Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Early Human Development (2007) 83, 293 — 305

available at www.sciencedirect.com

www.elsevier.com/locate/earlhumdev

Feeding skill performance in premature infants during


the first year
Karen Pridham a,*, Deborah Steward b,1, Suzanne Thoyre c,2
Roger Brown a,3, Lisa Brown c,2
a
University of Wisconsin-Madison School of Nursing, 600 Highland Avenue, Madison, WI, 53792, United States
b
Ohio State University College of Nursing, 1585 Neil Avenue, Columbus, Ohio, 43210, United States
c
University of North Carolina College of Nursing at Chapel Hill, Carrington Hall, NC, 27599, United States

Accepted 14 June 2006

KEYWORDS Abstract
Premature infant;
Infant feeding skills; Background: Little is known about premature infants’ feeding skill development and the
Rate of weight contribution to it of biologic and environmental conditions.
gain/day; Aims: Explore the level and variation in feeding skill performance through the first post-term
Maternal feeding year and examine the contribution to performance of infant neonatal condition and rate of
behavior; weight gain per day, maternal feeding behavior, and its interaction with neonatal condition.
Neonatal Medical Index Study design and subjects: In this longitudinal, descriptive study, data sources included
observed and videotaped in-home feeding for 45 infants b 1250 g birth weight and their mothers
(age z 17 years).
Outcome measure: Feeding skill performance (fdgskill): total number of expected skills at 1, 4,
8, and 12 months, post-term age (PTA).
Results: Feeding skill performance varied widely among infants at all four assessments. At 8 and
12 months, fdgskill indicated, for a minority of infants, delay and lack of opportunity to engage
in skills associated with new foods and new feeding modalities. Neonatal medical condition
contributed significantly to fdgskill at 1 and 4 months, but in the predicted (negative) direction
only at four months. Rate of weight gain per day contributed significantly to fdgskill at 1 and
8 months, but in the predicted direction (positive) only at one month. Maternal feeding
behavior did not contribute to fdgskill, nor did it interact with infant neonatal conditions to
affect fdgskill.

* Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 608 238 7536; fax: +1 608 263 5332.
E-mail address: kpridham@wisc.edu (K. Pridham).
1
Tel.: +1 614 292 4978.
2
Tel.: +1 919 966 8418; fax: +1 919 843 9969.
3
Tel.: +1 263 5281; fax: +1 263 5332.

0378-3782/$ - see front matter D 2006 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2006.06.004
294 K. Pridham et al.

Conclusions: Although infant neonatal medical status and rate of weight gain per day, before or
within the span of time between assessments, accounted for some variance in feeding skill
performance within the first three assessments (1, 4, and 8 months), much remains to be
explained, including neuro- and oral—motor capacities to manage new foods and feeding
modalities and opportunities to practice feeding skills during the last half of the first year.
D 2006 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction through the first post-term year, referenced to the


spectrum of skills expected for gestation-corrected age
Clinician interest in the feeding behavior of premature [7—9], is lacking.
infants is likely to focus on quantity of nutrient intake. Although a contribution of both premature infant
Feeding skills, in contrast, receive little attention. These biologic conditions and the feeding behavior of mother
skills, in addition to their potential contribution to intake, to infant feeding skill performance is supported by
are significant in their own right. Feeding skills mark an research findings [10—13], we know little about the
infant’s development through the first year from caregiv- effect, taken together, of both biologic conditions and
er- to predominantly self-regulated feeding. Social—emo- mother’s feeding behavior on infant feeding skill perfor-
tional and cognitive benefits accrue to an infant from mance through the first post-term year. Both the infant’s
participation in feeding in age-expected ways. Despite medical complications in the neonatal period and current
research observations [1,2] or parental report [3] that rate of gain in weight are biologic conditions that may
premature infants lag behind healthy, term-born infants in make a difference in infant feeding performance. We
self-feeding skills, little is known, for premature infants, learned that bronchopulmonary dysplasia (BPD), examined
about age-expected performance of feeding skills and in the context of intervention vs. standard care, poverty,
about factors that contribute to this performance. Knowl- and mothers’ symptoms of depression, had only a
edge of feeding skills would put clinicians in a better marginally significant ( p b .10) effect on feeding skill
position to understand how premature infants function performance at eight months PTA for VLBW infants
during feeding and support researchers in designing (Pridham et al. [14]). Korner and her colleagues [15],
interventions to promote development. We took a devel- however, found a significant effect of medical complica-
opmental science perspective [4—6] in this study to tions on the motor and mental development of VLBW
examine premature infants weighing b 1250 g at birth, infants using an index composed of multiple biologic
feeding skill performance at 1, 4, 8, and 12 months post- conditions, including BPD and neurologic problems (e.g.,
term age as points of observation likely to be associated periventricular leukomalacia, intraventricular hemorrhage)
with distinct phases of development. From this perspec- as well as prematurity and low birth weight [24]. An
tive, feeding skill arises from the collaboration of mother infant’s rate of weight gain is a sign of health, vigor, and
and infant and is a function of the support the mother physical robustness. A more robust infant is likely to be
offers the infant [6]. Therefore, our study included more active physically, to engage in more exploratory
examination of the independent and interactive contribu- behavior, and to have greater stamina for participation in
tion of infant biological and maternal feeding conditions to feeding. Wachs [16] observed that mothers offered more
feeding skill performance. robust infants greater opportunities for autonomy, motoric
activity, and exploration than mothers offered to smaller,
same-age infants. Mothers may give more robust prema-
1.1. Background ture infants greater opportunities for self feeding.
A mother’s regulation of her affect and behavior during
Feeding skill performance. Documentation for premature feeding may make a difference for her infant’s participa-
infants of developmental delay in feeding skills, for the tion in feeding and feeding skill learning [17]. A mother
most part, begins with the second half of the first post-term regulates her infant’s feeding by structuring both the
year when the adoption of new foods and feeding modal- feeding and the feeding environment and by responding
ities is expected. In one of the few observational studies to to an infant’s expression of need. Regulation of feeding is
date, Mathisen and colleagues [1] compared the oral— also expressed through inhibition of negative affect and
motor and self-feeding skills of 20 extremely premature, disruptive behaviors (e.g., intrusiveness) and through
low birth-weight (ELBW) infants (i.e., birth weight less than sensitive response. Abrupt, awkward, insensitive, or inef-
1000 g and born between 23 and 29 weeks gestation) at fective physical handling is one quality of maternal
6 months post-term age with those of 20 full-term infants at behavior that may adversely affect infant feeding [18].
the same age, and learned that a significantly lower Mathisen and her colleagues [1] observed mothers of 6-
percent of the premature infants performed the skill than month-old ELBW infants having difficulties in positioning
full-term infants. Holditch-Davis, Miles, and Belyea [2] the infant in a way that supported feeding. Maternal
observed a very low rate of self-feeding behavior at six feeding behavior may be influenced by perception of an
months post-term age among 29 very low birth-weight infant’s risk for medical problems [19]. How maternal
(VLBW), premature infants (i.e., birth weight between 1000 behavior contributes to infant feeding skill performance
and 1500 g), despite infants’ ability to bring objects to their through the first post-term year, accounting for infant
mouths. Description of premature infant feeding skills biologic conditions, is unknown.
Feeding skill performance in premature infants during the first year 295

1.2. Research questions and hypotheses study were collected in the home at 1, 4, 8, and 12 months
by research nurses. Training for data collection included
We addressed three questions in this study. (a) What laboratory scoring of video-taped feeding skills and in-home
expected oral—motor, hand—mouth—eye (manual), head— mentoring. Data-collection quality was monitored on ap-
trunk (postural), and communicative—social feeding skills proximately every fifth to tenth home visit, depending on
are observed in VLBW premature infants at 1, 4, 8, and the experience and performance of the data collector. In all,
12 months, post-term age? (b) What do infant biologic two trained observers concurrently coded feeding skills at
conditions, indexed by both neonatal medical status and 20 home visits.
rate of weight gain per day, and maternal regulation of During each of the four visits, the infant was weighed
affect and behavior during feeding contribute to feeding prior to a feeding that was initiated by the mother at a time
skill performance at each of these four ages? (c) How do she determined to be appropriate. Mothers were requested
infant biologic conditions and maternal feeding behavior to feed their infants as they would usually feed. The entire
interact to affect feeding skill performance at each of the feeding was video-taped for later, laboratory-coding of
four ages? maternal feeding behavior by one of two coders who were
uninformed about infant or maternal characteristics. To
assure full visibility, infant feeding skills were coded during
2. Methods the observed feeding.

This longitudinal, descriptive study used data collected in a 2.2. Data collection instruments and measures
randomized trial of an intervention to aid mothers in feeding
their VLBW infants. Because infants in the intervention and 2.2.1. Neonatal Medical Index
comparison groups did not significantly differ on feeding The Neonatal Medical Index [15], designed to assess the
skill performance [14], the data for the two groups were severity of the infant’s medical complications during the
pooled. The study was approved by the institutional review initial hospital stay, was one of the variables we used to
boards of the academic institution and the participating estimate infant biologic condition in this study. Infants were
hospitals. classified with the NMI ordinal scale, which ranges from 1
All infant ages are referred to or reported in this study (i.e., freedom from medical complications) to 5 (i.e.,
as post-term age, i.e., for the premature infants, the occurrence of the most serious medical problems.) Birth
number of days or months computed from the date of weight and medical complications (e.g., respiratory dis-
reaching 40 weeks post-conceptional age. This age was tress, assisted ventilation and or supplemental oxygen,
determined by the most reliable information available, patent ductus arteriosus requiring medical treatment or
either the mother’s menstrual dates, prenatally by ultra- major surgery, apnea and bradycardia, hyperbilirubinemia,
sound examination, or by the neonatologist’s post-birth meningitis, seizures, Grade III or IV intraventricular or
examination. periventirular hemorrhage, periventricular leukomalacia)
prior to hospital discharge were included in the classifica-
2.1. Participants tion algorithm. Predictive validity was supported, for VLBW
infants, by the significant contribution of the NMI score to
Mothers and their prematurely born infants were recruited the Bayley Motor Development Index at 12 months and to
from 1995 to 1997 during the infant’s stay at one of three the Bayley Mental and Motor Development Indices at
neonatal intensive care units (NICUs) in a mid-western US 24 months as well as to the Stanford Binet Intelligence
city. Inclusion criteria for infants were: (a) birth weight less Scale at 36 months [15].
than 1250 g and appropriate for gestational age (i.e.,
greater than the 10th percentile) as determined by the 2.2.2. Rate of weight gain per day
growth graph of Lubchenco et al. [20] and the gestational To obtain infant weight at 1-, 4-, 8-, and 12-month
age criteria of Ballard et al. [21]; and (b) at the time of assessments and to compute the rate of gain prior to each
recruitment, no medical conditions that interfere with oral assessment, the infant was weighed nude on a portable
intake of nutrients. Only the most severely ill infant of a digital electronic scale (Hopkins Medical Products, Balti-
multiple birth was included. The birth weight criterion more, MD) that dynamically integrates fluctuating weight to
established a population of higher-risk VLBW infants. the nearest 10 g. Weight was repeatedly assessed until two
Inclusion criteria for mothers were: (a) age 17 years old or readings agreed within 10 g. The average of these two
older; (b) the infant’s primary caregiver; and (c) English- readings was recorded. Rate of weight gain per day
speaking. Mothers who had multiple births were recruited to (rwtgain) was determined by dividing the amount of gain
the study. However, the infant of a multiple birth with the between assessments by the number of days spanned. The
longest length of hospital stay was the only infant included rate of weight gain/day prior to one month was computed by
in the study. Of the 80 eligible families, 57 (71.25%) agreed subtracting the infant’s hospital discharge weight from the
to participate. one-month weight, and dividing by the number of days from
After mothers and fathers, if available, had signed the discharge to the visit date.
study consent form, mothers were interviewed about their
plans for infant feeding and reported attribute and demo- 2.2.3. Child Feeding Skills Checklist
graphic data. Data about the infant’s birth and medical The Child Feeding Skills (CFS) Checklist was developed to
complications were obtained from the infant’s hospital assess, primarily from observation, skills of infants born at
record by a trained chart auditor. All other data for this term or prematurely at 1, 4, 8, and 12 months [22]. The
296 K. Pridham et al.

CSF items were drawn from Gesell and Ilg’s [7] list of skills, 2.2.4. Maternal regulation of feeding behavior and affect
except for two items concerning skill in taking non-milk We used an observation instrument, the Parent—Child Early
foods that were drawn from Morris and Klein’s [9] clinical Relational Assessment (PCERA) [25] to rate the quality of
observations. Gesell and Ilg [7] gave each item a beginning maternal regulation of feeding behavior and affect (mrab).
age or age period. We selected for non-overlapping The PCERA scales were derived from theoretical and empir-
assessments at 1, 4, 8, and 12 months items that could ical studies and confirmed by factor analysis on feeding data
be first observed within the infant’s first five weeks for the from a sample (n = 379) of mothers and their healthy, four-
1-month assessment, weeks 8—24 for the 4-month assess- month-old term infants [18,26]. The 14-item mrab scale
ment, 28—36 weeks for the 8-month assessment, and 40— includes items to assess on five-point ordinal scales the
52 weeks for the 12-month assessment. In the count of following concepts: (a) structuring a feeding and mediating
feeding skills, six CFS items were included at one month, the environment to support feeding; (b) responding sensi-
21 at four months, 31 at eight months, and 22 at twelve tively to infant needs and preferences; (c) responding
months. flexibly; and (d) expressing little or no negative feelings,
A count of skills observed or reliably reported by the intrusiveness, abrupt or rough physical handling of the infant,
mother as having been performed, if the data collector or contingent responding to infant behavior that had a
could not observe performance, was obtained for each negative meaning for the mother. The low end of the scale
assessment. This count (fdgskill) reflects the perspective for each item indicates less well regulated affect or behavior
of Fischer, Bullock, Rotenberg, and Raya [6] concerning the and the high end of the scale indicates regulated, well-
contribution of opportunity to skill and represents the structured feeding behavior, absence of negative affect,
feeding skills performed and not the skills an infant could presence of positive affect, or positive interactive behavior.
perform given the opportunity. Discharge from the special Scores may be defined categorically as follows: (a) 1 or 2, area
care nursery was contingent on suckling or sucking with of concern; (b) 3, area of some concern; (c) 4 or 5, area of
effective extraction of milk. Because all of the infants strength. Items were rated from the second 5 min of
performed this feeding skill at one month, it was not videotaped feeding interaction. A feeding is generally
included in the one-month skill count. Four of the CFS established and mothers and infants comfortable with and
items (i.e., grasps and draws bottle to mouth, hands close inattentive to the video taping within about five minutes [26].
on bottle, holds bottle, and tips bottle adaptively) are The PCERA has been used with both mothers of term and
applicable only to bottle-feeding infants. These skills were premature infants who were near term or post term [18,27—
included in the total count of performed skills because all 29,14]. Validity, both discriminant and concurrent, has been
of the infants in the sample were bottle fed at four demonstrated with women and children from a broad
months when these behaviors are expected. Skills related spectrum of the population, including African-American
to taking food from a spoon that Gesell and Ilg [7] women and children [27]. Coders were trained to at least
identified in their sample as early as 20 weeks were not 80% reliability, using the three category criterion. To maintain
included in the CFS count at four-months because pedia- inter-rater equivalency of coding, drift sessions were held
tricians were, in general, advising mothers to begin spoon every two weeks. In all, 20% of the total number of tapes was
feeding later. At eight and twelve months, the observer rated by two raters. Tapes with low coding agreement were
recorded each spoon-feeding, finger-feeding, and cup- discussed to obtain consensus on scale codes. Inter-rater
drinking skill and table-food handling skill that the infant agreement was 85% on average. The Kappa statistic averaged
had not experienced or had an opportunity to learn. If this .66 (SD = .23), signifying substantial agreement [30]. The
were the case, the infant was assumed not to have the internal consistency reliability of the mrab items ranged from
skill, and the skill was reported as not performed. For .81 to .91 across the four assessments.
each infant without experience or opportunity, we dis-
cussed with the mother her perspective on the infant’s 3. Results
ability to perform the skill and were informed that she did
not believe the infant could manage the skill. The 3.1. Sample description
communication—social skills primarily concerned age-spe-
cific vocal or behavioral cues of hunger and of satiety. Of the families recruited to the study, 45 (78.9%) completed
These skills were not included in the skill count because at least one observation of feeding skills, and 43 (75%)
the infant’s feeder could over-ride their expression, finished the study at the infant’s 12th month. All of the 14
confounding observance of performance. families that did not complete the study left it before the
Inter-rater agreement for approximately 10% of the infant was four months. Although we obtained weight data
observed feedings averaged 87% (range 83% to 91%) for for 44 infants at one month, we could observe infant feeding
the observed/not observed score for each item. Using skills for only 41 of these infants, primarily due to problems
data collected on developmental measures for the in scheduling the observation with the family. Feeding skills
larger study, concurrent validity of the measure of were observed for 39 infants at four months, 40 infants at
expected feeding skills performed (fdgskill) was sup- eight months, and 38 infants at twelve months.
ported by the correlation of eight-month fdgskill with
the eight-month score on the Bayley Infant Neurodeve- 3.2. Data collection procedure
lopmental Screener [23] and by the correlation of the
12-month fdgskill measure with the 12-month Motor Maternal, family, and infant attributes are described in Table
Development Index [24], respectively r = .54, p b .001 and 1. Most infants had two parents living in the home.
r = .49, p b .01. Approximately two-thirds of the mothers were married or
Feeding skill performance in premature infants during the first year 297

Table 1 Demographic and attribute data was breastfed at the one-month assessment. All other
infants were bottle fed at the 1-, 4-, 8-, and 12-month
Mean (SD) Range
assessments. Post-term age in days averaged 40.2 (SD = 21.4)
Maternal and family data for the first (1 month) assessment; 131.0 (SD = 26.7) for the
Mother’s age (years) 25.67 18— 37 second (4 month) assessment; 249.1 (SD = 20.9) for the third
(5.30) (8 month) assessment, and 375.1 (SD = 17.6) for the fourth
Mother’s education (years) 13.04 8—21 (12 month) assessment.
(2.62) Although the infants, on average, performed two-thirds
Number Percent of the expected skills in the set of skills included in the Child
Parents’ marital status: married 14 31.1 Feeding Skills Assessment at one month, by 12 months, the
Partnered, living together 16 35.6 average percent of skills performed had dropped to 54% of
Partnered, not living together 12 26.7 the expected skills (see Table 2). The probability of
Single, not living with a partner 2 4.4 performing less than 60% of the expected feeding skills at
Separated, divorced 1 2.2 a specific age, conditional on performance of less than 60%
Poverty statusa 29 64.4 of the expected feeding skills at the previous assessment,
Race/ethnicity: African American 27 60.0 was 41.7% at 4 months, 45.0% at 8 months, and 64.3% at
Euro-American 15 33.3 12 months. The variation in performance of the set of skills
Asian 1 2.2 assessed was wide, particularly at one month (M = 67.07%,
Latina 2 4.4 SD = 25.41; at four months, M = 56.78%, SD = 17.29; at eight
months, M = 65.56%, SD = 18.41; at 12 months, M = 54.39%,
Infant data SD = 18.45). Descriptive statistics for the number of skills
Birth weight (g) 876.58 530—1240 performed in relation to the set of skills assessed are shown
(205.90) in Table 3.
Gestational age (weeks) 26.37 23—30 At one month, on average, infants performed four of
(1.93) the six oral—motor and communication—social feeding
Days on assisted ventilation 27.80 0—83 skills included in the set expected at one month, PTA
(23.40) (see Table 2). At this age, almost all (93%) of the infants
Hospital days (special care) 82.73 35—158 demonstrated coordinated sucking and swallowing with
(27.43) breathing by swallowing liquids without choking. About
Number Percent 75% of the infants performed skills that are signs of an
Male gender 22 48.9 infant’s active participation in feeding (e.g., signaling
readiness for food with jaw excursion; sucking or mouthing
Neonatal Medical Index (NMI) b when placed in a feeding position). Only 51% of the infants
Rating = 1 0 mouthed or sucked their fists, a feeding readiness signal
Rating = 2 1 2.2 that has a wide age span for performance. We may have
Rating = 3 13 28.9 assessed this skill too early for the infants in our sample to
Rating = 4 7 15.6 demonstrate. About 75% of the infants engaged in looking
Rating = 5 24 53.3 at the mother’s face for more than a moment, a skill that
Note. N = 45. supports feeding alertness and maintenance. However, only
a
Estimated income below the poverty line, using U.S. about half of the infants communicated distress when
Census Bureau data specific to the time families were enrolled interrupted in feeding.
in the study (http://www.census.gov/hhes/poverty/threshld/ At four months, the infants, on average, performed
thresh98.html). approximately 12 of the 21 expected skills. About 77% of
b
A rating of 5 represents the most serious medical
the infants returned to feeding after brief socializing with
complications.
the mother, indicating ability to maintain and regain focus
living with a partner and 64% had a family income below the on the feeding task. All but 14% of the infants signaled
poverty level, determined by federal guidelines [31]. One- readiness to feed by opening the mouth and poising it to
third of the mothers were Euro-American; 60% were African receive the nipple. About 67 to 75% of the infants brought a
American. Although mothers averaged about 13 years of hand to the mouth or followed the bottle with the eyes at
education, the range in education years was 8 to 21. The least once during feeding, indicating engagement in the
infants, on average, were extremely low birth weight, and the feeding. However, except for keeping hands open for most
majority had high ratings (i.e., 4 or 5) of severity of medical of the feeding, demonstrated by about 70% of the infants,
complications on the Neonatal Medical Index. Twenty (44.4%) only about one-third of the infants performed skills involving
of the infants had a Grade I or II intraventricular hemorrhage hands, mouth, and perhaps eyes (e.g., making a hand-to-
(IVH). Although none of the infants had an IVH graded III or IV, mouth movement as the nipple is removed, closing in with
six infants had a diagnosis of periventricular leukomalacia. both hands to grasp objects held near). These skills are
precursors of skills in finger-feeding and in participating in
3.3. Performance of feeding skills through the first year, bottle feeding and, eventually, cup drinking. The skill of
post-term age maintaining a midline head position when the body is in
midline, performed by about 85% of the infants, and the skill
The feeding skills observed were almost entirely in the of sitting supported, performed by about 64% of the infants,
context of bottle- rather than breast-feeding. One infant contribute to an infant’s accepting food from a spoon.
298 K. Pridham et al.

Table 2 Feeding skill performance for four assessments during the first year, post-term age, by skill category
Infant post-term age (approximate) na Number of infants Percent of infants Post-term age or age period
at Assessment/Category of Skill performing skill performing skill of expected performanceb
1 month, post-term age
Oral—motor skills
Swallows liquids, no choking or coughing 41 38 92.7 Term (40 weeks)
Begins sucking/mouthing when in 40 30 75 Term—9 wks
feeding position
Moves jaw as mouth opens, signaling 40 30 75 4—12 wks
readiness for food
Mouths or sucks fist 40 17 42.5 Term—16 wks
Communication—social skills
Indicates distress when feeding 41 21 51.2 2 wks
is interrupted
Looks at caregiver’s face for more than 39 29 74.4 5 wks
a moment

4 months, post-term age


Oral—motor skills
Opens mouth and poises it to 38 33 86.8 16 wks
receive nipple
Does not choke with milk from bottle 38 28 73.7 16 wks
or breast
Hand—mouth—eye skills
Brings hand to mouth 38 28 73.7 16—24 wks
Grasps and draws bottle to mouth, 39 10 25.6 20—26 wks
usually with assistance
Hands close on bottle 37 18 48.6 20 wks
Hands are open for most of feeding 36 25 69.4 8 wks
Hand to mouth movement as nipple 38 10 26.3 16—24 wks
is removed
Regards hand 38 10 26.3 12—16 wks
Reaches for object on sight and grasps it 39 17 43.6 24 wks
Closes in with both hands to grasp 39 15 38.5 20 wks
objects held near
Looks at object in hand 38 19 50 20 wks
Head and trunk skills
Flexes neck towards breast or bottle 39 18 46.2 20—23 wks
when seen
Follows bottle with eyes at least 37 25 67.6 12 wks
once during feeding
Maintains midline head position 39 33 84.6 16 wks
when body is in midline
Turns head to sound as if to look at 39 36 92.3 24 wks
what is going on
Sits supported 39 25 64.1 16 wks
Communication—social skills
Regards breast or bottle eagerly 39 26 66.7 10 wks
Occasionally looks at caregiver’s 38 20 51.3 8 wks
face and smiles
Returns to feeding after brief 39 30 76.9 12 wks
socializing with caregiver
Regards caregiver’s face for a 38 24 63.2 8 wks
prolonged time
Coos 36 26 72.2 12 wks

8 months, post-term age


Oral—motor skills
Removes food quickly from spoon 41 32 78.0 28 wks
Uses upper lip to remove food from spoon 41 20 70.7 32 wks
Feeding skill performance in premature infants during the first year 299

Table 2 (continued)
Infant post-term age (approximate) na Number of infants Percent of infants Post-term age or age period
at Assessment/Category of Skill performing skill performing skill of expected performanceb
8 months, post-term age
Oral—motor skills
Draws in lower lip to remove food 40 27 65.9 28—32 wks
from spoon
Smacks tongue against palate 40 13 31.7 28—36 wks
Swallows solids; seldom chokes 41 39 95.1 28 wks
Takes pureed foods from spoon 40 36 87.8 27 wksc
Manipulates textured/lumpy foods 40 22 53.7 36 wksc
in mouth
Does up and down chewing 41 32 78.0 27 wks
Does rotary chewing 41 12 29.3 32 wks
Keeps lips closed while chewing, some 40 31 77.5 25 wks
of the time
Takes sips of liquid from a cup 40 23 56.1 28 wks
Takes one or two swallows from a cup 41 20 48.8 32 wks
Drinks from cup without choking 41 25 61.0 32 wks
Lips close on bottle nipple with little 41 34 82.9 28 wks
loss of liquid
Hand—mouth—eye skills
Hands reach for food 41 34 82.9 32 wks
Feeds self crackers, larger pieces of 41 24 58.5 28 wks
finger food
Helps with spoon 41 21 51.2 28 wks
Drinks from cup with caregiver control 41 19 46.3 32 wks
Holds bottle by self 41 28 68.3 28 wks
Adaptively tips bottle as it empties 40 23 57.5 32 wks
Uses inferior scissors grasp 40 25 62.5 32 wks
Grasps, brings food to mouth, and mouths 41 39 95.1 26 wks
or bites it
Voluntarily releases and re-secures 40 32 80.0 28 wks
food/object
Transfers food/object held in one hand 41 34 82.9 28 wks
to another
Head and trunk skills
Brings head forward to receive spoon 41 24 58.5 32 wks
Sits alone (unsupported) 41 29 70.7 32 wks
Balances trunk while manipulating 41 25 61.0 28—32 wks
food/object with hands
Communication—social skills
Vocalizes impatience or eagerness as sees 41 21 51.2 28—36 wks
caregiver preparing meal
Vocalizes eagerness as regards bottle, dish, 41 29 70.7 28—34 wks
or cup when placed in chair
Explores food with hands 41 24 58.5 32 wks
Vocalizes da-da or similar syllables 41 24 58.5 36 wks

12 months, post-term age


Oral—motor skills
Controls bite of soft solids or crunchy foods 38 28 73.7 52 wks
Manipulates food with definite 39 35 89.7 44 wks
chewing movements
Actively closes lips on spoon 39 31 79.5 52 wks
Uses tongue to lick morsels of food off 37 10 27.0 40 wks
lower lip
Uses upper or lower lip to clean food off 39 23 59.0 52 wks
the other lip
Eats mashed table foods 39 18 46.2 44 wks
Eats foods that vary in consistency 39 29 74.4 52 wks
(continued on next page)
300 K. Pridham et al.

Table 2 (continued)
Infant post-term age (approximate) na Number of infants Percent of infants Post-term age or age period
at Assessment/Category of Skill performing skill performing skill of expected performanceb
12 months, post-term age
Oral—motor skills
Adaptively approximates lips to rim or 39 26 66.7 40 wks
spout of cup
Does not spill from corners of mouth as 39 14 35.9 40—44 wks
drinks from cup
Continuously drinks 4—5 or more swallows 39 11 28.2 40 wks
from a cup
Does not drool 39 26 66.7 52 wks
Hand—mouth—eye skills
Finger feeds small pieces of food with a 39 28 71.8 46—48 wks
refined/ neat pincer grasp
Reaches for spoon 39 29 74.4 40 wks
Gets food into mouth with spoon, may 39 12 30.8 44 wks
grasp with pronated hand
Uses spoon independently; no spills 39 1 2.6 52 wks
Pokes at nipple or food with index finger 38 28 73.7 44 wks
Rubs spoon back and forth on tray 39 17 43.6 50 wks
Uses cup independently with two hands, 38 10 26.3 52 wks
spilling less than half
Grasps bottle or cup and brings to mouth 39 15 35.9 40 wks
Voluntary hand to mouth movements 38 32 84.2 40 wks
Communication—social skills
Vocalizes eagerness or fusses impatiently if 39 33 84.6 44—48 wks
caregiver is slow or presents an other than
wanted food
a
Number of infants participating in an observed feeding.
b
Derived from Gesell and Ilg [7] except as noted.
c
Derived from Morris and Klein [9].

At eight months, on average, infants performed 20 of spoon feeding. Five of the infants (12.5%) had not had an
the 31 skills assessed. At least two-thirds of the infants opportunity to take pureed food from a spoon. Sixteen of
performed finger-feeding skills (e.g., hands reach for food; the infants, almost 40% of the infants observed, had not
voluntarily releases and re-secures food or an object; had an opportunity to self-feed crackers, and 12 infants
transfers food or an object held in one hand to another; (30% of those observed) had had no opportunity to take sips
grasps and brings food to the mouth; and mouths or bites it) from a cup.
and spoon-feeding skills (e.g., removes food quickly from At the 12-month assessment, on average, infants per-
the spoon; uses upper lip to remove food from the spoon; formed 11 of the 21 expected skills. At least 70% of the
draws in the lower lip to remove food from the spoon). Up infants demonstrated feeding skills needed for eating table
to 60% of the infants demonstrated skills for cup drinking foods (e.g., controls bite of soft solids or crunchy foods,
and for managing textured or lumpy foods. For cup manipulates food with definite chewing movements, eats
drinking, these skills included taking one or two swallows foods that vary in consistency, finger feeds small pieces of
from a cup, drinking from a cup without choking, and food with a refined/neat pincer grasp. Advancement in
drinking from a cup with caregiver control. For managing spoon feeding skill was demonstrated in actively closing the
textured or lumpy foods, the demonstrated skills included lips on a spoon, for almost 80% of the babies, and in
manipulating the food in the mouth, rotary chewing, and reaching for the spoon, for just under 75% of the babies. Of
self-feeding crackers and larger pieces of finger food. At the approximately 75% of the infants who were given a
least 50—60% of the infants were showing signs of becoming spoon, either with food on it or to dip into food, less than
independent in bottle feeding in skills they performed one-third were skilled enough to get food into the mouth
(e.g., adaptively tips the bottle as it empties) and with it. Infants, in the range of 25—40%, performed more
progressing towards self-spoon feeding (e.g., helps with sophisticated cup drinking and spoon feeding skills (e.g.,
spoon). However, approximately 30% of the infants did not drinks from a cup without spilling from the corners of the
sit unsupported, and almost 40% did not balance the trunk mouth; continuously drinks 4—5 or more swallows from a
while manipulating food or an object with their hands, both cup; grasps bottle or cup and brings it to the mouth; gets
skills that are important for managing food that is a food into the mouth with a spoon, perhaps grasped with a
progression to table food and for independently bottle or pronated hand). Only one infant, however, used a spoon
Feeding skill performance in premature infants during the first year 301

Table 3 Descriptive statistics for continuous variables included in the model of feeding skill performance at 1, 4, 8, and 12
months, post-term age
Variable/PTA n Mean Standard deviation Min Max
a
fdgskill
1 month 41 4.02 1.52 1 6
4 months 39 11.92 3.63 4 20
8 months 40 20.32 5.71 5 28
12 months 38 11.42 3.87 1 17

mrab b
1 month 37 3.81 .41 2.93 4.36
4 months 39 3.52 .54 1.93 4.43
8 months 36 3.40 .59 2.14 4.36
12 months 40 3.37 .56 2.14 4.36

rwtgain c, g/day
NICU discharge to 44 29.96 12.02 8.05 54.28
1 month
Males 22 30.65 12.70 8.05 53.28
Females 22 29.27 11.55 11.67 51.50
1 to 4 months 35 22.33 7.46 4.79 33.49
Males 19 23.31 7.25 4.79 33.49
Females 16 21.17 7.78 5.14 21.17
4 to 8 months 38 14.04 3.76 7.46 25.68
Males 21 14.65 4.20 7.67 25.68
Females 17 13.29 3.09 7.46 18.20
8 to 12 months 41 9.50 4.23 2.76 23/4
Males 21 10.29 4.17 4.50 23.40
Females 20 8.67 4.23 2.76 15.55
Note. n refers to the number of observations made for the variable at a specific post-term age (PTA).
a
Number of feeding skills in the age-expected set of skills performed at least one time during feeding. The number of skills assessed at
each assessment was as follows: (a) 6 at 1 month; (b) 21 at 4 months; (c) 31 at 8 months; and (d) 21 at 12 months.
b
Mothers’ regulation of affect and behavior, rated on a 1—5 scale, 5 = adaptive feeding behavior and affect.
c
Rate of weight gain.

independently. Eight percent of the infants had not had an 31.7% of infants below the 10th percentile in daily weight
opportunity to hold and take a bite of soft solid or crunchy gain.
food, 51% had not had an opportunity to take mashed table We used fixed occasions repeated measures regression
foods, and 23% had not had an opportunity to take foods analysis [33—35] for the four assessments to explore the
that vary in consistency. Almost 13% of the infants had not contribution to feeding skills performance (fdgskill) of
had an opportunity to drink continuously (4—5 swallows) infant biological conditions (NMI and rwtgain), maternal
from a cup. regulation of affect and behavior during feeding (mrab), and
the interaction of NMI and mrab. This type of regression
3.4. The contribution of infant biologic conditions and analysis builds four simultaneous regressions and models the
maternal behavior to feeding skill correlation among fdgskill variables for the four assess-
ments. The regression is structured as a hierarchical,
Descriptive statistics for the infant and maternal conditions marginal model that uses all of the data that are available
explored as predictors of infant feeding skills are shown in at any one time and permits assessment of differences
Table 3. Mothers, on the whole, had scores on regulation of between times. For this analysis, fdgskill was log trans-
affect and behavior during feeding that ranged from within formed due to the positively skewed count data. We used a
an area of concern (b 3) to within an area of strength (N 4). hierarchical multivariate Poisson distribution [36,37] with
The rate of weight gain/day for female infants was not the Dispersion Phi measure to correct for over-dispersion in
significantly ( p N .05) lower than for male infants, and was, 8- and 12-month models [38].
on average, between the 10th and 90th percentiles for both Results of the fixed occasions repeated measures regres-
genders, using Fomon’s [32] tables of weight increments for sion analysis of feeding skill performance at 1, 4, 8, and
infants born at term. At one month PTA, 19.5% of the infants 12 months are shown in Table 4. At one month, both the
assessed had daily weight increments below the 10th infant’s NMI score and rate of daily weight gain (rwtgain)
percentile. The weight increment assessment at 4, 8, and contributed to the infant’s feeding skill performance
12 months PTA showed, respectively, 31.4%, 13.2%, and (fdgskill) in the context of maternal regulation of affect
302 K. Pridham et al.

Table 4 Parameter estimates for fixed occasions repeated measures regression analyses of feeding performance skills at 1, 4, 8,
and 12 months, infant post-term age
1 month 4 months 8 months 12 months
Intercept 0.792 (0.747) 3.378 (0.532) 3.219 (0.350) 2.423 (0.431)
NMIa 0.192 (0.086)* 0.157 (0.057)** 0.004 (0.049) 0.021 (0.064)
rwtgain b 0.018 (0.005)** 0.006 (0.007) 0.025 (0.010)* 0.011 (0.014)
mrab c 0.189 (0.156) 0.025 (0.093) 0.055 (0.066) 0.002 (0.113)

Covariance matrix
1 month 0.453 (0.137)
4 months 0.670 (0.086) 0.304 (0.159)
8 months 0.016 (0.173) 0.146 (0.187) 0.186 (0.174)
Note: N = 45. Standard errors are shown in parentheses. The data are adjusted for overdispersion in the 8- and 12-month models.
*p b .05; **p b .01.
a
Neonatal Medical Index.
b
Rate of gain in weight.
c
Mother’s regulation of affect and behavior during feeding.

and behavior during feeding (mrab). At four months, the NMI Furthermore, the variation of infants in post-term age
score made a significant contribution to fdgskill in the within each of the four assessments could have contributed
expected negative direction, but neither rwtgain nor mrab to variation in skill performance. The comprehensiveness of
did. At eight months, rwtgain made a significant contribu- the description and the longitudinal study, however, con-
tion to fdgskill, but in an unexpected negative direction. tribute an enlarged perspective of premature infant feeding
Feeding skill performance was lower with greater rate of skills across the first year and specify questions for further,
weight gain per day. Neither of the infant biologic condition in-depth study.
variables nor mrab made a significant contribution to The limitations of the method of assessing feeding skill
fdgskill at 12 months. The analysis showed significant performance, the Child Feeding Skills Checklist (CFS), must
correlation of fdgskill at one month with fdgskill at both be considered in reviewing findings for both study questions.
four and eight months. The 12-month fdgskill was not The age range identified from the Gesell and Ilg [7] norms
correlated with 1-, 4-, or 8-month measures of fdgskill. for each item may not have been appropriate for the
To estimate the contribution of the predictor variables to premature infants, many of whom were extremely low birth
fdgskill, we assessed the difference in the 2 log-likelihood weight. Furthermore, the age range given for several skills
value when the predictor variables were added to the initial we included in our assessments was very broad, and the
model (i.e., the constant or estimated logged count of likelihood of observing these skills at the ages selected for
feeding skills). The 2 log-likelihood at the initial model this study may have been low as a consequence. For
(i.e., constant only) was 823.06. With the predictors added, example, the item, mouths or sucks fist when in vicinity,
the 2 log-likelihood was 677.24. With the 12 new para- which we included in the one-month assessment, was
meters, or 12 degrees of freedom, chi square was 145.42, designated in the Gesell and Ilg reference as being
p b .05, indicating a statistically significant improvement in observable when an infant is term age through 16 weeks.
the predictability of the model. Moreover, it is likely that not all of the items included in the
CFS are of equivalent importance for characterizing feeding
3.5. Interaction of infant neonatal medical status and skill development at a specific age or for differentiating
maternal feeding behavior infants on feeding skill performance. The CFS only permits
recording of performance or non-performance. It does not
We examined the interaction of maternal regulation of capture the extent to which an infant performs a skill when
affect and behavior during feeding with the infant’s medical given the opportunity (once or repeatedly), nor does it
status in the neonatal period in all four models. However, permit description of the quality of performance.
the interaction was not significant in any of the models. For infants at high risk for neuro-developmental problems
[39—41], as were the ELBW infants who participated in this
study, the Child Feeding Skills Checklist may not be
4. Discussion adequate for assessment of feeding skill performance and
documentation of infant progression in self-regulated feed-
We described the feeding skills a small sample of premature ing. For VLBW and ELBW infants, supplementation of the
infants weighing b 1250 g at birth performed at four specific Child Feeding Skills Checklist with a more in-depth and
times during the first year (1, 4, 8, and 12 months), thereby precise assessment of skill in sucking, swallowing, breathing
extending knowledge gained from the observations Mathisen coordination, and use of the lips and tongue could aid
and colleagues [1] and Holditch-Davis, Miles, and Belyea [2] understanding of feeding outcomes [42—44]. Description of
made at six months. The study is limited by its small sample physiologic regulation and stability, including change in
and by its snapshot, rather than continuous, approach to heart and respiratory rates in relation to feeding, the
description of feeding skill performance across the first year. pattern and quality of breathing, and signs of respiratory
Feeding skill performance in premature infants during the first year 303

distress, as well as the quality of positional support an infant may lag behind expectations for self feeding. This exami-
has during feeding may also shed light on a premature nation may also help to better specify the reasons why
infant’s performance of feeding skills and their development infants may not be given opportunities for finger-feeding a
over time. cracker, cup drinking, and managing textured or lumpy
foods. At eight months, for example, delay in sitting
unsupported or in balancing the trunk while manipulating
4.1. Description of premature infant skills food with the hands may help to explain why a mother does
not give her infant finger food despite the infant’s capacity
One of the contributions of this study concerns the to bring a hand to the mouth and to maternal concern about
considerable variability among premature infants across an infant’s choking. At the 12-month assessment, potential
and within time. Exploring the data across time, we learned explanations for the lack of opportunity to eat such table
that the premature infants were lower in performance on foods as mashed potatoes, to self feed with a spoon, and to
the expected set of skills at four months relative to one take continuous swallows from a cup cannot be clearly
month and at 12 months relative to eight months, whereas conjectured from examination of performance on the total
at eight months, infants were similar in the percent of set of expected skills. This set, at 12 months, did not include
expected skills performed to the percent of skills observed head and trunk skills and more than one communication—
at one month. This change in performance relative to the social skill, and should be extended for a more comprehen-
set of expected skills may be due to a qualitative difference sive assessment.
in the challenge for the premature infants of the skills In sum, non-performance of finger-feeding, cup drinking,
included in a specific, age-related set or in the quality of table-food eating, and self-managed spoon feeding may be
maternal competence needed to support infant perfor- multi-determined and extend beyond lack of opportunity.
mance on these skills. The condition of the infant or environment at the time of
In addition to variability across time, feeding skill the observation may not have supported performance of
performance varied considerably within any one age, a feeding skills. Infants may not have had sufficient experi-
feature of the sample that began at one month. Our finding ence to become accustomed to a type of food or to feeding
that as many as 75% of the infants performed skills at this utensils by the time of feeding skill assessment. For
age that, to some degree, indicate active participation in a example, at about eight months, infants are often expected
feeding weakens the assumption that very young premature to manage food with texture, including lumps. Skill in
infants are passive in their feeding and not capable of managing lumpy food and in eating a mixed diet is likely to
contributing to the regulation of the feeding. However, at require practice as well as developmental changes in oral—
one month, infant signals may not be clear in all respects, pharyngeal structure and function that permit management
given that about 49% of the infants did not communicate of the lumps or varying consistency of the food. Practice and
distress when the feeding was interrupted. developmental change may be reciprocal [45,46].
Examination of feeding skills within the 4, 8, and
12 month assessments and across contiguous assessments
generates new questions about the progress of premature 4.2. Exploration of the contribution of biologic
infants in managing new and more sophisticated types of conditions and mothers’ feeding behavior to
food and in using new feeding modalities (e.g., fingers, cup, feeding skill performance
spoon), first with caregiver assistance and, by 12 months,
with some degree of independence. At each age, we We approached this study from a developmental science
observed a subset — on the whole, a minority — of the perspective and the assumption that assessment of feeding
premature infants performing the most advanced, for age, skill performance requires an understanding of the context
of the expected skills in respect to self feeding. At four of the infant’s performance to make it clinically and
months, for example, some premature infants may demon- theoretically useful. To explore feeding skill performance
strate postural and trunk skills that are prerequisite for in relation to a developmentally meaningful context, we
taking food from a spoon. To learn about patterns of used the parameter estimates for the fixed occasions
development within the population of premature infants in repeated measures regression analysis to examine the effect
respect to self-regulatory skill performance, the age span on feeding skill performance (fdgskill) of infant biologic
during which premature infants acquire head and trunk skills conditions (medical status in the neonatal period, estimated
that support being spoon fed as well as the hand—mouth— by the NMI score, and concurrent health and well being,
eye precursors of finger feeding should be explored with estimated by rate of daily weight gain (rwtgain) and
additional, serial skill assessments. These assessments could maternal regulation of feeding affect and behavior (mrab).
provide a foundation for addressing the question of how and In critique of our analytical model, the lack of statistical
when infants who demonstrate these precursor skills should significance for predictor coefficients may be due to low
be supported in developing finger-feeding and spoon- power rather than to true absence of an effect. Although
feeding skills. findings support the contribution of either the infant’s NMI
Examination of the four types of feeding skills (i.e., oral— score or rwtgain or both at the 1-, 4-, and 8-month
motor, hand—mouth—eye, head—trunk, communication— assessments, evidence was not found in this study for an
social) in the context of progression towards self feeding effect of mrab on feeding skill performance at any of the
suggests that a comprehensive observation of feeding skills four assessments.
is needed during the infant’s second half year in order to Several measurement issues may have affected the
better understand the reasons why some premature infants results. One issue is that the items of the mrab scale may
304 K. Pridham et al.

not be adequately sensitive to facets of maternal regulation Special thanks to the mothers and infants who partici-
of feeding affect and behavior that are relevant to feeding pated in this study; Rana Limbo, PhD, RN, and Michele
skill performance. Other qualities of maternal behavior, Schroeder, PhD, RN, for mentoring nurses who made feeding
such as expression of positive affect, may help infants to assessments and for supervision of data collection; the
regulate their behavior and, thus, make a difference in nurses who assessed feeding skills (Lorna Cisler-Cahill, MS,
feeding skill performance. How mothers structure a feeding RN; Wanda Frazier, BS, RN; Donna Harris, MSN, RN; Mary
and maintain an environment that supports infant partici- Krolikowski, MSN, RN, and Lois Seefeldt, PhD, RN); Wai Fong
pation has had little study. Moreover, how maternal Kwok for data entry; and Jeffrey Henriques, PhD, who gave
compensatory behavior [10,19] enhances or inhibits infant valuable assistance with data analyses.
performance of feeding skills is a question. Observation of
the quality of an infant’s participation in the feeding and
responses to opportunities to perform feeding skills could References
lead to identification of infant feeding behaviors that make
a difference in feeding skill performance. [1] Mathisen B, Worrall L, O’Callaghan M, Wall C, Shepherd RW.
Another issue concerns the unexpected direction of Feeding problems and dysphagia in six-month-old extremely
several of the predictor coefficients. The unexpected low birth weight infants. Adv Speech-Lang Pathol Audiol
2000;2(1):9 — 17.
positive contribution of the NMI score to feeding skill
[2] Holditch-Davis D, Miles MS, Belyea M. Feeding and nonfeeding
performance at one month may be explained by the greater interactions of mothers and prematures. West J Nurs Res
attentiveness by mothers to the feeding skills of more high- 2000;22:320 — 34.
risk infants. Mothers may have perceived these infants as [3] Hawdon JM, Beauregard N, Slattery J, Kennedy G. Identifica-
needing more caregiver support. The rwtgain variable made tion of neonates at risk of developing feeding problems in
a significant contribution to fdgskill at one month in the infancy. Dev Med Child Neurol 2002;42:235 — 9.
predicted positive direction, but at eight months, the [4] Lerner RM. Theories of human development: contemporary
significant contribution was in a negative direction. This perspectives. In Damon W, editor-in-chief, RM Lerner, volume
variable may inadequately reflect current status if an infant editor. Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical
has had a recent acute illness or severe or protracted acute models of human development. 5th ed. New York: Wiley;
1998. pp. 1—24.
illness during the interval between assessments and,
[5] Cairns RB, Elder GH, Costello EJ. Developmental science.
therefore, has had very high or very low catch-up growth. Cambridge, UK7 Cambridge University Press; 1996.
Physiologic and behavioral variables not included in this [6] Fischer KW, Bullock DH, Rotenberg EJ, Raya P. The dynamics of
study (e.g., the work of breathing, gastro-esophageal reflux competence: how context contributes to skill. In: Wozniak RH,
and associated swallowing difficulties) may influence feed- Fischer KW, editors. Development in context: acting and
ing skill performance [42]. thinking in specific environments. Hillsdale, NJ7 Lawrence
Too little is known about what is involved in premature Erlbaum; 1993. p. 93 — 117.
infants’ learning skills for new feeding modes and types of [7] Gesell A, Ilg FL. Feeding behavior of infants. Philadelphia, PA7
food. How infants can best gain experience in feeding tasks Lippincott; 1937.
that challenge their physical capacities to feed safely is an [8] Morris SE. Development of oral—motor skills in the neurolog-
ically impaired child receiving non-oral feedings. Dysphagia
important question for further study. How parents can be
1989;3:135 — 54.
effectively guided in offering opportunities and supporting [9] Morris SE, Klein MD. Pre-feeding skills. 2nd ed. Tucson, AZ7
their premature infants in performing and developing Therapy Skill Builders; 2000.
feeding skills is an important research and clinical agenda. [10] Holditch-Davis D, Black BP. Care of preterm infants: programs
An assessment of feeding skills that can reliably be done by of research and their relationship to developmental science.
clinicians in any setting where feeding can be observed Annu Rev Nurs Res 2003;21:23 — 60.
could advance this agenda. Although much work remains to [11] Holditch-Davis D, Cox MF, Miles MS, Belyea M. Mother—infant
be done, this study has advanced this agenda through a interactions of medically fragile infants and non-chronically ill
description of the variability in infant feeding skills within premature infants. Res Nurs Health 2003;26:300 — 11.
and across assessment ages and an exploration of infant [12] Landry SH, Denson SE, Swank PR. Effects of medical risk and
socioeconomic status on the rate of change in cognitive and
biologic conditions and maternal feeding behavior and
social development for low birth weight children. J Clin Exp
affect that may contribute to variability. Neuropsychol 1997;19:261 — 74.
[13] Sameroff AJ, Bartko WT, Baldwin A, Baldwin C, Seifer R. Family
and social influences on the development of child competence.
Acknowledgments In: Lewis M, Feiring C, editors. Families, risk, and competence.
Mahway, NJ7 Lawrence Erlbaum; 1998. p. 161 — 85.
This research was supported by the March of Dimes Birth [14] Pridham K, Brown R, Clark R, Limbo R.K, Schroeder M,
Defects Foundation (grant number FY-0103), Maternal and Henriques J, et al. Effect of guided participation on feeding
Child Health Bureau (MCJ-550806), the National Center for competencies of mothers and their premature infants. Res
Nurs Health 2005;28:252 — 67.
Research Resources to the University of Wisconsin Medical
[15] Korner AF, Stevenson DK, Kraemer HC, Spiker D, Scott DT,
School (NIH M01 RR03186), the University of Wisconsin-
Constantinou J, et al. Prediction of the development of low
Madison School of Nursing, and National Institute of birth weight preterm infants by a new neonatal medical index.
Nursing Research Post-Doctoral Fellowship 5 F32 NR07267- J Dev Behav Pediatr 1993;14:106 — 11.
02 to Deborah Steward and NIH Predoctoral Individual [16] Wachs TD. Going beyond nutrition: nutrition, context, and
National Research Service Award 5 F31 NR007703-02 to Lisa development. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 1993;58(7, serial No.
Brown. 235):100 — 10.
Feeding skill performance in premature infants during the first year 305

[17] Sroufe LA. Emotional development: the organization of [32] Fomon SJ. Nutrition of normal infants. St. Louis, MO7 Mosby;
emotional life in the early years. Cambridge, UK7 Cambridge 1993.
University Press; 1995. [33] Diggle PJ, Liang KY, Zeger S. Analysis of longitudinal data.
[18] Clark R, Hyde JS, Essex MJ, Klein MH. Length of maternity Oxford, UK7 Clarendon Press; 1994.
leave and quality of mother—infant interactions. Child Dev [34] Goldstein H. Multilevel statistical modeling. London, UK7
1997;68:264 — 82. Hodder Arnold; 2003.
[19] Holditch-Davis D, Cox MF, Miles MS, Belyea M. Mother—infant [35] Yang M, Heath A, Goldstein H. Multilevel models for repeated
interactions of medically fragile infants and non-chronically ill binary outcomes: attitudes and vote over the electoral cycle.
premature infants. Res Nurs Health 2003;26:300 — 11. J R Stat Soc, Ser A Stat Soc 2000;163:49 — 62.
[20] Lubchenco LO, Hansman C, Dressler M, Boyd E. Intrauterine [36] Frome EL, Kutner MH, Beauchamp J. Regression analysis of
growth as estimated from liveborn birth-weight data at 24 to Poisson-distributed data. J Am Stat Assoc 1973;68(344):935 — 40.
42 weeks of gestation. Pediatrics 1963;32:793 — 800. [37] Hutchinson MK, Holtman MC. Analysis of count data using
[21] Ballard JL, Khoury JC, Wedig K, Wang L, Eilers-Walsman BL, Poisson regression. Res Nus Health 2005;28:408 — 18.
Lipp R. New Ballard Score, expanded to include extremely [38] Hintze J. NCSS and PASS: Number cruncher statistical systems.
premature infants. J Pediatr 1991;119:417 — 23. Kaysville, UT: WWW.NCSS.COM; 2004.
[22] Pridham KF, Schroeder M, Limbo R, Thoyre S. Child feeding skills: [39] McGrath MM, Sullivan MC, Lester BM, Oh W. Longitudinal
Observational instrument and manual. Unpublished instrument, neurologic follow-up in neonatal intensive care unit survi-
University of Wisconsin-Madison, School of Nursing; 1999. vors with various neonatal morbidities. Pediatrics 2000;106:
[23] Aylward GP. Bayley Infant Neurodevelopmental Screener: 1397 — 1405.
Manual. Harcourt Brace, San Antonio, TX7 Psychological [40] Palta M, Sadek-Badawi M, Evans M, Weinstein M, McGuinness G.
Corporation; 1995. for the Newborn Lung Project. Functional assessment of a
[24] Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development. 2nd ed. New multicenter very low-birth-weight cohort at age 5 years. Arch
York7 Psychology Corporation; 1993. Pediatr Adolesc Med 2000;154:23 — 30.
[25] Clark R. Early parent—child relationship assessment. Unpub- [41] Singer L, Yamashita T, Lilien L, Collin M, Baley J. A longitudinal
lished manuscript, University of Wisconsin at Madison, Depart- study of developmental outcome of infants with bronchopul-
ment of Psychiatry; 1985. monary dysplasia and very low birth weight. Pediatrics 1997;
[26] Clark R. The parent—child early relational assessment: a 100:987 — 93.
factorial validity study. Educ Psychol Meas 1991;59:821 — 8. [42] Kerwin MLE, Osborne M, Eicher PS. Effect of position and
[27] Black MM, Hutcheson JJ, Dubowitz H, Starr RH, Berenson- support on oral-motor skills of a child with bronchopulmonary
Howard J. The roots of competence: mother—child interaction dysplasia. Clin Pediatr 1994;33:8 — 13.
among low income, urban, African American families. J Appl [43] Ramsay M, Gisel EG. Neonatal sucking and maternal feeding
Dev Psychol 1996;17:367 — 91. practices. Dev Med Child Neurol 1996;38:34 — 47.
[28] Pridham KF, Brown R, Clark R, Sondel S, Green C. Infant and [44] Ramsay M, Gisel EG, McCusker J, Bellavance F, Platt R. Infant
caregiving factors affecting weight-for-age and motor devel- sucking ability, non-organic failure to thrive, maternal char-
opment of full-term and premature infants at 1 (year post- acteristics, and feeding practices: a prospective cohort study.
term). Res Nurs Health 2002:394 — 410. Dev Med Child Neurol 2002;405 — 14.
[29] Pridham KF, Schroeder M, Brown R, Clark R. The relationship of [45] Young B, Drewett R. Methods for the analysis of feeding
a mother’s working model of feeding to her feeding behavior. behaviour in infancy: weanlings. J Reprod Infant Psychol 1998;
J Adv Nurs 2001;35:741 — 50. 16:27 — 44.
[30] Landis JR, Koch GG. The measurement of observer agreement [46] Drewett R, Young B. Methods for the analysis of feeding
for categorical data. Biometrica 1997;33:159 — 74. behavior in infancy: sucklings. J Reprod Infant Psychol 1998;
[31] U.S. Census Bureau (1998). Poverty 1998. Current Population 16:9 — 26.
Survey. Retrieved September 1, 2002, from http://www.
census.gov/hhes/poverty/threshld/thresh98.html.

You might also like