Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

Review

Special Issue: The Genetics of Cognition

Genetics of autism spectrum disorders


Daniel H. Geschwind1,2,3
1
Program in Neurogenetics, Department of Neurology, David Geffen School of Medicine, University of California, Los Angeles,
CA 90095, USA
2
Center for Autism Research and Treatment, Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, David Geffen School of
Medicine, University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA
3
Departments of Neurology, Psychiatry, and Human Genetics, David Geffen School of Medicine, University of California,
Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA

Characterized by a combination of abnormalities in lan- Kanner in 1943 [1], where he described 11 children with
guage, social cognition and mental flexibility, autism is autism, mostly boys with a combination of severe social and
not a single disorder but a neurodevelopmental syn- variable language dysfunction and the presence of repetitive
drome commonly referred to as autism spectrum disor- restrictive behaviors. Kanner made numerous interesting
der (ASD). Several dozen ASD susceptibility genes have observations based on these case studies, including the
been identified in the past decade, collectively account- identification of large head size in about half of the subjects
ing for 10–20% of ASD cases. These findings, although and postulated a biological genetic basis for the disorder.
demonstrating that ASD is etiologically heterogeneous, However, until the 1980s autism was not considered a
provide important clues about its pathophysiology. Di- distinct disorder in the manuals of psychiatric diagnosis,
verse genetic and genomic approaches provide evidence nor was it considered by most to be biologically based.
converging on disruption of key biological pathways, A major change in perspective came with the pioneering
many of which are also implicated in other allied neu- twin studies of Rutter and Folstein that demonstrated a
rodevelopmental disorders. Knowing the genes involved
in ASD provides us with a crucial tool to probe both the
specificity of ASD and the shared neurobiological and Glossary
cognitive features across what are considered clinically Complex genetics: a term used in contrast to Mendelian patterns of
distinct disorders, with the goal of linking gene to brain inheritance, whereby instead of being dominantly or recessively inherited a
phenotype is caused by multiple genes that could interact with each other and/
circuits to cognitive function. or the environment.
Concordance: the rate at which a second individual has the same phenotype as
Autism genetics: a decade of progress the first, for example in the second sibling of a pair, or the second twin.
Copy number variation: a greater than one Kb change in chromosomal
In many ways, autism is a mysterious disorder because it complement so that a particular region or chromosome diverges from the
involves core abnormalities in social cognition and lan- normal diploid human copy number (aneuploidy) over that region. A deletion
guage, both of which are central to what makes us human. would yield one copy, and a duplication of the region would yield 3 copies.
Copy number changes can be inherited or de novo.
Because of this, understanding autism will have a signifi- De novo: a genetic alteration that arises either in the gamete or in the fertilized
cant impact on our basic knowledge of these fundamental egg during its early development but is not observed in the somatic cells of the
cognitive processes, in addition to the obvious crucial role parent.
Endophenotype: a heritable phenotypic feature that is closely related to a
that such mechanistic understanding has on therapeutic disorder but represents a more narrow or simple component than the disorder
development. Until the past decade, virtually nothing was itself. An endophenotype should be observed in first-degree relatives more
known about the neurobiological basis of autism spectrum frequently than in the general population and might be quantitative rather than
binary.
disorder (ASD). There was no consistent neuropathology Epigenetic: non-sequence based changes to DNA that affect transcription,
and only scant knowledge of a few causal genetic factors. leading to changes in the expression of the gene product. Known major
epigenetic modifications include DNA methylation and histone modifications,
The past decade has brought an explosion of genetic find-
such as acetylation or methylation.
ings in ASD, surpassing most other common neuropsychi- Heritability: in general this refers to the extent to which a particular trait is due
atric disorders, so that we now have knowledge of the to inherited DNA sequence variation. A heritability of 1.0 indicates that a trait is
100% heritable and no environmental component contributes. However,
etiology of ASD for between 10 and 20% of cases. Here, heritability is an estimate based on the specific measurements taken in a
I review these exciting findings with respect to what they particular population, and therefore is only a rough guide.
tell us about the genetic architecture of ASD and how this Mendelian inheritance: a dominant or recessive pattern of inheritance of a trait
first defined by Gregor Mendel.
informs our mechanistic understanding of the disorder and Penetrance: the frequency at which a particular phenotype is observed, given a
in a broader sense, human brain function. specific genotype. Penetrance is always relative to the phenotype in question.
Pleiotropy: occurs when a gene influences more than one phenotype.
Polymorphism: a particular variant that is observed in more than 1% (common)
Definition and evolution of ASD in the population. A SNP is a single DNA base pair change.
Autism is a developmental neuropsychiatric syndrome with Proband: the index case being studied usually based on it having a particular
onset before the age of three. The fundamental conceptuali- phenotype or disorder.
Whole genome or whole exome sequencing: the use of new high-throughput
zation of the disorder is based on the initial observation of Next Generation Sequencing methods to sequence whole genomes or the
protein coding portion of the genome (exome) in patients and controls.
Corresponding author: Geschwind, D.H. (dhg@mednet.ucla.edu).

1364-6613/$ – see front matter ß 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.07.003 Trends in Cognitive Sciences, September 2011, Vol. 15, No. 9 409
Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences September 2011, Vol. 15, No. 9

genetic susceptibility to the disorder, providing incontro- rence rates [11], how shared environment would have a
vertible evidence as to its biological origins [2,3]. Over the more major role than genetics is not clear. Moreover,
past two decades, the concept of autism has broadened from studies in families show that first-degree relatives of autis-
the strict diagnosis of autistic disorder to include those with tic probands have a markedly increased risk for autism
normal intelligence and language (Asperger syndrome) and relative to the population, consistent with a strong famil-
those almost meeting the strict diagnostic criteria in all ial, or genetic, effect observed in twins [12]. This is not to
three domains (pervasive development disorder – not oth- dispute the role of the environment but to emphasize that
erwise specified, PDD-NOS). These diagnoses are not based genes play an important role.
on etiology but on expert observation and assessment of First-degree relatives of ASD probands have an in-
behavior and cognition; they can be considered arbitrary: crease in behavioral or cognitive features associated with
which category one fits into depends partially on subjective autism, such as social or language dysfunction, albeit in
criteria. This is further emphasized by the current trend in lesser forms, when compared with the population preva-
the DSM-V (http://www.dsm5.org/proposedrevision/Pages/ lence [13]. This has been called ‘the broader phenotype’
NeurodevelopmentalDisorders.aspx), in which the category and includes restrictive repetitive behaviors and sub-
of Asperger syndrome is removed and the diagnostic criteria threshold deficits in social cognition, as well as language
for autism are modified under the new heading of ASD. This dysfunction [14–17]. For example, language delay (LD) is
change in diagnostic criteria is not based on known similar- observed in a significant proportion of nonautistic siblings
ities or differences in causation between these clinically of autistic probands [18,19]. Similarly, autistic-like social
defined categories but rather on the consensus of opinions impairment clearly is heritable [20] and increased in
of expert clinicians. unaffected parents and children of autistic probands
Concurrently, over the past two decades, the notion [21]. Studies using multiple measures of subthreshold
that autism represents a quantitative spectrum of impair- autistic traits in population cohorts suggest that different
ments, rather than representing discrete disorders, has components, separately representing language, social
gained increasing traction for researchers [4,5]. From this function and repetitive or stereotyped behaviors contrib-
perspective, the term ASD attempts to crystalize the ute to ASD [22–24]. On aggregate, these data suggest that
notion that patients represent a clinically variable popu- different features of autism represent a quantitative con-
lation that suffers from pathologic levels of quantitative tinuum of function that could be inherited in distinct
variation in the major cognitive and behavioral domains patterns. This is consistent with the knowledge that spe-
that are disrupted, rather than a distinct clinical disorder. cific genetic factors contribute to the development and
How these two varying conceptualizations, autism as a function of specific brain structures, and the hypothesis
unitary disorder versus a spectrum of dysfunction, relate that distinct brain circuits might underlie different com-
to underlying etiologies is a key question facing the field. ponents of autism [25].
Furthermore, how the clinical domains relate to underlying It has also been known for several decades that many
dysfunction in specific cognitive domains is essentially un- rare medical or genetic conditions are associated with
known, although some clues are starting to emerge [6,7]. autism. Dozens of genetic syndromes including Joubert
Syndrome, Smith–Lemli–Opitz syndrome, Tuberous Scle-
Autism is a complex genetic disorder rosis and Fragile X are known to cause autism, although
Perhaps the biggest advance in understanding autism many with less than 50% penetrance [26,27]. Although this
pathophysiology has been the appreciation of a significant provided strong evidence in favor of a genetic cause for
genetic contribution to the etiology of ASD. Three main ASD, these syndromic forms were considered exceptional
areas of evidence support a genetic etiology in ASD: twin cases, and not relevant for common forms of idiopathic
studies, comparing monozygotic twins (MZ) and dizogotic autism because each was rare; none account for more than
twins (DZ), family studies comparing the rate of autism in 1% of ASD cases, and most are far less common [27].
first degree relatives of affected probands (see Glossary) Instead, similar to other common diseases with genetic
versus the population, and studies of rare genetic syn- contributions, autism was thought to fit a model in which
dromes with a comorbid autism diagnosis. Because MZ multiple common variants, each with small to moderate
twins share 100% of their genetic material and DZ twins effect sizes, interact with each other and perhaps in some
share 50% (similar to nontwin siblings) and both share the cases, environmental factors, to lead to autism; a situation
in utero environment with their twin, higher disease co- referred to as complex genetics [27]. In contrast to Mende-
occurrence in MZ twins than DZ twins supports a genetic lian genetics, under the complex genetic model, specific
etiology. This is what is observed in every twin study in common variants in genes increase susceptibility to ASD
ASD; overall consistent with heritability estimates of but each is not on its own sufficient to be causal (Figure 1).
about 70–80% [8,9]. One exception is a very recent study These same variants contribute to normal variation in
with a large sample of twins, which, despite showing a cognition and behavior in unaffected individuals but when
concordance of about 0.6 for MZ twins and 0.25 for DZ combined unfortunately as they are in the autistic pro-
twins (consistent with a Falconer heritability of 0.7), comes band, they cause ASD. This model would explain the
to the conclusion that shared environment plays a larger presence of subthreshold traits in nonautistic first-degree
role than genetic factors [10]. Given that the concordance relatives, such as the siblings or parents of autistic indi-
for strict autism in DZ twins (who can be considered viduals, because they would be expected to harbor a subset
genetic siblings who share the same in utero environment) of the genetic variants that cause autism in the affected
is not clearly different from modern infant sibling recur- individual.

410
(Figure_1)TD$IG][ Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences September 2011, Vol. 15, No. 9

Effect size this regard, two observations could be informative. First,


de novo CNV are observed in 5–10% of ASD probands but
they are also observed in 1–2% of unaffected controls; so all
Mutation
such CNV are not necessarily causal or fully penetrant
screens
[36]. From this perspective, some CNV could be acting as
complex genetic risk factors, with intermediate effect sizes,
Frequency
and variable penetrance and expressivity. Such is certainly
the case with 16p11 duplications, which are observed in
ASD, schizophrenia (SZ) and various forms of developmen-
tal delay (DD) [37–39], as well as Neurexin 1 CNV, which
Association are observed in about 50% of unaffected carriers [36], and
studies observed across several neurodevelopmental disorders
TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences (Table 1). Such pleiotropy and variable penetrance and
expressivity are the rule, rather than the exception (Table
Figure 1. Common and rare variants. We usually think of common and rare
variants representing two extremes on the allelic spectrum. SNPs with modest 1). Second, the frequency of de novo mutations is signifi-
effects sizes (gray oval) have been identified through association studies. Rare cantly lower in multiplex (familial) cases of autism versus
events that segregate with disease have also been identified through resequencing
those with only one affected proband, so called simplex
efforts and CNV studies. Less attention has been directed at events not at either
extreme but also likely to modulate risk. Larger studies will permit consideration of families [30,31,34]. This difference in the frequency of rare
lower frequency intermediate effect alleles (ovals with dotted lines). Current data de novo CNV in families with different structures (simplex
do not support the presence of common alleles of large effect for affection status
(bottom right corner). However, it is possible that such associations might be
vs multiplex) suggests that the contribution from different
observed in the future when appropriate quantitative endophenotypes are used. types of genetic variation between simplex and multiplex
Adapted with permission from [87]. autism might differ. Recent family data based on the social
responsiveness scale, which is a quantitative measure of
The role of rare mutations versus common autism-related features heavily weighted on social factors,
polymorphisms (CPs) in ASD suggests that this is indeed the case [40].
A series of important findings over the past four years This discussion of rare and common genetic variants
clearly challenges the notion that autism is mainly caused highlights an area of major tension in autism genetic
by combinations of common variants by identifying a large research. The causal nature of Mendelian (recessive or
number of rare, recurrent and nonrecurrent mutations dominantly acting) mutations is fairly easy to establish
that lead to ASD. At the same time, whole genome associ- because there is essentially a strong correspondence be-
ation studies with common variants, although identifying tween the presence of a mutation and a disease phenotype.
a few loci with very small effect sizes, have not yielded Certainly, dominant or recessive mutations are easier to
independently replicated results [28,29]. These rare muta- model in vitro and in vivo. However, few such variants
tions, mostly in the form of submicroscopic chromosomal cause autism alone but instead could cause a wide variety
structural variation, called copy number variants (CNV), of phenotypes (variable expressivity) with or without au-
are now known to account for up to 10% of cases of tism, ranging from intellectual disability (ID), epilepsy,
idiopathic autism (those with no obvious clinical syn- psychosis and global DD to neurotypical [27,41]. Thus,
drome) [30–34]. Because many of these CNV have large even for the simpler Mendelian mutations, understanding
effect sizes and thus are thought sufficient to cause ASD, which aspects of the phenotype observed in a model system
they are predicted to significantly reduce reproductive or a human patient relate to autism, and which might
fitness. Consistent with this, these causal CNV are often relate to more global aspects of cognitive dysfunction,
not transmitted from the parent but instead occur de novo needs to be carefully established. This provides a great
in the germline [33,35]. However, in some cases, such as opportunity for detailed phenotypic study of patients with
CNV at 16p11 and 15q11–13, the CNV are transmitted and without autism who harbor such rare causal muta-
from an unaffected parent to cause the disorder in an tions, so as to connect specific aspects of brain structure
offspring [33]. The genetic or epigenetic mechanism of and function with cognitive and behavioral phenotypes
reduced penetrance for ASD in the mutation-carrying that underlie the clinical features of the disease.
parent is not known. However, it is also probable that Common variants, although not causal for the disease,
the parent carriers of such CNV have more subtle neuro- seem to have more subtle effects, making them harder to
psychiatric or cognitive phenotypes that have not yet been model in vitro or in animal models. However, perhaps
systematically identified. because they are common in human populations and could
The first key issue in genotype–phenotype mapping in have more subtle or specific effects on particular aspects of
ASD is reconciling the role of causal rare de novo variants cognition and behavior, common risk variants might be
that occur in probands and are not inherited by the unaf- easier to relate to particular brain phenotypes or circuits in
fected siblings with the observations of broader subthresh- humans. This issue will be discussed below in more detail
old traits in siblings. The latter suggests the role of with respect to imaging-genetic findings.
common variation but as mentioned above, the genetic Last, it should also be emphasized that although they
evidence for specific common variants in ASD so far has are often presented as dichotomous models, the contribu-
been sparse. De novo events are major-effect genetic muta- tions of rare and common variants to ASD in an individual
tions that are mainly observed in probands, and are there- are not mutually exclusive [25]. For example, although
fore not expected to contribute to the broader phenotype. In rare de novo CNV are considered causal mutations, it is

411
Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences September 2011, Vol. 15, No. 9

Table 1. Pleiotropic effects of major genes/mutations associated with ASD and allied neurodevelopmental disorders
Gene/region Mechanism Disorders References
NRXN1 CNV, PM ASD, SZ [36], [50,72,73]
CNTNAP2 CNV, PM, CP ASD, ID, epilepsy, LD/SLI, TS [59,61,74,75]
16pdel CNV ASD, SZ, DD, LD, normal carrier [33,37,42,76–78]
16pdup CNV SZ, ID, DD, LD, ADHD, normal carrier [33,39,42,78]
15q13.3del CNV SZ, epilepsy, ASD, normal carrier [42,73,79]
17q12del CNV SZ, ASD, ID [80]
15q11–13dup CNV ASD, SZ/psychosis [33,81]
22q11 CNV ASD, ADHD, SZ, ID, epilepsy [73,82–84]
1q21 CNV ASD, SZ, ID, epilepsy [42,85,86]
Here we list genes, form of genetic risk variant, clinical disorders where the mutation has been observed, and some representative references. This table is not meant to be
exhaustive but illustrative of the pleiotropic effects of known ASD genes or loci with relatively large effect sizes (OR > 5–10 for ASD). New abbreviations are as follows: TS,
Tourette syndrome and PM, point mutation.

clear that mutations could have intermediate effect sizes Converging circuits and common pathways
and variable penetrance, as mentioned above [36,42], so Convergence of pathways could be assessed at many levels,
such mutations must be interacting with other factors. ranging from molecular mechanisms to brain circuitry. The
Penetrance less than 50% for ASD is also observed in many earliest synthetic molecular model was based on the notion
Mendelian forms of ASD, such as Fragile X, Jouberts that the primary area of convergence in ASD was the
Syndrome and Tuberous Sclerosis Complex (TSC), so other postnatal, experience-dependent development of the syn-
genetic, epigenetic or environmental factors must be con- apse [48]. This was a highly productive model based on
tributing to autism causation in such cases as well [27]. several forms of Mendelian mutations in ASD, which has
Thus, different rare variants could act as either suscepti- led to successful exploration of synaptic scaffolding mole-
bility or strongly causal alleles, and, in conjunction with cules and other synaptic genes as ASD susceptibility genes
common variation, might contribute in differing propor- [27,46]. Interestingly, because many mRNA for synaptic
tions to ASD risk in individual patients [25]. Fortunately, genes are alternatively spliced and the proteins regulated
over the next several years, whole genome or exome se- by ubiquitination, this paper was prescient in predicting
quencing, if performed in large enough populations, will alterations in mRNA splicing and ubiquitination pathways
settle this issue by providing an empiric assessment of the that have since been identified [49,50]. Nevertheless, be-
relative contributions of both rare and common alleles of cause the synapse is integral to the function of all neurons,
different effect sizes [43,44]. how the cognitive and behavioral specificity of ASD
emerges distinctly from ID or other synaptic disorders,
The molecular diversity of ASD is not yet explained by this molecularly focused model.
Several recent reviews summarize the growing list of Belmonte and colleagues have articulated a cogent
dozens of common and rare genetic variants that have unifying model based on imaging, electrophysiological
been associated with ASD at varying levels of statistical and anatomical data, which implicates abnormal neural
evidence [27,41,45–47], so I refer the reader to these connectivity in disrupting timing and information proces-
reviews for comprehensive gene lists. Here, instead, I will sing [51,52]. This general model was subsequently anatom-
try to synthesize what these findings might be telling us ically refined and modified to focus primarily on reduced
about ASD pathophysiology at what is still an early junc- long-range, and increased short-range frontal lobe connec-
ture in the search for ASD susceptibility genes. tivity [53]. Since the general connectivity model was pro-
The most obvious general conclusion from all of the posed, many subsequent studies provide strong supporting
published genetic studies is the extraordinary etiological data, including a recent study showing weak interhemi-
heterogeneity of ASD. No specific gene accounts for the spheric functional connectivity in young toddlers with ASD
majority of ASD; rather, even the most common genetic [54]. This study is particularly important because it shows
forms account for not more than 1–2% of cases [27]. Fur- altered connectivity in developing language circuits to be
ther, these genes, including those mentioned earlier, rep- an early (and thus potentially causal) feature of the disor-
resent a diversity of molecular mechanisms, ranging from der, which could not be inferred from studies of older
cell adhesion, synaptic vesicle release and neurotransmis- children or adults.
sion, synaptic structure, RNA processing/splicing, and ac- A more recent model [55] synthesizes several key fea-
tivity-dependent protein translation. On one hand, this tures of the synaptic and connectivity models above in
should not be surprising because autism is defined based conjunction with genetic findings, and is based on devel-
on observation of cognition and behavior, not etiology. On opmental disconnection of frontal and temporal cortical
the other hand, the diversity of potential mechanisms and regions. This model strives to explain the diversity of
the apparent lack of specificity of mutations for ASD begs genetic findings without necessitating a particular molec-
the question as to whether ASD should even be viewed as a ular mechanism. Instead, it relies on a behavioral neuro-
unitary disorder. Therefore, asking whether the diverse logic framework to integrate many potential levels of
genes implicated in autism might converge on common molecular dysfunction, from synapse formation and matu-
pathways becomes an important question for understand- ration, vesicle trafficking and signaling, to defective neu-
ing autism and developing new therapeutics. ronal migration, interneuron development, dendritic

412
(Figure_2)TD$IG][ Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences September 2011, Vol. 15, No. 9

Stochastic or
Environmental processes

(dup) 15q11-13
•Abnormal neuronal migration
•Abnormalities in patterning Developmental
TSC1
disconnection
(regionalization or connectivity)
of critical brain
FMR1 •Faulty wiring/axon pathfinding
circuits:
•Aberrant synaptogenesis or
•Fronto-striatal
(del) 22q synaptic function
•Fronto-temporal
•Dendritic abnormalities
•Fronto-parietal
(del) 16p •Dysfunctional neural
transmission
CACNA1C

ASD

Genetic modifier loci


(CNTNAP2, MET, rs4307059, etc…)

TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences

Figure 2. Convergence of genes on neural systems. Here we illustrate a working model for how autism risk alleles with major effects, such as those shown at the left, might
act to lead to autism. Because these Mendelian conditions are not specific for ASD and typically lead to ID, either environmental or genetic factors must modify their effects
on brain development. The biological pathways through which such genes are known to act (shown in the center box) are myriad. Although gene expression and proteomic
studies identify molecular and biological pathways that provide a source of convergence, the ultimate convergence must lie in neural systems. At a neural systems level, the
convergent process will likely be disconnection of the circuits outlined in the far right box because these systems are thought to underlie the core deficits of ASD. Adapted
with permission from [88].

maturation or axon path finding, all of which could con- that the broad syndrome of ASD can be broken down into
tribute to functional disconnection [55] (Figure 2). Indeed many component or intermediate phenotypes, referred to
several of the monogenic causes of ASD, including TSC [56] as endophenotypes. The familial segregation of endophe-
and Joubert Syndrome [57,58] show manifestations of notypes provides a genetic basis for the broader phenotype
abnormal axon path finding or migration abnormalities described earlier. A logical extension of this concept is that
that are known to lead to alterations in brain structural these endophenotypes represent one end of the continuum
connectivity. The study of common variants, for example in of the normal spectrum of behavior and cognition [25].
the gene CNTNAP2, also supports disconnection as a Several groups have demonstrated that this is indeed
causal phenomenon based on initial imaging-genetic evi- the case with respect to CNTNAP2 [60,61,63] because
dence. Two SNPs in a relatively circumscribed area of variants in the same region of the gene that increase risk
CNTNAP2 have been shown to modulate language func- for LD in ASD, increase risk for specific language im-
tion in ASD and other conditions [59–61]. Scott-Van Zee- pairment (SLI) and modify language ability in the general
land and colleagues [62] showed long-range disconnection population. Similarly, CNTNAP2 single nucleotide poly-
of medial prefrontal cortex and the precuneus, and short- morphism (SNP) [64] has been shown to modulate brain
range overconnectivity of the prefrontal region in carriers morphology in several ASD-related cortical regions in
of the CNTNAP2 risk allele, regardless of whether they normal controls [65].
were autistic or neurotypical. This finding is particularly However, specificity for ASD is not only an issue for rare
important because the genetic mediation of this disconnec- Mendelian variants but also for common variation [66].
tion supports its causal nature. Because the effects of genes on behavior are mediated
through neural circuits that mediate a multiplicity of
Endophenotypes, common variants and domain functions (e.g. frontal-striatal circuits), their pleiotropic
specificity in ASD effects on behavior and cognition should not be surprising.
The relations between specific genetic variants and specific In fact, pleiotropy should be the rule, rather than the
cognitive processes, such as language, highlight the notion exception. Recently, CNTNAP2 variants have been associ-

413
Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences September 2011, Vol. 15, No. 9

ated with mutism and social anxiety [67]. This complex the first strong evidence for both a shared genetic and
relation between genetic variation and phenotype high- an environmental/epigenetic basis for ASD and the pres-
lights the importance of in-depth exploration of cognitive ence of an early developmental patterning defect. It is
and neurobehavioral phenotypes in ASD patients, their tempting to speculate that the abnormalities in cortical
family members and in the general population. Phenotype patterning and interneuron function provide a molecular
discovery and the relation of core ASD-related endophe- basis for cortical–cortical and cortical–fugal disconnection,
notypes to each other and specific brain circuits in patients linking molecular abnormalities with anatomical, physio-
with ASD and related neurodevelopmental disorders logical and imaging findings.
remains a key but underexplored area. The second study on molecular convergence in ASD
identified protein interactors of known ASD or ASD-asso-
Evidence for converging molecular pathways ciated genes [70]. This interactome revealed several novel
Several recent studies have suggested that in addition to interactions, including between two ASD candidate genes,
convergent brain pathways, there could as well be conver- Shank3 and TSC1. The biological pathways identified in
gence at the level of molecular mechanisms in ASD. One this study include synapse, cytoskeleton and GTPase sig-
class of such studies has asked whether putative ASD naling, demonstrating a remarkable overlap with those
susceptibility genes are enriched in members for specific identified by the gene expression and CNV pathway stud-
molecular or biological processes more than expected by ies discussed above. This study differs from other genome-
chance. The value of this approach depends on the level of wide studies (such as mRNA expression) because it begins
experimental support for the specific genes tested and the with known ASD genes and asks about their relations. So
degree to which current pathway annotations represent although it is ‘–-omic’ in nature [71], it is not as unsuper-
reality [25,68]. For genes identified within CNV this can be vised as methods that survey the genome more agnosti-
particularly problematic because most known pathological cally. Despite the significant heterogeneity in ASD, these
CNV contain more than one gene and it is not expected that studies using diverse methods identify several common
all genes within the CNV contribute to ASD, potentially areas of molecular convergence in ASD. Understanding
increasing noise in this analysis. One recent study [69] how these pathways relate to individual differences now
reduced such background by using a new phenotype-driven becomes an important research priority.
method to group genes within high confidence de novo CNV
[34,69], identifying significant enrichment for several cat- Concluding remarks
egories of genes, including axon outgrowth, synaptogen- Many genes have been identified for ASD and some biologi-
esis, cell–cell adhesion, GTPase signaling and the actin cally coherent functional pathways that link these genes are
cytoskeleton. These results replicate and extend earlier emerging. Few of the genes, whether contributing by com-
composite pathway analysis of putative ASD susceptibility mon or rare variation, are specific to ASD but instead
genes compiled from the literature [68], and CNV pathway variably contribute to genetic risk for ASD, ID, SZ, SLI,
analysis in the Autism Genetic Resource Exchange and epilepsy and even attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
other cohorts [32,36]. However, these studies place ASD (ADHD). Therefore, understanding the specificity of indi-
genes within a multiplicity of pathways, several of which vidual variants to ASD is a crucial challenge that requires
are broad and do not necessarily demonstrate convergence several advances to be made (Box 1). Diseases of cognition
on final common molecular processes in individuals. and behavior have their basis in brain circuit dysfunction.
In this regard, two recent studies use different systems Therefore, we need to understand how specific genetic risk
biology approaches to provide a new perspective on the variants lead to changes in neural circuitry and function in
concept of molecular convergence. The first, an analysis of those with and without a specific diagnosis. This will proba-
gene expression in postmortem autism brain, provides bly involve understanding gene–gene and gene–environ-
strong evidence for a shared set of molecular alterations ment interactions, as well as epigenetics, areas that have
in a majority of cases of ASD. This included disruption of not yet been explored in ASD. We need to understand how
the normal gene expression pattern that differentiates
frontal and temporal lobes (consistent with an early devel-
Box 1. Priorities for future genetic research in ASD
opmental patterning defect), and two groups of genes
dysregulated in ASD brains: one related to neuronal func-  Delineate areas of molecular convergence between diverse
tion, and the other to immune/inflammatory responses mutational mechanisms underlying ASD susceptibility using
[49]. The neuronal function genes were enriched in genetic unbiased systems biology methods.
 Perform whole genome sequencing in large numbers of patients
association signals, providing evidence that these changes
with a variety of neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders to
were causal, rather than the consequence of the disease clearly define genetic overlap and differences in susceptibility.
[49]; in contrast, the immune/inflammatory changes did  Define epigenetic contributions to ASD.
not show a strong genetic signal, implicating environmen-  Define appropriate endophenotypes through family case-control
tal or epigenetic factors instead. It is also notable that the designs, so as to have a sound basis for understanding cross
disorder genotype–phenotype relations.
several of the same biological pathways identified in this  Translate the knowledge of mutational basis of ASD into
gene expression study overlapped with the pathway anal- mechanistic understanding at the cellular and circuit levels.
ysis of CNV described above. This analysis of postmortem  Characterize subtypes of ASD based on molecular or genetic
autism brain also showed downregulation of several mar- signatures and relate them to trajectory and treatment response.
kers of GABAergic interneurons, suggesting potential in-  Identify environmental modifiers and gene- by- environment
interactions.
hibitory interneuron dysfunction. These results provide

414
Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences September 2011, Vol. 15, No. 9

the implicated neural circuitry relates to specific cognitive 18 Gamliel, I. et al. (2007) The development of young siblings of children
with autism from 4 to 54 months. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 171–183
and behavioral elements, and we need to link the neural
19 Ben-Yizhak, N. et al. (2011) Pragmatic language and school related
circuitry and behaviors to disease. Additionally, such cir- linguistic abilities in siblings of children with autism. J. Autism Dev.
cuits are related to specific aspects of human higher cogni- Disord. 41, 750–760
tion and a more detailed notion of the autism cognitive 20 Constantino, J.N. and Todd, R.D. (2005) Intergenerational
phenotype is required. Components of this understanding transmission of subthreshold autistic traits in the general
population. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 655–660
have been achieved in a preliminary manner for only a few
21 Constantino, J.N. et al. (2006) Autistic social impairment in the
elements but to understand ASD from a systems level siblings of children with pervasive developmental disorders. Am. J.
mechanistic perspective, we will need to integrate all of Psychiatry 163, 294–296
these levels of understanding [71]. This will no doubt involve 22 Steer, C.D. et al. (2010) Traits contributing to the autistic spectrum.
the use of model systems, including mouse and inverte- PLoS ONE 5, e12633
23 Ronald, A. et al. (2006) Genetic heterogeneity between the three
brates, so it will be important to be cognizant of evolutionary components of the autism spectrum: a twin study. J. Am. Acad.
similarity and divergence in the interpretation of such data Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 45, 691–699
with respect to a disorder of human higher cognition. Re- 24 Ronald, A. et al. (2010) A twin study of autism symptoms in Sweden.
gardless of the challenges, the identification of causal ge- Mol. Psychiatry DOI: 10.1038/mp.2010.82
netic variants provides a starting point for a new 25 Geschwind, D.H. (2008) Autism: many genes, common pathways? Cell
135, 391–395
mechanistic understanding of ASD, enabling connection 26 Gillberg, C. and Coleman, M. (1985) The Biology of the Autistic
of genes to brain to cognition and behavior. Syndromes, Greenwood Publishing Group
27 Abrahams, B.S. and Geschwind, D.H. (2008) Advances in autism
Acknowledgments genetics: on the threshold of a new neurobiology. Nat. Rev. Genet. 9,
I thank Lauren Kawaguchi for her editorial assistance and Brett 341–355
Abrahams for permission to use a previously published figure. Our 28 Wang, K. et al. (2009) Common genetic variants on 5p14.1 associate
laboratory’s work in autism and the molecular basis of human higher with autism spectrum disorders. Nature 459, 528–533
cognition is supported by two Autism Center of Excellence grants from 29 Anney, R. et al. (2010) A genome-wide scan for common alleles affecting
NIMH: ACE Network 5R01MH081754-03 to D.H.G and ACE Center risk for autism. Hum. Mol. Genet. 19, 4072–4082
5P50HD055784-03 (S. Bookheimer PI, D.H.G. co-PI), a Merit award from 30 Sebat, J. et al. (2007) Strong association of de novo copy number
NIMH to D.H.G., 4R37MH060233-10, as well as funding from Autism mutations with autism. Science 316, 445–449
Speaks and The Simons Foundation. 31 Marshall, C.R. et al. (2008) Structural variation of chromosomes in
autism spectrum disorder. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 82, 477–488
References 32 Pinto, D. et al. (2010) Functional impact of global rare copy number
1 Kanner, L. (1943) Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nerv. Child variation in autism spectrum disorders. Nature 466, 368–372
2, 217–250 33 Sanders, S.J. et al. (2011) Multiple recurrent de novo CNVs, including
2 Folstein, S. and Rutter, M. (1977) Infantile autism: a genetic study of 21 duplications of the 7q11.23 Williams Syndrome region, are strongly
twin pairs. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 18, 297–321 associated with autism. Neuron 70, 863–885
3 Folstein, S. and Rutter, M. (1977) Genetic influences and infantile 34 Levy, D. et al. (2011) Rare de novo and transmitted copy-number
autism. Nature 265, 726–728 variation in autistic spectrum disorders. Neuron 70, 886–897
4 Wing, L. (1988) The continuum of autistic characteristics. In Diagnosis 35 Beaudet, A.L. (2007) Autism: highly heritable but not inherited. Nat.
and Assessment in Autism (Schopler, E.M.G., ed.), pp. 91–110, Plenum Med. 13, 534–536
5 Constantino, J.N. and Todd, R.D. (2003) Autistic traits in the general 36 Bucan, M. et al. (2009) Genome-wide analyses of exonic copy number
population: a twin study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 60, 524–530 variants in a family-based study point to novel autism susceptibility
6 Mundy, P. et al. (2010) Self-referenced processing, neurodevelopment genes. PLoS Genet. 5, e1000536
and joint attention in autism. Autism 14, 408–429 37 Fernandez, B.A. et al. (2010) Phenotypic spectrum associated with de
7 Charman, T. et al. (2011) Defining the cognitive phenotype of autism. novo and inherited deletions and duplications at 16p11.2 in individuals
Brain Res. 1380, 10–21 ascertained for diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder. J. Med. Genet.
8 Bailey, A. et al. (1995) Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: evidence 47, 195–203
from a British twin study. Psychol. Med. 25, 63–77 38 Bijlsma, E.K. et al. (2009) Extending the phenotype of recurrent
9 Rosenberg, R.E. et al. (2009) Characteristics and concordance of autism rearrangements of 16p11.2: deletions in mentally retarded patients
spectrum disorders among 277 twin pairs. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. without autism and in normal individuals. Eur. J. Med. Genet. 52,
163, 907–914 77–87
10 Hallmayer, J. et al. (2011) Genetic heritability and shared 39 McCarthy, S.E. et al. (2009) Microduplications of 16p11.2 are
environmental factors among twin pairs with autism. Arch. Gen. associated with schizophrenia. Nat. Genet. 41, 1223–1227
Psychiatry DOI: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.76 40 Virkud, Y.V. et al. (2009) Familial aggregation of quantitative autistic
11 Brian, J. et al. (2008) Clinical assessment of autism in high-risk 18- traits in multiplex versus simplex autism. Am. J. Med. Genet. B:
month-olds. Autism 12, 433–456 Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 150B, 328–334
12 Bolton, P. et al. (1994) A case-control family history study of autism. J. 41 Betancur, C. (2011) Etiological heterogeneity in autism spectrum
Child Psychol. Psychiatry 35, 877–900 disorders: more than 100 genetic and genomic disorders and still
13 Losh, M. et al. (2009) Neuropsychological profile of autism and the counting. Brain Res. 1380, 42–77
broad autism phenotype. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 66, 518–526 42 Vassos, E. et al. (2010) Penetrance for copy number variants associated
14 Warren, Z.E. et al. (2011) Neurocognitive and behavioral outcomes of with schizophrenia. Hum. Mol. Genet. 19, 3477–3481
younger siblings of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder at age 43 Gibb, B.C. (2009) Teetering towards chaos and complexity. Nat. Chem.
five. J. Autism Dev. Disord. DOI: 10.1007/s10803-011-1263-4 1, 17–18
15 Constantino, J.N. (2011) The quantitative nature of autistic social 44 Zeggini, E. (2011) Next-generation association studies for complex
impairment. Pediatr. Res. 69, 55R–62R traits. Nat. Genet. 43, 287–288
16 Gamliel, I. et al. (2009) Developmental trajectories in siblings of 45 Speicher, M.R. et al., eds (2009) Vogel and Motulsky’s Human Genetics:
children with autism: cognition and language from 4 months to 7 Problems and Approaches, Springer
years. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 39, 1131–1144 46 State, M.W. (2010) The genetics of child psychiatric disorders: focus on
17 Pickles, A. et al. (2000) Variable expression of the autism broader autism and Tourette syndrome. Neuron 68, 254–269
phenotype: findings from extended pedigrees. J. Child Psychol. 47 Cook, E.H., Jr and Scherer, S.W. (2008) Copy-number variations
Psychiatry 41, 491–502 associated with neuropsychiatric conditions. Nature 455, 919–923

415
Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences September 2011, Vol. 15, No. 9

48 Zoghbi, H.Y. (2003) Postnatal neurodevelopmental disorders: meeting 69 Gilman, S.R. et al. (2011) Rare de novo variants associated with autism
at the synapse? Science 302, 826–830 implicate a large functional network of genes involved in formation and
49 Voineagu, I. et al. (2011) Transcriptomic analysis of autistic brain function of synapses. Neuron 70, 898–907
reveals convergent molecular pathology. Nature 474, 380–384 70 Sakai, Y. et al. (2011) Protein interactome reveals converging
50 Glessner, J.T. et al. (2009) Autism genome-wide copy number variation molecular pathways among autism disorders. Sci. Transl. Med. 3,
reveals ubiquitin and neuronal genes. Nature 459, 569–573 86ra49
51 Belmonte, M.K. et al. (2004) Autism and abnormal development of 71 Geschwind, D.H. and Konopka, G. (2009) Neuroscience in the era of
brain connectivity. J. Neurosci. 24, 9228–9231 functional genomics and systems biology. Nature 461, 908–915
52 Belmonte, M.K. et al. (2004) Autism as a disorder of neural information 72 Gauthier, J. et al. (2011) Truncating mutations in NRXN2 and NRXN1
processing: directions for research and targets for therapy. Mol. in autism spectrum disorders and schizophrenia. Hum. Genet. DOI:
Psychiatry 9, 646–663 10.1007/s00439-011-0975-z
53 Courchesne, E. and Pierce, K. (2005) Why the frontal cortex in autism 73 Levinson, D.F. et al. (2011) Copy number variants in schizophrenia:
might be talking only to itself: local over-connectivity but long-distance confirmation of five previous findings and new evidence for 3q29
disconnection. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 225–230 microdeletions and VIPR2 duplications. Am. J. Psychiatry 168,
54 Dinstein, I. et al. (2011) Disrupted neural synchronization in toddlers 302–316
with autism. Neuron 70, 1218–1225 74 Strauss, K.A. et al. (2006) Recessive symptomatic focal epilepsy
55 Geschwind, D.H. and Levitt, P. (2007) Autism spectrum disorders: and mutant contactin-associated protein-like 2. N. Engl. J. Med.
developmental disconnection syndromes. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 17, 354, 1370–1377
103–111 75 Verkerk, A.J. et al. (2003) CNTNAP2 is disrupted in a family with
56 Tsai, P. and Sahin, M. (2011) Mechanisms of neurocognitive Gilles de la Tourette syndrome and obsessive compulsive disorder.
dysfunction and therapeutic considerations in tuberous sclerosis Genomics 82, 1–9
complex. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 24, 106–113 76 Kumar, R.A. et al. (2008) Recurrent 16p11.2 microdeletions in autism.
57 Poretti, A. et al. (2011) Joubert Syndrome and related disorders: Hum. Mol. Genet. 17, 628–638
spectrum of neuroimaging findings in 75 patients. AJNR Am. J. 77 Weiss, L.A. et al. (2008) Association between microdeletion and
Neuroradiol. DOI: 10.3174/ajnr.A2517 microduplication at 16p11.2 and autism. N. Engl. J. Med. 358, 667–675
58 Lee, J.E. and Gleeson, J.G. (2011) Cilia in the nervous system: linking 78 Shinawi, M. et al. (2010) Recurrent reciprocal 16p11.2 rearrangements
cilia function and neurodevelopmental disorders. Curr. Opin. Neurol. associated with global developmental delay, behavioural problems,
24, 98–105 dysmorphism, epilepsy, and abnormal head size. J. Med. Genet. 47,
59 Alarcon, M. et al. (2008) Linkage, association, and gene-expression 332–341
analyses identify CNTNAP2 as an autism-susceptibility gene. Am. J. 79 van Bon, B.W.M., et al. (1993) 15q13.3 Microdeletion. (Pagon RA, B.T.,
Hum. Genet. 82, 150–159 Dolan CR, Stephens K, ed), University of Washington, Seattle
60 Whitehouse, A.J. et al. (2011) CNTNAP2 variants affect early language 80 Moreno-De-Luca, D. et al. (2010) Deletion 17q12 is a recurrent copy
development in the general population. Genes Brain Behav. 10, 451–456 number variant that confers high risk of autism and schizophrenia.
61 Vernes, S.C. et al. (2008) A functional genetic link between distinct Am. J. Hum. Genet. 87, 618–630
developmental language disorders. N. Engl. J. Med. 359, 2337–2345 81 Mefford, H.C. et al. (2010) Genome-wide copy number variation in
62 Scott-Van Zeeland, A.A. et al. (2010) Altered functional connectivity in epilepsy: novel susceptibility loci in idiopathic generalized and focal
frontal lobe circuits is associated with variation in the autism risk gene epilepsies. PLoS Genet. 6, e1000962
CNTNAP2. Sci. Transl. Med. 2, 56–80 82 Fine, S.E. et al. (2005) Autism spectrum disorders and symptoms in
63 Peter, B. et al. (2011) Replication of CNTNAP2 association with children with molecularly confirmed 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. J.
nonword repetition and support for FOXP2 association with timed Autism Dev. Disord. 35, 461–470
reading and motor activities in a dyslexia family sample. J. Neurodev. 83 Niklasson, L. et al. (2009) Autism, ADHD, mental retardation and
Disord. 3, 39–49 behavior problems in 100 individuals with 22q11 deletion syndrome.
64 Arking, D.E. et al. (2008) A common genetic variant in the neurexin Res. Dev. Disabil. 30, 763–773
superfamily member CNTNAP2 increases familial risk of autism. Am. 84 Vorstman, J.A. et al. (2006) The 22q11.2 deletion in children: high rate
J. Hum. Genet. 82, 160–164 of autistic disorders and early onset of psychotic symptoms. J. Am.
65 Tan, G.C. et al. (2010) Normal variation in fronto-occipital circuitry and Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 45, 1104–1113
cerebellar structure with an autism-associated polymorphism of 85 Mefford, H.C. et al. (2008) Recurrent rearrangements of chromosome
CNTNAP2. Neuroimage 53, 1030–1042 1q21.1 and variable pediatric phenotypes. N. Engl. J. Med. 359,
66 State, M.W. (2011) The erosion of phenotypic specificity in psychiatric 1685–1699
genetics: emerging lessons from CNTNAP2. Biol. Psychiatry 69, 816–817 86 Stefansson, H. et al. (2008) Large recurrent microdeletions associated
67 Stein, M.B. et al. (2011) A common genetic variant in the neurexin with schizophrenia. Nature 455, 232–236
superfamily member CNTNAP2 is associated with increased risk for 87 Abrahams, B.S. and Geschwind, D.H. (2010) Genetics of autism. In
selective mutism and social anxiety-related traits. Biol. Psychiatry 69, Vogel and Motulsky’s Human Genetics (Speicher, M.R. et al., eds), pp.
825–831 699–714, Springer
68 Bill, B.R. and Geschwind, D.H. (2009) Genetic advances in autism: 88 Geschwind, D.H. (2011) Autism genetics and genomics: a brief
heterogeneity and convergence on shared pathways. Curr. Opin. Genet. overview and synthesis. In Autism Spectrum Disorders (Amaral,
Dev. 19, 271–278 D.G. et al., eds), pp. 812–824, Oxford University Press

416

You might also like