5.how To Say "No" Conviction and Identity Attributions in Persuasive Refusal

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 5

Intern. J.

of Research in Marketing 29 (2012) 390–394

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Intern. J. of Research in Marketing


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ijresmar

How to say “no”: Conviction and identity attributions in persuasive refusal


Vanessa M. Patrick a,⁎, Henrik Hagtvedt b
a
C. T. Bauer College of Business, University of Houston, Houston, TX 77204-6021, USA
b
Carroll School of Management, Boston College, MA 02467, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This research investigates the influence of refusal frames on persuasiveness in an interpersonal context. Spe-
First received in 29, June 2011 and was under cifically, the refusal frame “I don't” is more persuasive than the refusal frame “I can't” because the former con-
review for 6 months notes conviction to a higher degree. This perceived conviction is tied to the identity-signaling function of the
Available online 14 September 2012
refusal frame. Two studies demonstrate that 1) the “don't” frame is more persuasive than the “can't” frame,
2) perceived conviction mediates the influence of the refusal frame on persuasiveness, and 3) attributions to
Keywords:
the refuser's identity explain the perceived conviction.
Consumer identity
Language © 2012 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
Persuasion
Interpersonal communication

1. Introduction conveyed by the frame, and 3) the perceiver attributes this conviction
to the refuser's identity.
No man is an island. To function in society, we must interact, For example, if a hostess offers a rich slice of cake to a guest who
negotiate, and communicate with other people. A large part of that declines by saying “I don't eat cake,” we argue that the refusal would
interaction involves conversing effectively, not only to get what we engender the hostess’ compliance and not typically invite negotiation.
want but to avoid what we do not want. It reflects who the person is and what he or she stands for. However, if
One of the key factors that determines whether a refusal is effec- the guest declines by saying “I can't eat cake,” this statement reflects
tive is compliance from others (Cialdini & Goldstein, 2004). For an impediment or inability, and thus, it connotes less force or determi-
instance, anecdotal evidence from attendees of the Weight Watchers nation. In fact, it may connote external concerns that are often relied
program reveals that one of the key reasons why individuals gain upon in excuse-making (Snyder & Higgins, 1988) and therefore is
weight is because their friends, family, and spouses tempt them more likely to provoke discussion, debate, and negotiation.
with unhealthy food and they do not know how to refuse when This research contributes to theory in five key ways: 1) It contributes
confronted with this type of social pressure. From weight loss pro- to the extant literature on consumer identity by illustrating how
grams to Nancy Reagan's famous “Just Say No” campaign against language, in terms of the actual words used, can signal consumer iden-
drugs, individuals are often advised to say no to temptation. However, tity; 2) It contributes to the literature on language in social interactions
little research focuses on exactly how to do so effectively. to demonstrate how subtle differences in language can have a profound
In the current research, we propose that the persuasiveness of a influence on the outcome of the interaction; 3) It contributes to the
refusal depends on how it is framed. Recent research demonstrates literature on compliance-gaining and negotiation by demonstrating
that saying “I don't” is more empowering than saying “I can't” in the the effectiveness of refusal frames in an interpersonal context; 4) It con-
context of self-talk (Patrick & Hagtvedt, 2012). In the present tributes to the emerging literature on semantic framing (as distinct
research, we investigate the effectiveness of these two refusal frames from logically equivalent framing effects in the context of choice
in interpersonal communication. We theorize that when a refusal under uncertainty; Levin & Gaeth, 1988), in which two words used
evokes one's identity, it speaks of one's traits, characteristics, goals, interchangeably in conversation can have substantially different effects;
and beliefs (Oyserman, 2009). This attribution increases the per- 5) Because the current work is prescriptive, it provides recommenda-
ceived conviction of the refuser and consequently enhances the tions for how individuals can adopt refusal frames to more effectively
persuasiveness of the refusal. Specifically, we demonstrate that navigate interpersonal interactions in everyday life.
when refusing others 1) the “don't” frame is more persuasive than
the “can't” frame, 2) this persuasiveness stems from the conviction 2. The power of words: a brief review of the literature

⁎ Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 713 743 3661; fax: +1 713 743 4572. Whorf (1956) proposed that there is no thought without language
E-mail addresses: vpatrick@uh.edu (V.M. Patrick), hagtvedt@bc.edu (H. Hagtvedt). and suggested that individuals' perception of the world and their

0167-8116/$ – see front matter © 2012 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijresmar.2012.04.004
V.M. Patrick, H. Hagtvedt / Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 29 (2012) 390–394 391

interaction with it are based on the linguistic system that they use. Patrick and Hagtvedt (2012) identify the words “don't” and “can't”
Indeed, the semantic meaning of words depends on when and how as differentially effective in resisting temptation and motivating goal-
the words are used (Chafe, 1970; Grice, 1975). In the context of social directed behavior because the former provides a sense of psychological
interactions, Pennebaker, Mehl, and Niederhoffer (2003, p. 566) empowerment. For instance, study participants concerned with healthy
assert that “remarkably little research has been conducted on the eating were more likely to choose granola health bars (vs. chocolate
ways the interactants use words with each other.” However, a distinc- candy bars) if they engaged in self-talk by saying they “don't”
tion between linguistic content (what is said) and linguistic style (vs. “can't”) eat unhealthy foods. In the current context of interpersonal
(how it is said) can be seen in, for instance, two ways to make a communication, we theorize that the “don't” frame is more persuasive
simple request. “Would it be possible for you to pass me the butter?” than the “can't” frame because the former conveys conviction to a higher
and “Pass the butter” both express the speaker's desire for butter degree. As a speech act, “don't” conveys more force or determination and
and direct the listener's action, but the two utterances also reveal is therefore more final and non-arguable (Austin, 1975; Searle, 1969/
different features of the interactants' relationship, the speaker's per- 1999). While expressed commitment to a cause, attitude, or behavior
sonality, and perhaps the way the speaker understands himself. encourages individuals to be swayed by requests in line with that
Indeed, a growing body of literature has demonstrated that listeners commitment, the same commitment signals the unlikelihood of being
make inferences about the attitudes and characteristics of speakers swayed by requests that are incompatible with it (Cialdini & Goldstein,
based on the words those speakers employ. For instance, Douglas and 2004). Indeed, a determined stance thus tends to discourage argument
Sutton (2005) find that describers who use relatively abstract language or negotiation and increases compliance with the refusal (Cialdini &
are perceived by others to have biased attitudes and motives compared Goldstein, 2004; Patrick & Hagtvedt, 2012; Searle, 1969/1999).
with describers who use more concrete language. In the context of
favorable (unfavorable) word of mouth, Schellekens, Verlegh, and 2.3. The role of identity in perceived conviction
Smidts (2010) demonstrate that abstract language leads receivers to
infer that the sender has a more (less) favorable product attitude and One way to convey conviction is to provide a reason for the refusal. As
a higher (lower) buying intention for the product under consideration. Cialdini (2006) suggests, sometimes providing a reason that makes no
It does not seem unreasonable, then, that listeners can also infer aspects sense is more effective than not providing a reason at all. Reasons for re-
such as conviction and identity based on language use. fusal typically fall into two basic categories: those stemming from within
oneself and those stemming from outside oneself (Ross & Nisbett, 2011).
2.1. Semantic framing We propose that the former tends to convey more conviction.
A great deal of research has shown that consumers are likely to be-
Extant research has examined the role played by message framing in have in accordance with their identities, even if the identity is primed
persuasion processes (Mayer & Tormala, 2010), especially in by something as simple as a signature (Kettle & Häubl, 2011). They
health-related communications (Rothman, Salovey, Antone, Kelli, & may even distance themselves from others who do not resonate with
Drake, 1993; Tykocinski, Higgins, & Chaiken, 1994). Language use has their own identity (Berger & Heath, 2007; White & Dahl, 2007). One's
been shown to impact domains as disparate as sensory perception identity is deeply entrenched; it is not something one changes on a
(Hoegg & Alba, 2007), categorical perception (Schmitt & Zhang, 1998), whim. If a refusal is tied to one's identity, then it reflects conviction:
perceived durations of time intervals (Cheema & Patrick, 2008), health a committed, unwavering stance that does not invite negotiation. Con-
outcomes (Campbell & Pennebaker, 2003), and even the ability to dis- versely, when external causes are invoked, they come across as excuses
tinguish colors (Athanasopoulos, Damjanovic, Krajciova, & Sasaki, (Snyder & Higgins, 1988), thereby inviting discussion and negotiation.
2011). In the research on duration estimates, for example, framing Much extant research in sociolinguistics supports the notion that our
deadlines one way (“only three months to finish”) made individuals choice of language may be used to construct and signal identity (Eckert,
feel subjectively closer to that deadline and highlighted the difficulty 2000). This stream of research has roots in early work that revealed that
of completion compared to framing the deadline a different way (“still even seemingly trivial indicators such as sound changes could serve as
have three months to finish”), keeping the objective amount of time symbols of local identity (Labov, 1963). In research on voting behavior,
constant (Sanna, Parks, Chang, & Carter, 2005). Similarly, Cheema and Bryan, Walton, Rogers, and Dweck (2011) demonstrate that invoking
Patrick (2008) demonstrate that the framing of a time interval for cou- the self is sufficient to significantly influence whether individuals vote
pon redemption as “anytime between” or “only between” significantly in presidential and gubernatorial elections. In their case, the manipula-
influences coupon redemption behavior. The current research contrib- tion of identity relied on framing questions about voting behavior as a
utes to an emerging area of work that investigates semantic framing verb or a noun (e.g., “How important is it to you to vote (be a voter) in
in which words that are used interchangeably “may not always be psy- the upcoming election?”). Nouns were expected to invoke identity to
chologically interchangeable” (LeBoeuf, 2006, 60) and can have pro- a higher degree, and indeed, they encouraged voting to a higher degree.
foundly different influences on behavioral outcomes. In the current context, we suggest that attributions to the refuser's
identity result in “don't” conveying a strong sense of conviction. The
2.2. The role of conviction in persuasive refusal “I don't” refusal frame emphasizes that one simply does not do some-
thing or is not the kind of person to make that choice. It thus reflects a
The present research is concerned with the effective framing of a re- permanent state. Conversely, the “I can't” refusal frame emphasizes
fusal in an interpersonal context, the assumption being that effective- impediments or inabilities that may be temporary and not perma-
ness is indicated by compliance with the refusal. In other words, if nently linked to one's identity. We propose the following hypotheses
X says “no” and Y complies, the refusal is deemed effective. A great based on the above arguments:
deal of prior research on compliance-gaining strategies has focused on
how to influence the actions of others. Overarching themes such as H1. The “don't” refusal frame is more persuasive than the “can't”
“promise” (If you do this, I will reward you) or “threat” (If you do not refusal frame.
do this, you will be punished) reflect common compliance-seeking
strategies (Marwell & Schmitt, 1967; Schenck-Hamlin, Wiseman, & H2. Perceived conviction mediates the influence of refusal frame on
Georgacarakos, 1982). For a summary of compliance-gaining and persuasiveness.
compliance-resisting strategies, see Wilson (2002). While broad
themes have been identified, the use of specific words to gain compli- H3. Attributions to identity mediate the influence of refusal frame on
ance represents a gap in the extant literature. perceived conviction.
392 V.M. Patrick, H. Hagtvedt / Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 29 (2012) 390–394

3. Study 1: persuasive refusal and the role of conviction Table 1


Studies 1 and 2: ANOVA results for the influence of refusal frames on dependent
variables.
3.1. Method and procedure
Study 1 Study 2
The objective of this experiment was to investigate the effective- Don't Can't Don't Can't
ness of identity-based refusal framing in an actual interpersonal con-
M SD M SD M SD M SD
text. The study was focused on the domain of personal finance.
Identity attrib. 6.21⁎ 1.72 5.17⁎ 1.74
Forty-four undergraduate students (male = 63%) participated in this
Conviction 7.06⁎ 1.64 5.52⁎ 1.54 6.80† 1.61 5.63† 2.08
role-playing experiment as part of a lecture on professional selling. Persuasiveness 7.05⁎ 1.32 5.80⁎ 1.70 6.76⁎ 1.43 5.89⁎ 1.55
All of the participants were told that they would be participating in a
All means within each study significantly different across rows.
role-playing exercise in which they were to make a sales pitch to a ⁎ p b .05.
prospective customer. Their task was to sell an annual subscription †
p b .001.
of the local newspaper. They were provided with information about
the newspaper deal (frequency, price, discount rates, etc.). The partic-
ipants were told that the “first few minutes of the negotiation deter- 4.1. Pretest: refusal frames as identity signals
mines the success of the sale,” so they had approximately 3 min to
make their pitch. Each participant individually made a sales pitch to As a pretest, 60 participants completed a survey designed to test
a prospective customer. The prospective customers were confederates whether the words “I don't” are a stronger signal of identity in the
of the experimenter. There were five of these confederates (two male current context than the words “I can't.” The participants were told
and three female), who were told that they had a goal of saving money that one can use different words or phrases when refusing to do
and that they were to use the given strategies to refuse any attempt by something and that the purpose of this study was to understand the
the salesperson to sell them the subscription. Each confederate was language of refusal. They were asked to indicate the extent to which
given a script that acted as a rough guideline regarding how the re- the two phrases “I don't” and “I can't” connote identity. They com-
fusals were to be handled during the actual sales negotiation experi- pleted 3 sets of items with both the “don't” phrase and the “can't”
ment. Each confederate was individually trained by one of the phrase within-subjects on 9-point scales anchored by 1 = not at all
authors to rehearse their refusals. The script included suggestions and 9 = definitely. The items were as follows: “Saying “I don't/
such as “I don't/can't make spontaneous decisions about money,” or can't” do something reflects who one is as a person,” “Saying “I
“I don't/can't spend money without checking my budget first.” don't/can't” do something reflects one's identity,” and “Saying “I
Each participant was randomly assigned to one of five confeder- don't/can't” do something reflects one's beliefs as an individual.”
ates who refused the salesperson's attempts with either the “don't” The three “don't” items were combined to form an index (α = .89),
or the “can't” framing. Neither party was informed of the goals of and the three “can't” items were combined to form an index (α =
the study. After the dyad completed the sales negotiation, the respon- .86). A within-subjects means analysis revealed that the phrase
dents were given a feedback questionnaire in which they evaluated “I don't” is a strong signal of identity compared to the phrase
the prospective customer. Specifically, they reported how convinced “I can't” (Mdon't = 7.12, SD = 1.55 vs. Mcan't = 4.29, SD = 1.94, F(1,
they were by the prospective customer's response (1 = not at 59) = 59.11, p b .05, η 2 = .48).
all, 9 = extremely), the degree of effectiveness of the response
(1 = low degree, 9 = high degree), and how likely they would be 4.2. Method and procedure
to respect the customer's response (1 = not at all, 9 = extremely);
these items were later combined in a persuasion index: α = .75. The Fifty-five individuals participated in this between-subjects experi-
participants also evaluated the customer in terms of how determined, ment regarding the use of refusal framing in a health domain. The par-
committed, and confident they were (1 = not at all, 9 = very much; ticipants were asked to imagine that they were hosting a party. As
later combined in a conviction index: α = .88). they were serving some rich chocolate cake for dessert, one of their
guests refused the cake, saying either “I don't eat chocolate cake” or
“I can't eat chocolate cake,” depending on the condition. The partici-
3.2. Results pants reported the persuasion index (α = .69) and the conviction
index (α = .82) on the same scales as in Study 1. Additionally, they
A one-way ANOVA with the persuasion index as the dependent reported the degree to which the guest's response conveyed his/her
variable revealed the predicted main effect of framing (Mdon't = 7.05, identity (1 = low degree; 9 = high degree) and stance as an individ-
SD= 1.32 vs. Mcan't = 5.80, SD= 1.70, F(1, 42) = 7.33, p b .05, η2 = .15). ual (1 = low degree; 9 = high degree); the items were later combined
This result supports H1. in an identity attribution index (r = .77).
A similar ANOVA with the conviction index as the dependent var-
iable revealed a similar pattern of results (Mdon't = 7.06, SD = 1.64 vs. 4.3. Results
Mcan't = 5.52, SD = 1.54, F(1, 42) = 10.38, p b .05, η 2 = .19); see also
Table 1. Bootstrap estimation (Preacher & Hayes, 2004) with 5000 A one-way ANOVA with the persuasion index as the dependent
resamples, as well as a Sobel test, confirmed that conviction mediates variable revealed the expected main effect of framing (Mdon't = 6.76,
the influence of framing on persuasion (M = − .97, SE = .40, SD= 1.43 vs. Mcan't = 5.89, SD= 1.55, F(1, 53) = 4.73, p b .05, η 2 = .08).
95% CI = − 1.84, − .29; Sobel test: z = −2.71, p b .01). This result This result supports H1.
supports H2. A similar ANOVA with the conviction index as the dependent vari-
able also revealed the expected main effect of framing (Mdon't = 6.80,
SD= 1.61 vs. Mcan't = 5.63 SD= 2.08, F(1, 53) = 5.43, p b .001, η2 = .09).
4. Study 2: persuasive refusal and the role of identity A similar ANOVA with identity attribution as the dependent vari-
able revealed a main effect of framing (Mdon't = 6.21, SD = 1.72 vs.
This study replicates Study 1 in a laboratory experiment in the Mcan't = 5.17, SD = 1.74, F(1, 53) = 5.05, p b .05, η 2 = .09), thereby cor-
health domain. An additional goal was to demonstrate that the influ- roborating the pretest; see also Table 1.
ence of the “don't” framing on conviction stems from attributions to Taken together, H2 and H3 predict a three-path mediation with
the refuser's identity. identity attribution and conviction as successive mediators of the
V.M. Patrick, H. Hagtvedt / Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 29 (2012) 390–394 393

Table 2 framing as well the role of language-based identity attributions in


Study 2: regressions for the mediation of the influence of refusal frames on an interpersonal context.
persuasiveness.

Identity Conviction Persuasiveness 5.1. Implications and directions for future research
(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)
The current research contributes to the identity literature by dem-
Frame .52⁎ .58⁎ .26 .44⁎ .19 .06
Identity .61† .46† .15 onstrating not only how specific words may serve as identity signals
Conviction .52† but also how an identity signal may be used as a tool in persuasion.
R2 .09 .09 .39 .08 .35 .61 As previously discussed, this research also contributes to the litera-
Note: Frame is a binary variable (1 = don't, −1 = can't). Each column (numbered in ture on compliance-gaining and on language in social interactions.
second row) is a separate regression. Regressions (1), (3), and (6) correspond to Furthermore, the practical implications are notable, especially given
steps (a), (b), and (c), respectively. Regressions (2), (4), and (5) are included for that communication strategies based on the use of specific words
completeness. The first row indicates DV. The first column (rows 3–5) indicates
such as “don't” or “can't” may be easily implemented. However,
predictors. The table reports unstandardized regression coefficients.
⁎ p b .05. future research could investigate other word pairs of this kind to

p b .001. broaden the relevance of the current findings. Additionally, a limita-
tion of the current research is that conviction is arguably conceptually
close to persuasiveness; therefore, future research could provide both
effect of refusal frame on persuasiveness (refusal frame → identity → increased precision and increased richness to our understanding of
conviction → persuasiveness). We tested this with the approach the underlying process. Further, although the word “don't” conveys
developed by Baron and Kenny (1986), as adapted to three-path conviction and represents the firm stance of the speaker and conse-
mediation by Taylor, MacKinnon, and Tein (2008) (see Hamilton, quently works well in a negotiation, there is a fine line between firm-
Puntoni, & Tavassoli, 2010 for an example). Following this procedure, ness and rudeness. In adopting this refusal frame, we suggest that the
we estimated a regression for each of the dependent variables (iden- manner in which the words are spoken is as important as the words
tity attribution, conviction, and persuasiveness) with the preceding themselves. Firm but polite refusals are presumably the most effective.
variables in the proposed mediation chain as predictors. In other Future research should investigate linguistic differences, not only in
words, mediation was assessed by estimating the regression of terms of content but also in terms of style. It is also likely that additional
(a) identity attribution on the refusal frame, (b) conviction on the re- words presented alongside the refusal frames could influence the
fusal frame and identity attribution, and (c) persuasiveness on the re- extent to which they convey conviction and identity. For instance, pro-
fusal frame, identity attribution, and conviction; see Table 2 for the viding a time expression such as “always” may reduce the difference in
results. In step (a), the refusal frame is a significant predictor of iden- impact resulting from the “don't” and “can't” framing because “I always
tity attribution (b = .52, t (53) = 2.25, p b .05). In step (b), identity at- can't” implies a permanent rather than temporary stance.
tribution is a significant predictor of conviction (b = .61, t (52) = 4.99, As a general observation, the intersection between language and
p b .001), but the effect of the refusal frame is not significant. In step identity is a fruitful domain of investigation. Although research has
(c), conviction is a significant predictor of persuasiveness (b = .52, suggested that in order to be successful, brands need to tailor them-
t (51) = 5.76, p b .001), but the effects of the refusal frame and identity selves to fit a consumer's identity (Belk, 1988; Berger & Heath, 2007),
attribution are not significant. The results of these regressions are the language used to forge this bond needs to be further investigated.
consistent with the hypothesized three-path mediation. How does one effectively communicate identification with a consumer
We next estimated the magnitude of the indirect (i.e., mediated) or another brand?
effect by using the bias-corrected bootstrap test developed by Recent research on identity signals in the health domain (Berger &
Taylor et al. (2008), drawing 1000 random samples with replacement Rand, 2008) also presents some interesting avenues for future inves-
from the full sample (Hamilton et al., 2010). This bootstrap sample tigation. Expanding this work to understand how best to communi-
served as the basis to construct a bias-corrected confidence interval cate health risks that implicate and make accessible a consumer's
and thus test whether the indirect effect is significantly different identity is a critical issue that remains relatively unexamined. A
from 0. The indirect effect from the original sample is the product of recent research article by Puntoni, Sweldens, and Tavassoli (2011)
the effects revealed in the regressions from steps (a), (b), and investigates how communication cues can make gender identity
(c); in other words, .52 × .61 × .52 = .17 (allowing for more than two salient and consequently trigger defense mechanisms that prevent
decimals in the rounding). The 95% confidence interval (.03, .44) consumers from responding to the information communicated.
excludes 0, indicating a significant indirect effect. The results thus Further research of this nature will help close the gap in our knowledge
support the three-path mediation model predicted by hypotheses 2 about how identity salience can facilitate or interfere with consumer
and 3. information processing.
A related question pertains to how linguistic framing effects might
interact with other identity cues such as sentence structure, tone of
5. General discussion voice, gestures, and facial expressions. In general, future research
could investigate how such cues signal identity and perhaps also
This research is concerned with how to effectively say no. Empirical serve as a feedback mechanism for the individual exhibiting the cues.
findings from two experiments support the notion that the “I don't” Language is a tool that is so broadly used and has such far-
refusal frame is more persuasive than the “I can't” refusal frame and reaching applications and consequences that it is difficult to
conveys greater conviction on the part of speaker. This conviction overestimate the importance of mapping out the influences it has
mediates the influence of the refusal frame on perceived persua- on consumers. Importantly, these influences may not be universal
siveness. Furthermore, identity attributions explain the perceived and might depend on dimensions of accessible identity, cultural dif-
conviction of the refuser. A three-path mediation is therefore ferences, or even the specific circumstances in which they are used.
hypothesized and supported, in which conviction mediates the Furthermore, there may be linguistic influences that are common, or
influence of framing on persuasiveness and identity attributions that diverge markedly, across different languages. Undoubtedly, lan-
mediate the influence of framing on conviction. Study 1 is a guage use in communication is a complex issue, as is the concept of
role-playing experiment, whereas Study 2 is a scenario-based identity, and much research remains to be conducted to increase
experiment. Together, these studies provide insights into refusal our understanding in both these fields of inquiry.
394 V.M. Patrick, H. Hagtvedt / Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 29 (2012) 390–394

References Marwell, G., & Schmitt, R. D. (1967). Dimensions of compliance-gaining behavior: An


empirical analysis. Sociometry, 30(4), 350–364.
Athanasopoulos, P., Damjanovic, L., Krajciova, A., & Sasaki, M. (2011). Representation of Mayer, D. N., & Tormala, L. Z. (2010). “Think” versus “feel” framing effects in persuasion.
colour concepts in bilingual cognition: The case of Japanese blues. Bilingualism: Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(4), 443–454.
Language and Cognition, 14, 9–17. Oyserman, D. (2009). Identity-based motivation: Implications for action-readiness,
Austin, J. L. (1975). How to do things with words. Oxford: Oxford University Press. procedural-readiness, and consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology,
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in 19(3), 250–260.
social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic and statistical considerations. Patrick, V. M., & Hagtvedt, H. (2012). "I don't" versus "I can't": When empowered refusal
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–1182. motivates goal-directed behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(2), 371–381.
Belk, R. W. (1988). Possessions and the extended self. Journal of Consumer Research, Pennebaker, W. J., Mehl, R. M., & Niederhoffer, G. K. (2003). Psychological aspects of
15(2), 139–168. natural language use: Our words, our selves. Annual Review of Psychology, 54,
Berger, J., & Heath, C. (2007). Where consumers diverge from others: Identity-signaling 547–577.
and product domains. Journal of Consumer Research, 34(2), 121–134. Preacher, J. K., & Hayes, F. A. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect
Berger, J., & Rand, L. (2008). Shifting signals to help health: Using identity-signaling to effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, &
reduce risky health behaviors. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(2), 509–518. Computers, 36(4), 717–731.
Bryan, C. J., Walton, G. M., Rogers, T., & Dweck, C. S. (2011). Motivating voter turnout Puntoni, S., Sweldens, S., & Tavassoli, N. (2011). Gender identity salience and perceived
by invoking the self. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(31), vulnerability to breast cancer. Journal of Marketing Research, 48(3), 413–424.
12653–12656. Ross, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (2011). The person and the situation: Perspectives of social
Campbell, R. S., & Pennebaker, W. J. (2003). The secret life of pronouns: Flexibility in psychology (2nd edition). Pinter & Martin.
writing style and physical health. Psychological Science, 14(1), 60–65. Rothman, J. A., Salovey, P., Antone, C., Kelli, K., & Drake, C. M. (1993). The influence of
Chafe, W. (1970). Meaning and the structure of language. Chicago: University of Chicago message framing on intentions to perform health behaviors. Journal of Experimental
Press. Social Psychology, 29(5), 408–433.
Cheema, A., & Patrick, M. V. (2008). Anytime versus only: Mindsets moderate the effect of Sanna, L. J., Parks, C. D., Chang, E. C., & Carter, S. E. (2005). The hourglass is half full or
expansive versus restrictive frames on promotion evaluation. Journal of Marketing half empty: Temporal framing and the group planning fallacy. Group Dynamics:
Research, 45, 462–472. Theory, Research, and Practice, 9(3), 173–188.
Cialdini, B. R. (2006). Influence: The psychology of persuasion. New York: Harper Collins. Schellekens, G. A. C., Verlegh, P. W. J., & Smidts, A. (2010). Language abstraction in
Cialdini, B. R., & Goldstein, J. N. (2004). Social influence: Compliance and conformity. word of mouth. Journal of Consumer Research, 37, 207–223.
Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 591–621. Schenck-Hamlin, J. W., Wiseman, R. L., & Georgacarakos, G. N. (1982). A model of proper-
Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2005). When what you say about others says something ties of compliance gaining strategies. Communication Monographs, 30(2), 92–100.
about you: Language abstraction and inferences about describers' attitudes and Schmitt, B., & Zhang, S. (1998). Language structure and categorization: A study of
goals. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 500–508. classifiers in consumer cognition, judgment and choice. Journal of Consumer
Eckert, P. (2000). Linguistic variation as social practice: The linguistic construction of Research, 25, 108–122.
identity in Belten High. Oxford: Blackwell. Searle, J. R. (1969/1999). Speech acts: An essay in the philosophy of language. Cambridge,
Grice, P. H. (1975). In Peter Cole, & J. L. Morgan (Eds.), Logic and conversation in syntax UK: Cambridge University Press.
and semantics: Speech acts (pp. 41–58). (3rd ed.). New York: Academic. Snyder, C. R., & Higgins, L. R. (1988). Excuses: Their effective role in the negotiation of
Hamilton, R. W., Puntoni, S., & Tavassoli, N. T. (2010). Categorization by groups and reality. Psychological Bulletin, 104(1), 23–35.
individuals. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 112, 70–81. Taylor, A. B., MacKinnon, D. P., & Tein, J. -Y. (2008). Tests of the three-path mediated
Hoegg, J., & Alba, W. J. (2007). Taste perception: More than meets the tongue. Journal of effect. Organizational Research Methods, 11(2), 241–269.
Consumer Research, 33, 490–498. Tykocinski, O., Higgins, T. E., & Chaiken, S. (1994). Message framing, self-discrepancies,
Kettle, K. L., & Häubl, G. (2011). The signature effect: Signing influences consumption- and yielding to persuasive messages: The motivational significance of psychological
related behavior by priming self-identity. Journal of Consumer Research, 38, situations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20(1), 107–115.
474–489. White, K., & Dahl, D. W. (2007). Are all out-groups created equal? Consumer identity
Labov, W. (1963). The social motivation of a sound change. Word, 18, 1–42. and dissociative influence. Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 525–536.
LeBoeuf, A. R. (2006). Discount rates for time versus dates: The sensitivity of Whorf, L. B. (1956). Language, thought, and reality. Boston: MIT Press.
discounting to time-interval description. Journal of Marketing Research, 43(1), Wilson, R. S. (2002). Seeking and resisting compliance: Why people say what they do
59–72. when trying to influence others. Sage Publications.
Levin, P. I., & Gaeth, J. G. (1988). How consumers are affected by the framing of attribute
information before and after consuming the product. Journal of Consumer Research,
15, 374–378.

You might also like