Evaluating The Awakening Criterion in The Definition of Nightmares: How Certain Are People in Judging Whether A Nightmare Woke Them Up?

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

J. Sleep Res.

(2006) 15, 117–124

Evaluating the awakening criterion in the definition


of nightmares: how certain are people in judging whether
a nightmare woke them up?
M A R K B L A G R O V E and S U Z A N N E H A Y W O O D
Department of Psychology, University of Wales Swansea, Swansea, UK

Accepted in revised form 30 November 2005; received 29 November 2004

SUMMARY There is debate about whether to include in the definition of nightmares a criterion that
the imagery or emotions of the nightmare caused the person to wake up. This study
investigates whether people believe that they can judge this cause of awakening. 42
participants recorded for 14 nights whether they had a dream, and decided for each
dream whether it had or had not woken them. They then rated on a 5-point scale (where
1 ¼ very certain and 5 ¼ very uncertain) how certain they were in their decision of
whether or not the dream woke them. ParticipantsÕ mean certainty was high for
decisions that the dream woke them (mean certainty ¼ 1.60), and for very unpleasant
dreams this mean certainty that the dream woke them was very high (mean
certainty ¼ 1.27). Dreams judged to have caused awakening were found to be more
unpleasant than dreams judged not to have caused awakening. Although the inclusion
of the awakening criterion did not increase the association of nightmare frequency with
anxiety, there may be other advantages in the use of the awakening criterion.
keywords anxiety, awakening, definition, nightmares

content and an elaborated storyÕ (Wood et al., 1992), and


INTRODUCTION
Ôvery disturbing dreams, involving any unpleasant emotion,
There is debate about whether to include in the definition of which are usually vividly recalledÕ (Belicki, 1992a). In this
nightmares the criterion that the person was woken by the paper the former category of nightmares will be termed waking
events or emotions of the nightmare. For example, Levin and criterion nightmares and the latter category will be termed
Fireman (2002a), following the Diagnostic and Statistical inclusive definition nightmares. The category of inclusive
Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (American Psychi- definition nightmares thus includes waking criterion night-
atric Association, 1994) and the International Classification of mares and bad dreams: in bad dreams, as a matter of definition
Sleep Disorders-R (American Sleep Disorders Association, (see Zadra and Donderi, 2000), the events or emotions of the
1997), define idiopathic nightmares as Ôvivid dreams marked by dream are not the cause of the dreamer waking up.
intensified feelings of dread or terror that awaken the Awakenings due to nightmares are common. Zadra and
individualÕ. Similarly, Zadra and Donderi (2000) define a Donderi (2000) found that 47% of their participants in a
nightmare as Ôa very disturbing dream in which the unpleasant 1-month prospective log study reported having at least one
visual imagery and/or emotions wake the person upÕ. In waking criterion nightmare. Their log data gave for their
contrast, other definitions of nightmares do not require the sample an estimated yearly mean frequency of 11.04 for
person to have been woken by the nightmare: examples of waking criterion nightmares and 29.40 for bad dreams.
these definitions include Ôfrightening dreams with visual However, individuals can be inaccurate in their judgements
about sleep, such as about whether one was awake or asleep
Correspondence: Mark Blagrove, Department of Psychology, Univer-
when cued (Mendelson, 1998) and remembering brief experi-
sity of Wales Swansea, Swansea SA2 8PP, UK. Tel.: +44 1792 295586; mentally induced arousals (Kato et al., 2004). This problem of
fax: +44 1792 295679; e-mail: m.t.blagrove@swansea.ac.uk subjective reports may be present also in expecting people to

Ó 2006 European Sleep Research Society 117


118 M. Blagrove and S. Haywood

judge that a nightmare woke them. For example, there may be advantage are presented by Zadra and Donderi (2000). We
a halo effect when the person decides whether the nightmare aimed to test this hypothesized advantage by correlating
woke them, with awakening being erroneously attributed to a frequencies of waking criterion and inclusive definition night-
dream that had especially unpleasant events and emotions. mares with trait and state anxiety.
Further problems with the waking criterion are that it may be One of the few studies to have looked at correlates of
difficult to accurately introspect about what caused an awakenings from dreams, Kroth et al. (1997), did find that a
awakening, unless the cause is obvious, such as an alarm combined anxiety–depression measure correlated significantly
clock. Participants claiming that the dream woke them may be (r ¼ 0.42) with a retrospective measure of Ôawakening abruptly
Ôtelling more than we can knowÕ (Nisbett and Wilson, 1977). from a dreamÕ, but it is not clear whether the result was
However, contrary to this sceptical approach to introspection confounded by the emotional tone of dreams. Furthermore, in
is the work of Ericsson and Simon (1993), who show that the literature in general, there is no systematic difference
immediate retrospective descriptions of specific heeded behav- between correlations of anxiety with frequencies of waking
iours can be highly accurate. criterion and inclusive definition nightmares. A survey of, to
Using such introspective reports Suengas and Johnson our knowledge, all such between-subjects correlation studies in
(1988) found that memories for perceived events (for which the literature involving anxiety and nightmare frequency,
they include dreaming as one example) have greater clarity categorized by use or non-use of the awakening criterion, is
and sensory and contextual detail than do memories for events seen in Table 1. Similarly, in non-correlational studies, signi-
that the person had been told to imagine, and they conclude ficant relationships between nightmare frequency and trait
that therefore people can engage in accurate reality monito- anxiety have been found by Hersen (1971), Levin (1998) and
ring, that is, determining whether a memory is of an event that Ohayon et al. (1997) using the waking criterion, and by
had been perceived. Johnson (1988) reviews how time (on the Mindell and Barrett (2002), Nielsen et al. (2000) and Tanska-
scale of 24 or 48 h) and rehearsal can adversely affect reality nen et al. (2001) without the waking criterion. However, a
monitoring of perceptual and autobiographical memories, and concern with these correlational and non-correlational studies
for us this has implications for the long-term retrospective of waking criterion nightmares is that participants did not
recall of nightmares, but this work does indicate that, upon assess whether the nightmare woke them separately from
waking, people may be able to correctly judge whether or not assessing the degree of unpleasantness or distress of the
a dream woke them. In support of this is the finding of Zadra nightmare: in the present study participants rate unpleasant-
and Donderi (2000) that out of their 42 participants who had ness of dreams and judge whether the dream woke them as
at least one waking criterion nightmare during their 1-month separate decisions.
study, only one reported difficulties in distinguishing awaken-
ing caused by the dream content from spontaneous or
externally caused awakenings. METHOD
Our primary aim in the present study was to extend this
Participants
finding of Zadra and Donderi (2000) by ascertaining how
certain people are in deciding that a nightmare woke them. We recruited by advertisement 42 participants from the
This method follows from Ericsson and Simon’s (1993) review university population (33 females, nine males, mean
of the positive relationship between confidence and accuracy. age ¼ 21.79 years, SD ¼ 6.55) who were reporting at least
To reduce halo effects and attributions, decisions about one nightmare per month. Local ethics committee approval
awakenings were made for all dreams, whether pleasant or was obtained for the study and written consent to take part
unpleasant, and certainty was assessed for decisions that a was given by all participants.
dream had woken them, and also for decisions that a dream
had not woken them.
Materials and procedure
Our second aim was to (i) replicate the finding of Zadra
and Donderi (2003) that the mean hedonic tone of waking Participants completed the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory
criterion nightmares is more unpleasant than the mean (Spielberger, 1983) to obtain state anxiety (Ôhow you feel
hedonic tone of bad dreams, and (ii) to extend this right now, that is, at this momentÕ) and trait anxiety (Ôhow
comparison to dreams in general, by assessing whether you generally feelÕ). They next completed the Nightmare
dreams that cause awakening are more unpleasant than Distress Scale (Belicki, 1992b), a 13-item questionnaire on
dreams that do not cause awakening. which higher scores indicate a greater distress caused by
Our third aim was separate from the issue of the validity of nightmares, such as in making one feel afraid of going to sleep
the waking criterion, and concerned testing a possible or wishing to have therapy to stop the nightmares. They next
advantage of the criterion. This hypothesized advantage is made a retrospective estimate of their frequency of dream
that the frequency of waking criterion nightmares will have a recall and inclusive definition nightmares using the definition:
higher correlation with waking life psychopathology or poor Ôvery disturbing dreams, involving any unpleasant emotion,
well-being than would the frequency of inclusive definition which are usually vividly recalledÕ. They then estimated their
nightmares: data and theoretical reasons in favour of this frequency of waking criterion nightmares: the question here

Ó 2006 European Sleep Research Society, J. Sleep Res., 15, 117–124


Judging nightmare awakening 119

Table 1 Pearson and Spearman correlation coefficients between nightmare frequency and trait and state anxiety

Category of
Retrospective nightmare or Trait State
Authors /prospective bad dream anxiety anxiety

Blagrove et al., 2004 R W 0.16


Zadra and Donderi, 2000 R W 0.27 and 0.37a 0.28 and 0.35a
(Starker and Hasenfeld, 1976) R W (0.25)b
Cook et al., 1990 R I 0.33
Haynes and Mooney, 1975 R I 0.17 and 0.23c
Krakow et al., 2002 R I 0.29 and 0.28d
Miró and Martı́nez, 2005 R I 0.23 and 0.06e
Nguyen et al., 2002 R I 0.26 and 0.19f
Schredl et al., 1996 R I 0.31
Wood and Bootzin, 1990 R I 0.13
Zadra and Donderi, 2000 R BD 0.16 and 0.05a 0.09 and 0.09a
Blagrove et al., 2004 P– 2 weeks W 0.12
Levin and Fireman, 2002a P– 3 weeks W 0.23 0.20
Zadra and Donderi, 2000 P– 1 month W 0.28 0.27
Zadra et al., 1995 P– 2 weeks W 0.23
Blagrove et al., 2004 P– 2 weeks I 0.35
Krakow et al., 2002 P– 2 weeks I 0.33 and 0.37d
Wood and Bootzin, 1990 P– 2 weeks I 0.04
Wood et al., 1992 P– 3 weeks I 0.29, 0.38 and 0.23g
Wood et al., 1992 P– 3 weeks I All 3 correlations
non-significanth
Zadra and Donderi, 2000 P – 1 month BD 0.09 0.08
Zadra et al., 1995 P – 2 weeks BD 0.31

Studies are categorized as retrospective (R) or prospective (P); nightmares categorized as using the waking criterion (W) or as inclusive definition
nightmares (I). Studies of frequency of bad dreams, where, by definition it was not the dream that caused the person to wake, are labelled BD.
Studies using inclusive definition nightmares thus combine waking criterion nightmares and bad dreams.
a
First coefficient is for nightmare frequency assessed for 1 year retrospectively and second coefficient is for nightmare frequency assessed for
1 month retrospectively.
b
Measure used for correlation was anxious-distractible daydreaming style. As this is not a standard trait anxiety measure citation and coefficients
are placed in parentheses.
c
Coefficients are from study I and study II.
d
First coefficient refers to nights per week that a nightmare occurred and second coefficient refers to number of nightmares per week.
e
First coefficient refers to frequency of nightmares for participants who have nightmares on a weekly basis, i.e. 1–7 times per week and second
coefficient refers to frequency of nightmares for participants who have nightmares on a monthly basis, i.e. 1–3 times per month.
f
First coefficient is for global anxiety and second coefficient is for anxiety related to testing.
g
Coefficients reported for three groups; first two correlations are for individuals who were in the proximity of the 1989 San Francisco earthquake
and third correlation is for a non-earthquake control group. Anxiety assessed for time of occurrence of earthquake.
h
Coefficients not reported due to being non-significant for each of the three groups tested. Anxiety assessed for the 24 h before the log started.

was ÔHow often do you have a nightmare where the emotion or mares. It is not known whether or to what extent responses
imagery of the nightmare wakes you up?Õ would have altered if this question had also included reference
Participants then kept a prospective dream log for 14 nights, to dream imagery.
each morning answering the following:
1 Did you have a dream last night?
RESULTS
2 How pleasant/unpleasant was the dream? [responses made
on the 7-point dream hedonic tone scale (Foulkes et al., Table 2 shows the means, SDs and ranges of: retrospective
1966): 1 ¼ very pleasant to 7 ¼ very unpleasant] dream recall frequency; retrospective inclusive definition and
3 Did the emotion of the dream wake you up? waking criterion nightmare frequencies; dream recall and
4 Rate how certain you are of your answer to question 3 nightmare frequencies on the 2-week prospective log (log
(responses made on a 5-point scale: 1 ¼ very certain to inclusive definition nightmares defined as dreams rated 6 or 7,
5 ¼ very uncertain). i.e. moderately or very unpleasant, on the hedonic tone scale,
Participants could answer the questions for more than one log waking criterion nightmares defined as dreams rated 6 or 7
dream per night. Question 3 only referred to the emotion of the on the hedonic tone scale where the person also states that they
dream causing waking, and did not refer to the possibility that were woken by the emotion of the dream); hedonic tone of the
the imagery alone of the dream caused waking, unlike in the log dreams; confidence in making decisions about whether or
fuller retrospective question about waking criterion night- not a dream caused an awakening; trait anxiety and state

Ó 2006 European Sleep Research Society, J. Sleep Res., 15, 117–124


120 M. Blagrove and S. Haywood

Table 2 Means, SDs and ranges of: retrospective dream recall fre- number of dreams (279.2), inclusive definition (53.3) and
quency (DRF); retrospective inclusive definition and waking criterion waking criterion nightmares (31.5) were higher than estimates
nightmare frequencies (NFs); dream recall and nightmare frequencies
from the retrospective questions (169.7, 35.9 and 20.1 respect-
on the 2 week prospective log (log inclusive definition nightmares de-
fined as dreams rated 6 or 7, i.e. moderately or very unpleasant, on the ively).
hedonic tone scale, log waking criterion nightmares defined as dreams There were no significant sex differences in anxiety, night-
rated 6 or 7 on the hedonic tone scale and where the person also states mare distress, retrospective or log dream recall frequency,
that they were woken by the emotion of the dream); hedonic tone of dream hedonic tone, certainty in decisions about dreams
the log dreams; confidence in making decisions about whether or not a
causing awakening or not causing awakening, or in frequency
dream caused an awakening; trait anxiety and state anxiety
of retrospective waking criterion nightmares, but males did
Variable Mean SD Range report significantly fewer retrospective inclusive definition, log
Retrospective DRF (per year) 169.67 105.17 30–286 inclusive definition and log awakening criterion nightmares
Retrospective inclusive definition NF 35.86a 49.16 10–286 than did females (inferential statistics in notes to Table 2).
(per year) Tests of normality showed only the anxiety variables to be
Retrospective waking criterion NF 20.05 29.39 0–104 normally distributed and so non-parametric inferential statis-
(per year)
tics are used for tests involving the other variables. The use of
Nightmare distress 25.50 7.66 13–55
Log DRF (per 2 weeks) 10.74 4.75 4–23 non-parametric statistics for the nightmare data was also
Log inclusive definition NF (per 2 weeks) 2.05b 1.91 0–9 appropriate given the inherent inaccuracies in retrospective
Log waking criterion NF (per 2 weeks) 1.21c 1.41 0–7 and brief log methodologies.
Hedonic tone of log dreams 3.89 0.89 2.00–6.33 Table 3 shows the results for all 42 participants of number
Confidence in all decisions of whether 1.94 0.58 1.00–3.35
and percentage of dreams in each hedonic tone category, and,
dream did or did not cause awakening
Trait anxiety 44.05 8.51 31–64 for each hedonic category, number and percentage of dreams
State anxiety 39.38 9.71 20–57 judged to have caused awakening, percentage judged not to
have caused awakening, mean certainty of judgements that
Note: Some dreams rated 6 might not have been termed a nightmare or
awakening was not caused by a dream and mean certainty of
bad dream by the participant, or might not have met some of the
definitions of nightmare or bad dream present in the literature. judgements that awakening was caused by a dream.
a
Males reported significantly fewer inclusive definition nightmares
[mean (SD) ¼ 20.44 (31.33) per year] on the retrospective question-
naire than did females [mean (SD) ¼ 40.06 (52.59) per year]; Mann– Aim 1
Whitney U ¼ 74.00, z ¼ 2.49, P ¼ 0.021.
b Observing Table 3 suggests that the judgements of being
Males reported significantly fewer log inclusive definition nightmares
[mean (SD) ¼ 0.89 (1.17)] over the 2 weeks than did females [mean woken by a dream are generally made with high certainty and
(SD) ¼ 2.36 (1.97)]; Mann–Whitney U ¼ 74.00, z ¼ 2.35, P ¼ 0.021. with very high certainty for the very unpleasant dreams.
c
Males reported significantly fewer log waking criterion nightmares Judgements that dreams did not cause awakening were made
[mean (SD) ¼ 0.44 (0.73)] over the 2 weeks than did females [mean with moderate to high certainty. To assess the statistical
(SD) ¼ 1.42 (1.48)]; Mann–Whitney U ¼ 79.50, z ¼ 2.24. P ¼ 0.033.
significance of the difference in confidence between decisions of
dreams causing awakening and of dreams not causing
anxiety. On the logs 35 participants (83.3% of the sample) awakening a subset of participants was used, which would
reported one or more inclusive definition nightmares and 28 allow within-subject comparisons and adequate sampling of
participants (66.7% of the sample) reported one or more those dreams judged to have caused, and those judged not to
waking criterion nightmares. As has been found previously have caused awakening. On the prospective dream log 18
(e.g. Wood and Bootzin, 1990; Zadra and Donderi, 2000), the participants had at least three instances of a dream that they
prospective log provided higher frequency estimates than did judged woke them and at least three instances of a dream that
the retrospective questions. Here, log estimates of yearly they judged did not wake them. These participants were

Table 3 Number and percentage of dreams categorized by hedonic tone, and, for each hedonic tone category, percentage of dreams judged to have
not caused awakening, number and percentage of dreams judged to have caused awakening, mean certainty of participants that a dream did not
cause awakening, and mean certainty of participants that a dream caused awakening (1 ¼ very certain, 5 ¼ very uncertain)

Hedonic tone (1 ¼ very pleasant, 7 ¼ very unpleasant)

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Number of dreams with specified hedonic tone 36 65 81 119 64 52 34


Percentage of dreams with specified hedonic tone 8.0 14.4 18.0 26.4 14.2 11.5 7.5
Number of dreams judged to have caused awakening 9 5 12 8 16 24 26
Percentage of dreams judged not to have caused awakening 75.0 92.3 85.2 93.3 75.0 53.8 23.5
Percentage of dreams judged to have caused awakening 25.0 7.7 14.8 6.7 25.0 46.2 76.5
Mean certainty of judgements that a dream did not cause awakening 2.15 1.97 1.77 1.68 2.15 2.29 2.25
Mean certainty of judgements that a dream did cause awakening 1.44 1.80 2.08 1.62 2.31 2.08 1.27

Ó 2006 European Sleep Research Society, J. Sleep Res., 15, 117–124


Judging nightmare awakening 121

Table 4 Kendall’s tau-b partial correlation coefficients between retrospective and log measures of inclusive definition and waking criterion
nightmare frequencies (NFs), trait nightmare distress and mean hedonic tone of all dreams, and correlations between all nightmare variables and
dream hedonic tone with trait and state anxiety

Retrospective Retrospective
inclusive waking Log inclusive Log waking Nightmare Mean dream
definition NF criterion NF definition NF criterion NF distress hedonic tone

Retrospective waking criterion NF 0.57**


Log inclusive definition NF 0.31* 0.35**
Log waking criterion NF 0.34** 0.40** 0.64**
Nightmare distress 0.21 0.31* 0.01 0.14
Mean dream hedonic tone 0.30* 0.12 0.44** 0.43** )0.17
Trait anxiety 0.21 0.11 0.16 0.22 0.09 0.10
State anxiety 0.24 0.10 0.11 0.08 0.02 0.05

Critical values of tau-b for P-values taken from Maghsoodloo and Pallos (1981). Sex is partialled out for all correlations
*P < 0.005; **P < 0.001.

significantly more certain in judgements that dreams had unpleasant [mean hedonic tone ¼ 4.89 (SD ¼ 1.33)], than
woken them [mean certainty ¼ 1.60 (SD ¼ 0.62)] than in were dreams judged not to have caused awakening [mean
judgements that dreams had not woken them [mean cer- hedonic tone ¼ 3.70 (SD ¼ 0.88), Wilcoxon z ¼ 3.16,
tainty ¼ 2.11 (SD ¼ 0.74), Wilcoxon z ¼ 2.37, P ¼ 0.018]. P ¼ 0.002].
Mean certainty in judging that dreams caused the awakening
and mean certainty in judging that dreams did not cause
Aim 3
awakening each had negligible Spearman correlations with
state and trait anxiety (all |rsps | <0.08) and non-significant Finally, we used the data from all the participants for our third
correlations with nightmare distress (both |rsps | <0.15). aim, to investigate the relationships between the nightmare
variables and state and trait anxiety (the latter two being found
to correlate significantly, Pearson’s r ¼ 0.72, P < 0.001).
Aim 2a
Table 4 shows the Kendall tau-b partial correlation coefficients
Waking criterion nightmares were reported by 28 participants between the nightmare and hedonic tone variables, and
with a mean hedonic tone of 6.47 (SD ¼ 0.44), and bad between them and trait and state anxiety, sex is partialled
dreams were reported by 21 participants with a mean hedonic out for all correlations. (Note should be taken that values of
tone of 6.23 (SD ¼ 0.37). For inferential statistics we used the Spearman’s r are often 50% higher than Kendall’s tau-b for
participants who had at least one waking criterion nightmare the same data and that Spearman and Kendall rank correla-
and at least one bad dream (n ¼ 14). For the waking criterion tion measures cannot be directly compared; Chen and Popov-
nightmares, mean hedonic tone ¼ 6.48 (SD ¼ 0.43), which is ich, 2002.) Following Bonferroni’s correction trait and state
more unpleasant, but not significantly so, than for bad dreams, anxiety did not correlate significantly with any nightmare
where mean hedonic tone ¼ 6.23 (SD ¼ 0.37), Wilcoxon variable, nor with hedonic tone, and there were no systematic
z ¼ 1.65, P ¼ 0.10 (two-tail). Zadra and Donderi (2003) had differences between inclusive definition and waking criterion
found a significant difference between waking criterion night- nightmare frequencies in their correlation coefficients.
mares where emotional intensity ¼ 7.95 (SD ¼ 1.51) and bad
dreams where emotional intensity ¼ 7.24 (SD ¼ 1.15), using a
DISCUSSION
9-point scale.
Regarding our first aim, individualsÕ judgements that the
emotions of a dream woke them were made with high certainty
Aim 2b
and with very high certainty in the case of very unpleasant
Observing Table 3 suggests that more awakenings are judged dreams. Also, these judgements were made with significantly
to be caused by very unpleasant dreams than by moderately more certainty than when judging that it was not the dream
unpleasant dreams, and that moderately and very unpleasant that caused the awakening. If such certainty can be taken as
dreams are more likely to be judged to cause awakenings than evidence that people are accurate when ascribing their
more pleasant dreams. We used the data from the 18 awakening to the dream, there may indeed be validity in
participants who had at least three instances of a dream that including the awakening criterion in the definition of night-
they judged woke them and at least three instances of a dream mares. This view is further supported by the greater unpleas-
that they judged did not wake them, to investigate the antness/intensity of waking criterion nightmares in
relationship between being woken from a dream and dream comparison to bad dreams (Zadra and Donderi, 2003, and
pleasantness/unpleasantness. Here, dreams judged to have our results for Aim 2a), and by our finding that dreams in
caused awakening were found to be significantly more general that are judged to cause awakening are significantly

Ó 2006 European Sleep Research Society, J. Sleep Res., 15, 117–124


122 M. Blagrove and S. Haywood

more unpleasant than dreams judged not to cause awakening Donderi (2003), showing that waking criterion nightmares are
(Aim 2b). significantly more unpleasant than bad dreams, and our
Importantly, level of certainty was not related to state or similar but non-significant result for this comparison, it may
trait anxiety or nightmare distress. Levin and Fireman (2002b) be this intensity level that causes awakening, but that intensity
had found nightmare prevalence to be more a function of trait level is not related to anxiety, but rather to some other low
nightmare distress than of state nightmare intensity, calling well-being measure, or, again, to variables such as thin
into question some decisions of the presence of a nightmare boundaries or absorption, which are related to experiencing
because the decision is not simply a function of the actual nightmares (Belicki and Belicki, 1986; Hartmann, 1998).
nightmare’s characteristics, but is rather due to heightened However, although trait and state anxiety may only have
sensitivity to its contents. Also, Edinger et al. (2000) found weak relationships with nightmare frequency in between-
that anxiety is predictive of erroneous judgements of sleep subjects analyses, state anxiety is related to nightmare occur-
quality. Our findings that certainty judgements about awa- rence in the longitudinal study of Cellucci and Lawrence
kening are not related to anxiety, or to trait nightmare distress, (1978), where nightmares were defined as Ôa subjectively
do support the validity of these certainty judgements. disturbing or anxiety-provoking dream from which a person
We also found little difference between correlations of usually awakensÕ, and in the waking criterion nightmare
anxiety with frequencies of the two categories of nightmare treatment study of Kellner et al. (1992). There has not been,
(Aim 3). This accords with the general conclusion from the though, any longitudinal study that addresses within-subject
literature that the correlations between anxiety and nightmare correlations of both types of nightmare, or of bad dreams, with
frequency, however nightmares are defined, are small and often state anxiety, or with any other well-being measure.
insignificant. However, caution is needed here because we did Following our investigation of this one possible advantage
not recruit control participants who report few or no night- of the waking criterion, we will now briefly mention four
mares and this may have reduced the correlation coefficients of others.
either or both of the nightmare categories. Also, correlations 1 Does the use of the waking criterion aid nosology in
involving retrospective assessments may vary as a function of providing a more clear and exact definition of nightmares
dream recall frequency. Beaulieu-Prévost and Zadra (2005a) and hence a more reliable measure? One issue here is whether
found that the correlation of trait anxiety with retrospective the two categories of nightmare differ in the accuracy of
assessments of anxiety in dreams was significant for low retrospective frequency estimations. Such differences could
dream recall frequency participants (r ¼ 0.34), but negligible occur because of differences in the ease of making instances
(r ¼ 0.05) for high dream recall frequency participants. They of the two categories available to memory (Tversky and
conclude that the higher correlation occurs due to the influence Kahneman, 1974), which could be a function of the interest
of current affective state on beliefs about dream content for in and attitude towards dreams and nightmares that the
individuals with limited access to instances of dreams on which person has (Beaulieu-Prévost and Zadra, 2005b).
to base their beliefs about their dream content. This study also 2 Do waking criterion nightmares produce more distress on
found that the retrospective estimation of how much anxiety is average than do inclusive definition nightmares? As already
present in dreams has no significant relationship with pros- discussed, waking criterion nightmares are more unpleasant
pective measures in people with low dream recall frequency. than bad dreams, and so it may well be that the mean
Our finding of there being apparently certain knowledge of emotional response following waking criterion nightmares
when nightmares cause awakenings, but with taking this may be worse than for inclusive definition nightmares and for
occurrence of awakenings into account not leading to an bad dreams. Yet, although plausible, this has not been directly
augmentation of the correlation of nightmare frequency with addressed in the literature. State distress resulting from
anxiety, indicates that such awakenings are not a function of individual nightmares has been assessed for waking criterion
anxiety. Instead, being susceptible to being woken by a dream, nightmares (Levin and Fireman, 2002b; Ohayon et al., 1997)
may, for example, be related to: ability to wake oneself up, and inclusive definition nightmares (Köthe and Pietrowski,
such waking being a defence-response, as suggested by Haynes 2001; Krakow et al., 2002), but not for both definitions of
and Mooney (1975); measures of low well-being other than nightmare in one study. However, the related measure of trait
anxiety, such as depression or daily hassles or recent negative nightmare distress has been found to have a higher correlation
events (Cook et al., 1990); personality measures such as thin with prospective frequency of waking criterion nightmares
intrapsychic boundaries (Hartmann, 1998) or absorption and (tau-b ¼ 0.14 in the present study, r ¼ 0.36 in Blagrove et al.,
vividness of imagery (Belicki and Belicki, 1986); or individual 2004, r ¼ 0.47 in Levin and Fireman, 2002b) than with
differences in the orienting reaction, either to dreamt or to prospective frequency of inclusive definition nightmares
external (Fisher et al., 1970) stimuli. The personality and [tau-b ¼ 0.01 in the present study, r ¼ 0.25 in Blagrove et al.,
orienting response explanations may indeed be more fitting 2004, r ¼ 0.17 (nights per week) and r ¼ 0.30 (nightmares per
than psychopathology or low well-being based ones to address week) in Krakow et al., 2002].
our finding that 25% of very pleasant dreams caused awake- 3 Are waking criterion nightmares and inclusive definition
nings, and that the mean certainty of these awakening nightmares differentially responsive to treatment? For
judgements was very high. Following the result of Zadra and example, frequency of waking criterion nightmares is

Ó 2006 European Sleep Research Society, J. Sleep Res., 15, 117–124


Judging nightmare awakening 123

decreased by relaxation and desensitization (Miller and Although this hypothesized advantage of the waking criterion
DiPilato, 1983) and by desensitization and rehearsal treat- as an index of low well-being is currently under debate, there
ments (Kellner et al., 1992), but the effect of these treat- are other possible advantages that we have detailed that also
ments on nightmares defined without the criterion, or on warrant investigation.
bad dreams, has not been reported.
4 Defining nightmares with the waking criterion and hence
REFERENCES
making the distinction with bad dreams may be productive
for future research, as consistency in definitions is essential American Psychiatric Association. Diagnosis and Statistical Manual of
for results to be comparable across studies, and as in making Mental Disorders, 4th edn. APA, Washington, DC, 1994.
American Sleep Disorders Association. International Classification of
this distinction it is then possible to explore the possible Sleep Disorders: Diagnosis and Coding Manual, Revised. ASDA,
differential correlates of both types of dream. Data from Rochester, MN, 1997.
both dream experiences can also be combined to be Beaulieu-Prévost, D. and Zadra, A. How dream recall frequency
compared with studies reporting only on inclusive definition shapes people’s beliefs about the content of their dreams. North Am.
nightmares. However, data from studies that only use the J. Psychol., 2005a, 7: 253–264.
Beaulieu-Prévost, D. and Zadra, A. Dream recall frequency and
inclusive definition cannot be so usefully and fully compared attitude towards dreams: a reinterpretation of the relation. Pers.
to data from studies that use the waking criterion and bad Individ. Dif., 2005b, 38: 919–927.
dream categories. Belicki, K. Nightmare frequency versus nightmare distress: relations to
Although there may be such advantages as those mentioned psychopathology and cognitive style. J. Abnorm. Psychol., 1992a,
above in the use of the waking criterion, a possible confound is 101: 592–597.
Belicki, K. The relationship of nightmare frequency to nightmare
that frequent awakenings from sleep in general have been suffering with implications for treatment and research. Dreaming,
reported by individuals with nightmares compared to non- 1992b, 2: 143–148.
nightmare controls in studies of inpatients in a large state Belicki, K. and Belicki, D. Predisposition for nightmares: a study of
hospital (Hersen, 1971), a heterogeneous group including hypnotic ability, vividness of imagery, and absorption. J. Clin.
participants who had suffered trauma (Krakow et al., 1995) Psychol., 1986, 42: 714–718.
Blagrove, M., Farmer, L. and Williams, E. The relationship of
and insomniacs (Ohayon et al., 1997). These three studies used
nightmare frequency and nightmare distress to well-being. J. Sleep
subjective reporting, but poorer sleep in general has also been Res., 2004, 13: 129–136.
found in polysomnographical studies of people with trauma- Cellucci, A. J. and Lawrence, P. S. Individual differences in self-
related nightmares (Germain and Nielsen, 2003; Woodward reported sleep variable correlations among nightmare sufferers.
et al., 2000). It may be that individuals in such clinical J. Clin. Psychol., 1978, 34: 721–725.
Chen, P. Y. and Popovich, P. M. Correlation: Parametric and
populations are able to distinguish awakenings not caused by
Nonparametric Measures. Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA, 2002.
nightmares from instances of awakenings caused by night- Cook, C. A. L., Caplan, R. D. and Wolowitz, H. Nonwaking
mares, but as their frequent nocturnal awakenings would responses to waking stressors: dreams and nightmares. J. Appl. Soc.
sometimes occur at the same time as a nightmare, they may on Psychol., 1990, 20: 199–226.
some occasions erroneously believe that the nightmare caused Edinger, J. D., Fins, A. I., Glenn, D. M., Sullivan, R. J. Jr, Bastian, L.
A., Marsh, G. R., Dailey, D., Hope, T. V., Young, M., Shaw, E. and
the awakening. However, whereas there are findings for non-
Vasilas, D. Insomnia and the eye of the beholder: are there clinical
clinical populations of subjective reports of increased night- markers of objective sleep disturbances among adults with and
time awakenings for individuals with nightmares (Cellucci and without insomnia complaints? J. Consult. Clin. Psychol., 2000, 68:
Lawrence, 1978; Levin, 1994; Schredl, 2003), this is not found 586–593.
polysomnographically for idiopathic nightmares (Germain Ericsson, K. A. and Simon, H. A. Protocol Analysis: Verbal Reports as
Data (revised edition). MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1993.
and Nielsen, 2003).
Fisher, C., Byrne, J., Edwards, A. and Kahn, E. A psychophysiological
In conclusion, the clinical use of the waking criterion is study of nightmares. J. Am. Psychoanal. Assoc., 1970, 18: 747–782.
supported by our finding that participants have high certainty Foulkes, D., Spear, P. S. and Symonds, J. D. Individual differences in
in judgements that a nightmare caused them to wake, as this mental activity at sleep onset. J. Abnorm. Psychol., 1966, 71: 280–286.
suggests they may thus be accurate when they judge that they Germain, A. and Nielsen, T. A. Sleep pathophysiology in posttrau-
matic stress disorder and idiopathic nightmare sufferers. Biol.
were awoken by the nightmare. However, the waking criterion
Psychiatry, 2003, 54: 1092–1098.
may be problematic for populations with trauma-related Hartmann, E. Personality and dreaming: boundary structure and
nightmares, or those with primary insomnia, because of the dream content. Dreaming, 1998, 8: 31–39.
tendency of people in such groups to wake frequently from Haynes, S. N. and Mooney, D. K. Nightmares: etiological, theoretical
sleep in general. Regarding one of the hypothesized advantages and behavioral treatment considerations. Psychol. Rec., 1975, 25:
225–236.
of the waking criterion, the use of the criterion did not lead to
Hersen, M. Personality characteristics of nightmare sufferers. J. Nerv.
greater predictive power of nightmare frequency for trait or Ment. Dis., 1971, 153: 27–31.
state anxiety. However, caution over the latter correlational Johnson, M. K. Reality monitoring: an experimental phenomenolog-
results is required, as our sample size was small for such ical approach. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 1988, 117: 390–394.
analyses, we did not recruit control participants with few or no Kato, T., Montplaisir, J. Y. and Lavigne, G. J. Experimentally induced
arousals during sleep: a cross-modality matching paradigm. J. Sleep
nightmares, and the correlation coefficients were low, although
Res., 2004, 13: 229–238.
they were in accord with much of the previous literature.

Ó 2006 European Sleep Research Society, J. Sleep Res., 15, 117–124


124 M. Blagrove and S. Haywood

Kellner, R., Neidhardt, J., Krakow, B. and Pathak, D. Changes in adolescence and their relation to anxiety symptoms. Sleep, 2000,
chronic nightmares after one session of desensitization or rehearsal 23: 727–736.
instructions. Am. J. Psychiatry, 1992, 149: 659–663. Nisbett, R. E. and Wilson, T. D. Telling more than we can know:
Köthe, M. and Pietrowski, R. Behavioral effects of nightmares and verbal reports on mental processes. Psychol. Rev., 1977, 84: 231–259.
their correlations to personality patterns. Dreaming, 2001, 11: 43–52. Ohayon, M. M., Morselli, P. L. and Guilleminault, C. Prevalence of
Krakow, B., Tandberg, D., Scriggins, L. and Barey, M. A controlled nightmares and their relationships to psychopathology and daytime
comparison of self-rated sleep complaints in acute and chronic functioning in insomnia subjects. Sleep, 1997, 20: 340–348.
nightmare sufferers. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis., 1995, 183: 623–627. Schredl, M. Effects of trait and state factors on nightmare frequency.
Krakow, B., Schrader, R., Tandberg, D., Hollifield, M., Koss, M. P., Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci., 2003, 253: 241–247.
Yau, C. L. and Cheng, D. T. Nightmare frequency in sexual assault Schredl, M., Pallmer, R. and Montasser, A. Anxiety dreams in school-
survivors with PTSD. J. Anxiety Disord., 2002, 16: 175–190. aged children. Dreaming, 1996, 6: 265–270.
Kroth, J., Jensen, L. and Haraldsson, M. Correlations of splitting and Spielberger, C.D. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory.
phobic anxiety with dreaming. Percept. Mot. Skills, 1997, 85: 333– Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, 1983.
334. Starker, S. and Hasenfeld, R. Daydream styles and sleep disturbance.
Levin, R. Sleep and dream characteristics of frequent nightmare J. Nerv. Ment. Dis., 1976, 163: 391–400.
subjects in a university population. Dreaming, 1994, 4: 127–137. Suengas, A. G. and Johnson, M. K. Qualitative effects of rehearsal on
Levin, R. Nightmares and schizotypy. Psychiatry, 1998, 61: 206–216. memories for perceived and imagined complex events. J. Exp.
Levin, R. and Fireman, G. Nightmare prevalence, nightmare distress, Psychol. Gen., 1988, 117: 377–389.
and self-reported psychological disturbance. Sleep, 2002a, 25: 205– Tanskanen, A., Tuomilehto, J., Viinamäki, H., Vartiainen, E.,
212. Lehtonen, J. and Puska, P. Nightmares as predictors of suicide.
Levin, R. and Fireman, G. Phenomenal qualities of nightmare Sleep, 2001, 24: 844–847.
experience in a prospective study of college students. Dreaming, Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D. Judgment under uncertainty: heuris-
2002b, 12: 109–120. tics and biases. Science, 1974, 185: 1124–1131.
Maghsoodloo, S. and Pallos, L. L. Asymptotic behavior of Kendall’s Wood, J. M. and Bootzin, R. R. The prevalence of nightmares and
partial rank correlation coefficient and additional quartile estimates. their independence from anxiety. J. Abnorm. Psychol., 1990, 99: 64–
J. Stat. Comput. Simul., 1981, 13: 41–48. 68.
Mendelson, W. B. Effects of time of night and sleep stage on Wood, J. M., Bootzin, R. R., Rosenhan, D., Nolen-Hoeksema, S. and
perception of sleep in subjects with sleep state misperception. Jourden, F. Effects of the 1989 San Francisco earthquake on
Psychobiology, 1998, 26: 73–78. frequency and content of nightmares. J. Abnorm. Psychol., 1992,
Miller, W. R. and DiPilato, M. Treatment of nightmares via relaxation 101: 219–234.
and desensitization: a controlled evaluation. J. Consult. Clin. Woodward, S. H., Arsenault, N. J., Murray, C. and Bliwise, D. L.
Psychol., 1983, 51: 870–877. Laboratory sleep correlates of nightmare complaint in PTSD
Mindell, J. A. and Barrett, K. M. Nightmares and anxiety in inpatients. Biol. Psychiatry, 2000, 48: 1081–1087.
elementary-aged children: is there a relationship? Child Care Health Zadra, A. and Donderi, D. C. Nightmares and bad dreams: their
Dev., 2002, 28: 317–322. prevalence and relationship to well-being. J. Abnorm. Psychol., 2000,
Miró, E. and Martı́nez, M. P. Affective and personality characteristics 109: 273–281.
in function of nightmare prevalence, nightmare distress, and Zadra, A. and Donderi, D. C. Affective content and intensity of
interference due to nightmares. Dreaming, 2005, 15: 89–105. nightmares and bad dreams. Sleep (abstr suppl), 2003, 26: A93–A94.
Nguyen, T. T., Madrid, S., Marquez, H. and Hicks, R. A. Nightmare Zadra, A. L., Assaad, J. -M., Nielson, T. A. and Donderi, D. C. Trait
frequency, nightmare distress, and anxiety. Percept. Mot. Skills, anxiety and its relation to nightmares, bad dreams and dream
2002, 95: 219–225. content. Sleep Res., 1995, 24: 150.
Nielsen, T., Laberge, L., Paquet, J., Tremblay, R. E., Vitaro, F. and
Montplaisir, J. Development of disturbing dreams during

Ó 2006 European Sleep Research Society, J. Sleep Res., 15, 117–124

You might also like