Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Received 11/09/15

Revised 08/25/16
Accepted 10/01/16
DOI: 10.1002/jcad.12158

Loneliness as Moderator
Between Trauma and
Posttraumatic Growth
Melissa Zeligman, Jacquelyn A. Bialo, Jane L. Brack,
and Moriah A. Kearney
This study investigated the association between loneliness, trauma symptomatology, and posttraumatic growth (PTG)
in undergraduate students (N = 362). The study also explored whether loneliness moderated the relationship between
experiences of trauma and PTG. The results demonstrated that both loneliness and trauma symptoms predicted
levels of PTG, and loneliness moderated the relationship between trauma and PTG. Limitations, directions for future
research, and implications for counseling are discussed.

Keywords: posttraumatic growth, loneliness, trauma

The impact of traumatic events on individuals has been well begins when individuals start to cope with their trauma and
documented. Although not all survivors of trauma develop learn to assimilate or accommodate the trauma into their exist-
psychological distress, those who do may experience depres- ing belief system and cognitive schema (Bayer et al., 2007).
sion, anxiety, panic and stress disorders, phobias, dissociation, As individuals strive to make meaning from their trauma,
and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD; Briere & Scott, changes to their cognitive and emotional conceptualizations
2015; Calhoun, Tedeschi, Cann, & Hanks, 2010). According of themselves and the world they live in lay the foundation
to Janoff-Bulman (2006), when a trauma occurs, it shocks the for PTG (Janoff-Bulman, 2006). According to Tedeschi and
individual’s core and creates something of a “psychological Calhoun (2004), it is the struggle with this new reality “that
earthquake” (Bayer, Lev-Wiesel, & Amir, 2007, p. 5). The is crucial in determining the extent to which posttraumatic
trauma shatters one’s illusions of safety and security, as well growth occurs” (p. 5); trauma and PTG are not mutually
as one’s basic assumptions about the world (Bayer et al., exclusive. Although PTG is sometimes used interchangeably
2007). These basic assumptions are founded on the idea that with the term resilience, Tedeschi and Calhoun (1995, 1996)
if individuals do the right thing, are careful, and are overall saw these as two distinct concepts. Resilient personality traits
good people, they can prevent bad things from happening to may enable someone to better cope with crisis or trauma, but
them (Janoff-Bulman, 2006). Although individuals know that inevitably resilience refers to an individual maintaining emo-
bad things happen in the world, they do not expect those bad tional stability (Ogińska-Bulik, 2015). In contrast, individuals
things to happen to them personally until a traumatic event who experience PTG undergo a transformation following
happens (Janoff-Bulman, 2006). trauma, whereby there is movement past their pretrauma level
of functioning (Tedeschi & Calhoun, 1995, 1996).
Posttraumatic Growth The mechanisms underlying PTG, however, remain some-
what unclear. Various researchers have created theoretical
Although somewhat counterintuitive, researchers have found models and hypotheses to try to understand and explain how
that in some cases, the emotional healing process in the after- PTG occurs. Janoff-Bulman (2006) suggested there are three
math of a traumatic event can actually lead to positive psycho- pathways to PTG: strength through suffering, existential re-
logical changes and effects (Bayer et al., 2007; Bush, Skopp, evaluation, and psychological preparedness, which involves
McCann, & Luxton, 2011). This phenomenon is known as adjusting one’s schema to accept that there are random, un-
posttraumatic growth (PTG). PTG is a complex process that controllable events in the world. PTG happens by survivors

Melissa Zeligman, Jacquelyn A. Bialo, Jane L. Brack, and Moriah A. Kearney, Department of Counseling and Psychological
Services, Georgia State University. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Melissa Zeligman, Department
of Counseling and Psychological Services, Georgia State University, PO Box 3980, Atlanta, GA 30302-3980 (e-mail: mzeligman@
gsu.edu).

© 2017 by the American Counseling Association. All rights reserved.


Journal of Counseling & Development  ■  October 2017  ■  Volume 95 435
Zeligman, Bialo, Brack, & Kearney

acknowledging these elements, utilizing coping skills and (2009) erosion model described the relationship between so-
resources, and relying on their reestablished psychological cial support and PTSD as one in which the severity of PTSD
sense of coherence (Janoff-Bulman, 2006). Similarly, Taylor’s symptoms (e.g., social withdrawal) adversely influences uti-
(1983) model of cognitive adaptation includes three themes lization of supports and resources. In their study of survivors
that arise during the posttrauma readjustment process: (a) of traumatic motor vehicle accidents, Clapp and Beck were
searching for meaning, (b) trying to gain mastery and a specifically interested in looking at the moderating effect
sense of control over the event and over one’s life, and (c) of negative network orientation (i.e., when the traumatized
working toward self-enhancement and improved self-esteem. individual views social support as ineffective, inappropriate,
The individual reconciles these themes by using constructive or dangerous) on the indirect association between PTSD and
thoughts and actions to adapt and to grow from the trauma. social support. They found that after controlling for childhood
Another mechanism by which PTG could occur is through victimization and its interaction with PTSD symptoms, there
cognitive processing, which involves processing the trauma in was a relationship between severity of PTSD and increased
such a way as to enable the rebuilding of core beliefs (Tedes- negative network orientation, which subsequently was associ-
chi, 2011). More specifically, central to cognitive processing is ated with decreased perceptions of social support.
the idea of rumination. Tedeschi (2011) described rumination Although the findings have been somewhat inconsistent,
as initially involving intrusive, repetitive, automatic thoughts researchers have found that PTG has both psychological and
that eventually subside into a more deliberate reflection of the physical benefits for survivors. When individuals experience
event. Through the rumination process, survivors assimilate PTG, they may find an increase in their positive feelings, such
their new, changed reality into their cognitions (Tedeschi, as having more compassion and empathy, stronger ties to loved
2011; Triplett, Tedeschi, Cann, Calhoun, & Reeve, 2012). ones, and a greater appreciation for life (Bush et al., 2011).
Women are more likely to engage in thoughtful, deliberate In addition, individuals may experience changes in personal
rumination compared with men (Treynor, Gonzalez, & Nolen- strength, spirituality, self-concept, and life priorities, and they
Hoeksema, 2003), which Segerstrom, Stanton, Alden, and begin to recognize new life opportunities and possibilities (Cal-
Shortridge (2003) hypothesized is why women more regularly houn et al., 2010; Solomon & Dekel, 2007; Tedeschi, 2011). For
report experiences of PTG. For this reason, we controlled for example, Bush et al. (2011) used hierarchical linear regression
gender in the present study. Elements such as rumination and to analyze data related to PTG among 5,302 participants, 97.5%
cognitive processing could have either a mediating or a moder- of whom had served in the United States Army. They found not
ating effect on the relationship between a stressful experience only that PTG served as a protective factor against suicide risk
and psychosocial outcomes (Ben-Zur, 2012). For example, in the military but also that participants experienced positive
in their study of individuals after a myocardial infarction, change following the stress and trauma of deployment. Linley
Garnefski, Kraaij, Schroevers, and Somsen (2008) found and Joseph (2004) reviewed 39 studies and found that growth
that 24% of the variance of PTG was explained by cognitive after trauma led to a reduction in depressive symptoms, anxiety,
processing and coping strategies. Additionally, Barskova and and distress. Frazier, Conlon, and Glaser (2001) showed that
Oesterreich (2009) suggested that PTG may occur as family 12 months after a sexual assault, women who reported positive
and friends help survivors to identify what is truly meaningful life changes exhibited the least amount of distress and depres-
in their lives and to process the trauma experience. sive symptoms. Similarly, Milam (2004) found that among
Several factors (e.g., self-esteem, optimism, self-efficacy) HIV-positive individuals, those who indicated they had expe-
may contribute to an individual healing from trauma and rienced PTG exhibited less depression and pessimism as well
experiencing PTG (Epel, McEwen, & Ickovics, 1998; Linley as increased optimism. Physically, survivors who experienced
& Joseph, 2004; Updegraff, Taylor, Kemeny, & Wyatt, 2002). PTG exhibited decreased levels of the stress hormone cortisol
Two of the most salient variables appear to be personality traits (Epel et al., 1998). Milam also found that participants who
and social supports/networks (Clapp & Beck, 2009; Evans, had PTG ate healthier and drank less alcohol. Thus, it appears
Steel, & DiLillo, 2013; Linley & Joseph, 2004). Linley and that PTG is a complex phenomenon that captures the human
Joseph (2004) reviewed the literature on positive changes capacity for healing and, for some, can lead to significant and
following trauma and found that of the Big Five personality positive life changes.
traits, extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and consci-
entiousness were positively associated with PTG, whereas Loneliness as a Barrier
neuroticism was negatively correlated with PTG. In looking
at the role of social support, Evans et al. (2013) asserted that
to Trauma Recovery
individuals might evaluate a situation as being less stressful Some survivors experience PTG, whereas others struggle
if they perceive that others can offer support and resources to recover and regain a sense of normalcy in their lives. A
needed to help them cope with the event. Clapp and Beck’s particularly salient element that may inhibit and hinder PTG

436 Journal of Counseling & Development  ■  October 2017  ■  Volume 95


Loneliness as Moderator Between Trauma and Posttraumatic Growth

is loneliness. Loneliness is somewhat of a subjective concept suggested that social anxiety, a symptom that survivors of
in that it relies on an individual’s perceptions (Fees, Martin, trauma may experience (Briere & Scott, 2015), contributes to
& Poon, 1999). Eshbaugh (2010) suggested that loneliness loneliness in that individuals with anxiety are more sensitive
exists when “there is a discrepancy between the relationships to perceived negative evaluation from others, which in turn
one actually has and the relationship one wishes one has” (p. affects how they perceive social support.
8). Similarly, Ben-Zur (2012) proposed that loneliness has There remains a paucity of literature on the relationship
two components: emotional loneliness and social loneliness. between loneliness and PTG. Given that survivors of trauma
Emotional loneliness occurs when there is a “loss or absence can experience feelings of loneliness, it is essential that we
of confiding in and forming an attachment to a special and continue to study its moderating effects on factors such as
beloved person” (p. 24). Social loneliness occurs when in- PTG. Therefore, studying the association between loneli-
dividuals experience a lack of meaningful friendships and ness and PTG is important in further understanding how
relationships in their life. Furthermore, although similar in trauma survivors experience PTG and what factors may be
concept, there is a subtle, but important distinction between inhibiting this process of growth. To advance the knowledge
loneliness and perceptions of social support. Social isolation of PTG, and to further define the constructs that enable or
is considered an objective measure of social interactions and prevent individuals from experiencing such growth, we aim
relationships, whereas loneliness is based on perceptions of to identify the presence of trauma, loneliness, and PTG in
social isolation that cause distress and pain (J. T. Cacioppo et adults while further assessing the predictive nature of loneli-
al., 2015; Hawkley & Cacioppo, 2010; Masi, Chen, Hawkley, ness and trauma on PTG. We examined two primary research
& Cacioppo, 2011). questions: (a) Are perceived loneliness and experiences of
Loneliness is not limited to people who have been through trauma predictive of PTG in adults? and (b) Does loneliness
a trauma. Rather, it appears to be a common human condition serve as a moderator between experiences of trauma and PTG
reported by up to 80% of youth age 18 and under, and more in adults, even when gender is controlled for?
than 40% of adults age 65 and older (Hawkley & Cacioppo,
2010). Loneliness is associated with myriad negative physical, Method
cognitive, psychological, social, and mental health outcomes.
These include being at increased risk for depression, drug and Participants and Procedure
alcohol use, suicidal ideation and self-harm, and personality Participants consisted of 362 undergraduate students re-
disorder and psychoses (Hawkley & Cacioppo, 2010; Masi cruited from a large urban university in the southeastern
et al., 2011). In their meta-analysis, Holt-Lunstad, Smith, and United States. A post hoc power analysis was conducted using
Layton (2010) found that lonely people have a 45% increased G*Power (Version 3.1). With an alpha level of .05, a sample
risk for mortality, which is roughly comparable with the mor- size of 362, and the small observed effect size of .126 (Co-
tality risks associated with smoking and alcohol consumptions hen, 1992), achieved power for the study was .99. Inclusion
and which exceeds other factors such as obesity. Loneliness criteria for the study required that participants be 18 years
also contributes to decreases in executive functioning and is of age or older and able to consent to the sensitive nature
a risk factor for cognitive decline and dementia (S. Cacioppo, of the research. Potential participants were informed that
Capitanio, & Cacioppo, 2014). Among older adults, loneli- the research focused on experiences of trauma and personal
ness can lead to increased systolic blood pressure, depression, reactions to traumatic experiences. Following approval from
and poorer physical health (Chen & Feeley, 2014; Fees et al., the university institutional review board, data were collected
1999). Loneliness has been shown to affect quality of sleep, as part of a larger research project exploring the relationships
reduce immune system effectiveness, and increase stress of trauma and trauma symptomatology on PTG.
responses (Masi et al., 2011). Thus, studying loneliness and Participants were excluded from the larger data set if they
PTG becomes necessary for increasing wellness of clients had not reported experiencing a form of trauma as measured
who have experienced trauma. by the Trauma History Screen (Carlson et al., 2011). Partici-
Furthermore, similar to the ways in which certain personal- pants in the final sample ranged in age from 18 to 57 years
ity traits facilitate PTG, researchers have demonstrated how (M = 23.73 years, SD = 5.37), were primarily women (n =
other characteristics, such as social anxiety and low self- 215, 59.4%), and identified as heterosexual (n = 334, 92.3%).
concept, contribute to feelings of loneliness (Fees et al., 1999; The sample was diverse in terms of race, immigration history,
Gallagher, Prinstein, Simon, & Spirito, 2014). Additional and generational college experiences. The largest percentage
relationships exist between loneliness, blaming oneself for of students, 39.8% (n = 144), identified as Black or African
social shortcomings and failures, being more self-conscious American, 39.5% (n = 143) were first-generation college
in social settings, and experiencing increased likeliness of students, and 25.1% (n = 91) identified as first-generation
social rejection (Masi et al., 2011). Gallagher et al. (2014) U.S. citizens. The research was completed at a university with

Journal of Counseling & Development  ■  October 2017  ■  Volume 95 437


Zeligman, Bialo, Brack, & Kearney

a uniquely diverse student body; therefore, the sample was A reliability analysis on the current sample showed strong
representative of the larger university population. internal consistency (α = .94). This is consistent with alpha
coefficients gathered in other research on trauma (.92; Neal
Measures & Nagle, 2013).
Posttraumatic Growth Inventory (PTGI). Developed by Trauma History Screen. The Trauma History Screen
Tedeschi and Calhoun (1996), the PTGI measures personal (Carlson et al., 2011) is used to ensure that participants have
growth in individuals following a highly stressful or traumatic reported experiencing some sort of trauma prior to their
event. The subscales measure growth in five areas: Relating to participation. The measure screens for trauma in terms of the
Others (seven items), New Possibilities (five items), Personal following: car, boat, train, or airplane accidents; accidents at
Strength (four items), Spiritual Change (two items), and Ap- home or work; hurricanes, floods, earthquakes, tornados, or
preciation of Life (three items). A Likert-type scale ranging fires; being hit or kicked hard enough to incur injury (either
from 0 (I did not experience this change as a result of my as a child or an adult); being forced to make sexual contact
crisis) to 5 (I experienced this change to a very great degree (either as a child or an adult); being attacked with a gun,
as a result of my crisis) was used. Sample items include “I can knife, or weapon; seeing something horrible or being badly
better appreciate each day” and “I discovered that I’m stronger scared during military service; sudden death of close family
than I thought I was.” The scale results in a total score created or friend; seeing someone die suddenly or get badly hurt or
by taking the sum of each of the 21 items on the assessment. killed; sudden move or loss of home or possessions; suddenly
Items on the PTGI were previously shown to be reliable when abandoned by spouse, partner, parent, or family; or some
used with trauma survivors (e.g., α = .85 in Sattler, Boyd, & other sudden event that made participants feel very scared,
Kirsch, 2014; α = .94 in Taku, Cann, Tedeschi, & Calhoun, helpless, or horrified. Although this measure also contains a
2015). The present sample had a Cronbach’s alpha of .97, narrative component, only the trauma event portion was used
suggesting this scale has strong internal consistency. to determine whether a participant should be included in the
UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3). The third version of sample (i.e., did the participant report experiencing one of
the original UCLA Loneliness Scale (Russell, 1996) is a sim- these traumatic events?).
plified 20-item version designed to measure how individuals Demographics. Participants were given a demographic
describe their experience of loneliness. Respondents are asked questionnaire we created to capture a more detailed look at
to indicate how often they experience the feelings listed on a the sample. This questionnaire included items pertaining to
4-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (never) to 4 (always), age, gender, racial/ethnic identity, spiritual/religious affilia-
with higher scores indicating higher degrees of loneliness. tion, immigration history (e.g., first-generation immigrant),
Sample items include “How often do you feel that you are sexual orientation, year in school, grade point average, marital
no longer close to anyone?” and “How often do you feel status, disability status, and socioeconomic status. Not all
isolated from others?” The scale indicates strong convergent demographic information was included in the research ques-
validity in its correlations with other measures of loneliness tions of this study but rather was used to highlight the diversity
(e.g., NYU Loneliness Scale, Differential Loneliness Scale) of the sample, particularly in terms of race and immigration
and discriminant validity in its negative association with history. In this sense, the inclusion of this assessment helps
scores of social support. In addition, previous researchers to differentiate the present sample from previous samples
have reported strong internal consistency of the scale when exploring the construct of PTG.
used with college samples (.96; Demirli & Demir, 2014). The
scores presented strong reliability in the current sample, with Data Analysis
a Cronbach’s alpha of .92. We first conducted descriptive statistics to determine the
Trauma Symptom Checklist–40 (TSC-40). The TSC-40 presence and level of PTG within the sample. We also ran
(Briere & Runtz, 1989) consists of 40 items that were created descriptive statistics on the scales for loneliness and trauma
to determine how many symptoms associated with trauma symptomatology to assess the presence of both constructs
experiences participants are reporting and how often such within the data. We then specified and tested a series of
experiences have occurred. The TSC-40 is intended specifi- regression models to determine whether loneliness and
cally for research purposes and is not encouraged for clinical trauma symptomatology were predictive of experiences of
use. Items include symptoms such as restless sleep, flash- PTG within the sample. Multiple regression analyses were
backs, and feelings of guilt. Together these items constitute used to determine the predictive nature of loneliness and
six subscales: Dissociation (six items), Anxiety (nine items), trauma on the total experience of PTG, as well as on the five
Depression (nine items), Sexual Abuse Trauma Index (seven PTGI subscales (i.e., Relating to Others, New Possibilities,
items), Sleep Disturbance (six items), and Sexual Problems Personal Strength, Spiritual Change, and Appreciation of
(eight items), with some items used in multiple subscales. Life). In addition, the predictor variables (i.e., loneliness and

438 Journal of Counseling & Development  ■  October 2017  ■  Volume 95


Loneliness as Moderator Between Trauma and Posttraumatic Growth

trauma symptoms) were tested to determine if an interaction Ghee, Johnson, & Burlew, 2010; Victorson, Farmer, Burnett,
effect was present and to uncover if loneliness moderated Ouellette, & Barocas, 2005). Finally, the UCLA Loneliness
the relationship between trauma symptoms and PTG in the Scale resulted in a mean loneliness score of 44.39 (SD =
sample. We also controlled for gender during this process to 11.28, range = 20–77), which is similar to the mean found
account for significant variance in reported PTG scores, and by the authors of the scale when looking at a similar sample
the moderating effect of loneliness, over and above that ac- (Russell, 1996; M = 40.08, SD = 9.5).
counted for by gender. Women typically report greater levels Consistent with other research (e.g., Vanhalst, Luyckx,
of PTSD symptomatology and PTG than men (Vishnevsky, Raes, & Goossens, 2012; Vishnevsky et al., 2010), gender-
Cann, Calhoun, Tedeschi, & Demakis, 2010), leading gender based differences existed within the sample in terms of
to regularly be regarded as a control variable in PTG research, loneliness, trauma symptoms, and PTG. In exploring the
including in the present study. A two-step hierarchical regres- relationships among gender and the identified constructs, we
sion technique (Step 1 including the predictor variable and found that gender had weak positive correlations (p < .05) with
Step 2 including the interaction between the predictor and loneliness (r = .07), trauma symptoms (r = .16), and PTG (r
moderator) guided the testing of a moderation effect (Baron = .05). Using a t test, we determined that although mean PTG
& Kenny, 1986). Prior to conducting regression analyses, scores differed for men (M = 44.27) and women (M = 47.10),
we ran correlations to assess for potential issues of multicol- these differences were not significant (p > .05). In contrast,
linearity; in addition, centering of the predictor variables was mean differences in loneliness scores (men, M = 43.52;
completed to standardize the variables, as recommended by women, M = 45.14) and reports of trauma symptoms (men,
Aiken and West (1991). M = 19.16; women, M = 26.38) were significantly different
between men and women (p < .01), with women reporting
Results greater trauma symptoms and greater feelings of loneliness.
Descriptive Statistics Predictors of PTG
Correlations between constructs, and means and standards To examine the first research question, we performed regres-
deviations for the measures used in this study, are presented sion analyses to determine if loneliness and trauma symptoms
in Table 1. As shown in the table, trauma symptomatology were predictive of experiences of PTG within the sample.
was significantly and positively correlated with each of the Before conducting the analyses, we checked issues of multi-
other predictor variables (loneliness, r = .53; PTG, r = .26). collinearity by running correlations and collinearity statistics.
However, PTG scores were not correlated with feelings of These tests indicated that multicollinearity was not a concern
loneliness. Furthermore, the means shown in Table 1 give a within the present sample (for loneliness, tolerance = .70,
clearer picture of where the sample stood in terms of each of variance inflation factor [VIF] = 1.42; for trauma symptoms,
the constructs. Results indicated a PTG mean score of 45.73 tolerance = .70, VIF = 1.42). We used R2 to determine the ef-
(SD = 28.95, range = 0–105), which is considerably lower than fect size, because it is an appropriate measure often utilized
similar samples (e.g., Hooper, Marotta, & Depuy, 2009; M = in studies of regressions (McMillan & Foley, 2011). Results
67.03). A clinical cutoff score for the TSC-40 is not available; indicated that both predictor variables contributed to a model
however, the sample mean of 23.01 for trauma (SD = 21.55, of PTG in a significant way, accounting for 11% of the vari-
range = 0–120) is consistent with other means found in other ance in PTGI scores (R2 = .11), F(2, 359) = 23.85, p < .001.
samples of individuals who have experienced trauma (e.g., In addition, the analysis resulted in the following equation:
PTG = 59.1 – .57 (loneliness) + .53 (trauma symptoms).
Furthermore, PTG was significantly connected to loneliness
TABLE 1 (β = .23, p < .01) and trauma symptoms (β = –.41, p < .01)
Correlations, Means, and Standard Deviations within the model. The higher beta score associated with
Among the Variables trauma means this variable contributed to the outcome of PTG
slightly more than loneliness. It is also important to note that
Variable 1 2 3 trauma negatively predicted PTG, whereas loneliness was a
1. Trauma (.94) positive predictor.
2. Loneliness .53* (.92)
3. Posttraumatic growth .26* .01 (.94) To explore the predictive role of these variables within
the data, we next assessed whether loneliness served as a
M 23.01 44.39 45.73 moderator between trauma symptoms and PTG. Prior to
SD 21.55 11.28 28.95
testing a moderation effect, we took appropriate steps (i.e.,
Note. N = 362. Alpha values are listed in parentheses on the diagonal. data were centered and predictor variables were included
*p < .01. with the interaction variable; Frazier, Tix, & Barron, 2004).

Journal of Counseling & Development  ■  October 2017  ■  Volume 95 439


Zeligman, Bialo, Brack, & Kearney

Because gender was significantly associated with experi- support and loneliness are distinctly different constructs,
ences of PTG, this demographic variable was controlled for warranting separate research to uncover their respective con-
in the regression analysis. In addition, gender was controlled nections with PTG. At the time of this study, we did not find
for in the analysis due to the noted differences in PTG scores any other research exploring our research questions, includ-
between men and women in the sample. ing examining the role of loneliness as a moderator between
In Step 1of the regression analysis, we placed gender into trauma and PTG. More specifically, our research further
the first block due to theoretical rationale supporting gender measured the predictive relationship of trauma symptomatol-
differences in those who experience PTG (Vishnevsky et al., ogy and loneliness on PTG, and also controlled for gender
2010). In the second block, the remaining predictor variables when considering the moderating role of loneliness within
(i.e., trauma symptoms and loneliness) were added to the the sample of undergraduate students who had experienced
model, identifying their impact on PTG while partialing out trauma. Results indicated that both trauma symptoms and
the anticipated effects of gender from the first block. Lastly, loneliness were significantly predictive of experiencing PTG,
consistent with Frazier et al.’s (2004) steps for testing mod- and that loneliness moderated the relationship between trauma
erating effects, the interaction between trauma symptoms symptoms and PTG in the sample. Furthermore, loneliness
and loneliness was added into the third block to test the and PTG were both experienced in greater amounts among
moderating role of loneliness between trauma symptoms the women in our sample.
and PTG. Table 2 shows the order in which variables were In addition to confirming the predictive nature of each
entered to achieve the final model. Step 1 revealed that of the variables, correlations helped to determine the direc-
gender explained 0.3% of the variance in PTG within the tion of these relationships. A correlation matrix identified
sample (R2 = .003). In Step 2, the centered variables trauma that trauma symptoms had a significant, positive relation-
symptoms (β = –.40) and loneliness (β = .23) were entered, ship with PTG. This finding echoes the very definition of
with both significantly predicting experiences of PTG (p < PTG, which asserts that a marked amount of felt trauma
.01) and accounting for 11% of the variance in PTG (R2 = is necessary in order to experience personal gains after an
.11). The two-way interaction between trauma and loneli- event (Calhoun & Tedeschi, 1998). Furthermore, the beta
ness (as seen in Step 3 of Table 2) produced a significant scores produced in the regression equation suggest that
interaction effect (β = –.06, p < .01). Therefore, the pres- loneliness contributed slightly less to the experience of
ence of loneliness within the sample moderated the effect PTG than trauma symptoms, in a significant and positive
of trauma symptoms on PTG (R2 = .12), F(4, 357) = 12.18, manner. This finding helps to further define the experience
p < .01, and explained 12% of the variance in PTG scores. of PTG and confirms the unique contribution of loneliness
in the sample.
Discussion The second research question, which explored whether
loneliness moderated the presence of trauma symptoms on
The relationship between loneliness and PTG has been re- PTG, was found to be significant. In other words, the effect
viewed in the literature (e.g., Andrykowski et al., 2005; Bishop of trauma symptoms on PTG was tied to, or dependent on,
et al., 2007), but as with most PTG research, these findings scores of loneliness on the UCLA Loneliness Scale. This find-
have centered on cancer survivors. In addition, although the ing suggests that the presence of loneliness plays a significant
relationship between social support and PTG is regularly cited role in whether those who have experienced trauma are able to
in the literature, current thought tends to suggest that social experience PTG or not. Therefore, feelings of loneliness may
exacerbate trauma symptoms or likewise hinder individuals
TABLE 2 from being able to grow following their trauma. This finding is
Hierarchical Moderated Regression of perhaps even more meaningful because gender was controlled
Loneliness and Trauma Symptoms for, suggesting loneliness is capable of affecting one’s ability
on Posttraumatic Growth to experience PTG regardless of gender.

Predictor β t R2 F
Implications for Counseling
Step 1 .003 0.94 In this study, we examined the relationship between trauma
Gender .00 1.22 symptomatology and loneliness on PTG. Based on our find-
Step 2 .11 15.85
Trauma symptoms –.40 –6.81*
ings, it is clear that counselors need to pay attention to their
Loneliness .23 3.91* clients’ feelings of loneliness. They can do this in several
Step 3 .12 12.18 ways. First, counselors need to change their perspective on
Trauma × Loneliness –.06 –1.04*
trauma and learn about PTG, for example, that experiences
*p < .01. of adversity or trauma do not necessarily equate with

440 Journal of Counseling & Development  ■  October 2017  ■  Volume 95


Loneliness as Moderator Between Trauma and Posttraumatic Growth

permanent emotional damage (Joseph & Linley, 2006). This, event may have resulted in a different sample, and therefore
of course, comes with the caveat that because a person has different results. Finally, the research involved a cross-sectional,
not experienced PTG does not mean he or she has failed correlational research design. This design allows relationships
to make progress. By increasing their knowledge about to be explored but limits us from assessing causality.
PTG, counselors can shift from simply trying to alleviate
clients’ distress to facilitating clients’ PTG (Joseph & Linley, Directions for Future Research
2006). As Joseph and Linley (2006) noted, it is “important In addition to replicating this study on more traditional college
to recognize that growth is not simply the absence of post- campuses, researchers may determine if loneliness moderates
traumatic stress, but is rather an independent dimension of the relationship between trauma symptomatology and PTG
experience” (p. 1048). in other populations, such as high school students or young
Second, counselors should learn to recognize what con- adults in the workforce. Future studies may also collect longi-
tributes to feelings of loneliness and how to attend to them. tudinal data when examining factors related to PTG. Although
During intake, they can assess a client’s loneliness by using PTG generally increases over time in women who experience
a measure such as the UCLA Loneliness Scale. Counselors sexual assault, the biggest gains seem to occur between 2
need to use techniques that support the rebuilding of clients’ weeks and 2 months postassault (Frazier et al., 2001). The
core beliefs that have been shattered and work on ways to domains in which PTG developed also had different patterns
increase trauma survivors’ social support networks. As a way over time, with participants reporting more immediate change
to address loneliness, we suggest using strength counseling in some domains (e.g., empathy) and more gradual change in
(Smith, 2006), the foundation of which is establishing a others (e.g., personal strength). It could be that the relation-
trusting and caring therapeutic alliance while simultaneously ship between loneliness, trauma symptomatology, and PTG
not neglecting trauma histories. The utilization of clients’ changes over time as well, especially when examining the
strengths in strength counseling may have tremendous different domains of PTG. In more applied work, researchers
potential for developing PTG and diminishing loneliness. can determine the impact of different therapeutic approaches,
The collaborative nature of strength counseling can assist such as strength counseling, on client-reported levels of PTG
clients to identify where to begin, and, more importantly, and loneliness.
where to go.
Third, in a similar vein, counselors can work with their Conclusion
clients to build resilience. Although an in-depth discussion
This study examined the predictive nature of loneliness and
of resilience is beyond the scope of this article, Agaibi and
trauma symptomatology on PTG. We found that both loneli-
Wilson (2005) suggested, “In the most basic sense, resiliency
ness and trauma symptoms predict levels of PTG, with lone-
has been defined as the ability to adapt and cope successfully
liness contributing slightly more to the experience of PTG.
despite threatening or challenging situations” (p. 198). Like
Additional analyses also confirmed the moderating effect that
strength counseling, resilience is strengths based, and Con-
loneliness has on the relationship between trauma symptoms
nor, Davidson, and Lee (2003) found that higher levels of
and PTG. Findings from this study underscore the role that
personal resilience are associated with better mental health.
loneliness plays in the experience of PTG, by preventing
Factors that affect resilience include social support, effec-
or diminishing trauma survivors’ ability to grow after their
tive coping, determination, and flexible attitudes (Joseph
trauma. In working with clients who have experienced trauma,
& Linley, 2006).
counselors may be able to foster PTG by using therapeutic
Limitations approaches that address loneliness.
The study had a number of limitations. Although our sample
was diverse with regard to factors such as ethnicity and im-
References
migration history, participation in the study was limited to Agaibi, C. E., & Wilson, J. P. (2005). Trauma, PTSD, and resilience:
college students attending an urban university. Results may A review of the literature. Trauma Violence Abuse, 6, 195–216.
be more generalizable if future studies include students from doi:10.1177/1524838005277438
universities in more suburban or rural environments as well. In Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and
addition, inclusion criteria were based on whether participants interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
had experienced one or more potentially traumatic events listed Andrykowski, M. A., Bishop, M. M., Hahn, E. A., Cella, D. F., Beau-
on the Trauma History Screen. Although these events may have mont, J. L., Brady, M. J., . . . Wingard, J. R. (2005). Long-term
the potential to cause trauma, they might not have been viewed health-related quality of life, growth, and spiritual well-being
as traumatic by the participant. Another trauma measure as- after hematopoietic stem-cell transplantation. Journal of Clinical
sessing participants’ perceptions of the traumatic nature of the Oncology, 23, 599–608. doi:10.1200/JCO.2005.03.189

Journal of Counseling & Development  ■  October 2017  ■  Volume 95 441


Zeligman, Bialo, Brack, & Kearney

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator Chen, Y., & Feeley, T. H. (2014). Social support, social strain,
variable distinction in social psychological research: Con- loneliness, and well-being among older adults: An analysis of
ceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of the Health and Retirement Study. Journal of Social and Personal
Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. Relationships, 31, 141–161. doi:10.1177/0265407513488728
Barskova, T., & Oesterreich, R. (2009). Post-traumatic growth Clapp, J. D., & Beck, J. G. (2009). Understanding the relationship
in people living with a serious medical condition and between PTSD and social support: The role of negative network
its relations to physical and mental health: A system- orientation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 237–244.
atic review. Disability and Rehabilitation, 31, 1709–1733. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2008.12.006
doi:10.1080/09638280902738441 Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112,
Bayer, S., Lev-Wiesel, R., & Amir, M. (2007). The relation- 155–159. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155
ship between basic assumptions, posttraumatic growth, and Connor, K. M., Davidson, J. R. T., & Lee, L.-C. (2003). Spiritu-
ambiguity tolerance in an Israeli sample of young adults: ality, resilience, and anger in survivors of violent trauma: A
A mediation–moderation model. Traumatology, 13, 4–15. community survey. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 16, 487–494.
doi:10.1177/1534765607299908 doi:10.1023/A:1025762512279
Ben-Zur, H. (2012). Loneliness, optimism, and well-being among Demirli, A., & Demir, A. (2014). The role of gender, attachment,
married, divorced, and widowed individuals. The Journal of dimensions, and family environment on loneliness among Turk-
Psychology, 146, 23–36. doi:10.1080/00223980.2010.548414 ish University students. Australian Journal of Guidance and
Bishop, M. M., Beaumont, J. L., Hahn, E. A., Cella, D., An- Counseling, 24, 62–75. doi:10.1017/jgc.2013.11
drykowski, M. A., Brady, M. J., . . . Wingard, J. R. (2007). Late Epel, E. S., McEwen, B. S., & Ickovics, J. R. (1998). Em-
effects of cancer and hematopoietic stem-cell transplantation bodying psychological thriving: Physical thriving in re-
on spouses or partners compared with survivors and survivor- sponse to stress. Journal of Social Issues, 54, 301–322.
matched controls. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 25, 1403–1411. doi:10.111/j.1540-4560.1998.tb01220.x
doi:10.1200/JCO.2006.07.5705 Eshbaugh, E. M. (2010). Friend and family support as moderators
Briere, J., & Runtz, M. (1989). The Trauma Symptom Checklist of the effects of low romantic partner support on loneliness
(TSC-33): Early data on a new scale. Journal of Interpersonal among college women. Individual Differences Research, 8,
Violence, 4, 151–163. doi:10.1177/088626089004002002 8–16.
Briere, J. N., & Scott, C. (2015). Principles of trauma therapy: A Evans, S. E., Steel, A. L., & DiLillo, D. (2013). Child maltreat-
guide to symptoms, evaluation, and treatment (2nd ed.). Los ment severity and adult trauma symptoms: Does perceived
Angeles, CA: Sage. social support play a buffering role? Child Abuse & Neglect,
Bush, N. E., Skopp, N. A., McCann, R., & Luxton, D. D. (2011). 37, 934–943. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.03.005
Posttraumatic growth as protection against suicidal ideation Fees, B. S., Martin, P., & Poon, L. W. (1999). A model of loneliness
after deployment and combat exposure. Military Medicine, 176, in older adults. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences,
1215–1222. doi:10.7205/MILMED-D-11-00018 54, 231–239. doi:10.1093/geronb/54B.4.P231
Cacioppo, J. T., Cacioppo, S., Cole, S. W., Capitanio, J. P., Frazier, P., Conlon, A., & Glaser, T. (2001). Positive and negative
Goossens, L., & Boomsma, D. I. (2015). Loneliness across life changes following sexual assault. Journal of Consulting
phylogeny and a call for comparative studies and animal and Clinical Psychology, 69, 1048–1055. doi:10.1037/0022-
models. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10, 202–212. 006x.69.6.1048
doi:10.1177/1745691614564876 Frazier, P. A., Tix, A. P., & Barron, K. E. (2004). Testing moderator
Cacioppo, S., Capitanio, J. P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2014). Toward and mediator effects in counseling psychology research. Journal
a neurology of loneliness. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 1464– of Counseling Psychology, 51, 115–134. doi:10.1037/0022-
1504. doi:10.1037/a0037618 0167.51.1.115
Calhoun, L. G., & Tedeschi, R. G. (1998). Beyond recovery from trauma: Gallagher, M., Prinstein, M. J., Simon, V., & Spirito, A.
Implications for clinical practice and research. Journal of Social (2014). Social anxiety symptoms and suicidal ideation in
Issues, 54, 357–371. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.1998.tb01223.x a clinical sample of early adolescents: Examining loneli-
Calhoun, L. G., Tedeschi, R. G., Cann, A., & Hanks, E. A. (2010). ness and social support as longitudinal mediators. Journal
Positive outcomes following bereavement: Paths to posttrau- of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42, 871–883. doi:10.1007/
matic growth. Psychologica Belgica, 50, 125–143. doi:10.5334/ s10802-013-9844-7
pb-50-1-2-125 Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V., Schroevers, M. J., & Somsen, G. A.
Carlson, E., Palmieri, P., Smith, S., Kimerling, R., Ruzek, J., & (2008). Post-traumatic growth after a myocardial infarction: A
Burling, T. (2011). Development and validation of a brief self- matter of personality, psychological health, or cognitive cop-
report measure of trauma exposure: The Trauma History Screen. ing? Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 15,
Psychological Assessment, 23, 463–477. doi:10.1037/a0022294 270–277. doi:10.1007/s10880-009-9136-5

442 Journal of Counseling & Development  ■  October 2017  ■  Volume 95


Loneliness as Moderator Between Trauma and Posttraumatic Growth

Ghee, A. C., Johnson, C. S., & Burlew, A. K. (2010). Measurement Segerstrom, S. C., Stanton, A. L., Alden, L. E., & Shortridge,
equivalence of Trauma Symptom Checklist–40 for chemically B. E. (2003). A multidimensional structure for repetitive
dependent African American and Caucasian women: A prelimi- thought: What’s on your mind, and how, and how much?
nary study. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 19, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 909–921.
820–838. doi:10.1080/10926771.2010.522949 doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.5.909
Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Loneliness matters: Smith, E. J. (2006). The strength-based counseling model. Counsel-
A theoretical and empirical review of consequences and ing Psychologist, 34, 13–79. doi:10.1177/0011000005277018
mechanisms. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 40, 218–227. Solomon, Z., & Dekel, R. (2007). Posttraumatic stress dis-
doi:10.1007/s12160-010-9210-8 order and posttraumatic growth among Israeli ex-POWs.
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., & Layton, J. B. (2010). Social re- Journal of Traumatic Stress, 20, 303–312. doi:10.1002/
lationships and mortality risk: A meta-analytic review. PLoS jts.20216
Medicine, 7, e1000316. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1000316 Taku, K., Cann, A., Tedeschi, R. G., & Calhoun, L. G.
Hooper, L. M., Marotta, S. A., & Depuy, V. (2009). A confir- (2015). Core beliefs shaken by an earthquake correlate
matory factor analytic study of the Posttraumatic Growth with posttraumatic growth. Psychological Trauma: Theory,
Inventory among a sample of racially diverse college stu- Research, Practice, and Policy, 7, 563–569. doi:10.1037/
dents. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 18, 335–343. tra0000054
doi:10.1080/09638230802522502 Taylor, S. E. (1983). Adjustment to threatening events: A
Janoff-Bulman, R. (2006). Schema-change perspectives on post- theory of cognitive adaptation. American Psychologist, 38,
traumatic growth. In L. G. Calhoun & R. G. Tedeschi (Eds.), 1161–1173. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.38.11.1161
Handbook of posttraumatic growth: Research and practice (pp. Tedeschi, R. G. (2011). Posttraumatic growth in combat vet-
81–99). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. erans. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings,
Joseph, S., & Linley, P. A. (2006). Growth following adversity: Theo- 18, 137–144. doi:10.1007/s10880-011-9255-2
retical perspectives and implications for clinical practice. Clinical Tedeschi, R. G., & Calhoun, L. G. (1995). Trauma and trans-
Psychology Review, 26, 1041–1053. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2005.12.006 formation: Growing in the aftermath of suffering. Thousand
Linley, P. A., & Joseph, S. (2004). Positive change following trauma Oaks, CA: Sage.
and adversity: A review. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 17, 11–21. Tedeschi, R. G., & Calhoun, L. G. (1996). The Posttraumatic
doi:0894-9867/04/0200-0011/1 Growth Inventory: Measuring the positive legacy of trauma.
Masi, C. M., Chen, H.-Y., Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2011). Journal of Trauma and Stress, 9, 455–471. doi:10.1002/
A meta-analysis of interventions to reduce loneliness. Personal- jts.2490090305
ity and Social Psychology Review, 15, 219–266. Tedeschi, R. G., & Calhoun, L. G. (2004). Posttraumatic
McMillan, J. H., & Foley, J. (2011). Reporting and discussing effect growth: Conceptual foundations and empirical evidence.
size: Still the road less traveled? Practical Assessment, Research Psychological Inquiry, 15, 1–18. doi:10.1207/s15327965p-
& Evaluation, 16, 1–12. doi:10.1177/1088868310377394 li1501_01
Milam, J. E. (2004). Posttraumatic growth among HIV/AIDS pa- Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003).
tients. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34, 2353–2376. R u m i n a t i o n r e c o n s i d e r e d : A p s y c h o m e t r i c a n a ly -
doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2004.tb01981.x sis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247–259.
Neal, T. M. S., & Nagle, J. E. (2013). Measuring abuse sequelae: doi:10.1023/A:1023910315561
Validating and extending the Trauma Symptom Checklist–40. Triplett, K. N., Tedeschi, R. G., Cann, A., Calhoun, L., &
Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 22, 231–247. Reeve, C. L. (2012). Posttraumatic growth, meaning in
doi:10.1080/10926771.2013.764953 life, and life satisfaction in response to trauma. Psycho-
Ogińska-Bulik, N. (2015). The relationship between resiliency logical Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy,
and posttraumatic growth following the death of someone 4, 400–410. doi:10.1037/a0024204
close. OMEGA: Journal of Death and Dying, 71, 233–244. Updegraff, J. A., Taylor, S. E., Kemeny, M. E., & Wyatt, G.
doi:10.1177/0030222815575502 E. (2002). Positive and negative effects of HIV infec-
Russell, D. W. (1996). UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3): Reli- tion in women with low socioeconomic resources. Per-
ability, validity, and factor structure. Journal of Personality As- sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 382–394.
sessment, 66, 20–40. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6601 doi:10.1177/0146167202286009
Sattler, D. N., Boyd, B., & Kirsch, J. (2014). Trauma-exposed fire- Vanhalst, J., Luyckx, K., Raes, F., & Goossens, L. (2012).
fighters: Relationships among posttraumatic growth, posttrau- Loneliness and depressive symptoms: The mediating and
matic stress, resource availability, coping and critical incident moderating role of uncontrollable ruminative thoughts. The
stress debriefing experience. Stress and Health, 30, 356–365. Journal of Psychology, 146, 259–276. doi:10.1080/002239
doi:10.1002/smi.2608 80.2011.555433

Journal of Counseling & Development  ■  October 2017  ■  Volume 95 443


Zeligman, Bialo, Brack, & Kearney

Victorson, D., Farmer, L., Burnett, K., Ouellette, A., & Barocas, J. Vishnevsky, T., Cann, A., Calhoun, L. G., Tedeschi, R. G., & Dema-
(2005). Maladaptive coping strategies and injury-related distress kis, G. J. (2010). Gender differences in self-reported posttrau-
following traumatic physical injury. Rehabilitation Psychology, matic growth: A meta-analysis. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
50, 408–415. doi:10.1037/0090-5550.50.4.408 34, 110–120. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2009.01546.x

444 Journal of Counseling & Development  ■  October 2017  ■  Volume 95


Copyright of Journal of Counseling & Development is the property of Wiley-Blackwell and
its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.

You might also like