Understanding Donors Motivations A Study

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Pergamon Soc. Sci. Med. Vol. 45, No. 1, pp.

137-147, 1997
© 1997 ElsevierScience Ltd
Pll: S0277-9536(96)00327-9 All rights reserved. Printed in Great Britain
0277-9536/97 $17.00 + 0.00

U N D E R S T A N D I N G DONORS' MOTIVATIONS: A STUDY


OF UNRELATED BONE MARROW DONORS

G A L E N E. S W I T Z E R , ~'2. M A R Y A M A N D A D E W , 2"3"4V I C T O R I A
A. B U T T E R W O R T H , 2 R O B E R T A G. S I M M O N S 2'~ and M I N D Y S C H I M M E L 6
'Department of Medicine, 2Department of Psychiatry, 3Department of Psychology, 4Department of
Epidemiology, ~Department of Sociology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, U.S.A. and
6JDC-Brookdale Institute of Gerontology and Human Development, Jerusalem, Israel

Abstract--Medical advances in bone marrow transplantation techniques and immunosuppressive medi-


cations have dramatically increased the number of such transplants performed each year, and conse-
quently, the demand for bone marrow from unrelated donors. Although physiological aspects of bone
marrow donation have been thoroughly investigated, very few studies have examined psychosocial fac-
tors that may impact individuals' donation decisions and outcomes. To examine one particular set of
donor psychosocial issues, this study investigated motives for bone marrow donation among 343 unre-
lated bone marrow donors who donated through the National Marrow Donor Program. Six distinct
types of donor motives were identified from open-ended questionnaire responses. Donors most fre-
quently reported motives reflecting some awareness of both the costs (to themselves) and potential ben-
efits (to themselves and the recipient) of donation. A desire to act in accordance with social or religious
precepts, expected positive feelings about donating, empathy for the recipient, and the simple desire to
help another person were also commonly cited reasons for donating. Among a series of donor back-
ground characteristics, donors' gender was the variable most strongly associated with motive type;
women were most likely to cite expected positive feelings, empathy, and the desire to help someone.
Central study findings indicated that donor motives predicted donors reactions to donation even after
the effects of donor background characteristics (including gender) were controlled. Donors who
reported exchange motives (weighing costs and benefits) and donors who reported simple (or idealized)
helping motives experienced the donation as less positive in terms of higher predonation ambivalence
and negative postdonation psychological reactions than did remaining donors. Donors who reported
positive feeling and empathy motives had the most positive donation reactions in terms of lower
ambivalence, and feeling like better persons postdonation. These findings add substantially to the body
of work concerning medical volunteerism generally, and also have important practical implications for
the recruitment and education of potential bone marrow donors. © 1997 Elsevier Science Ltd

Key words--bone marrow donation, altruism

INTRODUCTION Our ongoing interest in living organ donation in


g e n e r a l - - a n d bone marrow donation in particular
Since the National M a r r o w D o n o r Registry was
(Butterworth et al., 1992; Butterworth et al., 1993;
established nearly a decade ago, the number of indi-
Simmons et al., 1993; Switzer et al., under review;
viduals who have volunteered to donate bone mar-
Switzer et aL, in press: Switzer et al., in press;
row to unrelated strangers has grown exponentially.
Switzer et al., 1996)--has led us in this current
Each month, approximately 30000 persons join the
study to investigate one critical aspect of the donor
National Marrow D o n o r Program ( N M D P ) which
decision process: donor motivation. Specifically, we
now numbers over 2 million (National Marrow
set out to (a) describe the motives of unrelated
D o n o r Program, 1996). Despite the fact that medi- bone marrow donors, (b) examine the association
cal innovations have elevated bone marrow trans- of donor background characteristics (e.g., sex, age,
plantation from an experimental procedure to one religion) with motives for donation, and (c) examine
that is routinely used for some blood-related dis- the relationship of predonation factors, including
eases, and despite increased awareness and accep- donor motives, to psychosocial and physical out-
tance of unrelated donation among members of the comes of the donation process.
general public, few studies have systematically In many ways, bone marrow donation is similar
investigated psychosocial issues surrounding the to other types of medical volunteerism such as kid-
decision to become a bone marrow donor. ney and blood donation which have enjoyed more
research attention, especially with regard to donor
*Author for correspondence: 3811 O'Hara Street, motivation. In the following section, we briefly dis-
Pittsburgh, PA 15213, U.S.A. cuss motives for medical volunteerism in general

137
138 Galen E. Switzer et al.

before we move to more specifically consider bone The availability of cadaveric and living related
marrow donation. organs--coupled with concerns that any system of
unrelated living organ donation could be abused--
has restricted the number of unrelated solid organ
Medical volunteerism transplants performed in the U.S. However, unre-
The importance of investigating motivation in the lated donors do provide a substantial amount of
context of organ and bone marrow donation often bone marrow for transplantation (Silberman et al.,
has been noted in reviews of the donation literature, 1994). Bone marrow donation bears similarities
although systematic attempts to assess motives have both to blood donation (in that it involves a less
been rare (Andrykowski, 1994; Andrykowski, invasive medical procedure of a physical material
1994). The frequent failure of researchers studying that is easily regenerated)* and to solid organ do-
donor issues to empirically assess donor motives nation (in that it involves surgery under general
seems surprising given the demonstrated relation- anesthesia to assist a specific patient). Blood donor
ship of donor motives to donor outcomes in such motivations have been examined in detail by sev-
settings. For example, research with blood donors eral researchers and are relevant to bone marrow
has shown that donors' motivations a r e r e l a t e d to donation because of similarities in the donation
longer-term donation and donor satisfaction procedures and because bone marrow donors have
(Callero and Piliavin, 1983; Callero et al., 1987; often been recruited directly from blood donor
Piliavin, 1990; Piliavin and Callero, 1991). lists.
Although some outcomes such as long-term active The first studies of motivations for blood do-
participation may not be a central concern for cer- nation were conducted in the same decade as the
tain types of organ or tissue donation, understand- first studies of motivations for kidney donation,
ing how donors' self-evaluations, evaluations of the and reached similar conclusions; altruistic/humani-
donation process, and psychological and physical tarian motives for donating were most commonly
difficulty with donation may be affected by their reported (Oswalt, 1977; Simmons et al., 1977;
reasons for donating seems critical. Simmons et al., 1987). A more recent study examin-
Donors are likely to be motivated by a variety of ing temporal changes in blood donor motivation
intrinsic factors (e.g., acting in accordance with reli- determined that first-time donors were often motiv-
gious convictions), and extrinsic factors (e.g., social ated to donate by less altruistic forces such as exter-
pressures) that may operate simultaneously to nal social pressures (e.g., obligations to a group),
inspire and/or dissuade the donor. Furthermore, it but that if donation continued, these donors devel-
is probable that the particular combination of moti- oped an internal "donor self-image" (Callero et al.,
vational forces differs depending on whether or not 1987; Piliavin and Callero, 1991; Callero, 1985;
the donor is related to the recipient. Members of Callero et al., 1987; Charng et al., 1988; Gardner
the medical profession and transplantation research- and Cacioppo, 1995; Royse and Doochin, 1995).
ers have assumed that family members or emotional Increased commitment to blood donation and cor-
partners are naturally motivated by the prospect of responding changes in self-image are thus produced
saving the life of a loved one. In contrast to this in small incremental steps as donation continues; in-
view of related donors' motivations, expressions of dividuals begin to see themselves as "the kind of
willingness to donate to a stranger have often been person who donates." A similar process of increas-
viewed as unusual or even as pathological (Fellner ing commitment and changes in self-identity may be
and Schwartz, 1971). In the early years of kidney operating for bone marrow donors. Such donors
transplantation, for example, individuals who con- are often recruited to the registry in conditions that
tacted transplant centers and volunteered to donate may involve high levels of social pressure, but sub-
a kidney to a stranger were regarded with distrust sequently make a series of small incremental
and suspicion by transplant physicians (Hamburger decisions leading to actual donation (Stroncek et
and Crosnier, 1968). However, in an investigation aL, 1989).
of public attitudes toward kidney donation, Fellner Finally, there is some evidence that one's self-
and Schwartz (1971) found that 54% of metropoli- image as a "medical donor" may generalize across
tan residents expressed willingness to donate a kid- donation types. For example, studies of bone mar-
ney to an unrelated stranger. They concluded that row donor recruitment from blood donor registries
attitudes toward unrelated organ donation were have found that frequent blood donors are 3-12
much more favorable than the medical community times more likely to join the bone marrow registry
previously had anticipated. than less frequent blood donors (Beatty et al., 1989;
Briggs et al., 1986; Cacioppo and Gardner, 1993;
Sarason et al., 1993).
*Newer medical procedures including stem cell donation
that may eventually replace the traditional surgical Unrelated bone marrow donation
procedure for harvesting bone m a r r o w involve a pro-
cedure that is almost identical to whole blood and Unrelated bone marrow donors volunteer to
apheresis donation. engage in an act which is time consuming, painful,
Donation motives 139

and frequently frightening, to help someone they METHODS


have never met. Prospective donors who are part of
Subjects and procedure
the national registry are matched with individuals
who need a bone marrow transplant (most often to Potential subjects for this study were the 962 in-
treat leukemia, aplastic anemia, or some other form dividuals from the N M D P registry who donated
bone marrow between December 1987 and
of cancer or blood disorder) but who can not ident-
December 1991 (see Butterworth et al., 1993 for a
ify a related donor. Donors consent to undergo
more complete description of donor recruitment
general or spinal anesthesia for 1-2 hours while
procedures). Although a series of psychosocial ques-
bone marrow is aspirated from the illiac crest of the
tionnaires was sent to all donors, participants for
pelvis through small incisions in the donor's lower the current study included the cohort of 343 indi-
back. Following donation, donors may experience viduals who donated marrow through the N M D P
anesthesia--related side-effects such as sore throat, during this timeperiod and returned predonation,
nausea, headaches, vomiting, or temporary numb- 1-2 week postdonation, and one year postdonation
ness in the legs. Although long-term physical costs questionnaires. During the first few years of N M D P
to donors are likely to be small because bone mar- operation, virtually all donors were recruited
row regenerates, donors often spend a day or two through blood banks and thus were regular platelet
at home recovering and report feeling the lingering or blood donors who volunteered to join the regis-
effects of donation for up to a few weeks. try. Once a volunteer in the N M D P registry was
Unrelated bone marrow donation is a particularly identified through laboratory blood tests as the best
interesting form of medical volunteerism because it potential match for a patient needing marrow, the
involves costs and benefits that are higher than volunteer attended an hour-long information ses-
those of blood donation, but potentially lower than sion with donor center medical personnel. Before
consenting to become a bone marrow donor, all po-
those involved in the donation of nonregenerating
tential donors were fully informed about the mar-
organs (e.g., kidney, liver lobe, lung lobe) to a rela-
row collection procedure, risks and benefits of bone
tive. In addition to time missed from work and
marrow donation, and anesthesia options, and
home activities, the inconvenience of rearranging
often viewed a videotape which described a related
one's schedule, and anxiety induced by the medical donor transplant. Shortly after giving final consent
procedure, the costs of bone marrow donation to bone marrow donation, donors received a predo-
include physical discomfort and the small possibility nation questionnaire including the measures
of severe life-threatening complications (estimated described below. Follow-up questionnaires were
as about 1 in 400; [Bortin and Buckner, 1983]) or mailed 1-2 weeks postdonation and 1 year postdo-
even anesthesia-related death (1 in 10000; [Hirsh, nation.
1982]). The potential benefits include feeling that Longitudinally, of the 849 predonation respon-
one has acted in accordance with deep emotional dents, 716 (84%) subsequently returned the shortly
sentiments or moral convictions, and the very tangi- postdonation questionnaire. Of these 716, 343
ble possibility of saving someone's life. (48%) returned the one year postdonation ques-
In the course of our discussions with bone. mar- tionnaire producing a final panel of 343 donors
row donors in previous studies, it became evident who completed all three questionnaires. To deter-
that different donors weighed the costs and benefits mine whether particular types of donors were
of donation in very different ways and, in many likely to be excluded from our sample because of
cases, seemed to be so strongly motivated to donate nonresponse to one of the study questionnaires,
the 343 donors in our study cohort were compared
that they discounted any possible negative outcomes
to donors who returned predonation but not post-
of the donation process altogether (Butterworth et
donation questionnaires. Chi-square analyses indi-
al., 1993; Switzer et al., in press). We therefore
cated that these two groups did not differ on any
became deeply interested in investigating donors' of the demographic characteristics measured predo-
motives in a more systematic fashion, both to nation. In addition to gathering questionnaire
describe such motives and their demographic ante- data, we conducted one hour in-depth telephone
cedents, and to determine whether motives pre- interviews with every second donor of approxi-
dicted psychosocial and physical reactions to the mately the first one hundred donors. Quotations
donation process. For the current study, we exam- from these 50 interviews are included below to il-
ined these issues qualitatively--by creating a typol- lustrate the categories of motives developed from
ogy of the six types of non-mutually exclusive the full sample.
motives reported by this group of d o n o r s - - a n d
quantitatively--by examining the relationships Measures
among donor background characteristics, donor Three classes of variables were included in the
motives, and donors' reactions to the donation ex- present study. First, motives were coded from open-
perience. ended responses to the predonation questionnaire
140 Galen E. Switzer et al.

item, "In your own words, what do you feel your whether the process was more or less painful
reasons are for donating?" A typology was devel- than the donor expected, how many days
oped inductively; as open-ended responses were passed until the donor felt physically "back to
reviewed they were collapsed into six distinct motive normal," and whether the donor had experi-
categories which were based on Simmons' prior enced fatigue or lower back pain. These items
work (Simmons et al., 1977). Research staff were were scored along 3-point scales (e.g., fatigue:
trained to code up to four motives for each respon- 0=not at all, 1 = a little, 2 = a lot; days
dent based on the responses to the open-ended until donor felt physically normal: 0-5, 6-14,
question. All responses were initially coded by one >14 days). An overall physical difficulty scale
staff member and an additional twenty percent were was created by summing the 5 items [6]. Thus,
subsequently coded by a second staff member. scores ranged from 0 = no physical difficulty,
Agreement among the two ratings, corrected for to 10 = high physical difficulty with donation
chance, was measured using Cohen's Kappa. A (Cronbach's alpha = .70).
kappa of .85 (p < .05) was achieved between the Psychological reactions shortly postdonation
two raters, indicating a strong degree of interrater (and also measured at one year postdonation)
reliability. Over half (63%) of respondents reported were measured with a four-item scale developed
more than one motive. The average number of mo- for use with kidney donors (Simmons et al., 1977),
tives reported was 1.97 (sd = .96). and modified slightly for use with bone marrow
Second, donor background characteristics includ- donors (Switzer et al., in press). Items asked
ing gender, age, religion, marital status, whether the whether donors felt like they had given something
donor had any children, and educational level for nothing in return (0 = disagree a lot,
attained were collected from predonation question- 1 = disagree a little, 2 = agree a little, 3 = agree a
naires. Because 99% of these donors were white, lot; dichotomized for analysis as 0 versus all other
ethnicity was not included as a variable in these categories), whether they were generally happy
analyses. about having donated (0 = very happy about hav-
Finally, reaction to the donation process was ing donated, 1 = a little happy, 2 = a little
measured by a total of seven outcomes gathered in unhappy, 3 = very unhappy about having
the three questionnaires (predonation: ambivalence; donated; dichotomized as 0 versus all other cat-
shortly postdonation: physical difficulty with do- egories), how worried they were about the effects
nation, negative psychological reactions; 1 year of marrow donation on their own body
postdonation: negative psychological reactions, self- (0 = almost never, 1 = sometimes, 2 = often;
esteem, feeling like a better person, concern about dichotomized as 0 versus all other categories), and
recipient). The one reaction to the donation process how worried they were about their own current
measure collected predonation was an additive health (0 = not at all, 1 = a little, 2 = somewhat,
seven-item ambivalence scale used previously with 3 = very; dichotomized as 0 versus all other cat-
kidney donors (Simmons et al., 1977). (Cronbach's egories). Because the number of response cat-
alpha for the present sample was .78). It was egories per item ranged from 3-4, and because the
assumed that the formulation of donor motives distribution of responses on each item was skewed,
temporally preceded donors' emotional responses items were dichotomized as noted above (Switzer
(e.g., ambivalence) toward donation--thus, predo- et al., in press; Simmons et al., 1977). The four
nation ambivalence is conceptualized as a donation items were summed to form a final scale ranging
reaction potentially affected by motives. from 0 = no negative psychological reactions, to
Ambivalence items assessed doubts and worries 4 = very negative psychological reactions. The in-
about donating (e.g., "I sometimes feel unsure ternal reliability coefficient for this scale
about donating.") and commitment to donating (alpha = .44) is lower than the coefficient obtained
(e.g. "I would really want to donate myself even if in previous uses of the scale (alpha = .73;
someone else could do it.") To be consistent with (Simmons et al., 1977). However, the four items
previous applications of this measure (Switzer et al., were all positively intercorrelated (median r =.10),
in press; Simmons et al., 1977), all items were and excluding one or more items did not produce
dichotomized and an index of ambivalence was any improvement in reliability. The low coefficient
formed by summing the number of items checked probably reflects measurement error in large part,
by the respondent (0 = no items endorsed: not at and suggests that statistics using this scale will
all ambivalent, to 7 = all items endorsed: extremely underestimate the degree of relationship of donor
ambivalent). motives to psychological reactions.
Physical difficulty with donation and negative The four other measures of donor reaction to the
psychological reactions to donation were donation process were measured one year postdona-
measured on the 1-2 week postdonation ques- tion. These included measures of negative psycho-
tionnaire. Physical difficulty with donation was logical reactions (discussed above), self-esteem,
measured with five items asking how the donor feelings that one is a better person as a result of the
felt physically during and after the donation, donation, and concern about the recipient. A nine-
Donation motives 141

Table 1. Means and standard deviations for all donation reactions


Donation reaction Range Mean SD

Predonation
Ambivalence 0-7 1.78 1.95
Shortly postdonation
Physical difficulty 0-10 4.85 2.49
Psychological 0-4 1.55 .80
difficulty
One year
postdonation
Psychological 0-4 1.40 .62
difficulty

item scale measuring self-esteem was derived from RESULTS


the scale developed by Rosenberg* (Rosenberg and
Simmons, 1972; Rosenberg, 1965), (Cronbach's Describing donor motives
alpha for this sample was .75). Donors responded
Our motive typology included six types of mo-
to statements about themselves (e.g. "I feel I have a
tives: empathy-related motives, exchange-related
number of good qualities", "On the whole, I am
motives, normative motives, idealized helping mo-
satisfied with myself,. . . . I feel I do not have much
tives, positive feeling motives, and past experience-
to be proud of", "At times I think I am no good at
based motives. Each are defined below and are
all") using four response categories (1 = strongly
summarized. These motives are similar to those
agree, 2 = agree, 3 = disagree, and 4 = strongly
found among other types of volunteers, both in nat-
disagree). To be consistent with previous appli-
uralistic settings--such as volunteer "buddies" for
cations of the scale (Simmons et al., 1993; Switzer
AIDS patients (Clary and Snyder, 1991; Snyder,
et al., in press; Simmons et al., 1977) responses
1993; Clary and Orenstein, 1991; Omoto and
were dichotomized (0 = low self-esteem, 1 = high
Snyder, 1990), 4-H volunteers (Rohs, 1986), social
self-esteem) and summed to form a composite scale
service volunteers (Rubin and Thorelli, 1984),
ranging from 1 to 9.
elderly volunteers (Okun and Eisenberg, 1992), and
The final three variables were adapted from
kidney donors (Simmons et al., 1977; Simmons et
Simmons' studies of kidney donors (Simmons et al.,
al., 1987)--as well as in more controlled experimen-
1977). The ten-item "perception of oneself as a bet-
tal studies of helping (Batson et al., 1989; Baumann
ter person" measured the extent to which a donor
et al., 1981; Karylowski, 1982; Perry et al., 1986;
felt like a better person due to the donation
Bar-Tal, 1982; Cialdini et al., 1987). Table 2
(Cronbach's alpha =.75). Items were questions
includes donor quotations from our in-depth inter-
about respondents' self-feelings when they thought
views and open-ended questionnaire responses illus-
about the transplant (e.g. "When you think about
trating each motive category.
the transplant have you felt..." 1 = very proud,
Exchange-related motives were the most com-
2 = a little proud, 3 = not at all proud; "Donating
monly reported type of motive; 45% of donors
bone marrow makes one feel that he/she is some- reported such motives. Motive descriptions were
how a bigger and more worthwhile person. Do
categorized as exchange-related if they stated or
you..." 1 = agree a lot, 2 = agree a little, implied donors' awareness of the relative costs (to
3 = disagree a little, or 4 = disagree a lot). Items themselves) and benefits (to themselves or the mar-
were dichotomized (0 = low "better person," row recipient) of donating. Variations of exchange
1 = high "better person") and summed to form a motives included statements indicating that donors
composite scale ranging from 0 to 10. were comparing their own good fortune with the
Donors' concern about the recipient at one year bad fortune of the recipient and wanting to "give
postdonation was measured with a single item. something back," and statements concerning the
Donors were asked to respond to the statement, "I hope that should they or their family were in a
think about the recipient..." by checking one of similar desperate situation, others would do the
nine frequencies ranging from, 1 = never to same for them.
9 = more often than once a day. Table 1 lists More than one-third of donors (37%) gave ideal-
means and standard deviations for all donation ized helping motive responses. These donors seem to
reaction variables. have been responding "automatically" without deep
thought about the potential costs of donation or
*Nine-item and eight-item versions of this scale have been even their own motivations to become volunteers.
extensively used and validated in previous work (e.g., Approximately one-quarter (26%) of donors cited
Simmons et al., 1993; Simmons et aL, 1977; Rosenberg feeling that donating was their social obligation/
and Simmons, 1972). In such cases, the elimination of
one or two items from the original ten-item scale has duty or that donating was morally correct or appro-
substantially improved the internal consistency of the priate (normative motives). In many cases, state-
composite measure. ments included in this category contained direct
142 Galen E. Switzer et al.

Table 2. Six Donor Motivation Categories Illustrated with Donor Quotations


Exchange-related motives
The main thing on the bone marrow, I think, (was)a the.., information they gave when I received the pamphlet. It gave the statistics on
survival and how much it increased. I felt, oh boy, if I can do that (it's such a little thing on my part). I can't remember the exact
statistics, but it increased from (about) 20% to 70%.
My healthy marrow can prolong her life, I hope. That's the whole drive. I got something freely, and if I can give it back, let's do it.
Idealized helping motives
I feel fortunate that I am able to be a donor... I guess (I was) just glad to be able to help.
(I wanted) to try to help somebody out, give somebody a chance for a normal life.
Normative motives
There was no doubt I wouldn't do it. It's something I feel I should do. I was brought up to help others whenever possible.
Well, I guess being raised Catholic and being pro-life, it gives me a real good feeling to say I helped someone.
Positivefeeling motives
The bottom line is here I can do something that's going to make me feel pretty good about myself.
From the beginning I felt very privileged I could do this.
Empathy-related motives
I don't think I got emotional until they told me they notified her they had a donor. That's when 1 got emotional (thinking of them telling
her). I was real excited for her.... The fact that she's close to my age makes me feel more emotional.
... (the donor coordinator) told me it was a little five-year-oldboy. That decided me. I have a little five-year-oldboy at home now. I keep
looking at him and thinking, "What if it was my boy?"
Past experience-based motives
It's just the next step after giving pheresis. I started out giving blood, then pheresis, now this. If can help someone (I might as well).
I'm a science teacher and I had read about children with Combined Immunosuppression Syndrome and that really appealed.... I did it
because it was medical.
aParenthetical words or phrases within quotes indicate that the words may not be precisely those used by the respondent, although they
are quite close. Such slight inaccuracies are the inevitable result of verbatim documentation of interviews which were not tape
recorded.

references to religious tenets concerning helping c o m m o n motive types a n d did not include motives
others. A n o t h e r q u a r t e r (25%) o f d o n o r s discussed from these two categories.
the d o n a t i o n in terms o f the good feeling it gave Intercorrelations a m o n g the motive types were in-
t h e m to p e r f o r m this helping act (positive feeling itially calculated to determine, in part, whether our
motives). These positive feelings often included the a priori motive categories were in fact distinct mo-
expectation t h a t the d o n o r would have improved tive types. Low intercorrelations a m o n g motive cat-
self-esteem or feel better a b o u t him/herself after egories would bolster o u r claim t h a t we are
donating. D o n o r s also reported feeling special or measuring a well-defined heterogeneous set of
privileged to be able to donate, especially w h e n reasons for donating. W e did find t h a t motives were
they were the only p e r s o n whose m a r r o w m a t c h e d n o t highly intercorrelated; the only statistically re-
t h a t o f the recipient. A p p r o x i m a t e l y one-fifth o f liable relationships were between exchange a n d
d o n o r s (18%) described their m o t i v a t i o n s to give idealized helping motives (r = - . 1 4 ; p < .01), nor-
mative a n d idealized helping motives (r = - .21;
b o n e m a r r o w in terms of their feelings t o w a r d the
p < .01), a n d n o r m a t i v e a n d e m p a t h y motives
recipient (empathy-related motives). D o n o r s seemed
(r = - . 1 6 ; p < .01). The correlation coefficients are
to be putting themselves in the place o f the person
all relatively small a n d negative indicating t h a t
w h o needed b o n e m a r r o w or t h a t d o n o r ' s family.
e n d o r s e m e n t o f one o f these motive pairs slightly
E m p a t h y seemed to be triggered by a variety of
decreases the likelihood t h a t the other motive o f the
stimuli including similarities between the d o n o r a n d
pair will also be endorsed.
the recipient, similarities between the recipient a n d
s o m e o n e close to the d o n o r , or simply seeing some- Association o f background characteristics with mo-
one else w h o was ill. tives
Finally, smaller n u m b e r s o f d o n o r s reported past
Two-thirds of the sample were m e n (66%). M o r e
experience-based motives ( 8 % ) which included state-
t h a n h a l f (65%) o f the d o n o r s were currently mar-
m e n t s a b o u t the d o n o r ' s previous life experiences
ried a n d h a d at least one child (64%). M o r e t h a n
such as participation as a b l o o d or apheresis d o n o r ,
9 9 % were high school graduates a n d 52% h a d com-
o t h e r b a c k g r o u n d as a volunteer, or experiences pleted some college. D o n o r s ' age ranged from 22 to
t h a t m a d e the d o n o r especially aware o f the need 55 years (X = 38, sd =7.6). Forty-three percent
for b o n e marrow. A n additional 9 % o f motives did were Protestant, 35% were Catholic, 3 % were
n o t seem to fit any o f the a b o v e categories. The Jewish, a n d 19% did n o t consider themselves part
m o s t c o m m o n t h e m e a m o n g these remaining mo- of those religions.
tives were statements t h a t did n o t so m u c h give Chi-square analyses were conducted in order to
reasons for their decision as express incredulity t h a t determine whether d o n o r b a c k g r o u n d character-
a n y o n e would decide otherwise. Because the pro- istics were associated with specific d o n a t i o n mo-
p o r t i o n o f d o n o r s reporting past experience-based tives. W o m e n were significantly more likely to
motives, or motives t h a t did not fit any o f the pre- report e m p a t h y a n d positive feeling motives a n d
established categories was relatively small, sub- s o m e w h a t m o r e likely to report idealized helping
sequent analyses focused o n only the five m o s t motives t h a n were male donors. Twenty-three per-
Donation motives 143

cent of females compared to 13% of males reported feeling, empathy) were entered in the second block.
empathy motives (Z2(1, n = 342) = 5.02, p < .05) Variables were entered in separate, hierarchical
and 31% of females compared to 21% of males blocks because, temporally, donors' background
reported positive feeling motives (~2(1, characteristics were likely to have preceded develop-
n = 342) = 3.83, p < .05). In addition, younger ment of their motives for donation, and because
donors were more likely to base their reasons for our goal was to assess the effect of donation mo-
donating on exchange-related motives and idealized tives on outcomes while controlling for the effects
helping motives than were older donors. Forty-eight of donor background characteristics (Cialdini et al.,
percent of younger donors (those under 40) 1987).
reported exchange motives, while 38% of older The linear combination of donor background
donors reported exchange motives (X2(I, characteristics and motives produced statistically
n = 343) = 3.66, p = .056) and 41% of younger significant (or nearly significant in the case of
donors compared to 31% of older donors reported Physical Difficulty and Psychological Difficulty 1
idealized helping motives (;(2(1, n = 343)= 3.67, year postdonation) R-values for the full equation
p = .055). Finally, Protestants (30%) were slightly for all outcomes except donor self-esteem.
more likely than members of other religious groups Ambivalence toward donation was predicted by
(22%) to report normative motives for donating gender and education level; female donors and
(Z2(1, n = 343) = 2.91, p = .088). Other than these donors who were more highly educated were more
associations no other background characteristics likely to experience higher levels of predonation
were associated with specific motives for donating. ambivalence. Even after the effects of background
characteristics were taken into account, exchange-
Association o f background characteristics and mo- related and positive feeling motives were related to
tives with donation reactions ambivalence. Donors who seemed to be weighing
A series of hierarchical multiple regression analy- the costs and benefits of donating were more likely
ses was conducted to assess the relationship of to experience predonation ambivalence while those
background characteristics and the 5 most com- who were motivated by the expectation of positive
monly reported predonation motives to psychoso- feelings for themselves as a result of donation were
cial and physical reactions to the donation process less likely to be ambivalent.
(see Table 3). Seven such regression equations were Of the two outcomes measured shortly postdona-
constructed. For each equation, a single outcome tion, psychological difficulty with donation was
variable (ambivalence, physical difficulty with do- most strongly related to predonation background
nation, shortly postdonation negative psychological factors and motives. The only factor that signifi-
reactions, one year postdonation negative psycho- cantly predicted physical difficulty with donation
logical reactions, self-esteem, better person, and was gender; women were much more likely to
concern about recipient) served as the dependent report experiencing physical side-effects of donation
variable. Predictor variables were entered in two than were men. Psychological difficulty, in contrast,
blocks: background variables (gender, age, religion, was predicted by age (younger donors were more
education level, marital status, and presence of chil- likely to report psychological difficulty than were
dren) were entered in the first block; motives older donors), and by marital status (married
(exchange, idealized helping, normative, positive donors experienced greater psychological difficulty

Table 3. Relationship of Background Characteristics and Motives to Donation Reactionsa


Ambivalence Physical Psych. difficulty Psych. difficulty Self-esteem Better person Concern about
toward donation difficulty shortly post 1 yr. post recipient

Background
Gender .14" .24*** .01 .07 .03 -.20** .03
Age -.04 -.07 - . 16* - . 14* -.06 -.03 -.04
Religion -.03 -.01 -.01 .05 .03 .13" .06
Education .21"* .01 -.07 .03 .06 -,04 -.09
Married -.06 .05 .19* -.05 .11 -.02 -.04
Children -.03 -.07 -.04 .03 -.09 -.04 -.01
Motives
Exchange .13* -.04 ,08 .03 -.03 .02 -.08
Ideal help .11 .03 .18"* .14" -.04 .04 -.06
Normative -.08 -.02 -.05 .01 .07 -.09 .03
Pos. feeling - . 18"* -.07 -.06 -.04 -.02 .14" .11 #
Empathy .05 .03 -.01 - . I 1# -.02 .12* .22** *
Adjusted R .30*** .18# .29*** .16# .13 .24** .22*
a Numbers in the Table are standardized regression coefficients (beta values).
#p < =.10.
*p < =.05.
**p < =.01.
***p < =.001.
144 Galen E. Switzer et al.

than did unmarried donors). In addition, donors tral goal was to determine whether donors' self-
who reported idealized helping motives were signifi- reported motives for donating predicted donors'
cantly more likely to experience shortly postdona- reactions to the donation process even after back-
tion psychological difficulty than were their ground characteristics were taken into account.
counterparts who did not endorse such motives. Although the majority of donors reported more
A pattern of results similar to that for shortly than one motive, motives were not highly intercor-
postdonation psychological difficulty was found for related. This indicates that particular groups of mo-
one year postdonation psychological difficulty. tives were not consistently reported together,
Older donors and donors reporting idealized help- suggesting that a number of different psychosocial
ing motives again experienced less psychological dif- processes rather than a single motivation may guide
ficulty than their counterparts. In addition, donors this type of altruistic behavior. This interpretation
who were motivated to donate by empathetic feel- is strengthened by our findings that different motive
ings for the recipient were slightly less likely to ex- types were related to different sets of background
perience psychological difficulty at one year and outcome variables.
postdonation than were donors who did not report The reasons donors gave for performing such an
such motives. altruistic act varied widely. These reasons included
The perception that one was a better person for weighing the risks and benefits of such an oper-
having donated bone marrow was predicted by two ation, a simple desire to help, normative consider-
background characteristics and two types of mo- ations, the positive feelings related to performing
tives. Men were significantly more likely to feel like such an act, and feelings of empathy for the recipi-
better persons for having donated than were women ent or the recipient's family. Motives were coded
and members of Protestant religions were more from an open-ended questionnaire item into a priori
likely to feel like better persons than were donors motive categories based on previous work with kid-
with other religious affiliations. Donors who ney donors (Simmons et al., 1977). Although inter-
reported positive feeling and empathy motives were rater reliability for the presence or absence of each
also more likely to feel like better persons for hav- motive was excellent, this manner of assessing and
ing donated than were donors who did not report coding donor motives is likely to have produced
such motives. motive groupings that are somewhat less precise
Finally, concern with the recipient was signifi- than those that would have been produced with
cantly predicted by empathy-related motives; those closed-ended ordinal-level items. Ultimately, how-
donors who were motivated to donate because they ever, allowing donors to describe motives in their
could understand what the recipient was feeling own words may have yielded more valid responses
were more likely to be concerned about the reci- than closed-ended items would have. In addition,
pient's well-being one year postdonation. In ad- the fact that several important and intuitively mean-
dition, donors who reported positive feeling motives ingful relationships of donor motives with donation
were somewhat more likely to be concerned about reactions were found lends credence to the robust-
the recipient postdonation. ness of these motive categories; more precise
measures could be expected to produce even stron-
ger relationships.
DISCUSSION
There were surprisingly few reliable associations
Donors who agree to undergo a surgical pro- of donor background characteristics with donation
cedure to donate bone marrow to a stranger engage motives. Perhaps the most interesting was that
in a unique and potentially life-threatening form of women were more likely than men to report donat-
helping. The fact that the number of unrelated bone ing because of empathic feelings for the recipient,
marrow transplants performed in the U.S. increases because they expected to feel good about donating,
dramatically each year makes it critical to better and for unspecified "idealized" helping reasons.
understand the psychosocial processes involved in These motives make sense in the context of the so-
donors' decision-making. Although researchers have cietal norms for women, which encourage--and
long been interested donor motives in the broader often impel--them to be more nurturing and caring
context of living medical donation (e.g., blood do- than men (Eagly, 1987; Eagly and Crowly, 1986;
nation and kidney donation), motives for donating Parsons and Bales, 1955). The incorporation of
bone marrow have not been systematically exam- these norms into women's daily roles may, in turn,
ined to date. Thus, we have undertaken an investi- lead women to help others for a variety of reasons
gation of donor motives in a cohort of the first that are consistent with such roles (e.g., empathy
individuals to donate through the National Marrow for someone in need), or for the possible outcomes
Donor Program. In addition to describing such mo- of having fulfilled the role (e.g., feeling good for
tives, we have examined the relationship of donor having helped someone else).
background characteristics to motives, as well as The most interesting and important findings in
the relationship of background characteristics and this study of donor motives resulted from the set of
motives to a set of key donation reactions. Our cen- regression analyses concerning the relationship of
Donation motives 145

donor background characteristics and motives with significantly lower levels of predonation ambiva-
donation reactions. In general, men had more posi- lence, elevated feelings of being a better person for
tive donation experiences than did women; men had having donated, and somewhat more concern about
lower levels of predonation ambivalence, less physi- the recipient than donors who were not motivated
cal difficulty with donation, and felt like better per- by positive feelings. Although it is not necessarily
sons following donation. This gender difference clear that concern about the recipient is a positive
favoring men is one that is fairly consistently reaction, the fact that this outcome is associated
reported throughout the donation/transplantation with other positive outcomes (both for positive feel-
literature (Switzer et al., in press; Piliavin and ing and empathy motives) indicates that it certainly
Cailero, 1991; Simmons et al., 1987), and may is not causing donors to experience the full do-
result from women's greater willingness to express nation process less positively. As noted previously,
less positive feelings or from an actual difference in the fact that positive feeling motives are associated
the way that bone marrow donation is experienced with lower ambivalence is important because
by men and women. Two other groups--more edu- ambivalence may lead to a variety of other negative
cated donors and younger donors--also reported donation reactions. The link between positive feel-
less positive reactions in terms of greater predona- ing motives and feeling like a better person after
tion ambivalence or psychological difficulty one donating is not surprising, and may represent, in
year postdonation. part, a fulfillment of donors' expectations about the
Even after the effects of donor background donation process. Donors with empathy motives
characteristics were controlled, however, there were had somewhat less psychological difficulty at one-
notable relationships of pre-donation motives to do- year postdonation, felt like better persons for hav-
nation reactions. Two types of donors, those report- ing donated, and were significantly more concerned
ing exchange-related motives and those reporting about the recipient than were donors who did not
idealized helping motives, seemed to experience do- report empathy motives.
nation as less positive than donors who did not Although expecting positive feelings for oneself
report such motives. First, donors who indicated and being concerned about the well-being of a reci-
that they had weighed the costs and benefits of pient seem to represent two divergent motivational
donating were much more likely to experience pre- states--one focused internally on the potential ben-
donation ambivalence than were other donors. efits of donation to oneself and one focused exter-
Although such exchange-related motives were not nally on the potential benefits to the recipient--they
directly associated with other less positive donation do share a common element. More than any of the
reactions, other investigations involving this same other motive categories, these two motive types are
population indicate that predonation ambivalence is affectively rather than cognitively based. Although
itself a central predictor of other negative postdona- the evidence presented here is preliminary, it is
tion outcomes (e.g., physical and psychological diffi- possible that donors who are emotionally engaged
culty; Switzer et al., 1996). Thus, a cost-benefit in the donation process may experience more posi-
analysis approach to donation may place donors tive donation outcomes than those who donate for
directly or indirectly at risk for a variety of less other reasons.
positive donation outcomes. Second, donors report- In sum, our findings demonstrate several things.
ing idealized helping motives were also likely to First, it is clear that unrelated bone marrow donors
have less positive experiences in the form of elev- agree to donate for a variety of reasons, none of
ated psychological difficulty (e.g., feeling like they which--in contrast to some of the earliest assump-
had given up something for nothing in return) both tions about unrelated donors' motivations--seem
shortly postdonation and one year postdonation. It particularly pathological. Second, specific motives
is possible that exchange-related motives and ideal- for donation appear to be associated with donor
ized helping motives are actually two aspects of a background characteristics. This relationship seems
single dimension--exchange donors seem to have especially strong for donors' gender, which was as-
heightened awareness of the potential costs of do- sociated with three of the five motive types central
nation while idealized helping donors seem to be to our analysis. Finally, donors' motives are associ-
unaware of such costs. Differing levels of sensitivity ated with their reactions to donation; at least one
to the potential underlying costs of donation would motive type was associated with five of the seven
explain why exchange donors experience predona- donation reactions. Specifically, two motive types
tion ambivalence while idealized helping donors' (exchange and idealized helping) were associated
negative reactions (psychological difficulty) are with less positive donation outcomes and two
delayed until after the donation experience. others (positive feeling and empathy) were associ-
Two donation motives--positive feeling and ated with more positive donation outcomes. The
empathy--were related to positive reactions to the implications of these findings for the process of
donation process. Donors who said that they felt donor recruitment and education are important. If
privileged to donate or that they expected to feel subsequent studies confirm the relationship of
good about themselves after donation experienced donor motivation to donation outcomes, donor
146 Galen E, Switzer et al.

recruitment and continuing education strategies Callero, P. L. and Piliavin, J. A. (1983) Developing a com-
mitment to blood donation: the impact of one's first ex-
could be tailored to compensate for any liabilities
perience. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 13, 1-16.
invoked by certain motives, and to encourage Callero, P. L. (1985) Role-identity salience. Social
donors to view the donation process more compre- Psychology Quarterly 48, 203-215.
hensively. F o r example, if a donor coordinator Callero, P. L., Howard, J. A. and Piliavin, J. A. (1987)
informally assessed a donor's motives shortly before Helping behavior as role behavior: disclosing social
structure and history in the analysis of prosocial action.
donation and discovered that the donor was focuss- Social Psychology Quarterly 50, 247-256.
ing narrowly on a process of weighing costs and Charng, H. W., Piliavin, J. A. and Callero, P. L. (1988)
benefits of donating (exchange motives), the donor Role identity and reasoned action in the prediction of
coordinator could help the donor to broaden his/ repeated behavior. Social Psychology Quarterly 51, 303-
her perspective to consider the potential good that 317.
Cialdini, R. B., Schaller, M., Houlihan, D., Arps, K.,
could result from donating or on feelings of empa- Fultz, J. and Beaman, A. L. (1987) Empathy-based
thy for the recipient. We believe that the findings helping: is it selflessly or selfishly motivated?. Journal of
from the present study both enhance our conceptual Personality and Social Psychology 53, 749-758.
and theoretical understanding of the process of Clary, G. E. and Snyder, M. (1991) A functional analysis
of altruism and prosocial behavior: the case of volun-
medical volunteerism and further our ability to
teerism. Review of Personality and Social Psychology 12,
make practical suggestions for the recruitment and 119-148.
education of bone marrow donors. Clary, E. G. and Orenstein, L. (1991) The amount and
effectiveness of help: the relationship of motives and
abilities to helping behavior. Personafity and Social
UNLINKED REFERENCES Psychology Bulletin 17, 58-64.
Cohen, J. and Cohen, P. (1983) Applied Multiple
Cohen and Cohen, 1983 Regression~Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral
Sciences. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.
Eagly, A. (1987) Sex Differences in Social Behavior: A
Acknowledgements--This work was supported by grant Social Role Interpretation. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.
HL48883 from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Eagly, A. and Crowly, M. (1986) Gender and helping
Institute, Bethesda, MD. behavior: a meta-analytic review of the social psycho-
logical literature. PsychologicalBulletin 100, 283-308.
Fellner, C. H. and Schwartz, S. H. (1971) Altruism in dis-
REFERENCES repute: medical versus public attitudes toward the living
organ donor. New England Journal of Medicine 284,
Andrykowski, M. A. (1994) Psychiatric and psychosocial 582-585.
aspects of bone marrow transplantation. Psychosomatics Gardner, W. E. and Cacioppo, J. T. (1995) Multi-gallon
35, 13-24. blood donors: why do they give?. Transfusion 35, 795-
Bar-Tal, D. (1982) Segmental development of helping 798.
behavior: a cognitive learning model. Development Hamburger, J. and Crosnier, J. (1968) Moral and ethical
Review 2, 101-124. problems in transplantation. In Human Transplantation,
Batson, C. D., Batson, J. G., Griffitt, C. A., Barrientos, eds. F. Rapaport and J. Dausset, pp. 37-44. Grune &
S., Brandt, J. R.., Sprengelmeyer, P. and Bayly, M. Stratton, New York.
J. (I989) Negative-state relief and the empathy-altruism Hirsh, R. A. (1982) An approach to assessing periopera-
hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology tive risk. In Medical Care of the Surgical Patients: A
56, 922-933. Problem Oriented Approach to Management, eds F. H.
Baumann, D. J., Cialdini, R. B. and Kenrich, D. T. (1981) Goldmann, W. K. Brown, W. K. Levy, G. B. Slap, E.
Altruism as hedonism: helping and self-gratification as J. Sussman, et al., pp. 31-39. Lippincott, Philadelphia,
equivalent responses. Journal of Personality and Social PA.
Psychology 40, 1039-1046. Karylowski, J. (1982) Two types of altruistic behavior:
Beatty, P. G., Atcher, C., Hess, E., Meyer, D. M. and doing good to feel good or to make the other feel good.
Slichter, S. J. (1989) Recruiting blood donors into a In Cooperation and Helping Behavior: Theories and
local bone marrow donor registry. Transfusion 29, 778- Research, eds V. J. Derlega and J. Grzelak. Academic
782. Press, New York.
Bortin, M. M. and Buckner, C. D. (1983) Major compli- National Marrow Donor Program (1996) CP1 Statistics.
cations of marrow harvesting for transplantation. Okun, M. A. and Eisenberg, N. (1992) Motives and intent
Experimental Hematology 11( 11), 916-921. to continue organizational volunteering among residents
Briggs, N. C., Piliavin, J. A. and Becker, G. A. (1986) On of a retirement community area. Journal of Community
willingness to be a bone marrow donor. Transfusion 26, Psychology 20, 183-187.
324-330. Omoto, A. M. and Snyder, M. (1990) Basic research in
Butterworth, V. A., Simmons, R. G. and Schimmel, action: volunteerism and society's response to AIDS.
M. (1992) When altruism fails: reactions of unrelated Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 16, 152-165.
bone marrow donors when the recipient dies. Omega 26, Oswalt, R. M. (1977) A review of blood donor motivation
161-173. and recruitment. Transfusion 17, 123-135.
Butterworth, V. A., Simmons, R. G., Bartsch, G., Parsons, T. and Bales, R. F. (1955) Family Socialization
Randall, B., Schimmel, M. and Stroncek, D. F. (1993) and Interaction Process. Free Press, Glencoe, IL.
Psyehosocial effects of unrelated bone marrow donation: Perry, L. C., Perry, D. G. and Weiss, R. J. (1986) Age
experiences of the National Marrow Donor Program. differences in children's beliefs about whether altruism
Blood 81, 1947-1959. makes the actor feel good. Social Cognition 4, 236-269.
Cacioppo, J. T. and Gardner, W. L. (1993) What underlies Piliavin, J. A. (1990) Why do they give the gift of life? A
medical donor attitudes and behavior?. Health review of research on blood donors since 1977.
Psychology 12, 269-271. Transfusion 30, 444-459.
Donation motives 147

Piliavin, J. A. and Callero, P. L. (1991) Giving Blood." The donors. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 34, 285-
Development of an Altruistic Identity. The Johns 301.
Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, MD. Simmons, R. G., Marine, S. K. and Simmons, R. L.
Rohs, F. R. (1986) Social background, personality, and (1987) Gift of Life: The Effect of Organ Transplantation
attitudinal factors influencing decision to volunteer and on Individual, Family, and Societal Dynamics.
level of involvement among adult 4-H leaders. Journal Transaction Books, New Brunswick, NJ.
of Voluntary Action Research 15, 87-99. Simmons, R. G., Klein, S. D. and Simmons, R. L. (1977)
Rosenberg, M. and Simmons, R. G. (1972) Black and Gift of Life: The Social and Psychological Impact of
White Self-Esteem: The Urban School Child. American Organ Transplantation. Wiley, New York.
Sociological Association, Arnold and Caroline Rose Snyder, M. (1993) Basic research and practical problems:
Monograph Series, Washington, DC. the promise of a "functional" personality and social
Rosenberg, M. (1965) Society and the Adolescent Self- psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin
Image. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. 19, 251-264.
Stroncek, D., Strand, R., Scott, E., Kamstra-Halvorson,
Royse, D. and Doochin, K. E. (1995) Multi-gallon blood
L., Halagan, N., Rogers, G. and McCulough, J. (1989)
donors: who are they?. Transfusion 35, 826-831.
Attitudes and physical condition of unrelated bone mar-
Rubin, A. and Thorelli, I. M. (1984) Egoistic motives and row donors immediately after donation. Transfusion 29,
longevity of participation by social service volunteers. 317-322.
Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 20, 223-235. Switzer, G. E., Dew, M. A., Simmons, R. G. (in press).
Sarason, I. G., Sarason, B. R., Slichter, S. J., Beatty, P. Donor ambivalence and post-donation outcomes: impli-
G., Meyer, D. M. and Bolgiano, D. C. (1993) cations for living donation. Proceedings of the
Increasing participation of blood donors in a bone-mar- International Transplant Society: XVI Biennial Congress.
row registry. Health Psychology 12, 272-276. Switzer, G. E., Simmons, R. G. and Dew, M. A. (1996)
Silberman, G., Crosse, M. G., Peterson, E. A., Weston, R. Helping unrelated strangers: physical and psychological
C., Horowitz, M. M., Appelbaum, F. R. and Cheson, reactions to the bone marrow donation process among
B. D. (1994) Availability and appropriateness of allo- anonymous donors. Journal of Applied Social
geneic bone marrow transplantation for chronic myeloid Psychology 26(6), 469-490.
leukemia in 10 countries. New England Journal of Switzer, G. E., Dew, M. A., Magistro, C. A., Regalski, J.
Medicine 331, 1063-1067. M., Twillman, R. K. and Simmons, R. G. (under
Simmons, R. G., Schimmel, M. and Butterworth, V. review). Sibling bone marrow donors' donation experi-
A. (1993) The self-image of unrelated bone marrow ences: psychosocial issues and family relationships.

You might also like