Changes in General Self-Efficacy and Mindfulness Are Associated With Short-Term Improvements in Mood During Art-Making in A Partial Hospital Program

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 49

Journal Pre-proof

Changes in General Self-Efficacy and Mindfulness are Associated with


Short-Term Improvements in Mood during Art-Making in a Partial
Hospital Program

Marie Forgeard, Alexandra Silverman, Jennifer Buchholz, Courtney


Beard, Thröstur Björgvinsson

PII: S0197-4556(21)00044-7
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aip.2021.101799
Reference: AIP 101799

To appear in: The Arts in Psychotherapy

Received Date: 13 May 2019


Revised Date: 17 March 2021
Accepted Date: 21 March 2021

Please cite this article as: Forgeard M, Silverman A, Buchholz J, Beard C, Björgvinsson T,
Changes in General Self-Efficacy and Mindfulness are Associated with Short-Term
Improvements in Mood during Art-Making in a Partial Hospital Program, The Arts in
Psychotherapy (2021), doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aip.2021.101799

This is a PDF file of an article that has undergone enhancements after acceptance, such as
the addition of a cover page and metadata, and formatting for readability, but it is not yet the
definitive version of record. This version will undergo additional copyediting, typesetting and
review before it is published in its final form, but we are providing this version to give early
visibility of the article. Please note that, during the production process, errors may be
discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal
pertain.

© 2020 Published by Elsevier.


ART-MAKING AND MOOD 1

Changes in General Self-Efficacy and Mindfulness are Associated with Short-Term

Improvements in Mood during Art-Making in a Partial Hospital Program

Marie Forgeard1,2 Alexandra Silverman3, Jennifer Buchholz4, Courtney Beard1, Thröstur

Björgvinsson1

1
McLean Hospital and Harvard Medical School

of
2
William James College

ro
3
University of Virginia
4
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

-p
re
Author Note

Corresponding Author: Marie Forgeard, McLean Hospital/Harvard Medical School & William
lP

James College, 115 Mill Street, Belmont MA 02478, 617-855-4456,

mforgeard@mclean.harvard.edu. The authors do not have conflicts of interests to disclose. They


na

would like to thank Allen Bailey and Shannon Haley, as well as the staff and patients of the

Behavioral Health Partial Program at McLean Hospital.


ur

Highlights
 •Art-making may improve mood; more research is needed to examine what might explain
Jo

such benefits

 •Intensive treatment programs often offer art groups to patients

 •Increases in self-efficacy and mindfulness may relate to mood improvements during art-
making
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 2

Abstract

A growing body of scholarship suggests that art-making provides symptom relief as well as

short-term mood benefits. However, more research is needed to explore how artistic activities

may lead to mood improvements within the course of sessions conducted in clinical contexts.

To this end, we examined short-term outcomes of participation in an art group offered within a

brief partial hospital program in which intensive day treatment is delivered. In a preliminary

study (Study 1) conducted on Amazon MTurk (N = 193), we validated the use of a brief 7-item

of
self-assessment scale to capture potential outcomes of art-making identified based on a review of

ro
the literature: positive/negative mood, general self-efficacy, creative self-efficacy, activation,

mindfulness, and social connectedness. In our main study (Study 2), 175 patients in a partial

-p
hospital program completed the brief instrument validated in Study 1 at the beginning and end of
re
a 50-minute unstructured art group. All psychological outcomes improved over the course of the

group. Changes in general self-efficacy and mindfulness were associated with improvements in
lP

mood over and above changes in other outcomes. Results are limited by the naturalistic,

uncontrolled, design of the group, which was not led by art therapists. These findings suggest
na

that further studies examining patients’ experiences during art-making in clinical settings may

provide useful insights into how this activity enhances mood.


ur

Keywords: art; mood; self-efficacy; mindfulness


Jo
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 3

A growing body of scholarship suggests that art-making provides symptom relief as well

as short-term mood benefits. However, more research is needed to explore how artistic activities

may lead to mood improvements within the course of sessions conducted in clinical contexts. To

date, most of the research specifically examining what might account for mood improvements

during art-making has been conducted in experimental laboratory settings and more work is

therefore needed to understand what might account for mood improvements associated with art-

making in real-world clinical contexts (Gruber & Oepen, 2018). Relatedly, intensive

of
psychological treatment programs often offer patients the opportunity to engage in art groups

ro
alongside other interventions (Malchiodi, 2013), but little research has examined associated

short-term benefits beyond changes in mood. Many clinicians not trained in art therapy and its

-p
scientific foundation intuitively see art as a useful medium (Rosal, 2016). Nonetheless, others
re
may at first glance regard artistic activities as unrelated to the main goals of treatment. Further

research can therefore help clarify the nature and extent of the potential short-term benefits of
lP

art-making during psychological treatment. To address this gap in the literature, this project

examined short-term outcomes of an art group delivered within a partial hospital program
na

delivering intensive treatment for individuals experiencing acute psychiatric symptoms.

Treatment in a partial program takes place during the day (typically for 1-2 weeks), with
ur

individuals returning home in the evenings/weekends.

The Benefits of Art-Making


Jo

Clinicians and mental health institutions often offer opportunities for creative self-

expression during treatment, using an array of possible modalities (e.g., the visual arts, music,

drama, dance, or play) to provide individuals with opportunities to generate potentially original

and valuable ideas or products (Malchiodi, 2013; Stein, 1953; Sternberg & Lubart, 1995). A
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 4

growing body of evidence suggests that art-making may be beneficial for individuals presenting

with psychological difficulties (American Art Therapy Association, 2017). Most of the evidence

bearing on this question has however examined medium- to long-term outcomes of participation

in art-making for psychological symptoms, general health, and/or quality of life. Comparatively

less research has examined short-term changes in mood and other outcomes experienced within

one session, as further explained below. Reviews of controlled and uncontrolled studies in this

area conclude that art-making interventions (delivered as a primary intervention, or as an

of
adjunctive intervention to treatment-as-usual) overall appear to be useful for symptom reduction

ro
and psychological adjustment (Maujean, Pepping, & Kendall, 2014; Slayton, D’Archer, Kaplan,

2010) for a wide range of problems including depression (e.g., Gussak, 2006, 2007, 2009;

-p
Thyme et al., 2007), schizophrenia (e.g., Richardson, Jones, Evans, Stevens, & Rowe, 2007), and
re
trauma-related symptoms (e.g., Decker, Deaver, Abbey, Campbell, & Turpin, 2018; Schouten, de

Niet, Knipscheer, Kleber, & Hutschemaekers, 2015), among others. Despite these promising
lP

results, some studies have found either minimal or no effect of art-based interventions (primary

or adjunctive) on symptom outcomes (e.g., Crawford et al., 2012). Existing evidence speaking to
na

the benefits of art therapy for specific populations remains limited. For example, a systematic

review of both randomized and non-randomized controlled trials of art therapy for anxiety
ur

disorders only found three eligible studies; in spite of preliminary evidence of effectiveness,

results were hampered by the paucity of high quality controlled trials and risk of bias in existing
Jo

research (Abbing et al., 2018).

To complement research assessing medium- to long-term outcomes of participation in

art-based interventions, additional work is needed to investigate potential short-term benefits for

participants (Gruber & Oepen, 2018). Examining participants’ immediate experiences could help
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 5

maximize the effectiveness of artistic interventions by clarifying the conditions under which art-

making improves mood in clinical settings. In other words, such research can help shed light on

not just whether but also how these experiences might serve the goals of treatment (Forgeard &

Elstein, 2014).

Experimental research conducted with nonclinical samples has started to address this

question by examining participants’ changes in mood resulting from art-making. More

specifically, much of this research has assessed whether and how engaging in artistic activities

of
(mostly drawing) can help repair mood following an experimental negative mood induction. For

ro
example, De Petrillo and Winner (2005) found that participants who were asked to draw a

picture based on their feelings exhibited improved mood after drawing, whereas those who were

-p
asked to copy shapes or to complete a word puzzle did not. In addition, participants’ mood
re
improvements did not differ based on whether they expressed negative emotions or redirected

their attention toward positive content in their pieces (as observed by examining the contents of
lP

the drawings produced). These results suggest that creating a personally relevant product (not

just the physical act of drawing) is important for mood improvement to occur. Additional studies
na

have shown that drawing may be particularly effective for mood repair when it is used to

implement a specific emotion regulation strategy - redirecting one’s attention toward positive
ur

content. Dalebroux, Goldstein, and Winner (2008) demonstrated that drawing to elicit positive

mood (e.g., drawing something happy) led to larger improvements in mood than drawing to vent
Jo

(i.e., drawing to express negative mood), or engaging in a control activity (scanning a sheet for

specific symbols). Similarly, Drake, Coleman, and Winner (2011) found that drawing was a

more effective means of immediate mood repair than writing, and that both activities repaired

mood more effectively through distraction than through venting negative mood (see also
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 6

Diliberto-Macaluso & Stubblefield, 2015; Drake, Hastedt, & James, 2016; Drake & Winner,

2012, 2013; Kimport & Robbins, 2012; Smolarski, Leone, & Robbins, 2015).

Despite the growing body of work documenting mood benefits of art-making in

experimental settings, little empirical evidence speaks to predictors of such benefits in real-world

clinical settings in which art is offered as a complement to other treatment modalities. Related to

this, little research speaks to whether the mood benefits of art-making are best explained by

general processes common to most therapeutic activities or by processes specific to creative

of
activities such as art (Forgeard & Elstein, 2014). Understanding how art-making influences a

ro
range of psychological experiences during psychological treatment has the potential to (a) more

accurately and comprehensively capture the benefits of art-making, (b) help identify the types of

-p
psychological changes that are most closely related to mood improvements during art-making,
re
and (c) enhance clinicians’ ability to target goals other than mood repair through art-making,

thus potentially enhancing engagement with, and the effectiveness of, the specific type of
lP

treatment(s) received.

To that end, the present study examined associations between changes in


na

positive/negative mood and changes in five psychological variables hypothesized to be

associated with mood improvements based on a review of the literature (general self-efficacy,
ur

creative self-efficacy, activation, mindfulness, and social connectedness) during an art group

occurring in a partial hospitalization program for acute psychopathology. More specifically, this
Jo

naturalistic setting allowed us to examine how patients might benefit from an art group delivered

as part of an intensive treatment program delivering Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy (CBT; Beck,

Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979) and Dialectical Behavior Therapy (DBT; Linehan, 1993) skills

training, two modalities known to be effective in acute settings (e.g., Webb, Beard, Kertz, Hsu,
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 7

& Björgvinsson, 2016). Although other variables besides the five reviewed below may be

relevant to understand changes in mood associated with art-making, we selected these in order to

(1) construct and validate a very brief measure that is feasible and acceptable to use in a clinical

setting, and (2) select variables most relevant to the type of treatment participants were receiving,

and that they might therefore find useful to assess and reflect on during an art group.

General Self-Efficacy

Self-efficacy, also often called “mastery,” is defined as the degree to which individuals

of
perceive that they can control events in their lives, and take action to cope with challenges or to

ro
attain valued goals (Bandura, 1997; Pearlin & Schooler, 1978). Self-efficacy is associated with

lower levels of depression and anxiety, as well as higher levels of well-being (Bandura,

-p
Pastorelli, Barbaranelli, & Caprara, 1999; Chorpita, Brown, & Barlow, 2016; Zalta &
re
Chambless, 2011). Helping individuals recognize which activities provide them with feelings of

self-efficacy and mastery is an important strategy in cognitive-behavioral therapy. In particular,


lP

the partial program in the present study emphasized practicing behavioral activation techniques

to address symptoms. Behavioral activation refers to a specific set of strategies used to increase
na

activity levels and structure; engaging in activities even when motivation or mood is low can

help break the vicious cycle of depressive/anxious avoidance, maintain functioning, and facilitate
ur

learning which behaviors produce positive reinforcement (Dimidjian et al., 2006; Jacobson,

Martell, & Dimidjian, 2001). When practicing behavioral activation, individuals are explicitly
Jo

encouraged to monitor both mood and feelings of self-efficacy (or mastery) before and after

activities. In keeping with this skill, artistic activities may provide a fertile ground for developing

general self-efficacy (Kaimal & Ray, 2017) by enabling individuals to select activities at
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 8

appropriate levels of challenge in relation to their skills (Csikszentmihalyi, 1996); this may in

turn lead to feelings of competence and accomplishment (Fisher & Specht, 1999).

Creative Self-Efficacy

We also examined a more specific form of self-efficacy – creative self-efficacy. Creative

self-efficacy is defined as the subjective belief that one is able to come up with novel and useful

(i.e., creative) ideas, products, or behaviors (Beghetto, 2006; Tierney & Farmer, 2002), and has

been linked to psychological adjustment and well-being (Forgeard & Benson, 2019; Karwowski

of
& Lebuda, 2016). To date, and perhaps surprisingly, studies examining the benefits of art-

ro
making have rarely empirically examined whether creativity-related variables account for art-

making’s effects on mood. With the exception of the nonclinical experimental studies cited

-p
above that compared the effects of drawing novel material vs. copying images, past studies have
re
typically not tested whether the act of generating a novel and valuable product (Stein, 1953;

Sternberg & Lubart, 1995) could account for the benefits of art-making. Examining changes in
lP

creative self-efficacy in addition to general self-efficacy can help researchers understand whether

the benefits of art-making are specific to creative activities, or whether they may be shared by
na

other non-creative activities.

Activation
ur

Perhaps more simply, engaging in art-making may provide participants with beneficial

stimulation, leading them to feel more activated, alert, and energized. Again, this outcome could
Jo

reflect participants’ use of behavioral activation strategies during a group. Although activation is

often conceptualized as a component of mood, we decided to include this construct separately

from positive/negative mood in order to isolate changes in participants’ arousal and energy levels

during the art group.


ART-MAKING AND MOOD 9

Mindfulness

Art may also help participants feel more mindful, by focusing on a specific activity and

set of stimuli during a defined time period. Mindfulness is a key component of several evidence-

based treatments for psychological disorders including Dialectical Behavior Therapy (DBT;

Linehan, 1993), Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (Hayes & Lillis, 2012), and Mindfulness-

Based Cognitive Therapy (Segal, Williams, & Teasdale, 2012), among others. This skill is

defined as “paying attention in a particular way: on purpose, in the present moment, and

of
nonjudgmentally” (Kabat-Zinn, 1994, p. 4). The benefits of mindfulness may stem from

ro
individuals’ enhanced ability to intentionally and flexibly allocate their attention, by increasing

awareness and understanding of, as well as the ability to cope with, external circumstances and

-p
internal states (Chambers, Gullone, & Allen, 2009). Several authors have described how art-
re
making can be used within the framework of DBT to practice mindfulness and help with emotion

regulation (for reviews see Clark, 2016; Rosal, 2016). Focusing on a concrete activity, as is done
lP

in art groups, may provide a fertile space for individuals to practice mindful awareness of the

present moment (Monti et al., 2006).


na

Social Connectedness

Art groups may also help participants develop a sense of social connection based on a
ur

shared experience. Fostering the development of meaningful, rewarding, and supportive

relationships may constitute an important positive factor in treatment (regardless of orientation),


Jo

whether between patients and treatment providers, or with each other (e.g., Martin, Garske, &

Davis, 2000; Marziali, Munroe-Blum, & McCleary, 1997). In a review and case study of the

usefulness of art therapy for older adults, Johnson and Sullivan-Marx (2006) suggested that

creating art in small group settings may help foster social connectedness by decreasing isolation,
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 10

participating in an exchange, and creating a safe place for offering/receiving support. In addition,

engaging in altruistic behavior during groups (e.g., helping others with their artistic projects,

providing support and validation for others, etc.) can help connect individuals with their

strengths. For this reason, some clinicians use approaches specifically designed to foster social

connectedness, for example by asking participants to explore experiences they have in common

(Levine-Madori, 2013).

The Present Studies

of
This project investigated short-term outcomes of an art group offered within a naturalistic

ro
partial hospital program delivering intensive CBT/DBT skills training for acute

psychopathology. These outcomes were specifically selected because they may help explain

-p
mood improvements experienced by participants engaged in art-making. More specifically, we
re
examined relationships between changes in positive/negative mood and changes in five other

outcomes hypothesized to be associated with mood improvements (general self-efficacy, creative


lP

self-efficacy, activation, mindfulness, and social connectedness). Because we needed a very brief

instrument to measure these constructs in a clinical setting to minimize the burden placed on
na

participants and facilitate the completion of the questionnaire within the regular course of a brief

art group session, we conducted a preliminary study (Study 1) to validate a scale for this purpose.
ur

In the main study (Study 2), which was conducted in a partial hospital program, we expected

participants attending a 50-minute art group to report significant increases in all outcomes except
Jo

negative mood (for which we expected participants to report significant decreases). We also

hypothesized that changes in general self-efficacy, creative self-efficacy, activation, mindfulness,

and social connectedness would all be independently associated with changes in mood.

Study 1
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 11

We conducted a preliminary study to develop and validate the brief Art Group

Questionnaire used in Study 2. We needed a very brief instrument that would be feasible to use

without disrupting the normal sequence of the 50-minute art group, and that participants might

find acceptable/useful as a self-assessment exercise. Thus, instead of using existing measures

which were too long for use during the art group given the number of variables were interested

in, we designed and validated a new brief instrument. Because it was therefore also not feasible

to validate this instrument within a sample of patients at the partial hospital program (which

of
would have been ideal), we used Amazon MTurk to collect a large enough sample of participants

ro
who would be able to complete a longer survey. Amazon MTurk is an online crowdsourcing

marketplace through which individuals (or businesses) can recruit others to perform tasks

-p
virtually in exchange for monetary compensation. This platform is increasingly being used by
re
researchers to find participants and enroll them in online studies or experiments; participants

recruited through MTurk tend to be diverse (for reviews see Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling,
lP

2011; Paolacci & Chandler, 2014), though they may present with elevated mood and anxiety

symptoms in comparison to the general population (Arditte, Çek, Shaw, & Timpano, 2016;
na

Shapiro, Chandler, & Mueller, 2013). This was not a limitation for the present study given that

we aimed to use the instrument developed in a clinical population.


ur

Method

Participants. We recruited 193 participants through Amazon MTurk (Mage = 34.60 years,
Jo

SDage = 9.93, 38.3% women, 75.1% White and non-Latinx). They were compensated $0.75 for

completing a 5 to 10-minute survey. All participants provided informed consent to participate in

this study and procedures were reviewed and approved by our local Institutional Review Board.
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 12

Materials and Procedures. Participants completed all measures through the survey

administration platform REDCap (Harris et al., 2009). These included the brief Art Group

Questionnaire to be validated for use in Study 2, a disaggregated version of the same

questionnaire, and previously validated measures to assess convergent/divergent validity. In

addition, participants completed two attention check items (e.g., “please select ‘extremely’ for

this question”). We excluded 14 participants who failed one (n = 7) or both (n = 3) attention

checks or had missing data (n = 4) on these variables, resulting in a final sample size of 179

of
participants for analyses. All measures had adequate levels of internal consistency in the present

ro
study (all αs > .70, as described below). For all instruments presented below, we invited

participants to report on their experiences “right now, in the present moment” (modifying

original instructions if needed).


-p
re
Art Group Questionnaire. As explained above, participants completed a very brief 7-

item questionnaire designed for this project in order to assess different states in the present
lP

moment (see Appendix). Participants indicated how they were feeling “right now” on a 5-point

Likert-type scale (from “not at all,” to “extremely”). Each item assessed one construct of interest
na

using three adjectives: general self-efficacy (“competent, effective, capable”), creative self-

efficacy (“creative, imaginative, innovative”), activation (“energized, invigorated, stimulated”),


ur

mindfulness (“mindful, present, attentive”), social connectedness (“supported, connected, cared

for”), positive mood (“pleased, content, happy”), and negative mood (“frustrated, anxious, sad”).
Jo

The order of items was determined using a random number generator. The content of items was

designed by examining existing measures of similar constructs (e.g., Baer, Smith, Hopkins,

Krietemeyer, & Toney, 2006; Beghetto, Kaufman, & Baxter, 2011; Carver & White, 1994;

Diener et al., 2010; Schwarzer & Jerusalem, 1995; Zimet, Dahlem, Zimet, & Farley, 1988) and
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 13

through piloting with research team members to ensure feasibility and clarity of the terms used.

Single-item measures, though usually inferior to multi-item instruments, can be acceptable if the

constructs measured are narrow enough (Wanous, Reichers, & Hudy, 1997) and when their use

enables the feasibility of measurement in clinical settings (Zimmerman et al., 2006), which was

the ultimate goal of this validation effort.

Disaggregated version of the Art Group Questionnaire. To validate the use of items

combining three adjectives in the brief 7-item scale described above, we also asked participants

of
in this preliminary study to complete a 21-item version of the same scale in which adjectives

ro
were presented one by one (instead of in groups of three).

Positive and negative affect. Participants completed the International Positive and

-p
Negative Affect Schedule – Short Form (I-PANAS-SF; Thompson, 2007), a 10-item
re
questionnaire assessing broad positive (α = .80) and negative affect (α = .93) in the present

moment using a 5-point rating scale.


lP

General and creative self-efficacy. Participants completed two brief 6-item scales

measuring state general (α = .94) and creative self-efficacy (α = .92) using a 5-point rating scale
na

(Forgeard & Benson, 2019). These two scales assessed the degree to which participants currently

felt (a) generally able to accomplish their goals and persevere through obstacles, and (b) able to
ur

use their imagination and come up with novel and useful ideas.

Activation. Participants completed the Vigor subscale of the Abbreviated Profile of Mood
Jo

States questionnaire (A-POMS; Grove & Prapavessis, 1992), a 5-item subscale assessing current

levels of vigor (α = .93) using a 5-point rating scale.

Mindfulness. Participants completed the Toronto Mindfulness Scale (Lau et al., 2006), a

13-item measure using a 5-point rating scale which yields two subscale scores corresponding to
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 14

Curiosity about the present moment (α = .92) and Decentering, defined as disidentification with

one’s immediate thoughts and feelings (α = .83).

Social connectedness. Participants completed the Social Connectedness Scale – Revised

(SCS-R; Lee, Draper, & Lee, 2001), a 20-item scale assessing perceived social connection (α =

.96) using a 6-point rating scale.

Data Analytic Strategy. First, we used Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) conducted

in Mplus 7 (Muthén & Muthén, 2012) to verify that the three adjectives chosen to measure each

of
construct indeed captured the same latent variable using responses from the 21-item

ro
disaggregated version of the Art Group Questionnaire (as these were combined into a single item

for the briefer 7-item version used in Study 2). We followed recommendations to assess the fit of

-p
the model using multiple indices given that sample size has an important effect on the
re
significance of chi-square tests (Hu & Bentler, 1999; Kline, 2005): we examined absolute values

of the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA = or < .08 indicates adequate fit),
lP

Comparative Fit Index (CFI = or > .90 indicates adequate fit) and Standardized Root Mean

Squared Residual (SRMR = < .08 indicates adequate fit). We then examined whether items
na

loaded significantly on their respective constructs.

Second, we examined bivariate correlations between each single-item measure from the
ur

7-item Art Group Questionnaire and its corresponding convergent validity measures, expecting

rs to reflect moderate-to-strong associations. We used the negative affect subscale of the


Jo

PANAS-SF as a measure of divergent validity (except for the item assessing negative affect, for

which we used the positive affect subscale of the PANAS-SF as a measure of divergent validity),

expecting rs to reflect weak associations. Finally, we compared rs using Fisher’s r-to-z


ART-MAKING AND MOOD 15

transformation, expecting correlations with convergent validity measures to be significantly

stronger than correlations with divergent validity measures.

Results

Confirmatory factor analysis. We used CFA to determine whether the three adjectives

comprised within each item of the Art Group Questionnaire reflected the same latent variable.

The fit of the model was adequate, χ2 (168) = 342.24, p < .001, RMSEA = .08 (90% CI = .07-

.09), CFI = .93, SRMR = .06. All items loaded significantly on their hypothesized latent

of
variables (see Figure 1). Thus, the three adjectives chosen to measure each construct grouped

ro
together as expected. All latent variables correlated significantly with each other (ps < .05) with

the exception of social connectedness and negative affect, as well as activation and negative

affect (ps > .05).


-p
re
Convergent and divergent validity. As expected, we found moderate-to-strong

associations (rs = .47 - .77) between each item and its convergent validity measure, with one
lP

exception: the mindfulness item only correlated weakly with the Toronto Mindfulness Scale

Curiosity (r = .28) and Decentering (r = .27) subscales. We also found weak correlations
na

between each item and its divergent validity measure (rs = -.12-.25). Fisher’s r-to-z

transformations confirmed that each item’s correlation with its convergent validity measure was
ur

significantly stronger than with its divergent validity measure (all ps < .001). Table 1 provides all

results.
Jo

Discussion

Results of Study 1 indicated that the brief 7-item Art Group Questionnaire designed for

use in clinical settings demonstrated coherent measurement of several relevant psychological

variables (as assessed using CFA) and adequate convergent/divergent validity. However,
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 16

correlations between the mindfulness item and our convergent validity measure (two subscales of

the Toronto Mindfulness Scale) were weaker than expected. It is likely that there was a mismatch

between the brief item designed to capture temporary states of mindfulness (“mindful, present,

attentive”) and this convergent validity measure, which focused on beliefs/attitudes (e.g., “I

experience my thoughts more as events in my mind than as a necessarily accurate reflection of

the way things ‘really’ are”). Despite the instructions provided, it may have been difficult for

participants to rate these items in the present moment. Given that the three adjectives chosen did

of
group together in our CFA and the convergent correlations were significantly stronger than the

ro
divergent measure correlation, we kept this item in our final scale but interpreted any relevant

results with caution in Study 2.

Study 2
-p
re
In Study 2, we used the brief Art Group Questionnaire validated in Study 1 to examine

psychological experiences of individuals attending a brief 50-minute art group as part of a partial
lP

hospital program focusing on CBT/DBT skills training, and to determine whether changes in

mood experienced during the art group were associated with changes in other psychological
na

outcomes captured by this questionnaire.

Method
ur

Participants. Participants included 175 patients attending a partial hospitalization

program at a private psychiatric hospital in the United States between September of 2015 and
Jo

April of 2016. All participants consented for their data to be used for research purposes and

attended at least one art group during treatment at the partial hospital program. Participants were

predominantly in young to middle adulthood (M = 33.68 years old, SD = 13.70), women (54.9%,

men = 42.3%, non-binary= 1.7%, 1.1% missing) and non-Latinx White (86.9%, 13.1% other
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 17

race/ethnicity, 5% multiracial). Approximately half of participants (49.1%) had less than a 4-year

college degree, were unemployed (53.7%), and had been hospitalized for psychiatric reasons in

the past 6 months (53.7%). This sample was high in comorbidity, with 58.9% of participants

currently meeting criteria for two or more psychological disorders and 33.3% for three or more

psychological disorders (as determined by a structured clinical interview). The most common

diagnosis was a major depressive disorder (58.2%) (see Table 2 for more information about

training of interviewers and all diagnostic characteristics of this sample).

of
Treatment at this brief intensive partial hospitalization program focuses on evidence-

ro
based treatment to manage acute symptoms. All treatment takes place during the day, with

patients returning home on evenings/weekends. As explained above, the majority of treatment

-p
focuses on the acquisition and practice of skills drawn from CBT (e.g., behavioral activation,
re
cognitive restructuring) and DBT (e.g., mindfulness, distress tolerance, emotion regulation,

interpersonal effectiveness). The program serves individuals with a wide range of psychological
lP

disorders (principally mood, anxiety, personality, and psychotic disorders). The average length

of stay in the program for this sample was 12.57 days (including non-treatment weekend days).
na

Treatment consists of pharmacotherapy, case management, and group and individual therapy

(provided by psychiatrists, social workers, psychologists, and psychology trainees, as well as


ur

nurses and mental health counselors). Patients attend a variety of 50-minute therapy groups

during the day, including the art group examined in this study (as described below) (for more
Jo

information about this program including its effectiveness see Forgeard, Beard, Kirakosian, &

Björgvinsson, 2018).

Art Group. All participants took part in at least one art group during their stay in the

partial program. These groups were offered four days per week and were led by either a
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 18

postdoctoral fellow in clinical psychology, or one of three bachelor’s level community residence

counselors supervised by the postdoctoral fellow. The treatment program in which this study

took place did not have an art therapist on staff. The art group in this treatment program was

originally developed by an occupational therapist with specific training in art therapy, who

trained group leaders prior to leaving her position (and prior to the start of data collection for this

study). This staff member had completed a semester-long course in art therapy with an art

therapist as part of her occupational therapy studies. She had also worked alongside an art

of
therapist in several psychiatric facilities following completion of her degree. Nonetheless, the art

ro
groups offered in this program were not led by art therapists and therefore did not constitute art

therapy. Instead, the primary focus of art groups in this partial hospital program was to build

-p
coping skills and leisure interests through art-making, allowing group leaders to assess how
re
patients function in their everyday life and approach new tasks.

In this partial hospital program, case managers collaborate with patients to determine
lP

which groups they will attend (including the art group). Because 65 out of 175 participants

attended the group more than once, our main analyses only included each person’s first group
na

participation (responses from participants who attended the group twice were used for the

purpose of test-retest reliability analyses, see below).


ur

At the beginning of each art group session, the group leader introduced the brief Art

Group Questionnaire validated in Study 1 as a self-assessment exercise, asking participants to


Jo

take a few minutes to think about how they were currently feeling. Participants then worked on

an artistic project of their choice during the next 45-minute period. Group leaders encouraged

participants to explore materials available in the art studio: 34.3% engaged in coloring, 20.6% in

drawing, 15.4% in painting, 3.4% in collage, 7.4% in crafts, 5.7% in origami/paper-folding,


ART-MAKING AND MOOD 19

9.1% in jewelry-making, 2.3% in writing, and 8.6% in other activities (e.g., clay/sculpting,

crochet, etc.)1. Group leaders did not structure the session around specific instructions, with one

exception: one session per week (out of four) also included a facilitated discussion in which

participants were asked to specifically describe a transition they were currently experiencing,

thus adding a “processing” component. In the other three sessions, participants were free to

informally converse with peers on topics of their choice. Because we wanted to examine

participants’ experiences in art groups as they ran in naturalistic settings, we did not modify the

of
group protocols in place, but still took care to examine in our analyses whether outcomes

ro
differed by group type (processing vs. no processing).

After working on a project of their choice for 45 minutes, participants were asked to

-p
repeat the self-assessment exercise they completed at the beginning of the session by filling out
re
the 7-item Art Group questionnaire again. They also indicated which type of activity they chose

(using the list of activities provided above) and provided optional open-ended comments
lP

regarding their experience in the group. The session concluded with a 5-minute discussion about

what participants learned by doing self-assessment (i.e., filling out the Art Group Questionnaire
na

at the beginning/end of the group).

Materials and Procedures. All participants provided consent for their data to be used in
ur

research and the local Institutional Review Board approved all study procedures.

Art Group Questionnaire. Participants completed the 7-item Art Group Questionnaire
Jo

validated in Study 1 at the beginning and end of the art group. As explained above, the brevity of

the questionnaire was necessary to help patients monitor and reflect on what they experienced

during the art group without creating undue burden or limitations on the way the group would

1 Some participants engaged in more than one activity.


ART-MAKING AND MOOD 20

typically run. To further validate this brief instrument, we also used examined test-retest

reliability in the subset of participants who attended the art group twice (n = 65), as this measure

can help establish the appropriateness of single-item measures (Youngblut & Casper, 1993;

Zimmerman et al., 2006). Correlations between the first and second administrations were

moderate and significant (positive mood r = .49, negative mood r = .50, general self-efficacy r =

.49, creative self-efficacy r = .52, activation r = .36, mindfulness r = .36, social connectedness r

= .58, all ps < .005), as expected given that items assessed how participants were feeling “right

of
now” (Crocker & Algina, 2006).

ro
Depressive symptoms at admission. Participants completed the Patient Health

Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9; Kroenke & Spitzer, 2002) upon admission to the partial program as

-p
part of routine clinical care. This measure was administered using REDCap (Research Electronic
re
Data Capture), a secure, web-based application designed to support data collection for research

studies (Harris et al., 2009). The PHQ-9 is a validated 9-item self-report measure assessing the
lP

frequency of symptoms of depression (e.g., “feeling tired or having little energy”) over the past

two weeks on a 4-point scale. We used this measure to determine whether patients who attended
na

the art group during their stay in the program differed in depression symptom severity from those

who did not.


ur

Data Analytic Strategy.

Attendance to art group. We conducted preliminary one-way ANOVAs and chi-square


Jo

tests to assess whether participants who took part in art groups differed from patients who did not

based on demographic characteristics (age, gender, race/ethnicity, education level) and baseline

depressive symptoms.
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 21

Overall outcomes during group. We conducted a repeated-measures MANOVA to

assess whether participants experienced significant changes in all outcomes measured by the Art

Group Questionnaire during the art group.

Predictors of outcomes. To prepare for building a path model (see below), we ran

correlations and one-way ANOVAs to determine whether changes in all outcomes during the art

group were associated with demographic variables (age, education, depressive symptoms at

admission), group features (group leader, group size, group type: processing vs. no processing),

of
and artistic medium/activity selected by participants.

ro
Relationships between mood and other outcomes. Finally, we examined relationships

between changes in positive/negative mood and changes in all other outcomes simultaneously

-p
(general self-efficacy, creative self-efficacy, activation, mindfulness, social connectedness) using
re
path analysis. The model controlled for variables associated with changes in outcomes in

preliminary analyses, as well as all outcomes at the beginning of the art group. Path analysis was
lP

implemented in Mplus 7 using full-information maximum likelihood (FIML) estimation (Muthén

& Muthén, 2012). We tested saturated models in order to assess relationships between all
na

variables; thus, fit indices are not available. We opted for path analysis rather than multiple

regression analyses in order to take advantage of FIML to adjust results for missing data, and to
ur

be able to simultaneously assess and examine how changes in outcomes related to both changes

in positive and negative mood.


Jo

Results

Attendance to art group. A total of 391 participants received treatment at the partial

program during the period of time this study was conducted and consented for their data to be
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 22

used in research. Individuals who attended at least one art group (n = 202)2 did not differ from

non-attenders (n = 189) on gender3, ethnicity (non-Latinx White vs. others), age, education level,

or depressive symptoms at admission (all ps > .05). Attenders had longer lengths of stay in the

program (M = 12.66 days, SD = 3.63) compared to non-attenders (M = 10.71 days, SD = 5.57), F

(1, 387) = 16.90, p < .001, Cohen’s d = .41.

Overall outcomes during group. A repeated-measures MANOVA (including all

outcomes as dependent variables) revealed a significant overall effect of time, Wilks’ Lambda =

of
.44, F (7, 151) = 27.66, p < .001, η2 = .56. Follow-up ANOVAs indicated significant changes in

ro
the expected direction for all outcomes (all ps < .05, Cohen’s ds = .15-.68, see Table 3).

Participants therefore reported experiencing decreases in negative affect, but increases in positive

-p
affect, general self-efficacy, creative self-efficacy, activation, mindfulness, and social
re
connectedness over the course of the art group.

Predictors of outcomes. Age, depressive symptoms at admission, and group size were
lP

not related to any of the outcomes (all ps > .05). Education was negatively related to changes in

positive mood (partial r = -.17, p = .03), but not to any other outcomes (all ps > .05). Women (M
na

= .64, SD = .88) reported larger increases in general self-efficacy than men (M = .15, SD = .84),

F (1, 157) = 4.70, p = .03, Cohen’s d = .57. Outcomes did not vary according to race/ethnicity,
ur

group type (processing vs. no processing), and group leader. Change in positive affect was the

only outcome to vary by medium/activity selected by participants, F(7, 128) = 2.18, p = .04;
Jo

2
Attenders (n = 202) included patients who attended any art groups, even if they did not complete the main
questionnaire for this study and were not included in the sample described above or used in our statistical analyses
(n = 175).
3
Because of the low number of individuals identifying as a gender other than man or woman, statistical analyses do
not include them.
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 23

however, Bonferroni-corrected posthoc tests did not detect any reliable differences (all ps > .05).

Based on these results, education and gender were covaried in path analyses.

Relationship between mood and other outcomes. We ran a path-analytic model to

assess differential relationships between changes in positive/negative mood and changes in other

outcomes during the art group. We covaried education, gender, and all outcomes measured at the

beginning of the group by entering them as additional predictors in the model. Table 4 presents

the raw correlation matrix among all variables in the model. These correlations indicated that

of
changes in positive mood were significantly associated with changes in all other outcomes

ro
during the art group (all ps < . 001), and changes in negative mood were significant associated

with changes in all outcomes (ps < .05) except activation and social connectedness (both ps >

-p
.05). Figure 2 and Table 4 present results for the path model. Examination of standardized path
re
coefficients indicated that changes in general self-efficacy (β = .22, p = .046) and changes in

mindfulness (β = .30, p = .001) were significantly associated with changes in positive mood
lP

when controlling for other predictors. Changes in general self-efficacy (β = -.23, p = .03) were

associated with changes in negative mood when controlling for other predictors. All other paths
na

were not significant (all ps > .05). The covariance path between changes in positive and negative

mood was significant (β = -.15, p = .02).


ur

Discussion

Results of our main study showed that participants enrolled in a partial hospitalization
Jo

program reported experiencing improvements in mood as well as in general self-efficacy,

creative self-efficacy, activation, mindfulness, and social connectedness while engaging in a 50-

minute art group. Changes in positive and negative mood during art groups were differentially

associated with changes in other outcomes. Specifically, increases in general self-efficacy and
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 24

mindfulness related to increased positive mood, and increases in general self-efficacy related to

decreased negative mood. Changes in other psychological variables (creative self-efficacy, social

connectedness, and activation) did not relate to changes in mood when all variables were

simultaneously entered as predictors in the model.

General Discussion

Results of the present study provide new insights into experiences of patients attending

art groups offered as part of intensive psychiatric partial hospitalization. These results contribute

of
to the literature examining the benefits of art-making in several ways. They highlight that the

ro
mood benefits of such activities could be predicted by other variables (Gruber & Oepen, 2018),

including variables not specifically contingent on the creative and/or artistic process. Indeed, to

-p
date, past research has not yet demonstrated that creativity-related variables account for the
re
benefits of creative activities such as art (Forgeard & Elstein, 2014).

Increases in general self-efficacy following the art group predicted improvement in both
lP

positive and negative mood. More research is needed to assess whether/how art groups foster

general self-efficacy defined as the belief that one is competent and able to execute and
na

accomplish personal goals (see also Kaimal & Ray, 2017). In contrast, creative self-efficacy was

not associated with changes in mood when both types of self-efficacy were examined
ur

simultaneously. Similarly, in a recent study, general self-efficacy derived from extracurricular

activities for youths was also more closely related to psychological adjustment than creative self-
Jo

efficacy (Forgeard & Benson, 2019). Our findings are consistent with the literature documenting

associations between self-efficacy and psychological adjustment across pathologies (Maddux,

2009). Providing brief and concrete activities for individuals experiencing acute psychological
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 25

difficulties (such as creating a piece of art during a 50-minute period) may set the stage for

increasing beliefs in one’s abilities to take action that lead to satisfying outcomes.

This study also showed that increases in mindfulness related to increases in positive

mood over the course of the art group. Art-making may be particularly useful in this regard by

encouraging patients to focus their attention on an experiential process, moving past verbal

description and intellectual comprehension to facilitate direct personal experience (Clark, 2016).

Of note, participants attended other therapy groups in our program, including groups where DBT

of
skills (such as mindfulness) were discussed and practiced. It is therefore possible that

ro
participants took advantage of art groups to apply the mindfulness skills they had previously

learned, though more research would be needed to substantiate this hypothesis. Findings

-p
pertaining to mindfulness are limited by the fact this item showed limited convergent validity
re
with another measure assessing mindful attitudes and beliefs in Study 1 (the Toronto

Mindfulness Scale; Lau et al., 2006), though Confirmatory Factor Analysis confirmed that the
lP

three adjectives used (“mindful, present, attentive”) adequately captured the same construct. The

state captured by the item used in the Art Group Questionnaire might therefore differ from
na

mindfulness conceptualized more broadly, and most likely simply referred to whether

participants felt attentive and present in the current moment they completed the questionnaire
ur

(regardless of whether they more generally practiced principles of mindfulness in their lives).

This study was conducted in a naturalistic setting, which enhances the external validity
Jo

of findings by testing questions among patients experiencing clinically significant and

transdiagnostic challenges. This consideration is important given that previous research

investigating the short-term benefits of art-making for mood has typically been conducted in

nonclinical settings (Gruber & Oepen, 2018). This naturalistic setting however also presents
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 26

important limitations. First and foremost, this study did not include a control group to assess

short-term changes in mood and other outcomes during another intervention. Thus, this study

could not determine whether the benefits experienced by participants were unique to the art

group; rather, it sought to investigate which psychological variables most closely related to mood

improvements during art-making. Changes in mood and other outcomes during art groups may

not differ from changes occurring in other groups. In fact, our results suggest that it is likely that

similar experiences are involved (since the most specific outcome to art-making activities,

of
creative self-efficacy, was not more highly correlated with mood than other outcomes). Our

ro
uncontrolled pre/post design also did not allow us to examine whether the outcomes assessed

here may also act as processes explaining the benefits of the group. Future research using

-p
controlled and mediational designs, which would require measurement at more time points
re
during the group, is needed to ascertain the degree to which changes in specific variables actually

cause changes in positive/negative mood (Kazdin, 2007) and may act as mechanisms of change.
lP

It is possible changes in mood drove changes in other outcomes, or that third variables (e.g.,

constructs not measured in this study) accounted for the present results. For example, it may be
na

interesting to further study the degree to which variables related to the therapeutic relationship

(e.g., alliance with clinician, quality of interactions with other group members) could explain the
ur

benefits of art-making in clinical settings.

Second and related, approximately half of patients attending the partial program did not
Jo

attend art groups during their stay, raising the possibility of a selection bias. Preliminary analyses

however showed that the only distinguishing feature of individuals attending these groups was

that they tended to, on average, attend the program for a longer period of time. Individuals who
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 27

attended art groups did not differ from others on a range of demographic variables (age, sex,

race/ethnicity, education levels) or on depressive symptoms at admission.

Third, the art groups in this study were not led by art therapists (as these were not on staff

at this program) but by other treatment team members who also delivered CBT and DBT skills

groups. Although this is a common practice in clinical settings and makes our results relevant to

other similar contexts/interventions, this also means that results can only speak to the use of

unstructured art-making during treatment, and not to the use of other art-based interventions

of
including art therapy more specifically. Further research should assess whether outcomes might

ro
differ based on the content of the session, including whether group leaders implemented a

specific art-making intervention. It might be especially informative to study short-term outcomes

-p
of CBT- and DBT-informed art therapy which directly and explicitly integrate relevant cognitive
re
and behavioral principles within sessions (e.g., Clark, 2016; Heckwolf, Bergland, & Mouratidis,

2014; Rosal, 2016). Indeed, artistic activities may be well-suited to help clinicians flexibly
lP

implement principles of CBT and DBT by tailoring interventions to the needs and preferences of

each individual (e.g., Kendall & Beidas, 2007; Kendall, Chu, Gifford, Hayes, & Nauta, 1999). A
na

therapist may, for example, encourage a patient to engage in artistic activities to increase energy

levels and encourage value-driven behavior, to practice mindfulness by focusing on the task at
ur

hand, or to gradually confront anxiety-provoking memories by using art as a vehicle for

expression (among other possible uses).


Jo

Fourth, this study used a very brief measure (the Art Group Questionnaire) to assess

constructs of interest; the brevity of the measure was needed for the study to be feasible in a

clinical context, and to balance the burden/benefit ratio in order to maximize clinical relevance

and usability for patients (see also Zimmerman et al., 2006). This method allowed us to directly
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 28

compare outcomes to one another and isolate relationships between each outcome and

positive/negative mood. Although we took care to validate this measure in a preliminary study

and to examine its test-retest reliability in our main study, future research should ideally use

multi-item scales whenever feasible.

Despite these limitations, the present studies extend the scientific understanding of the

benefits of art-making during psychological treatment as delivered in real-world clinical settings.

In particular, Study 2 suggested that patients perceived a number of adaptive consequences of art

of
group participation beyond mood improvements. The roles of general self-efficacy and

ro
mindfulness should be further studied given the links between these outcomes and mood benefits

during art-making. Though we could not assess this directly in the present study, it possible that

-p
participants used this activity to implement skills learned during treatment without being
re
specifically prompted to do so (though the self-assessment questionnaire likely acted as a

reminder). Future research could assess the usefulness of art groups explicitly designed to
lP

practice specific skills, as well as potential moderators of the usefulness of art in treatment (e.g.,

patient preferences and motivation, personality traits, previous experience with treatment, etc.).
na

Nonetheless these findings suggest that studying patients’ experiences while they make art in

naturalistic clinical settings may help further shed light on how artistic activities may benefit
ur

mood.
Jo
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 29

References

Abbing, A., Ponstein, A., van Hooren, S., de Sonneville, L., Swaab, H., & Baars, E. (2018). The

effectiveness of art therapy for anxiety in adults: A systematic review of randomised and

non-randomised controlled trials. PloS one, 13, e0208716.

American Art Therapy Association. (2017). History and background. Retrieved on 05/01/2017

from http://www.arttherapy.org/aata-history-background.html

of
Arditte, K. A., Çek, D., Shaw, A. M., & Timpano, K. R. (2016). The importance of assessing

ro
clinical phenomena in Mechanical Turk research. Psychological Assessment, 28, 684-

691. doi.org/10.1037/pas0000217

-p
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report
re
assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assesssment, 13, 27-45.

Barlow, D. H. (Ed.). (2014). Clinical handbook of psychological disorders: A step-by-step


lP

treatment manual. New York, NY: Guilford.

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, NY: W. H. Freeman and
na

Company.

Bandura, A., Pastorelli, C., Barbaranelli, C., & Caprara, G. V. (1999). Self-efficacy pathways to
ur

childhood depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 258.

doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.2.258
Jo

Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression.

New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Beghetto, R. A. (2006). Creative self-efficacy: Correlates in middle and secondary students.

Creativity Research Journal, 18, 447-457. doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1804_4


ART-MAKING AND MOOD 30

Beghetto, R. A., Kaufman, J. C., & Baxter, J. (2011). Answering the unexpected questions:

Exploring the relationship between students' creative self-efficacy and teacher ratings of

creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 5, 342-349.

doi.org/10.1037/a0022834

Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon's Mechanical Turk: A new source

of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 3-5.

doi.org/10.1177/1745691610393980

of
Caddy, L., Crawford, F., & Page, A. C. (2012). ‘Painting a path to wellness’: Correlations

ro
between participating in a creative activity group and improved measured mental health

outcome. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing, 19, 327-333.

doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2850.2011.01785.x
-p
re
Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective

responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales. Journal of Personality
lP

and Social Psychology, 67, 319-333. doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319

Chambers, R., Gullone, E., & Allen, N. B. (2009). Mindful emotion regulation: An integrative
na

review. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 560-572. doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.06.005

Chorpita, B. F., Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. (2016). Perceived control as a mediator of family
ur

environment in etiological models of childhood anxiety. Behavior Therapy, 29, 457-476.

doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2016.08.008
Jo

Clark, S. M. (2016). DBT-informed art therapy: Mindfulness, cognitive behavior therapy, and

the creative process. Philadelphia, PA: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.

Crawford, M. J., Killaspy, H., Barnes, T. R. E., Barrett, B., Byford, S., Clayton, K., et al.

(2012). Group art therapy as an adjunctive treatment for people with


ART-MAKING AND MOOD 31

schizophrenia: Multicentre pragmatic randomised trial. The BMJ, 344, e846.

doi.org/10.1136/bmj.e846

Crocker L., & Algina, J. (2006). Introduction to classical and modern test theory. Belmont, CA:

Wadsworth Publishing Company.

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Creativity: Flow and the psychology of discovery and invention

(Vol. viii). New York, NY: HarperCollins Publishers.

Dalebroux, A., Goldstein, T. R., & Winner, E. (2008). Short-term mood repair through art-

of
making: Positive emotion is more effective than venting. Motivation and Emotion, 32,

ro
288-295. doi.org/10.1007/s11031-008-9105-1

Decker, K., Deaver, S., Abbey, V., Campbell, M., & Turpin, C. (2018). Quantitatively improved

-p
treatment outcomes for combat-associated PTSD with adjunctive art therapy:
re
Randomized controlled trial. Art Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy

Association, 35, 184-194. https://doi.org/10.1080/07421656.2018.1540822


lP

De Petrillo, L., & Winner, E. (2005). Does art improve mood? A test of a key assumption

underlying art therapy. Art Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy Association,
na

22, 205-212. doi.org/10.1080/07421656.2005.10129521

Diener, E., Wirtz, D., Tov, W., Kim-Prieto, C., Choi, D. W., Oishi, S., & Biswas-Diener, R.
ur

(2010). New well-being measures: Short scales to assess flourishing and positive and negative

feelings. Social Indicators Research, 97, 143-156. doi.org/10.1007/s11205-009-9493-y


Jo

Diliberto-Macaluso, K. A., & Stubblefield, B. L. (2015). The use of painting for short-term mood

and arousal improvement. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9, 228-234.

doi.org/10.1037/a0039237
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 32

Dimidjian, S., Hollon, S. D., Dobson, K. S., Schmaling, K. B., Kohlenberg, R. J., Addis, M. E.,

Gallop, R., et al. (2006). Randomized trial of behavioral activation,

cognitive therapy, and antidepressant medication in the acute treatment of adults with

major depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 658-670.

doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.74.4.658

Drake, J. E., Coleman, K., & Winner, E. (2011). Short-term mood repair through art: Effects of

medium and strategy. Art Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy Association, 28,

of
26-30. doi.org/10.1080/07421656.2011.557032

ro
Drake, J. E., Hastedt, I., & James, C. (2016). Drawing to distract: Examining the psychological

benefits of drawing over time. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10,

325–331. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000064
-p
re
Drake, J. E., & Winner, E. (2012). Confronting sadness through art-making: Distraction is more

beneficial than venting. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6, 255-261.
lP

doi.org/10.1037/a0026909

Drake, J. E., & Winner, E. (2013). How children use drawing to regulate their emotions.
na

Cognition & Emotion, 27, 512-520. doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2012.720567

Fisher, B. J., & Specht, D. K. (2000). Successful aging and creativity in later life. Journal of
ur

Aging Studies, 13, 457-472. doi.org/10.1016/S0890-4065(99)00021-3

Forgeard, M., Beard, C., Kirakosian, N., & Björgvinsson, T. (2018). Research in partial hospital
Jo

settings. In Dodd, R. T. (Ed.), Practice-based research: A guide for clinicians (pp. 212-

239). New York: Routledge.

Forgeard, M. J. C., & Benson, L. (2019). Extracurricular involvement and psychological

adjustment in the transition from adolescence to emerging adulthood: The role of mastery
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 33

and creative self-efficacy. Applied Developmental Science, 23, 41-58.

doi.org/10.1080/10888691.2017.1288124

Forgeard, M. J., & Elstein, J. G. (2014). Advancing the clinical science of creativity. Frontiers in

Psychology, 5, 30-33. doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.00613

Grove, J. R., & Prapavessis, H. (1992). Preliminary evidence for the reliability and validity of an

abbreviated Profile of Mood States. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 23, 93-

109.

of
Gussak, D. (2006). Effects of art therapy with prison inmates: A follow-up study. The Arts in

ro
Psychotherapy, 33, 188-198. doi.org/10.1016/j.aip.2005.11.003

Gussak, D. (2007). The effectiveness of art therapy in reducing depression in prison populations.

-p
International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 51, 444-460.
re
doi.org/10.1177/0306624X06294137

Gussak, D. (2009). Comparing the effectiveness of art therapy on depression and locus of control
lP

of male and female inmates. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 36, 202–207.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aip.2009.02.004
na

Harris, P. A., Taylor, R., Thielke, R., Payne, J., Gonzalez, N., & Conde, J. G. (2009). Research

electronic data capture (REDCap)—a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for
ur

providing translational research informatics support. Journal of Biomedical Informatics, 42, 377-

381. doi.org/10.1016/j.jbi.2008.08.010
Jo

Hayes, S. C., & Lillis, J. (2012). Acceptance and commitment therapy. Washington, DC:

American Psychological Association.

Heckwolf, J. I., Bergland, M. C., & Mouratidis, M. (2014). Coordinating principles of art therapy

and DBT. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 41, 329-335. doi.org/10.1016/j.aip.2014.03.006


ART-MAKING AND MOOD 34

Hofmann, S. G., Asnaani, A., Vonk, I. J., Sawyer, A. T., & Fang, A. (2012). The efficacy of

cognitive behavioral therapy: A review of meta-analyses. Cognitive Therapy and

Research, 36, 427-440. doi:10.1007/s10608-012-9476-1

Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis:

Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1-55.

doi:10.1080/10705519909540118

Jacobson, N. S., Martell, C. R., & Dimidjian, S. (2001). Behavioral activation treatment for

of
depression: Returning to contextual roots. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 8, 255-

ro
270. doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.8.3.255

Johnson, C. M., & Sullivan-Marx, E. M. (2006). Art therapy: Using the creative process for

-p
healing and hope among African American older adults. Geriatric Nursing, 27, 309-316.
re
doi.org/10.1016/j.gerinurse.2006.08.010

Kabat-Zinn, J. (2013). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body and mind to face
lP

stress, pain, and illness (revised edition). New York, NY: Bantam.

Kaimal, G., & Ray, K. (2017). Free art-making in an art therapy open studio: Changes in affect
na

and self-efficacy. Arts & Health, 9, 154-166. doi.org/10.1080/17533015.2016.1217248

Karwowski, M., & Lebuda, I. (2016). The big five, the huge two, and creative self-beliefs: A
ur

meta-analysis. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10, 214-232.

doi.org/10.1037/aca0000035
Jo

Kazdin, A. E. (2007). Mediators and mechanisms of change in psychotherapy research. Annual

Review of Clinical Psychology, 3, 1-27. doi.org/

10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091432
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 35

Kendall, P. C., & Beidas, R. S. (2007). Smoothing the trail for dissemination of evidence-based

practices for youth: Flexibility within fidelity. Professional Psychology: Research and

Practice, 38, 13-20. doi.org/10.1037/0735-7028.38.1.13

Kendall, P. C., Chu, B., Gifford, A., Hayes, C., & Nauta, M. (1999). Breathing life into a

manual: Flexibility and creativity with manual-based treatments. Cognitive and

Behavioral Practice, 5, 177-198. doi.org/10.1016/S1077-7229(98)80004-7

Kimport, E. R., & Robbins, S. J. (2012). Efficacy of creative clay work for reducing negative

of
mood: A randomized controlled trial. Art Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy

ro
Association, 29, 74-79. doi.org/10.1080/07421656.2012.680048

Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New

York, NY: Guilford Press.


-p
re
Kroenke, K., & Spitzer, R. L. (2002). The PHQ-9: A new depression diagnostic and severity

measure. Psychiatric Annals, 32, 509-515. doi.org/10.3928/0048-5713-20020901-06


lP

Lau, M. A., Bishop, S. R., Segal, Z. V., Buis, T., Anderson, N. D., Carlson, L., . . . Devins, G.

(2006). The Toronto Mindfulness Scale: Development and validation. Journal of Clinical
na

Psychology, 62, 1445-1467. doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20326

Lee, R. M., Draper, M., & Lee, S. (2001). Social connectedness, dysfunctional interpersonal
ur

behaviors, and psychological distress: Testing a mediator model. Journal of Counseling

Psychology, 48, 310-318. doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.48.3.310


Jo

Levine-Madori, L. (2013). Utilizing a thematic approach to art therapy with seniors: Enhancing

cognitive abilities and social interactions. In P. Howie, S. Prasad, & J. Kristel (Eds.),

Using art therapy with diverse populations: Crossing cultures and abilities (pp. 317-

327). Philadelphia, PA: Jessica Kingsley.


ART-MAKING AND MOOD 36

Linehan, M. (1993). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline personality disorder.

New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Lynch, T. R., Morse, J. Q., Mendelson, T., & Robins, C. J. (2003). Dialectical behavior therapy

for depressed older adults: A randomized pilot study. The American Journal of Geriatric

Psychiatry, 11, 33-45. doi.org/10.1097/00019442-200301000-00006

Maddux, J. E., (2009). Self-efficacy: The power of believing you can. In S. J. Lopez & C. R.

Snyder (Eds.), Oxford handbook of positive psychology (2nd edition, pp. 335-344). New

of
York, NY: Oxford University Press.

ro
Malchiodi, C. A. (Ed.). (2013). Expressive therapies. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Martin, D. J., Garske, J. P., & Davis, M. K. (2000). Relation of the therapeutic alliance with

-p
outcome and other variables: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
re
Psychology, 68, 438-450. doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.68.3.438

Marziali, E., Munroe-Blum, H., & McCleary, L. (1997). The contribution of group cohesion and
lP

group alliance to the outcome of group psychotherapy. International Journal of Group

Psychotherapy, 47, 475-497. doi.org/10.1080/00207284.1997.11490846


na

Maujean, A., Pepping, C. A., & Kendall, E. (2014). A systematic review of randomized

controlled studies of art therapy. Art Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy
ur

Association, 31, 37-44. doi.org/10.1080/07421656.2014.873696

Monti, D. A., Peterson, C., Kunkel, E. J. S., Hauck, W. W., Pequignot, E., Rhodes, L., &
Jo

Brainard, G. C. (2006). A randomized, controlled trial of mindfulness-based art therapy (MBAT)

for women with cancer. Psycho-Oncology, 15, 363-373. doi.org/ 10.1002/pon.988

Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2012). Mplus statistical modeling software: Release 7.0. Los

Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.


ART-MAKING AND MOOD 37

Paolacci, G., & Chandler, J. (2014). Inside the Turk: Understanding Mechanical Turk as a

participant pool. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 184-188.

doi.org/10.1177/0963721414531598

Pearlin, L. I., & Schooler, C. (1978). The structure of coping. Journal of Health and Social

Behavior, 19, 2-21.

Harris, P. A., Taylor, R., Thielke, R., Payne, J., Gonzalez, N., & Conde, J. G. (2009). Research

electronic data capture (REDCap)—a metadata-driven methodology and workflow

of
process for providing translational research informatics support. Journal of Biomedical

ro
Informatics, 42, 377-381.

Richardson, P., Jones, K., Evans, C., Stevens, P., & Rowe, A. (2007). Exploratory RCT of art

-p
therapy as an adjunctive treatment in schizophrenia. Journal of Mental Health, 16, 483-491.
re
doi.org/10.1080/09638230701483111

Rosal, M. L. (2016). Cognitive‐behavioral art therapy revisited. In D. Gussak & M.L. Rosal
lP

(Eds.), The Wiley handbook of art therapy (pp. 68-76). Malden, MA, US: John Wiley & Sons,

Ltd.
na

Sandblom, P. (1992). Creativity and disease: How illness affects literature, art and music. New

York: Marion Boyars.


ur

Schouten, K. A., de Niet, G. J., Knipscheer, J. W., Kleber, R. J., & Hutschemaekers, G. J.

(2015). The effectiveness of art therapy in the treatment of traumatized adults: A systematic
Jo

review on art therapy and trauma. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 16, 220-228.

doi.org/10.1177/1524838014555032
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 38

Schwarzer, R., & Jerusalem, M. (1995). Generalized Self-Efficacy scale. In J. Weinman,

S. Wright, & M. Johnston (Eds.), Measures in health psychology: A user’s portfolio.

Causal and control beliefs (pp. 35-37). Windsor, UK: NFER-NELSON.

Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2012). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy

for depression. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Shapiro, D. N., Chandler, J., & Mueller, P. A. (2013). Using Mechanical Turk to study clinical

populations. Clinical Psychological Science, 1, 213-220.

of
doi.org/10.1177/2167702612469015

ro
Sheehan, D., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Sheehan, K., Amorim, P., Janavs, J.,Weiller, E., et

al. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and

-p
validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of
re
Clinical Psychiatry. 59, 22-33. doi.org/10.1016/S0924-9338(97)83296-8

Slayton, S. C., D'Archer, J., & Kaplan, F. (2010). Outcome studies on the efficacy of art therapy:
lP

A review of findings. Art Therapy: Journal of the American Art Therapy Association, 27,

108-118. doi.org/10.1080/07421656.2010.10129660
na

Smolarski, K., Leone, K., & Robbins, S. J. (2015). Reducing negative mood through drawing:

Comparing venting, positive expression, and tracing. Art Therapy: Journal of the
ur

American Art Therapy Association, 32, 197-201.

doi.org/10.1080/07421656.2015.1092697
Jo

Stein, M. I. (1953). Creativity and culture. The Journal of Psychology, 36, 311-322.

doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1953.9712897

Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1995). Defying the crowd: Cultivating creativity in a culture of

conformity. New York, NY: Free Press.


ART-MAKING AND MOOD 39

Thompson, E. R. (2007). Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of

the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS). Journal of Cross-Cultural

Psychology, 38, 227-242. doi.org/10.1177/0022022106297301

Thyme, K. E., Sundin, E. C., Stahlberg, G., Lindstrom, B., Eklof, H., & Wiberg, B. (2007). The

outcome of short-term psychodynamic art therapy compared to short-term psychodynamic verbal

therapy for depressed women. Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy, 21, 50-264.

of
doi.org/10.1080/02668730701535610

Tierney, P., & Farmer, S. M. (2002). Creative self-efficacy: Its potential antecedents and

ro
relationship to creative performance. Academy of Management Journal, 45, 1137-1148.

doi.org/10.2307/3069429

-p
Wanous, J. P., Reichers, A. E., & Hudy, M. J. (1997). Overall job satisfaction: How good are
re
single-item measures? Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 247-252. doi.org/10.1037/0021-

9010.82.2.247
lP

Webb, C. A., Beard, C., Kertz, S. J., Hsu, K. J., & Björgvinsson, T. (2016). Differential role of

CBT skills, DBT skills and psychological flexibility in predicting depressive versus
na

anxiety symptom improvement. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 81, 12-20.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2016.03.006
ur

Youngblut, J. M., & Casper, G. R. (1993). Focus on psychometrics single‐item indicators in

nursing research. Research in Nursing & Health, 16, 459-465. doi.org/ 10.1002/nur.4770160610
Jo

Zalta, A. K., & Chambless, D. L. (2011). Testing a developmental model of anxiety with the

Parental Facilitation of Mastery Scale. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25, 352-361.

doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.10.009
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 40

Zimet, G. D., Dahlem, N. W., Zimet, S. G., & Farley, G. K. (1988). The Multidimensional Scale

of Perceived Social Support. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52, 30-41.

doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5201_2

Zimmerman, M., Ruggero, C. J., Chelminski, I., Young, D., Posternak, M. A., Friedman, M. et

al. (2006). Developing brief scales for use in clinical practice: The reliability and validity of

single-item self-report measures of depression symptom severity, psychosocial impairment due

to depression, and quality of life. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 67, 1536-1541.

of
doi.org/10.4088/JCP.v67n1007

ro
-p
re
lP
na
ur
Jo
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 41

of
ro
-p
re
lP
na
ur
Jo

Figure 1. Results of the confirmatory factor analysis on the 21-item disaggregated version of the

Art Group Questionnaire. From left to right: residual variances, indicator variables (items),

standardized path coefficients, and latent variables (constructs).


ART-MAKING AND MOOD 42

of
ro
-p
re
lP

Figure 2. Results of the path analysis examining relationships between changes in

positive/negative mood and changes in other outcomes during the art group (controlling for
na

gender, education, as well as all outcomes measured at the beginning of the art group),

including significant standardized path coefficients and residual variances (nonsignificant paths
ur

are omitted).
Jo
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 43

Table 1

of
Correlations Examining Convergent and Divergent Validity of Items Included in the Brief 7-Item Art Group Questionnaire, and

Fisher’s r-to-z Transformation Comparing Correlation Coefficients.

ro
-p
Convergent Validity Divergent Validity
Art Group Questionnaire Item Pearson's r Pearson's r Fisher's r-to-z
Measure Measure

re
1. creative, imaginative, innovative State CSE .582*** PANAS-SF NA 0.246** 3.89***

2. supported, connected, cared for SCS-R .475*** PANAS-SF NA .013 4.72***

3. energized, invigorated, stimulated

4. mindful, present, attentive


lP
A-POMS Vigor

TMS Curiosity
.773***

.284***
PANAS-SF NA

PANAS-SF NA
0.216**

-.115
7.58***

3.82***
na
TMS Decentering 269*** PANAS-SF NA -.115 3.67***

5. pleased, content, happy PANAS-SF PA .468*** PANAS-SF NA -.101 5.71***

6. competent, effective, capable State GSE .507*** PANAS-SF NA -.111 6.29***


ur

8. frustrated, anxious, sad PANAS-SF NA .714*** PANAS-SF PA .035 8.07***


Jo

Note. *** p < .001, ** p < .01, * p < .05


ART-MAKING AND MOOD 44

Table 2

Diagnostic Characteristics of the Present Sample, Based on the Miniature International

Neuropsychiatric Interview.

MINI Diagnosis N Percentage of Sample

Major Depressive Disorder (lifetime) 93 69.4%

Major Depressive Episode (current) 78 58.2%

of
Bipolar Disorder (lifetime) 28 20.9%

ro
Major Depressive Episode (current) 19 14.2%

Mania or hypomania (current) 2 1.48%

-p
Generalized Anxiety Disorder (current) 54 40.3%

Social Anxiety Disorder (current) 40 29.9%


re
Alcohol Use Disorder (current) 31 23.1%

Panic Disorder (current) 25 18.7%


lP

Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (current) 20 14.9%

Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (current) 15 11.2%


na

Psychotic Disorder (current) 6 4.5%

Note. Participants completed the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI; Sheehan et al.,
1998) to assess for DSM-IV disorders. The MINI was completed by 134 participants (77% of the sample);
ur

41 participants did not complete the MINI due to a variety of clinical reasons including early discharge,
symptom acuity, or other concerns. Participants completed the MINI with a clinician at the beginning of
treatment in the PHP (typically on their second day of treatment). The MINI has strong reliability and
validity in relation to the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-IV), with inter-rater
Jo

reliabilities ranging from kappas of .89-1.0 (Sheehan et al., 1998). In this study, the MINI was
administered by doctoral practicum students and interns in clinical psychology who received weekly
supervision by a postdoctoral fellow. Training included reviewing administration manuals and
completing mock interviews, as well as participating in bi-annual reliability ratings.
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 45

Table 3

Means and Standard Deviations for All Outcomes Before and After Group; Results of One-Way

ANOVAs Including F Statistics, P-Values, and Effect Sizes (Cohen’s d).

Before After

Group Group

of
Cohen's

Outcome M SD M SD F(1,157) p d

ro
Positive Mood 2.64 1.02 3.32 0.99 94.38 <.001 0.68

Negative Mood 2.58 1.12 2.01 1.05 58.02 <.001 0.52

Creative Self-Efficacy 2.64 1.11 3.28


-p
1.02 83.27 <.001 0.60
re
General Self-Efficacy 2.91 1.07 3.32 0.90 34.34 <.001 0.41

Activation 2.55 1.11 3.23 0.98 84.56 <.001 0.65


lP

Mindfulness 2.92 0.96 3.50 0.93 57.44 <.001 0.61

Social Connectedness 3.39 1.00 3.54 0.96 5.59 .02 0.15


na
ur
Jo
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 46

Table 4

of
Bivariate Correlations (r) for all Variables Included in the Path Model.

ro
pm nm cse gse act mind soc edu gender pm nm cse gse act mind soc
(chg) (chg) (chg) (chg) (chg) (chg) (chg) (pre) (pre) (pre) (pre) (pre) (pre) (pre)
pm (chg) 1

-p
nm (chg) -.29*** 1
cse (chg) .23** .18* 1
gse (chg) .22** -.27*** .17* 1
act (chg) .36*** -.15 .29*** .33*** 1

re
mind (chg) .39*** -.24** .24** .28*** .40*** 1
soc (chg) .35*** -.06 .15* .17** .44*** .27*** 1
edu -.15 .12 .03 .05 .00 -.08 -.16* 1
gender -.19* .15 -.15 -.28*** -.14 -.18* .04 -.24** 1
pm (pre)
nm (pre)
cse (pre)
gse (pre)
act (pre)
-.48***
.17*
.02
-.11
-.12
.26***
-.51***
.21**
.26***
.16*
-.08
.14
-.50***
-.14
-.18*
-.02
.08
-.13
-.58***
-.10
lP-.21**
-.08
-.15
-.24**
-.56***
-.10
.06
.01
-.07
-.15
-.06
-.04
.10
.05
-.11
-.01
-.06
-.10
-.14
-.08
.20**
-.22**
.09
.25***
.14
1
-.45***
.43***
.56***
.65***
1
-.19*
-.33***
-.29***
1
.56***
.61***
1
.60*** 1
na
mind (pre) -.15 .27*** -.09 -.14 -.23** -.55*** -.04 -.01 .19* .56*** -.33*** .39*** .54*** .58*** 1
soc (pre) -.21** .08 -.03 -.03 -.23** -.19* -.48*** .13 -.01 .52*** -.19* .32*** .40*** .58*** .42*** 1
ur

Note. *** p < .001, ** p < .01, * p < .05; chg = change score; pre = pretest score; pm = positive mood; nm = negative mood; cse =
creative self-efficacy; gse = general self-efficacy; act = activation; mind = mindfulness; soc = social connectedness; edu = education;
gender = gender (1 = woman, 2 = man)
Jo
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 47

Table 5

Unstandardized and Standardized Path Estimates, Standard Errors, and P-Values for the Path

Model.

Unstd B SE Std β SE p
cse (chg) → pm (chg) 0.13 0.10 .13 .10 .17
gse (chg) → pm (chg) 0.21 0.11 .22 .11 <.05
act (chg) → pm (chg) 0.22 0.12 .23 .13 .07
mind (chg) → pm (chg) 0.28 0.09 .30 .09 <.001
soc (chg) → pm (chg) 0.05 0.10 .05 .10 .60

of
pm (pre) → pm (chg) -0.81 0.08 -.93 .09 <.001
nm (pre) → pm (chg) -0.06 0.04 -.08 .05 .12
cse (pre) → pm (chg) 0.05 0.09 .06 .10 .59

ro
gse (pre) → pm (chg) 0.17 0.12 .20 .14 .15
act (pre) → pm (chg) 0.29 0.13 .36 .17 .04
mind (pre) → pm (chg) 0.25 0.11 .27 .11 .02
soc (pre)
gender
edu



pm (chg)
pm (chg)
pm (chg)
0.00
-0.08
-0.05
0.10
0.08
0.05
.00

-p
-.05
-.05
.11
.05
.05
.98
.32
.32
re
cse (chg) → nm (chg) 0.04 0.09 .04 .09 .64
gse (chg) → nm (chg) -0.24 0.11 -.23 .10 .03
lP

act (chg) → nm (chg) 0.04 0.10 .04 .10 .72


mind (chg) → nm (chg) -0.16 0.10 -.16 .10 .11
soc (chg) → nm (chg) -0.07 0.12 -.06 .11 .54
pm (pre) → nm (chg) 0.09 0.12 .10 .12 .42
na

nm (pre) → nm (chg) -0.39 0.07 -.47 .08 <.001


cse (pre) → nm (chg) 0.17 0.09 .20 .11 .06
gse (pre) → nm (chg) -0.05 0.13 -.05 .15 .73

ur

act (pre) nm (chg) -0.06 0.12 -.07 .14 .60


mind (pre) → nm (chg) -0.04 0.12 -.04 .12 .76
soc (pre) → nm (chg) -0.13 0.10 -.14 .10 .19

Jo

gender nm (chg) -0.04 0.15 -.02 .08 .79


edu → nm (chg) 0.07 0.06 .07 .06 .26

pm (chg) ↔ nm (chg) -0.07 0.03 -.16 .07 .02

Note. chg = change score; pre = pretest score; pm = positive mood; nm = negative mood; cse =
creative self-efficacy; gse = general self-efficacy; act = activation; mind = mindfulness; soc =
social connectedness; edu = education; gender = gender (1 = woman, 2 = man)
ART-MAKING AND MOOD 48

Appendix
Art Group Questionnaire

Part 1 (To be completed at the beginning of group): Please use the scales below to describe
how you feel RIGHT NOW. There are no right or wrong answers! We are asking you to
complete this questionnaire as an opportunity to practice self-assessment.

not at all a little somewhat very extremely


creative, imaginative, innovative 1 2 3 4 5
supported, connected, cared for 1 2 3 4 5
energized, invigorated, stimulated 1 2 3 4 5
mindful, present, attentive 1 2 3 4 5
pleased, content, happy 1 2 3 4 5

of
competent, effective, capable 1 2 3 4 5
frustrated, anxious, sad 1 2 3 4 5

ro
Thanks, you are ready to start working on your project! You will fill out the other side of
this document at the end of the group.

-p
Part 2 (To be completed at the end of group): Please answer the questions below at the end of
group, in preparation for a brief discussion with other group members.
re
First, what activity (or activities) did you choose? Check all that apply.
Coloring ❏ Drawing ❏ Painting ❏ Collage ❏ Crafts ❏
Origami / Paper folding ❏ Jewelry ❏ Writing ❏
lP

Other ❏ Describe: _____________________________________________________

Second, we’d like you to fill out this questionnaire again. Please use the scales below to
describe how you feel RIGHT NOW.
na

not at all a little somewhat very extremely


creative, imaginative, innovative 1 2 3 4 5
supported, connected, cared for 1 2 3 4 5
ur

energized, invigorated, stimulated 1 2 3 4 5


mindful, present, attentive 1 2 3 4 5
pleased, content, happy 1 2 3 4 5
Jo

competent, effective, capable 1 2 3 4 5


frustrated, anxious, sad 1 2 3 4 5

Finally, we would like to ask you for your opinion about this group. What do you like the
best/least about this group? In what ways do you think that creative self-expression is (or is
not) helpful? Any comments/suggestions?

[Open-Ended Response]

You might also like