47 .08 .02 –.03 –.16 —
14 6 Helmert-D2 .01 .41 .14 –.08 .03 –.08 .02 —
15 7 Indicator-D1 .32 .47 –.08 .08 –.04 –.01 .49*** –.86*** —
16
17 8 Indicator-D2 .34 .48 .16 –.06 .01 –.15 .51*** .87*** –.50*** —
18 *
Note. N = 140. p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001. Dummy coding of multicategorical independent variable is as follows: Helmert coding D1 = low PFP condition (–2/3), moderate PFP condition
19 (1/3), high PFP condition (1/3); Helmert-D2 = low PFP condition (0), moderate PFP condition (–1/2), high PFP condition (1/2); Indicator-D1 = low PFP condition (0), moderate PFP condition
20
(1), high PFP condition (0); Indicator-D2 = low PFP condition (0), moderate PFP condition (0), high PFP condition (1). HPE indicates helping-performance expectancy. Reliability coefficients
21
22 are displayed within parentheses on the diagonal.
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
Academy of Management Journal Page 66 of 72

1
2
3
4 TABLE 7 Process Macro Analysis Results in Study 3
5 Objective Helping Behavior Subjective Helping Behavior
6 Variables
High PS Low PS High HPE Low HPE High PS Low PS High HPE Low HPE
7
8 Helmert-D1 .59 (.77) .40 (.78) .46 (.75) 1.24 (.86) .30 (.25) –.19 (.25) .11 (.24) .08 (.27)
9 [–.93, 2.13] [–1.13, 1.94] [–1.02, 1.93] [–.46, 2.95] [–.19, .78] [–.68, .30] [–.36, .58] [–.47, .62]
10
Helmert-D2 2.80** (.91) –.54 (.88) 2.25* (.93) .29 (.85) .14 (.29) –.55† (.28) .53† (.30) –.77** (.27)
11
12 [1.00, 4.61] [–2.29, 1.20] [.41, 4.10] [–1.39, 1.98] [–.44, .71] [–1.10, .01] [–.06, 1.11] [–1.30, –.23]
13 Indicator-D1 –.80 (.91) .67 (.89) –.67 (.89) 1.10 (1.00) .23 (.29) .08 (.28) –.15 (.28) .46 (.32)
14 [–2.61, 1.00] [–1.09, 2.44] [–2.44, 1.10] [–.88, 3.07] [–.35, .80] [–.48, .64] [–.71, .41] [–.17, 1.09]
15
16 Indicator-D2 2.00* (.88) .13 (.89) 1.58† (.87) 1.39 (.92) .37 (.28) –.46 (.28) .37 (.28) –.31 (.29)
17 [.25, 3.75] [–1.63, 1.90] [–.13, 3.30] [–.43, 3.21] [–.19, .92] [–1.02, .10] [–.17, .92] [–.89, .27]
18 Note. N = 140. Bootstrapping sample size = 1000. PS indicates performance subjectivity. HPE indicates helping-performance expectancy. “High PS” reflects high PS condition (PS = 1). “Low
19
20 PS” reflects low PS condition (PS = 0). “High HPE” reflects high HPE condition (HPE = mean + 1 SD = 6.45). “Low HPE” reflects low HPE condition (HPE = mean – 1 SD = 4.40). All
21 coefficients displayed are unstandardized. Standard errors are in parentheses. 95% confidence intervals are in square brackets. As another test of the differences between groups, we re-analyzed
22 the data using MPLus (verison 8.4; Muthen & Muthen, 2019), calculating the bias-corrected bootstrapped intervals for both the Helmert coding and the Indicator coding dummy variables. We
23
used the bias-corrected version of the bootstrapping method as recommended by MacKinnon, Lockwood, and Williams (2004), with 10,000 resamplings to ensure a high precision in the
24
25 confidence interval. This robust analysis generated similar empirical results, which are available from the corresponding author.
26 †p < .10. * p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001.
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
Page 67 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
FIGURE 1 The Overall Conceptual Model
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
FIGURE 2 The Interactive Effects of PFP with Performance Subjectivity Dummy on
27 Employee Helping Behavior in Study 1
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53 Note. High performance subjectivity dummy = 1; low performance subjectivity dummy = 0.
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 68 of 72

1
2
3
FIGURE 3 The Interactive Effects of PFP with Performance Subjectivity Score on
4
5 Employee Helping Behavior in Study 1
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27 FIGURE 4 The Interactive Effects of PFP with Performance Subjectivity on
28 Employee Helping Behavior in Study 2
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55 Note. PFP intensity ranged from 1 (0-5%) to 6 (70-85%) in Study 2.
56
57
58
59
60
Page 69 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
FIGURE 5 The Interactive Effects of PFP with Helping-Performance Expectancy on
4
5 Employee Helping Behavior in Study 2
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36 Note: HPE indicates helping-performance expectancy. PFP intensity ranged from 1 (0-5%) to 6 (70-85%)
37 in Study 2.
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 70 of 72

1
2
3
FIGURE 6 The Interactive Effects of PFP with Performance Subjectivity on
4
5 Participant Objective Helping Behavior in Study 3
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32 Note: PS indicates performance subjectivity.
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
Page 71 of 72 Academy of Management Journal

1
2
3
FIGURE 7 The Interactive Effects of PFP with Helping-Performance Expectancy on
4
5 Participant Objective Helping Behavior in Study 3
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32 Note: HPE indicates helping-performance expectancy.
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
Academy of Management Journal Page 72 of 72

1
2
3
Wei He (whe@nju.edu.cn) is an associate professor of management at the School of
4
5 Business, Nanjing University, China. He received his PhD in business administration from
6 Huazhong University of Science and Technology, China. His research interests include
7 compensation, leadership, employee organizational citizenship behavior, and workplace
8
9 deviance.
10
11 Shao-Long Li (tli@whu.edu.cn) is an assistant professor at the Economics and Management
12
13
School of Wuhan University, China. He received his PhD in business administration from
14 Huazhong University of Science and Technology, China. His research interests include
15 compensation, team leadership, and workplace deviance.
16
17
18 Jie (Jasmine) Feng (jie.feng@rutgers.edu) is an assistant professor of human resource
19 management in the Rutgers School of Management and Labor Relations. She received her
20 PhD from the Department of Management and Human Resources at the Wisconsin School of
21
22 Business. Her primary research interests include employee turnover, compensation, and
23 interfaces between HR and Entrepreneurship.
24
25
26
Guanglei Zhang (zhangguanglei@whut.edu.cn) is an associate professor at the School of
27 Management, Wuhan University of Technology, China. He received his PhD in business
28 administration from Huazhong University of Science and Technology, China. His research
29
interests include human resource management practices and dirty work.
30
31
32 Michael C. Sturman (michael.sturman@rutgers.edu) is a professor of human resource
33 management in the Rutgers School of Management and Labor Relations. He received his
34
35 PhD from Cornell University’s School of Industrial and Labor Relations. His research
36 interests include the prediction of individual job performance over time, the influence of
37 compensation systems, and the use of HR Analytics and Metrics to improve HR decision
38
39
making.
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60

You might also like