Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Food Quality and Preference 71 (2019) 279–285

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Food Quality and Preference


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/foodqual

Short Communication

Utilizing hedonic frame for projective mapping: A case study with Korean T
fermented soybean paste soup

Mi-Ran Kima, Kwang-Pyo Kimb, Seo-Jin Chunga,
a
Department of Nutritional Science & Food Management, Ewha Womans University, 52, Ewhayeodae-gil, Seodaemun-Gu, Seoul 120-750, Republic of Korea
b
Department of Food Science & Technology, Chonbuk National University, Republic of Korea

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Projective mapping (PM) based techniques are frequently used to develop consumer perception maps holistically
Hedonic-frame for identifying and characterizing samples with similar characteristics. In the present study, the criteria for
Projective mapping locating samples in projective mapping are narrowed from the original projective mapping methodology. This
Consumer acceptance study proposes the use of a hedonic frame (i.e. reasons for liking similarity), H_PM, and comparing it using a
Korean fermented soybean paste
sensory frame (i.e. sensory similarity), S_PM, with the aim of understanding how consumers perceive soups made
with various Korean fermented soybean pastes. The participants comprised a total of 69 consumers. Fifteen
fermented soybean paste products from different regions of Korea were selected. All consumers evaluated
samples using both S_PM and H_PM, which were conducted in separate sessions. The order of the two mapping
sessions was balanced between the subjects. In the S_PM method, subjects grouped samples with similar sensory
characteristics. In the H_PM method, subjects grouped samples which had similar reasons for liking or disliking
on a mapping sheet. Ultra flash profiling was conducted in both S_PM and H_PM after the mapping tasks.
Multiple factor analysis was used for statistical analysis. S_PM and H_PM resulted in different product positions.
Although some samples shared very similar sensory characteristics with each other in S_PM, distinct differences
appeared in the reasons for (dis)liking in H_PM. Critical attributes that affected sample positioning differed when
using different criteria for mapping the samples which resulted in discrete perceptual maps of S_PM and H_PM.
H_PM can identify important hedonic drivers of samples that may not be caught by a sensory based approach.

1. Introduction samples on a sheet.


Projective mapping has often been proposed as an alternative quick
Free sorting and projective mapping (a.k.a. napping) related tech- method for descriptive analysis (Valentin, Chollet, Lelièvre, & Abdi,
niques are popular methods for product characterization due to their 2012). Previous studies have investigated the reproducibility, validity,
simplicity as well as effectiveness in holistically discriminating samples and reliability of projective mapping techniques by comparing methods
(Risvik, McEwan, & Rødbotten, 1997; Tuorila & Monteleone, 2009; with descriptive analysis (Kennedy & Heymann, 2009; Heymann,
Cadoret & Lê, 2010). These methods basically identify and characterize Hopfer & Bershaw, 2014). The effectiveness of the projective mapping/
samples with (dis)similar characteristics (Faye et al., 2004; Abdi, napping methodology as a sensory analysis tool has also been compared
Valentin, Chollet, & Chrea, 2007; Lelièvre, Chollet, Abdi, & Valentin, with rapid descriptive analysis methods such as free multiple sorting,
2008). In global projective mapping techniques, subjects use holistic flash profiling, ultraflash profiling, conventional profiling, and the
similarity criteria, which include a broad spectrum of similarity in CATA method (Perrin et al., 2008; Nestrud & Lawless, 2010; Dehlholm,
sensory, hedonic or other aspects of food, to position samples on a Brockhoff, Meinert, Aaslyng, & Bredie, 2012; Santos et al., 2013; Liu,
sheet. More recently, researchers conducted projective mapping using a Grønbeck, Di Monaco, Giacalone, & Bredie, 2016). The usage of pro-
narrower criteria (i.e. partial projective mapping), specific reference jective mapping as an alternative for descriptive analysis implies that
sample (i.e. polarized projective mapping), or specific attribute (i.e. the sensory based similarity between samples are one of the main im-
freshness). Table 1 lists various projective mapping/napping techniques plicit frames adapted to determine the sample position on a map, al-
which utilize different evaluation frame of similarity for positioning the though subjects evaluate the samples holistically.


Corresponding author at: Department of Nutritional Science & Food Management, College of Science & Industry Convergence, Ewha Womans University, 52,
Ewhayeodae-gil, Seodaemun-Gu, Seoul 03760, Republic of Korea.
E-mail address: sc79d@ewha.ac.kr (S.-J. Chung).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2018.07.014
Received 12 January 2018; Received in revised form 31 July 2018; Accepted 31 July 2018
Available online 01 August 2018
0950-3293/ © 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
M.-R. Kim et al. Food Quality and Preference 71 (2019) 279–285

Table 1
List of projective mapping/napping techniques utilizing diverse evaluation frame for positioning the samples.
Type of method Evaluation Frame Publications

Global Projective Mapping/Napping Holistic_ Similarity/dissimilarity Risvik et al. (1994)


Projective Mapping Risvik et al. (1997)
Pagès (2005)
Perrin et al. (2008)
Dehlholm et al. (2012)
Kim et al. (2013)
Vidal, Cadena, Antunez, Gimenez, and Varela, Ares
(2014)
Marcano et al. (2015)
Varela et al. (2017)
Moelich et al. (2017)
Esmerino et al. (2017)
Projective Mapping/Napping with Sorting Holistic_Similarity/dissimilarity Pagès, Cadoret, and Le (2010)
Hopfer and Heymann (2013)
Partial Mapping/napping - Single attribute (Usage, Freshness)_similarity King et al. (1998)
- Single modality (appearance, aroma, taste, or texture)_Similarity Zhang, Lusk, Mirosa, and Oey (2016)
- Multi modalities (aroma & taste, palate or flavor & texture) _Similarity/ Dehlholm et al. (2012)
dissimilarity Reinbach, Giacalone, Ribeiro, Bredie, and Frøst
(2014)
Marcano et al. (2015)
Louw et al. (2015)
Moelich et al. (2017)
Partial Mapping/napping with Sorting - Single modality (appearance, aroma, taste, or texture)_Similarity Blancher, Clavier, Egoroff, Duineveld, and Parcon
- Multi modalities (aroma & taste, palate: flavor & texture) _Similarity/ (2012)
dissimilarity
Polarized projective mapping 3 Reference samples_ Similarity/dissimilarity Ares et al. (2013)
Horita et al. (2017)
Projective Mapping on Choice/Preference Preference_Similarity/dissimilarity Lezaeta et al. (2017)
Varela et al. (2017)
Structured/directed mapping Usage & liking King et al. (1998)
Healthy & Liking Varela and Salvador (2014)

When evaluating samples for projective mapping, subjects will use (called doenjang) is a very important culinary ingredient in Korean
their own holistic criteria, and as mentioned earlier, the types of criteria foods and the flavors of fermented soybean pastes can vary widely
involved and the weight of these criteria (if there are more than 2) will depending on the raw materials, fermentation methods, aging periods,
vary among subjects but will probably be consistent within a subject. and other factors (Kim & Rhee, 1988; Lee, 2004; Ahn et al., 2012; Jeon,
For example, foods that taste similar to each other should elicit similar Lee, Kim, & Kim, 2016).
hedonic responses. However, since the components that build similarity
perception of food are multifaceted, similar sample properties may not
necessarily mean similar sensory property nor similar hedonic proper- 2. Material and methods
ties of food. That is, the importance (or weight) of sample attributes
affecting the perceived similarity of samples from sensory aspects and 2.1. Sample and sample preparation
hedonic aspects may well be different. Thus, similar sensory char-
acteristics of samples will not always correspond to similar acceptance Fifteen Korean fermented soybean paste (doenjang) products were
ratings. And partly due to these reasons, researchers attempt to narrow chosen as samples of interest (Table 2). A large set of samples was
the frame for evaluating similarity among samples. obtained from major and local producers located in various regions of
A few researchers have attempted to incorporate hedonic aspects of Korea. Two products were national brands with the largest market
food evaluation into mapping/napping techniques. King, Cliff, and Hall shares in Republic of Korea. Nine products were produced by various
(1998) proposed a structured projective mapping method that defined small-scale local producers in five different provinces. Four products
the x and y axes as liking and usage, respectively, and asked the subjects were obtained from a single local producer and these were produced by
to position the samples in this two-dimensional space based on their similar methods but used different starter cultures to ferment the soy-
individual liking and usage. Varela and Salvador (2014) applied a si- bean paste.
milar approach to children. Very recently, projective mapping based on Since fermented soybean paste is a seasoning ingredient that is
preference was introduced and compared with classical projective rarely consumed on its own, samples were evaluated as soup prepara-
mapping resulting in a different perceptual map of the product (Varela tions (Kim, Hong, Song, Shin, & Kim, 2010; Kim & Lee, 2014). In the
et al., 2017). The present study proposes a projective mapping method first step, stock for fermented soybean paste soup was made by boiling
which utilizes hedonic frame (H_PM), and compared this with a pro- 160 g of dried anchovy and dried kelp at a ratio of 3:7 (Seokha, Busan,
jective mapping utilizing a sensory based similarity strategy (S_PM) Korea) in 4 L of water (Jeju Sam Da Soo, Kwangdong Pharmaceutical,
using soups made with fermented soybean paste samples as products of Jeju, Korea) for 40 min. The salinity of the stock was approximately
interest. S_PM was conducted using previously described protocols 0.09%. The fermented soybean paste sample was added to the stock at a
(Risvik, McEwan, Colwill, Rogers, & Lyon, 1994; Pagès, 2005; Park, Lê, concentration of 10%. The soup was then boiled for 5 min, and soup
Hong, & Kim, 2014). For the H_PM method, consumers were required to samples were kept in a heating cabinet (LH-1041G, Daeyeong E&B,
sort the samples based on similarity of reasons for liking and disliking Changwon, Korea) at 70 °C until being evaluated. Fifty milliliters of
the samples. each sample was poured into a disposable cup (diameter 7 cm, height
Fermented soybean paste soup was selected as a product category of 4 cm; Samboopack, Incheon, Korea) just before the sample evaluation,
interest in this experiment since Korean fermented soybean paste and each sample was labeled with a three-digit random code.

280
M.-R. Kim et al. Food Quality and Preference 71 (2019) 279–285

Table 2
Sample information, consumer acceptability scores and rank (between brackets) of 15 fermented soybean pastes.
Sample identification Province Ingredient Overall acceptability

JS Jeolla-do Soy bean, Sea salt 4.0bcd(11)


JN Soy bean, Sea salt, Purified water 5.0ghi(3)
KS Gyeongsang-do Meju/Soy bean, Sea salt 5.5i(1)
KN Meju, Sea salt, Purified water 3.1a(15)
CS Chungcheong-do Soy bean, Sea salt 4.6efgh(7)
CN Meju, Salt Purified water 3.8bc(12)
KW_1 Kangwon-do Meju, Sea salt 4.8fgh(5)
KW_2 Organic soy bean, Sea salt 4.1cde(10)
JJ Jeju-do Soy bean, Refined salt 3.2a(14)
MJ_1 Major company Soy bean, Purified water, Refined salt 4.7fgh(6)
MJ_2 Soy bean, Purified water, Sea salt, Soybean flour, Seed malt, ethanol 5.2hi(2)
R Jeolla-do Meju (Rice straw based starter culture1), Sea salt, Purified water, ethanol 3.4ab(13)
RA Meju (Rice straw based starter culture1 + A. oryzae), Sea salt, Purified water, ethanol 4.9fgh(4)
BA Meju (B. licheniformis + A. oryzae), Sea salt, Purified water, ethanol 4.4defg(8)
RBA Meju (Rice straw based starter culture1 + B. licheniformis + A. oryzae), Sea salt, Purified water, ethanol 4.3cdef(9)

Different superscripts within a column indicate significant difference according to Duncan’s multiple range test (p < 0.05). The samples were evaluated on a 9-point
hedonic scale (1: Dislike extremely, 9: Like extremely).

2.2. Subjects 20 cm based on its description, such that samples that elicited more
similar sensory properties were located closer to each other, while those
Seventy-eight female consumer subjects consisting of young female showing different sensory profiles were farther away from each other.
college students (n = 40, aged 20–29) as well as housewives (n = 38, The subjects were allowed to retaste the samples to confirm or change
aged 40–59) actively involved in cooking were recruited from local their positions. Once the subjects had finalized the positions of all of the
community. Subjects who were interested in fermented soybean paste samples, the three-digit code of each sample was marked on the sheet.
soup tasting participated in the study. A total of sixty-nine subjects The subjects were then asked to group the samples into as many groups
completed both the S_PM and H_PM experiments. as they wished based on the similarity of their sensory profiles. The
number of groups ranged from 2 to 14, and each sample could only
2.3. Procedure belong to a single group. After grouping the samples, the subjects
conducted ultra-flash profiling (Perrin & Pagès, 2009) freely describing
Fifteen fermented soybean paste samples were evaluated using both the sensory characteristics of the groups they had classified.
the S_PM and H_PM methods. Half of the subjects evaluated the samples
using S_PM initially and then re-evaluated the samples using H_PM 2.3.2. Hedonic based projective mapping (H_PM)
one week later. The other half of the subjects evaluated the samples first For the H_PM method, subjects were first asked to rate the overall
using H_PM followed by S_PM one week later. A detailed description of liking (OL) of each sample on a 9-point hedonic scale and the reasons
the tasting and evaluation procedure for each mapping method along for liking and disliking its taste. Similar to the S_PM method, a list of
with a handout describing the procedure were provided to the subjects sensory and non-sensory terms were given to the subjects during sample
at the start of each session. Preliminary experiments were carried out to evaluation upon request to help them describe the reasons for liking
find an optimal protocol for maintaining the correct temperature and disliking accurately. The subjects were then asked to position the
(70 °C) and to minimize variations between the panelists in sample samples on a rectangular sheet of the same size as that used in S_PM
handling. Unlike most PM studies, in which samples are served all at the based on the similarity and dissimilarity of the liking level(score) and
same time, samples were served in sequential monadic manner to the reasons for liking and disliking the samples; that is, samples rated
control the temperature of the samples during evaluation since tem- with similar liking scores due to similar reasons for (dis)liking were
perature drastically affects the flavors of soups. Additionally, partici- positioned close to each other, while samples with similar acceptance
pants commented on the need for writing down distinctive notes of ratings were separated when the reasons for (dis)liking differed.
each sample since there were too many samples to remember prior to Additionally, samples were separated when their acceptance ratings
positioning them on the sheet. Thus, in the main experiment, subjects were different but the reasons for (dis)liking them were similar.
were allowed to freely write memos of each sample in both projective Subjects were asked to retaste each sample before finalizing its position
mapping conditions (Fleming, Ziegler, & Hayes, 2015). The serving on the sheet. Subjects were allowed to retaste the samples as often as
order of the 15 samples was determined by a Williams Latin Square they wanted to. Again, the three-digit code of each sample was marked
design (Williams, 1949). Panelists were allowed to retaste the samples on the sheet when its position had been finalized, and the subjects then
during the projective mapping process. Upon request from the subjects, categorized the samples into 2–14 groups based on the similarity of the
warm samples were served again for re-evaluation when the samples hedonic level and the reasons for (dis)liking them, with each belonging
became cold. Warm water and unsalted water crackers (Carr’s Original to a single group. After grouping the samples, the subjects freely de-
Table Water, Carr’s of Carlisle, Carlisle, UK) were provided to cleanse scribed the reasons for (dis)liking the groups they had classified.
the palate between samples.
2.4. Statistical analysis
2.3.1. Sensory based projective mapping (S_PM)
The following procedure was implemented for the S_PM evaluations For both the S_PM and H_PM methods, multiple factor analysis
of 15 fermented soybean paste soup samples. Consumers tasted each (MFA) was applied to the projective mapping data that each panelist
sample and wrote descriptions of the perceived sensory characteristics. had generated to extract data for and produce the common perception
Upon request, a list of lexicons often used to describe the sensory map of the products and the corresponding distinctive attributes per-
characteristics of fermented soybean paste soup were provided to help ceived by the subjects (Pagès, 2005; Kim, Jombart, Valentin, & Kim,
the subjects to describe samples using appropriate attributes. The sub- 2013). In the case of descriptors obtained from the UFP, similar words
ject positioned each sample on a rectangular sheet of size 30 cm by were first merged together (e.g. sour and tart were merged to sour). The

281
M.-R. Kim et al. Food Quality and Preference 71 (2019) 279–285

Fig. 1. MFA plot of 15 fermented soybean paste samples (A) and the corresponding sensory descriptors used as supplementary variables (B) evaluated by S_PM
method.

merged attributes were reviewed by two other sensory researchers for understand the perceptual map of the samples. The most frequently
validation. Then the frequencies of attributes mentioned per sample mentioned reasons of liking attributes were meaty odor/flavor (15.2%)
were counted. In order to understand the common descriptors used to and mild odor/flavor (11.8%) while the most frequently mentioned
characterize each sample group, an attribute was included as a sup- disliking reasons were saltiness (27.2%), nauseating (14.6%), lack of
plementary variable in the MFA if that attribute was elicited by more flavor (13.5%), and sourness (13.2%) detected from the samples. The
than 10% of the subjects for at least one sample. subjects used similar number of words, 27.7 and 32.3 on the average, to
Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to analyze the effects describe all the samples using ultra flash profile in S_PM and H_PM,
of fermented soybean paste type and age group on the overall accep- respectively.
tance of the samples. Subject was considered as a random factor.
Additionally, two types of cluster analysis, hierarchical cluster analysis 3.2. Overall liking of fermented soybean paste samples
using Ward’s method and K-means cluster analysis, were conducted on
the overall liking scores of the 15samples to investigate the presence of The mean overall liking (OL) ratings and preference rank order of
heterogeneity in terms of consumers’ preference patterns for the sam- the samples are presented in Table 2. The OL rating varied significantly
ples. MFA was conducted using the FactoMineR package (Husson, with the fermented soybean paste sample type (F = 13.708,
Josse, Lê, & Mazet, 2011) of the R statistical system, ANOVA was im- p < 0.001). Samples KS and MJ_2 were commonly preferred, while
plemented using SPSS (version 23, IBM, Chicago, IL, USA), and Cluster samples KN, JJ, and R were on average the least preferred samples.
analysis was carried out by XLSTAT (Paris, France). When the effect of Scale usage between the two age groups significantly differed
age group on the preference and the perceptual configuration (S_PM (F = 15.602, p < 0.001) while the overall preference order of the
and H_PM) of samples were examined by ANOVA and MFA, the young samples was similar in the young and older subjects (F = 1.493,
and old subject groups showed little differences in sample perception. p = 0.107). When cluster analysis was conducted on the liking scores of
Thus the data from the two groups were merged for analysis and results the samples, both hierarchical and K-means commonly resulted in 2
from group as a whole are presented in the results and discussion sec- clusters consisting of approximately 70% of subjects in cluster 1 and
tion. 30% in cluster 2. This observation implies that the majority of the
subjects shared similar preference patterns for the samples.
3. Results
3.3. Sensory profile of fermented soybean paste by S_PM
3.1. Descriptors generated by S_PM and H_PM to profile fermented soybean
paste samples MFA maps of the 15 samples profiled by the S_PM method and the
corresponding sensory descriptors used as supplementary variables are
Applying S_PM to the 15 fermented soybean paste samples resulted shown in Fig. 1A and B, respectively. Factor 1 (F1) and factor 2 (F2)
in ninety-three attributes to characterize the samples. Sensory terms explained 17.1% and 11.5% of the total variance, respectively. When
with similar meanings were merged into 47 attributes, and ultimately perceived sensory characteristics were analyzed as supplementary
16 attributes were used as supplementary variables for MFA. Hedonic variables in the MFA, the darkness of color (brown vs. yellow) was the
or emotional terms were not mentioned by the subjects during the main driver characterizing the samples in F1. A brown color itself
profiling. Saltiness (25.4%), acetic acid odor/flavor (18.4%), brown showed strong positive correlations with saltiness, meju odor/flavor
(18.9%), meju (fermented soy) odor/flavor (14.4%), meaty odor/flavor and soy sauce odor/flavor attributes, while a yellow color showed a
(13.7%), and sweetness (10.8%) were the most frequently mentioned strong correlation with a roasted soybean odor. The negative axis of F2
attributes. showed high loadings with sour-related attributes (sourness, acetic acid
Concerning H_PM method, the subjects originally used 58 and 87 odor), while the positive F2 axis showed high loadings with sweet,
attributes to describe the reasons for liking and disliking the samples, umami taste, and meaty odor/flavor, which are reported to be distinct
respectively. Combining the attributes with similar meanings resulted characteristics of commercially manufactured fermented soybean paste
in 24 liking and 28 disliking attributes. At final analysis, 16 attributes (Chung & Chung, 2007).
(7 liking and 9 disliking) were used as supplementary variables to Concerning the sample map (Fig. 1A), samples were roughly

282
M.-R. Kim et al. Food Quality and Preference 71 (2019) 279–285

clustered into 4 distinct groups. Samples KN, KW_2, CS, and JJ were Samples KS (OL = 5.5) and MJ_2 (OL = 5.2) were ranked as the
located on the positive F1 axis and negative F2 axis. These samples most acceptable samples, and they were commonly liked because of
were distinctly characterized as salty [average of KN, KW_2, CS, & JJ their mild [average of KS & MJ_2 13.3% vs. average of all samples
39.6% vs. average of all samples 25.4%] and brown [24.0% vs. 18.9%], 7.4%], salty [13.3% vs. 7.4%], meaty flavor [16.7% vs. 11.7%], and
and marginally as soy sauce [8.7% vs. 5.6%], and sour [12.2% vs. umami taste [8.7% vs. 4.9%]. Rich [24.0% vs. 9.4%], ripened [17.3%
9.9%]. Samples CN, JN, KS, and KW_1 were perceived similarly to each vs. 6.3%], and familiar flavors [14.7% vs. 8.1%] were additional liking
other and these were loaded on the positive F1 and F2 axis largely due reasons of KS. Thus KS came closer with MJ_2 in H_PM than in S_PM
to their brown color [average of CN, JN, KS, & KW_1 26.0% vs. average which showed only marginal ellipses overlap between the two samples.
of all samples 18.9%] and marginally due to saltiness [28.1% vs. Regarding samples MJ_1 and MJ_2, which were perceptually very close
25.4%]. Samples MJ_1 and MJ_2 (national-brand products) were posi- in S_PM, the distance of the two samples increased although the ellipses
tioned at positive F2. These samples were described as eliciting sweet were slightly overlapping when evaluated in the hedonic contextual
[average of MJ_1 & MJ_2 22.9% vs. average of all samples 10.8%], framework. For lower rating samples, different attributes drove the
umami tastes [16.0% vs. 9.3%], and meaty odor/flavor [27.1% vs. disliking of different samples. Samples KN (OL = 3.1), CN (OL = 3.8),
13.7%] and were perceived as being very similar. Samples RA, RBA, and KW_2 (OL = 4.1) which were positioned on the negative F2 axis,
BA, and R were located on the negative F1 and F2 axis and were were clearly disliked due to their strong saltiness [average of KN, CN, &
characterized noticeably by yellowness [average of RA, RBA, BA, & R KW_2 40.9% vs. average of all samples 27.2%], and tendencies of eli-
19.1% vs. average of all samples 8.1%], and weakly by soybean odor citing slightly more sourness [14.7% vs. 13.2%], and strange flavor
[5.9% vs. 3.0%], and sourness [11.8% vs. 9.9%]. [11.6% vs. 9.3%] than other samples. Bitterness [KN 10.7% vs. average
of all samples 3.6%] and dark color [18.7% vs. 6.1%] detected in
sample KN were additional reasons for subjects disliking this sample.
3.4. Hedonic based sensory profile of fermented soybean paste by H_PM Samples JJ (OL = 3.2), R (OL = 3.4) and JS (OL = 4.0) were disliked
rather due to lack of flavor [17.8% vs. 13.5%], fermented odor/flavor
In the MFA, 28.3% of the total variance was explained by F1 [11.6% vs. 7.5%], and strange-flavor characteristics [13.3% vs. 9.3%],
(15.4%) and F2 (12.9%) when the samples were profiled by H_PM which were also associated with emotional terms such as “nauseating
method (Fig. 2A & B). Overall, the positive F1 axis was defined by the [29.8% vs. 14.6%]”. Close to these samples, samples BA (OL = 4.4), RA
reasons of liking attributes while the negative F1 axis was defined by (OL = 4.9), and RBA (OL = 4.3) were commonly positioned together
the reasons of disliking attributes. Liking familiar, rich, meaty, ripened because they were all disliked for lacking flavor [average of BA, RA, &
flavor and umami taste of samples were strongly loaded on the positive RBA 21.8% vs. average of all samples 13.5%] but were liked due to
F1 axis whereas disliking fermentation flavor, nauseating, strange having a mild flavor [15.6% vs. 11.7%].
flavor were strongly loaded on the negative F1 axis. F2 axis was
strongly related to attribute strength. That is, liking mild flavor and
disliking lack of flavor were positively loaded on F2, while disliking 4. Discussion
saltiness, bitterness, and color were negatively loaded.
Unlike the distinct clustering of samples observed in S_PM, the po- This study sorted and positioned 15 fermented soybean paste sam-
sition of the samples were scattered more in the diagonal direction of ples on mapping sheets by applying two types of criteria (sensory vs.
negative F1 and F2 to positive F1 and F2 in H_PM. The samples were hedonic), with these criteria used by consumers to profile their char-
aligned based on their acceptance levels of the samples. The lowest- acteristics. In most of the previous projective mapping studies, subjects
rated sample was located on the negative F1 and F2 axes. Starting from were instructed to locate samples based on the samples’ holistic simi-
these axes, the acceptance rating increased in the diagonal direction larities. While in most cases the subjects did sort the samples based on
toward the positive F1 and F2 axes, where the highest-rated sample was the similarities of sensory properties, in some instances hedonic or
located. Additionally, 2 clusters of samples were located on the right quality dimensions were identified in the results of sorted napping or
and left side of the diagonal. The samples on the right side of diagonal projective mapping (Ares, Varela, Rado, & Giménez, 2011; Torri et al.,
were characterized with reasons of liking attributes while samples on 2013). Kim et al. (2013) observed that consumers were able to sort
the left were delineated using reasons of disliking terms. samples based on sensory attributes related to the processing method

Fig. 2. MFA plot of 15 fermented soybean paste samples (A) and the corresponding sensory descriptors used as supplementary variables (B) evaluated by H_PM
method. Average consumer acceptability scores of 15 types of fermented soybean paste samples are shown in brackets.

283
M.-R. Kim et al. Food Quality and Preference 71 (2019) 279–285

when they were familiar with the samples. However, when the products 2017; Esmerino et al., 2017). Thus subjects first evaluate samples hol-
were unfamiliar, consumers had difficulty detecting subtle differences istically but eventually have to characterize the samples verbally. Ad-
between the samples, and the hedonic aspects of the samples influenced ditionally holistic nature of PM becomes somewhat analytical under the
their napping positions. context of partial PM since specific criteria are given for positioning the
The present study has proposed using similar liking levels(scores) samples (refer to Table 1). In the present study, subjects were rather
and reasons of liking as criteria for positioning samples in the projective urged to become analytical mind set in both S_PM and H_PM. Particu-
mapping procedure. Unlike global projective mapping, the present larly in H_PM, subjects were instructed to position the samples on very
H_PM method asks the subjects to locate the samples based on specific specific criteria, reasons of liking and disliking. In addition, the num-
criteria and implicitly urges subjects to become somewhat analytical bers of samples were relatively large and from a practical point of view,
rather than holistic. Applying two different strategies to locate samples it was difficult to locate 15 samples holistically by remembering all
on mapping sheet resulted in discrete product maps. There were several their characteristics. It has been reported that individual’s cognitive
occasions where the relative distances between the samples were in- style, wholistic vs. analytic, significantly affected sample positioning in
consistent between the S_PM and H_PM methods. Samples MJ_1 and PM (Varela et al., 2017). The performance of projective mapping, when
MJ_2, which were perceptually very close in S_PM, moved apart and subjects explicitly utilize holistic or analytical frame as a psychological
ellipses were only slightly overlapping when evaluated in the hedonic strategy for sample positioning, especially in the case of large sample
contextual framework. Subjects gave higher acceptance ratings for number should be investigated further in future research.
sample MJ_2 because it had fewer off-odors/off-flavors (MJ_2 4.0% vs. For both S_PM and H_PM methods, the total variance explained by
MJ_1 12.0%), as well as an artificial flavor (MJ_2 0.0% vs. MJ_1 6.7%) factor1 and 2 in the MFA was relatively low, being approximately
than MJ_1. These two samples were difficult to differentiate in the 28–29%. Factor 3 and 4 did not provide any additional insightful in-
sensory based mapping, but based on liking levels and reasons of liking, formation to understand the sample configuration thus these dimen-
they were perceived as being different when these traits were captured sions were not mentioned further. Total explained variance of dimen-
using the H_PM method. A similar example is samples CN and JN: in the sion 1 and 2 for multivariate analysis such as MFA and GPA are often
S_PM map, sample JN (which was liked by consumers) was situated critically dependent on the number of samples, latent structure of
very close to sample CN (which was relatively disliked). The presence sample set, and subjects’ level of training. Usually the larger the number
or absence of a meaty odor/flavor was an important factor determining of samples, the lower the % of cumulative explained variance. The total
the hedonic levels of samples in the case of samples JN (presence) and variance of dimension 1 and 2 in previous projective mapping or sorting
CN (absence). Similarly, samples KN, KW_2, CS were characterized as studies were approximately 38–56% when 7–9 samples were evaluated
salty, traditional soy sauce, and brown color, and perceived rather si- (Lelièvre et al., 2008; Lezaeta, Bordeu, Næs, & Varela, 2017). The total
milarly in terms of their sensory properties. However, the liking of these variance, however, dropped drastically to 21–31% when the sample
samples varied quite widely. Varela et al. (2017) also observed similar number increased to 12–18 (Hopfer & Heymann, 2013; Torri et al.,
discrepancies between sample mapping by global similarity and global 2013; Park et al., 2014). These are similar % level with the present
choice/preference. These disagreements in the product configurations study which evaluated 15 samples. Moreover, as reported previously for
between S_PM and H_PM are probably related to differences in the re- other projective mapping (or napping) studies, some subjects find the
lative importance given to certain sensory attribute when the task is task difficult and may require training in order to get used to the pro-
based on sensory vs. hedonic criteria. For example, samples may be cedure prior to participating in the actual experiments (Ares, Deliza,
liked/disliked for having more/less intensity of certain sensory attri- Barreiro, Giménez, & Gámbaro, 2010; Liu et al., 2016). Thus familiar-
butes, which may not be responsible for the main differences/simila- izing subjects to the procedures of S_PM or H_PM may increase the %
rities in terms of sensory attributes. variance explained by each dimension.
When profiling samples utilizing UFP method, different frameworks
applied during the evaluation had led to different descriptions for the 5. Conclusions
samples. That is, when subjects were inquired to describe sample
characteristics in sensory based framework they were not able to suf- This study has proposed a projective mapping method that accounts
ficiently express their perceptions using sensory terms—emotional he- for the hedonic aspects of product evaluation. The results showed that
donic terms were somewhat more useful. In the case of samples, R, JS, the subjects used different criteria to sort samples in the S_PM and
BA, RBA, which were disliked for their lack of flavor, very few de- H_PM methods. The inconsistent results between S_PM and H_PM imply
scriptors were used to characterize these samples other than yellowness that projective mapping utilizing sensory properties of samples as the
and chemical odor in S_PM. When they were evaluated from the per- main criteria for locating samples may overlook the hedonic drivers
spective of reasons for (dis)liking, the samples were described as having that significantly influence the liking of samples in the mapping task.
an unripened and fermented flavor and off-flavors, and were frequently The H_PM method seems to be appropriate when it is used to screen
associated with emotional terms such as “nauseating” and “disgusting.” samples for further analysis in consumer taste testing. Nevertheless,
The main advantage of H_PM is that the samples can be roughly asking subjects to evaluate a large number of samples by either S_PM or
grouped based on similar hedonic properties that are important drivers H_PM resulted in low % of total variance explained by the main two
influencing the acceptance of the samples. H_PM will be especially factors using MFA. Since the projective mapping task by itself may be
useful when researchers try to screen and select samples from a large more challenging for some subjects, initial familiarization to the eva-
pool of candidates to conduct further consumer studies within a target luation procedure itself may be necessary in order to reduce potential
product category. Projective mapping techniques have been proposed errors occurring due to the complicated nature of the task.
as screening tools to select samples from a large pool of candidates
(Louw et al., 2013; Moelich, Muller, Joubert, Næs, & Kidd, 2017). We Acknowledgements
suggest H_PM as a potential screening tool to select samples for a
consumer acceptance test. This research was supported by Sunchang Jangryu Co. We deeply
The basis of projective mapping is to have subjects evaluate samples thank the anonymous reviewer for the valuable comments.
under holistic mindset and intuitively position samples in such a way
that cannot be verbally or rationally explained. However in many References
global projective mapping, researchers step further and ask subjects to
characterize the samples often in the form of ultraflash profiling Abdi, H., Valentin, D., Chollet, S., & Chrea, C. (2007). Analyzing assessors and products in
(Marcano, Ares, & Fiszman, 2015; Varela et al., 2017; Moelich et al., sorting tasks: DISTATIS, theory and applications. Food Quality and Preference, 18,

284
M.-R. Kim et al. Food Quality and Preference 71 (2019) 279–285

627–640. Lezaeta, A., Bordeu, E., Næs, T., & Varela, P. (2017). Exploration of consumer perception
Ahn, J. B., Park, J. A., Jo, H. J., Woo, I. H., Lee, S. H., & Jang, K. I. (2012). Quality of Sauvignon Blanc wines with enhanced aroma properties using two different de-
characteristics and antioxidant activity of commercial fermented soybean paste and scriptive methods. Food Research International, 99, 186–197.
traditional Fermented soybean paste in Korea. The Korean Journal of Food and Liu, J., Grønbeck, M. S., Di Monaco, R., Giacalone, D., & Bredie, W. L. (2016).
Nutrition, 25, 142–148. Performance of Flash Profile and Napping with and without training for describing
Ares, G., Deliza, R., Barreiro, C., Giménez, A., & Gámbaro, A. (2010). Comparison of two small sensory differences in a model wine. Food Quality and Preference, 48, 41–49.
sensory profiling techniques based on consumer perception. Food Quality and Louw, L., Malherbe, S., Naes, T., Lambrechts, M., van Rensburg, P., & Nieuwoudt, H.
Preference, 21, 417–426. (2013). Validation of two Napping® techniques as rapid sensory screening tools for
Ares, G., de Saldamando, L., Vidal, L., Antunez, L., Gimenez, A., & Varela, P. (2013). high alcohol products. Food Quality and Preference, 30, 192–201.
Polarized projective mapping: Comparison with polarized sensory positioning ap- Louw, L., Oelofse, S., Naes, T., Lambrechts, M., van Rensburg, P., & Nieuwoudt, H.
proaches. Food Quality and Preference, 28, 510–518. (2015). Optimisation of the partial napping approach for the successful capturing of
Ares, G., Varela, P., Rado, G., & Giménez, A. (2011). Identifying ideal products using mouthfeel differentiation between brandy products. Food Quality and Preference, 41,
three different consumer profiling methodologies. Comparison with external pre- 245–253.
ference mapping. Food Quality and Preference, 22, 581–591. Marcano, J., Ares, G., & Fiszman, S. (2015). Comparison of partial and global projective
Blancher, G., Clavier, B., Egoroff, C., Duineveld, K., & Parcon, J. (2012). A method to mapping with consumers: A case study with satiating cheese pies. Food Research
investigate the stability of a sorting map. Food Quality and Preference, 23, 36–43. International, 67, 323–330.
Cadoret, M., & Lê, S. (2010). The sorted napping: A new holistic approach in sensory Moelich, E. I., Muller, M., Joubert, E., Næs, T., & Kidd, M. (2017). Validation of projective
evaluation. Journal of Sensory Studies, 25, 637–658. mapping as potential sensory screening tool for application by the honeybush herbal
Chung, L., & Chung, S. J. (2007). Cross-cultural comparisons among the sensory char- tea industry. Food Research International, 99, 275–286.
acteristics of fermented soybean using Korean and Japanese descriptive analysis Nestrud, M. A., & Lawless, H. T. (2010). Perceptual mapping of apples and cheeses using
panel. Journal of Food Science. 72, S676–S688. projective mapping and sorting. Journal of Sensory Studies, 25, 390–405.
Dehlholm, C., Brockhoff, P. B., Meinert, L., Aaslyng, M. D., & Bredie, W. L. (2012). Rapid Pagès, J. (2005). Collection and analysis of perceived product inter-distances using
descriptive sensory methods–comparison of free multiple sorting, partial napping, multiple factor analysis: Application to the study of 10 white wines from the Loire
napping, flash profiling and conventional profiling. Food Quality and Preference, 26, Valley. Food quality and preference, 16, 642–649.
267–277. Pagès, J., Cadoret, M., & Le, S. (2010). The sorted napping: A new holistic approach in
Esmerino, E. A., Tavares Filho, E. R., Carr, T. B., Ferraz, J. P., Silva, H. L. A., Pinto, L. P. F., sensory evaluation. Journal of Sensory Studies, 25, 637–658.
... Bolini, H. M. A. (2017). Consumer-based product characterization using Pivot Park, H. S., Lê, S., Hong, J. H., & Kim, K. O. (2014). Sensory perception of Yackwa
Profile, Projective Mapping and Check-all-that-apply (CATA): A comparative case (Korean traditional fried cookie) by consumer groups of different age using the sorted
with Greek yogurt samples. Food Research International, 99, 375–384. napping procedure. Journal of Sensory Studies, 29, 425–434.
Faye, P., Brémaud, D., Daubin, M. D., Courcoux, P., Giboreau, A., & Nicod, H. (2004). Perrin, L., & Pagès, J. (2009). Construction of a product space from the ultra-flash pro-
Perceptive free sorting and verbalization tasks with naive subjects: An alternative to filing method: Application to 10 red wines from the Loire Valley. Journal of Sensory
descriptive mappings. Food Quality and Preference, 15, 781–791. Studies, 24, 373–395.
Fleming, E. F., Ziegler, G. R., & Hayes, J. E. (2015). Check-all-that-apply (CATA), sorting, Perrin, L., Symoneaux, R., Maître, I., Asselin, C., Jourjon, F., & Pagès, J. (2008).
and polarized sensory positioning (PSP) with astringent stimuli. Food Quality and Comparison of three sensory methods for use with the Napping® procedure: Case of
Preference, 45, 41–49. ten wines from Loire valley. Food Quality and Preference, 19(1), 1–11.
Heymann, H., Hopfer, H., & Bershaw, D. (2014). An exploration of the perception of Reinbach, H. C., Giacalone, D., Ribeiro, L. M., Bredie, W. L. P., & Frøst, M. B. (2014).
minerality in white wines by projective mapping and descriptive analysis. Journal of Comparison of three sensory profiling methods based on consumer perception: CATA,
Sensory Studies, 29, 1–13. CATA with intensity and Napping®. Food Quality and Preference, 32, 160–166.
Hopfer, H., & Heymann, H. (2013). A summary of projective mapping observations – The Risvik, E., McEwan, J. A., Colwill, J. S., Rogers, R., & Lyon, D. H. (1994). Projective
effect of replicates and shape, and individual performance measurements. Food mapping: A tool for sensory analysis and consumer research. Food Quality and
Quality and Preference, 28, 164–181. Preference, 5, 263–269.
Horita, C. N., Esmerino, E. A., Vidal, V. A. S., Farah, J. S., Amaral, G. V., Bolini, H. M. A., Risvik, E., McEwan, J. A., & Rødbotten, M. (1997). Evaluation of sensory profiling and
... Pollonio, M. A. R. (2017). Sensory profiling of low sodium frankfurter containing projective mapping data. Food Quality and Preference, 8, 63–71.
garlic products: Adequacy of polarized projective mapping compared with trained Santos, B. A., Pollonio, M. A. R., Cruz, A. G., Messias, V. C., Monteiro, R. A., Oliveira, T. L.
panel. Meat Science, 131, 90–98. C., ... Bolini, H. M. A. (2013). Ultra-flash profile and projective mapping for de-
Husson, F., Josse, J., Lê, S., & Mazet, J. (2011). FactoMineR: Multivariate Exploratory scribing sensory attributes of prebiotic mortadellas. Food Research International, 54,
Data Analysis and Data Mining with R. R package version 1.16. http://factominer. 1705–1711.
free.fr,http://www.agrocampus-rennes.fr/math/. Torri, L., Dinnella, C., Recchia, A., Naes, T., Tuorila, H., & Monteleone, E. (2013).
Jeon, H., Lee, S., Kim, S., & Kim, Y. (2016). Quality characteristics of modified fermented Projective Mapping for interpreting wine aroma differences as perceived by naïve and
soybean paste and traditional fermented soybean paste. Journal of the Korean Society experienced assessors. Food Quality and Preference, 29, 6–15.
of Food Science and Nutrition, 45, 1001–1009. Tuorila, H., & Monteleone, E. (2009). Sensory food science in the changing society:
Kennedy, J., & Heymann, H. (2009). Projective mapping and descriptive analysis of milk Opportunities, needs, and challenges. Trends in Food Science & Technology, 20, 54–62.
and dark chocolates. Journal of Sensory Studies, 24, 220–233. Valentin, D., Chollet, S., Lelièvre, M., & Abdi, H. (2012). Quick and dirty but still pretty
Kim, H. G., Hong, J. H., Song, C. K., Shin, H. W., & Kim, K. O. (2010). Sensory char- good: a review of new descriptive methods in food science. International Journal of
acteristics and consumer acceptability of fermented soybean paste (Fermented soy- Food Science and Technology, 47, 1563–1578.
bean paste). Journal of Food Science, 75, S375–S383. Varela, P., Antúnez, L., Berget, I., Oliveira, D., Christensen, K., Vidal, L., ... Ares, G.
Kim, M. J., & Rhee, H. S. (1988). The contents of free amino acids nucleotides and their (2017). Influence of consumers' cognitive style on results from projective mapping.
related compounds in soypaste made from native and improved meju and soypaste Food Research International, 99, 693–701.
product. Journal of the Korean Society of Food Science and Nutrition, 17, 69–72. Varela, P., Berget, I., Hersleth, M., Carlehög, M., Asioli, D., & Næs, T. (2017). Projective
Kim, M. K., & Lee, K. G. (2014). Correlating consumer perception and consumer ac- mapping based on choice or preference: An affective approach to projective mapping.
ceptability of traditional fermented soybean paste in Korea. Journal of Food Science, Food Research International, 100, 241–251.
79, S2330–S2336. Varela, P., & Salvador, A. (2014). Structured sorting using pictures as a way to study
Kim, Y. K., Jombart, L., Valentin, D., & Kim, K. O. (2013). A cross-cultural study using nutritional and hedonic perception in children. Food Quality and Preference, 37,
Napping®: Do Korean and French consumers perceive various green tea products 27–34.
differently? Food Research International, 53, 534–542. Vidal, L., Cadena, R. S., Antunez, L., Gimenez, A., Varela, P., & Ares, G. (2014). Stability
King, M. C., Cliff, M. A., & Hall, J. W. (1998). Comparison of projective mapping and of sample configurations from projective mapping: How many consumers are ne-
sorting data collection and multivariate methodologies for identification of similarity cessary? Food Quality and Preference, 34, 79–87.
of use of snack bars. Journal of Sensory Studies, 13(3), 347–358. Williams, E. J. (1949). Experimental designs balanced for the estimation of residual ef-
Lee, S. B. (2004). Fermented foods. Seoul: HyoIl65–82. fects of treatments. Australian Journal of Chemistry, 2(2), 149–168.
Lelièvre, M., Chollet, S., Abdi, H., & Valentin, D. (2008). What is the validity of the sorting Zhang, T., Lusk, K., Mirosa, M., & Oey, I. (2016). Understanding young immigrant
task for describing beers? A study using trained and untrained assessors. Food Quality Chinese consumers’ freshness perceptions of orange juices: A study based on concept
and Preference, 19, 697–703. evaluation. Food Quality and Preference, 48, 156–165.

285

You might also like