Brooks, 1999

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

Developmental Psychology

Copyright 1999 by the American Psychological Association, Inc


1999, Vol. 35, No. 1,29-44
0012-1649/99/S3.00

Young Children Learn to Produce Passives With Nonce Verbs

Patricia J. Brooks Michael Tomasello


The College of Staten Island of the Emory University
City University of New York

Younger and older children (mean ages = 2 years 11 months and 3 years 5 months) learned 2 nonce verbs
in a full passive or active transitive construction. When asked patient-focused questions encouraging
passive-voice replies (e.g., "What happened to the ball?") or agent-focused questions encouraging
active-voice replies (e.g., "What did Elmo do?"), children used a variety of strategies to meet the
demands of the questions, usually without changing the construction in which the verb occurred. In
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Study 2 in which passive and active constructions were primed, 40% of the almost 3-year-old children
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

used an active-introduced verb in a passive construction and 35% used a passive-introduced verb in an
active transitive construction when discourse demands encouraged them to do so. Thus, before their 3rd
birthdays, some children have an understanding of the passive and active transitive constructions general
enough to support productive usages with newly learned verbs.

The English passive comprises a family of related constructions although they occasionally produce truncated passives somewhat
whose primary function is to focus attention on the patient of a earlier (often with get, as in The bunny got caught; Harris & Flora,
transitive action and what happened to it (defocusing the agent in 1982). In elicitation experiments in which discourse pressure is
the process; Shibitani, 1985). Thus, in the full passive construc- used to encourage children to produce passives (e.g., children are
tion, The cat was chased by the dog, the focus is on the cat and asked questions such as "What happened to the bunny?"), the
what happened to it, rather than on the dog's act of chasing as in majority of 3-year-olds can produce truncated passives with fa-
the active description of this event. In the so-called truncated miliar verbs, with about one quarter able to produce full passives
passive construction, The cat was chased up a tree, the fact that a (Marchman, Bates, Burkhardt, & Good, 1991; see also Horgan,
dog did the chasing is not indicated at all. In addition to these two 1978). The following are the most obvious reasons for the rela-
general forms, Budwig (1990) showed that the "get" and "be" tively late acquisition of full passives in English: (a) It is a marked
forms of the passive are prototypically associated with distinct construction, containing several additional linguistic elements rel-
discourse perspectives. Thus, the prototypical "get" passive in ative to active-voice constructions (an auxiliary be or get, a marked
Wendy got drenched in the sudden downpour tends to be used form of the verb in the past participle, and the preposition by to
when an animate patient is adversely affected by an inanimate explicitly mark the agent), and (b) it is not a frequent construction
entity or nonagent source. In contrast, the "be" passive in This in child-directed speech. Empirical support for the latter point is
book was written by a warped personality, or simply This book supplied by studies of children's early linguistic experience in
was written in three weeks, is prototypically used when an inani- which full passives constitute only a tiny fraction of the speech
mate entity undergoes a more neutral change of state in which the children hear. In an informal study reported by Maratsos, Fox,
agent causing the change of state is unknown or irrelevant. In most Becker, and Chalkley (1985), 37.5 hr of parent-child interaction
accounts, adjectival passives such as / am scared of dogs are yielded a total of one full passive in the speech directed to the
distinguished from other passive constructions. child. In a more systematic study of the Adam, Eve, and Sarah
English-speaking children typically do not produce full passive corpora in the CHILDES database, Gordon and Chafetz (1990)
sentences in their spontaneous speech until 4 or 5 years of age, examined 86,655 child-directed utterances (with the children rang-
ing in age from 1 year 6 months to 5 years 1 month) and identified
only four full passive utterances; there were 84 tokens, represent-
Patricia J. Brooks, Department of Psychology, Sociology, and Anthro- ing 52 types of truncated passives.
pology, The College of Staten Island of the City University of New York;
Michael Tomasello, Department of Psychology, Emory University. It is important to note that children acquiring certain other
This work was supported by a grant from the Spencer Foundation. languages, mostly non-Indo-European, do not experience the same
We thank the parents, teachers, and children at the Oak Grove Preschool obstacles to the acquisition of passives as children acquiring En-
and Day Care Center and the Decatur First United Methodist Preschool and glish, and so they typically learn to produce them much earlier in
Parent's Morning Out programs in metropolitan Atlanta, Georgia. We also development. This is true for children learning Inuktitut (Allen &
thank Deanne Swan for her assistance with data collection and Felix
Crago, 1996), K'iche' Mayan (Pye & Quixtan Poz, 1988), Sesotho
Scherer for constructing wonderful toys for us.
(Demuth, 1989, 1990), and Zulu (Suzman, 1985). For example,
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Patricia J.
Brooks, Department of Psychology, Sociology, and Anthropology, The Allen and Crago (1996) found that a child at age 2 years 0 months
College of Staten Island of the City University of New York, 2800 Victory to 2 years 9 months (as well as two slightly older children)
Boulevard 4S-223, Staten Island, New York 10314. Electronic mail may be produced both truncated and full passives quite regularly. Al-
sent to pbrooks@postbox.csi.cuny.edu. though a majority of these were with familiar actional verbs, also

29
30 BROOKS AND TOMASELLO

present were passives with experiential predicates and several to produce passive sentences with that verb. If children below 3
clearly innovative forms with verbs that do not passivize in adult years of age are productive with the passive construction, one
Inuktitut, indicating the productivity of the construction for these could argue that a generalized representation of this construction
children. The reasons for this precocity relative to English- underlies even their very earliest productions of the passive. If they
speaking children would seem to involve both of the factors are not productive, then a verb-by-verb account would be sup-
invoked to explain the late acquisition of English passives: (a) ported, implying that it is only after children have learned to
Inuktitut passives are very common in child-directed speech; and produce passive sentences with some number of verbs individually
(b) passive utterances are actually simpler than active-voice con- that their understanding of the construction becomes general
structions in Inuktitut because the passivized verb has to agree enough to support productive usages. It should be noted that in our
only with the grammatical subject, whereas the active transitive study the children who first learned to produce passive utterances
verb has to agree with both the subject and the object. containing the nonce verbs were also placed under discourse
Given that English-speaking children hear passive sentences pressure to produce active transitive sentences with their newly
only infrequently and are relatively late in acquiring them, it is not learned verbs. We were thus able to test the same two
surprising that they are not productive with the passive construc- hypotheses—verb-by-verb acquisition or productive use of a gen-
eralized construction—for the active transitive construction as
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tion until relatively late in development. A number of researchers


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

have observed that English-speaking children occasionally pro- well.


duce passives with verbs that in adult English do not passivize, for Our third question was the extent to which young children
example, He get died and It was bandaided, indicating some would use pronouns for agents and patients in passive and active
productivity with the construction (Bowerman, 1982, 1988; Clark, transitive constructions, and whether their use of pronouns would
1982). Such utterances are produced almost exclusively by chil- interact with the different kinds of questions we asked them (i.e.,
dren over 3 years of age, mostly by children over 4 years of age agent-focused questions such as "What did [agent] do?"; patient-
(see the compilation of Pinker, Lebeaux, & Frost, 1987). In the focused questions such as "What happened to [patient]?"; and
only experimental study to examine the issue of productivity, neutral questions such as "What happened?"). The issue of pro-
Pinker et al. (1987) trained 3- to 4-year-old English-speaking nouns is particularly important because of the different ways in
children to produce active sentences with nonce verbs, as in The which passive and active constructions affect the discourse prom-
boy pilked the dog. They then put the children under discourse inence of the agent and patient. This would be expected to interact
pressure to produce a passive with that novel verb by asking, for with the level of specificity with which agents and patients are
example, "What happened to the dog?" Even though they had linguistically indicated using full nouns and pronouns.
never heard this verb in the passive voice, children as young as 3
years 10 months were successful in producing passives, indicating
Study 1
that they came to the experiment in possession of a generalized
understanding of the passive construction to which they could Method
assimilate the new verb. Unfortunately, it was not reported
whether any of the children's utterances were full passives. Participants. Twenty-eight younger children (mean age = 34.6
months, range = 33-36 months; 14 boys and 14 girls) and 28 older
In the present study, we asked three questions about young children (mean age = 40.9 months, range = 39-44 months;14 boys and 14
children's acquisition of the English passive. First, we wanted to girls) participated in the study. One boy in the older group did not use the
know if very young children—below 3 years of age in the young- target verbs during the course of the experiment and was replaced. The
est age group—could learn to produce full passive sentences with majority of the children came from White, middle-class families and were
the appropriate kind of exposure and training. If they could, it recruited and tested at four preschools and day-care centers in the Atlanta,
would provide evidence that the linguistic complexities of the Georgia, metropolitan area. Some children were recruited through the
English passive are not an insurmountable obstacle for children Emory University Child Participant Pool and were tested at the Department
this young, but that the essential problem is the low frequency with of Psychology at Emory University.
which they are typically exposed to passives in their natural Materials and design. Two novel verbs, meek and tarn, were used to
describe two transitive actions that could be performed with two novel
environments. Unlike previous studies with older children that
toys. Meeting was used to refer to a puppet pulling a small object up a
simply provided them with "input" of full passive sentences, we
ramp (the object was inside a clear jar attached to a string and was pulled
chose to train children in a more discourse-intensive manner in up through a clear tube attached to the ramp). For each enactment, 1 of 10
which the adult asked questions about various elements of the different puppets representing familiar animals and characters was used to
novel event in a way that helped children to build up gradually to pull the object up the ramp (i.e., as agent), and 1 of 10 inanimate objects
the full passive structure (e.g., "Who got meeked?" or "The car got (e.g., a toy banana, ball, bus, stove) was used as the object to be pulled up
meeked by who?"). Moreover, we also set up a situation in which (i.e., as patient). Tamming was used to refer to the action of an object
both the agent and the patient were variable across different attached to a pendulum that hit and knocked another object off a pedestal.
enactments of the event so that explicit mention of the participants For each enactment, 1 of 10 different inanimate toy objects was attached to
was clearly discourse appropriate (e.g., "Look! This time the ball the pendulum using Velcro and was used to do the hitting (i.e., as agent),
and 1 of 10 animate objects (e.g., a toy robot, frog, dog, dinosaur) was set
is getting meeked by the frog!").
on the pedestal to be knocked off (i.e., as patient). Thus, for the action
Our second question concerned the extent to which young called meeking, all of the possible actors were animate and the patients
children could be productive in their use of the English passive. were inanimate; for tamming, the actors were inanimate and the patients
We thus also taught some children to produce active-voice transi- were animate.
tive utterances with the same novel verbs and then, as in previous There were two experimental conditions: the passive training group and
studies with older children, placed them under discourse pressure the active transitive training group. Each child learned both of the exper-
YOUNG CHILDREN LEARN TO PRODUCE PASSIVES 31
imental verbs in the same way, that is, in the get passive construction (e.g., consisted of an agent-focused question, which directed attention to the
"The stove's gonna get meeked by Ernie" and "The dog got tammed by the object or puppet performing the action (e.g., "What did Big Bird do?").
clock") or in the active transitive construction (e.g., "Ernie's meeking the The second session began with two sequences of models for the first
stove" and "The clock tammed the dog"). At each age, 20 children were in novel verb. The sequences again consisted of eight utterances in which the
the passive training group and 8 children were in the active transitive experimenter modeled usage of the verbs and scaffolded the child's dis-
training group. Children were randomly assigned to passive and active course. In addition, neutral and matching questions were administered with
transitive training groups with the conditions that the mean ages for the each sequence of models. After the two sequences were completed, an
passive and active groups were equal and both groups had equal numbers elicited production task was administered. In the elicited production task,
of boys and girls. the experimenter did not generate any utterances containing the novel
Training and elicitation procedures. Children were tested individually verbs. After two new objects were introduced and placed on the appropriate
in two 30-min sessions conducted within the span of 1 week. Two tape apparatus, the experimenter asked three questions in a random order. The
recorders were used to record each session; in addition, a handwritten log questions were (a) a neutral question ("What happened?" or "What is going
was kept of the children's utterances and relevant gestures (e.g., pointing). to happen now?"), (b) a patient-focused question (e.g., "What happened to
Two experimenters conducted the procedure: One experimenter interacted [patient]?" or "What is going to happen to [patient] now?"), and (c) an
with the child, whereas the other wrote down what the child said. The first agent-focused question ("What did [agent] do?" or "What is [agent] going
to do now?"). The patient-focused question matched the discourse perspec-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

experimenter's goal was to teach the children how to use the verbs through
tive of the passive construction and mismatched the discourse perspective
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

scaffolded discourse. In the first session, 10 sequences of models were


provided for each novel verb with each sequence consisting of eight of the active transitive construction, whereas the agent-focused question
utterances describing the action of a particular pair of objects. For instance, mismatched the discourse perspective of the passive construction while
children exposed to the novel verb meeking in the passive construction matching that of the active transitive construction. The purpose of the
would hear the experimenter (E) say a sequence of models such as the elicited production task was (a) to determine whether children would use
following: the novel verbs in the targeted constructions under conditions in which the
experimenter no longer provided models and scaffolded discourse, and (b)
1. Look, the car is going to get meeked. to examine children's strategies in answering questions that mismatched
2. The car is going to get meeked by Big Bird. the discourse perspective of the construction in which they had heard the
3. What's going to get meeked? (E points to the car) novel verbs used. The elicited production task was administered three times
with unique pairs of objects, providing the child with three opportunities to
4. That's right, the car is going to get meeked.
answer neutral, matching, and mismatching questions with each novel
5. The car is going to get meeked by who? (E points to Big Bird)
verb. The order of the questions was randomized such that each question
6. Yes, the car is getting meeked by Big Bird, (while performing action)
was presented first, second, or third an approximately equal number of
7. Did you see what got meeked by Big Bird? (E points to the car)
times. After the elicited production task was administered three times for
8. Exactly! The car got meeked by Big Bird.
the first verb, the procedure (two complete discourse sequences followed
Children exposed to meeking in the active transitive construction would by elicited production) was repeated for the second verb.
hear a sequence such as: Observational and analytic procedures. During both sessions, the sec-
1. Look, Big Bird is going to meek something. ond experimenter wrote down all of the child's utterances that used one of
2. Big Bird is going to meek the car. the experimental verbs. The accuracy of the handwritten log was subse-
3. Who's going to meek the car? (E points to Big Bird) quently checked against the audiotaped recording. In all cases, the hand-
4. That's right, Big Bird is going to meek the car. written log was corrected if the audiotape indicated that the child had said
something in addition to, or different from, what was recorded by hand. In
5. Big Bird is going to meek what? (E points to the car)
general, the handwritten log was highly accurate except on rare occasions
6. Yes, Big Bird is meeking the car. (while performing action)
when the child produced several utterances in a rapid sequence.
7. Did you see who meeked the car? (E points to Big Bird)
In all analyses, we examined children's responses for the elicited pro-
8. Exactly! Big Bird meeked the car.
duction task to determine the effects of training condition (passive or
In the first session, children heard 10 sequences of models for the first verb, active), age (younger or older), and question type (neutral, matching, or
then 10 for the second verb. The order of actions (meeking and tamming) mismatching). Children's multiword utterances with the novel verbs during
varied across participants and sessions. In general, the experimenter started this task were categorized as passive (full or truncated), active transitive, or
with whichever toy the child approached first to maximize the child's intransitive. We also tabulated children's errors with the target verbs (e.g.,
engagement with the activity. usage of an incorrect noun or nonce verb) and their utterances containing
English verbs. Occasionally a child's reply to a given question consisted of
Children's participation in the discourse was encouraged in several
more than one complete utterance, and so, although children were given six
ways. First, at regular intervals the experimenter invited the child to name
opportunities to answer each question type (three opportunities per novel
the action by saying, "This is called meeking (or tamming). Can you say
verb), the actual number of utterances recorded per question type some-
meeking? Say meeking." Second, at regular intervals the experimenter
times exceeded six utterances. The frequencies of the most common
paused in mid-sentence (with an encouraging final intonation contour) to
utterance types (passive, active transitive, intransitive, English verb, and
let the child finish the sentence (e.g., "The car is gonna get meeked by . . . "
errors) were analyzed using mixed-design analyses of variance (ANOVAs)
or later "The car is gonna.. ."). Third, after one or two sequences of
with training condition and age group as between-subjects variables and
models were completed, the experimenter provided an additional opportu-
question type as within-subjects variable.
nity for children to use the novel verb spontaneously by asking a neutral
question such as "What happened?" or "What is going to happen now?"
And finally, to further encourage children to use the novel verb in the target Results
construction, in the 6th through 10th sequences, we introduced a "match-
ing" question. For children in the passive training condition, the matching Results are presented for each of the types of sentence construc-
question consisted of a patient-focused question, which directed the child's tions children produced with the experimental verbs, with analyses
attention to the object affected by the action (e.g., "What happened to the focusing on the discourse contexts in which they produced them.
car?"). For children in the active training condition, the matching question We start by presenting the results of preliminary analyses that
32 BROOKS AND TOMASELLO

show that children demonstrated equal proficiency in learning the were analyzed in a mixed-design ANOVA with age (younger vs.
two novel verbs. older children) and training group (passive vs. active) as between-
To compare the numbers of utterances children produced with subjects variables and question type (neutral, patient, or agent) as
each of the two verbs, we conducted an ANOVA with age (young- a within-subjects variable. First, children's tendency to use the
er vs. older children) and training group (passive vs. active) as novel verbs in the passive construction varied greatly as a function
between-subjects variable and verb (meek vs. tarn) as a within- of training condition, F(l, 52) = 53.30, p < .001. The majority of
subjects variable. The dependent variable was the total number of children (90%) who heard the experimenter use the novel verbs in
multiword utterances containing the target verb (meek or tarn). The a passive construction produced passives themselves in the elicited
ANOVA revealed significant main effects of age and training production task (with the passive-trained group averaging about 10
group only, and no significant interactions. The older group pro- passive utterances per child), whereas only 2 children (12%) who
duced an average of 8.3 utterances with each verb, whereas the heard the novel verbs used in an active transitive construction used
younger children averaged only 6.0 utterances per verb, F(l, 52) = the verbs in a passive construction (with the active-trained group
8.23, p < .01. Children in the active training group averaged 8.7 averaging less than one quarter of one passive utterance per child).
utterances with each verb, with children in the passive training Frequencies of passives also varied as a function of the question
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

group averaging only 6.6 utterances per verb, F(l, 52) = 7.23, p < posed, with the vast majority of children's passives coming in
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

.01. Children produced statistically equivalent numbers of utter- response to neutral or patient-focused questions (86%). This effect
ances with meek and tarn, averaging 7.5 utterances with meek was highly significant, F(2, 104) = 25.66, p < .001, with planned
and 6.9 utterances with tarn, F(l, 52) < 1. comparisons indicating that children produced roughly equal num-
To confirm this result for children's production of passive bers of passive utterances in response to neutral and patient-
utterances only, we conducted an additional ANOVA with age as focused questions, F(l, 104) < 1, and fewer passives in response
a between-subjects variable and verb as a within-subjects variable. to agent-focused questions than for neutral or patient-focused
For this ANOVA, the dependent variable was the number of questions, Fs(l, 104) > 34.33, ps < .001. There was also a
passive utterances produced by children in the passive training significant interaction between the variables of training group and
group only (children in the active training group were not included question type, F(2, 104) = 22.46, p < .001, which was due to the
because they produced almost no passive utterances). The fact that almost no passive utterances were produced by the active-
ANOVA revealed no main effects or interactions. Children in the trained children. Thus, the effect of question type was carried
passive training group averaged 5.2 passive utterances with meek almost entirely by children in the passive training condition. There
and 4.7 passive utterances with tarn, F(l, 38) = 1.17, ns. Older was no main effect of age, nor did age interact with either of the
children produced an average of 5.4 passives with each novel verb other variables.
with younger children averaging 4.5 passives per verb, F(l, 38) = Table 2 presents the average numbers of full and truncated
1.19, ns. passives produced by the younger and older children of the passive
Because the preliminary analyses found no significant differ- training group (children in the active training group were not
ences in utterance frequencies for the verbs meek and tarn, utter- included because they produced a total of only three passive
ances for both verbs were collapsed in all of the analyses reported utterances, all of which were truncated). These data were analyzed
below. We look first at children's passive utterances, then at active in a mixed-design ANOVA with age as a between-subjects vari-
transitive utterances, then at intransitives, and finally at sentences able and question type and passive type (full vs. truncated) as
with English verbs in which children avoided use of the experi- within-subjects variables. The main effect of passive type was
mental verbs altogether. We conclude with a look at children's use significant, with children producing more full than truncated pas-
of pronouns in passive and active transitive utterances. sives, F(l, 38) = 11.68,/? < .01. Also, the main effect of question
Passives. Table 1 presents the average numbers of passive type was significant, F(2, 76) = 61.97, p < .001, which matched
utterances produced in the elicited production task. These data the results for the overall analysis reported earlier. The interaction

Table 1
Average Numbers of Passive Utterances and Percentage of Children in the Passive and Active
Training Groups Who Produced Them in Study 1

Question type

Neutral Patient Agent Total

Group M % M % M % M %

Passive training
Younger 3.75 85 3.85 85 1.55 45 9.15 85
Older 4.60 95 5.00 95 1.20 40 10.80 95
Active training
Younger 0.00 0 0.12 12 0.00 0 0.12 12
Older 0.12 12 0.12 12 0.00 0 0.25 12

Note, n = 20 at each age for the passive training group; n = 8 at each age for the active training
group. Percentages indicate the percentage of children who produced at least one utterance of a given type.
YOUNG CHILDREN LEARN TO PRODUCE PASSIVES 33
Table 2
Average Numbers of Full and Truncated Passive Utterances and Percentage of Children in the Passive Training Group
Who Produced Them in Study 1

Question type

Neutral Patient A;*ent Total


Full Truncated Full Truncated Full Truncated Full Truncated
Group hi % M % M % M % M % hi % M % M %
Younger 2.50 75 1.25 45 2.50 70 1.35 50 0.90 30 0.65 35 5.90 80 3.25 50
Older 3.25 95 1.35 60 4.15 95 0.85 40 0.65 25 0.60 30 8.00 95 2.80 60

Note, n - 20 at each age. Percentages indicate the percentage of children who produced at least one utterance of a given type.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

of question type and passive type also reached significance, F(2, omitted the verb altogether accounted for 38% of the younger
76) = 15.72, p < .001, with planned comparisons indicating that children's passive errors and 6% for the older children.
children produced a greater number of full passives than truncated In sum, the overwhelming majority of children (90%) in the
passives in response to neutral or patient-focused questions, Fs(l, passive training group produced passive utterances, whereas only 2
76) > 33.75, ps < .001, and roughly equal numbers of full and children (12%) in the active training group ever produced a passive
truncated passives in response to agent-focused questions, F(l, utterance, even when they were put under discourse pressure to do
76) < 1. There was no main effect for age, although there was a so. This latter finding would seem to indicate that the children did
significant interaction of age with question type, F(2, 76) = 3.40, not bring to the experiment a verb-general passive construction to
p < .05, and a three-way interaction of age with question type and which they could assimilate their newly learned verbs. Passive-
passive type, F(2, 76) = 5.00, p < .01. These interactions were trained children produced most of their passive utterances in
due to the fact that the older children's preferences for passive response to neutral or patient-focused questions (not agent-focused
utterances, and for full passives over truncated passives in partic- questions), indicating that they were sensitive to the discourse
ular, were stronger in response to neutral and patient-focused demands of the different question types. Consistent with this
questions than the preferences displayed by the younger children interpretation, children made most of their reversal errors in re-
(although the basic pattern was the same across age groups). sponse to agent-focused questions in which there was a mismatch
Children in the passive training group made several types of between the construction they had been using and the discourse
errors involving the passive construction. On average, each demands of the experimenter's question. Children produced more
younger child produced 3.8 passive errors (65% of subjects), and full than truncated passives, most likely because they heard this
each older child averaged 3.4 errors (50% of subjects). Of special construction most often.
interest for current purposes were errors in which the child re- Active-voice transitive utterances. Table 3 presents the aver-
versed the canonical order of the agent and patient nominals, with age numbers of active transitive utterances produced in the elicited
the agent of the action in the preverbal position and the patient of production task. These data were analyzed in a mixed-design
the action in the fry-phrase. These accounted for 42% of errors for ANOVA with age (younger vs. older children) and training group
the younger children and 39% of the errors for the older children (passive vs. active) as between-subjects variables and question
(with 45% of the younger children and 40% of the older children type (neutral, patient, agent) as a within-subjects variable. First,
producing at least one reversal error). Importantly, these reversal children's tendency to use the novel verbs in the active transitive
errors came mostly in response to agent-focused questions (67% of construction varied greatly as a function of training condition, F( 1,
the total). Recall that in agent-focused questions, the child was put 52) = 413.08, p < .001. All 16 children in the active training
under discourse pressure to produce a sentence with the agent as group produced a wide variety of active-voice transitive utterances
subject, and the passive-trained children had never heard the containing the novel verbs, averaging 15.9 active transitive utter-
experimental verbs in any active construction in which the agent ances per child. In contrast, in the passive training group only 4 of
was sentence subject. With some frequency in this situation, chil- the 20 younger children and 11 of the 20 older children ever used
dren responded to questions such as "What did Big Bird do?" by a novel verb in an active transitive utterance (with the passive-
saying such things as "He got meeked by the car," when the trained children averaging 1.5 active transitive utterances per
reverse was actually the case in the real-world situation. It seems child). There was also a main effect of age, F(l, 52) = 11.77, p <
that the children may have treated the verb plus passive morphol- .001, with older children producing more active transitive utter-
ogy (e.g., got meeked by) as an unanalyzed whole and thus were ances with the experimental verbs compared with younger chil-
productively varying word order in response to discourse pressure dren. In addition, the main effect of question type was significant,
encouraging them to make the agent be the sentence subject. F(2, 104) = 10.47, p < .001, with children producing the greatest
In addition to the reversal errors, lexical errors in which the number of active transitive utterances in response to the agent-
child substituted an incorrect nonce verb or nominal in the utter- focused questions. Of the tests for the interaction effects, only the
ance accounted for 20% of the younger children's passive errors three-way interaction of training condition, age, and question type
and 55% for the older children. Utterances in which the child reached significance, F(2, 104) = 5.22, p < .01. This reflects the
34 BROOKS AND TOMASELLO

Table 3
Average Numbers of Active Transitive Utterances and Percentage of Children in the Passive and
Active Training Groups Who Produced Them in Study 1

Question type

Neutral Patient Agent Total

Group M % M % M % M %

Passive training
Younger 0.05 5 0.05 5 0.40 15 0.50 20
Older 0.50 35 0.15 10 1.85 55 2.50 55
Active training
Younger 4.63 100 4.38 100 5.50 100 14.50 10U
Older 6.63 100 5.00 100 5.75 100 17.38 100
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Note, n = 20 at each age for the passive training group; n = 8 at each age for the active training
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

group. Percentages indicate the percentage of children who produced at least one utterance of a given type.

fact that, for the active-trained children, active transitive utterances Intransitives. Intransitive usages of the novel verbs were sur-
were numerous for both age groups in response to all three ques- prisingly infrequent: Only 9 of 40 children (22%) in the passive
tion types, whereas this was not true for the passive-trained chil- training group and 3 of 16 children (19%) in the active training
dren. For the passive training group, there was an interaction group ever used the novel verbs in intransitive utterances such as
between age and question type, F(2,76) = 6.57, p < .01, such that "It meeked" or "He made it meek." Thus, the majority of children
the younger children produced very few active transitive utter- never used the novel verbs in intransitive sentences in the absence
ances with the novel verbs in response to any question type, of hearing the experimenter use the novel verbs in this manner. A
whereas the older children produced 50 active transitive utter- mixed-design ANOVA with between-subjects variables of age and
ances, the majority of which (74%) were in response to agent- training condition and a within-subjects variable of question type
focused questions. showed no main effects and no interaction. Children produced a
Errors involving the active transitive construction were less total of 18 simple intransitive utterances (e.g., "It meeked") and 5
prevalent than errors involving the passive construction. Children utterances using a "make" + intransitive construction (e.g., "He
in the active training group produced an average of 1.69 active pulled the string and made it meek"). Most (78%) of the simple
transitive errors (less than half the rate of passive errors produced intransitives were referentially ambiguous (e.g., "It meeked" could
by the passive training group), with 62% of the children in the refer to either the agent or patient's action), 17% expressed the
younger and older groups producing at least one error. Children in agent as subject (e.g., "Big Bird meeked"), and 6% expressed the
the passive training group made few active transitive errors (1 patient as subject (e.g., "The car meeked"). The 5 utterances
child produced two such errors), mainly because they produced involving usage of a "make" + intransitive construction were
relatively few active transitive sentences. Errors involving substi- produced by 2 older children in the passive training group in
tution of an incorrect noun or nonce verb accounted for 76% of response to agent-focused questions, indicating sensitivity to the
children's active transitive errors. Reversal errors accounted for discourse demands of the agent-focused questions.
24% of active transitive errors and, as expected, were almost English verbs. Children produced utterances containing famil-
always in response to patient-focused questions (43%) or neutral iar English verbs fairly frequently: 19 of 40 children (48%) in the
questions (43%). These errors, produced by 25% of the active- passive training group and 6 of 16 children (38%) in the active
trained children at each age, show how the children attempted to training group used English expressions such as knock down, hit,
manipulate word order in response to discourse pressure to make fall, pull up, and did it to describe the actions of the toys. Although
the patient be the sentence subject. the majority of usages of English verbs were in active-voice
In sum, children in the active training group produced many transitive or intransitive constructions, six passive-trained children
active transitive utterances containing the experimental verbs. The used English verbs in a passive construction (e.g., "The whale got
number of active transitive utterances produced by the active- done on that" and "It was zoomed up"). Seven out of 10 passive
trained children did not vary much as a function of the question utterances were truncated and 3 were full (e.g., "He knocked down
posed, indicating that the children considered an active transitive by shoe" and "He's gonna get hit by a car"). It seems that the
utterance to be a suitable response to any of the elicitation ques- passive training may have "primed" these children to use the
tions. In contrast, only 20% of the younger children and 55% of the passive construction with other verbs.
older children in the passive training group ever used the novel The frequencies of utterances containing English verbs and the
verbs in active transitive utterances—even when they were put percentages of children in each age group and training condition
under discourse pressure to do so. The passive-trained children who produced them are presented in Table 4. A mixed-design
who produced active transitive utterances used these utterances ANOVA revealed a main effect of question type, F(2, 104) =
almost exclusively to answer agent-focused questions; these chil- 3.70, p < .05, which was mitigated by a highly significant inter-
dren preferred to use passive utterances when responding to neu- action of training condition and question type, F(2, 104) = 10.06,
tral or patient-focused questions. p < .001. There was no main effect of training condition. Children
YOUNG CHILDREN LEARN TO PRODUCE PASSIVES 35
in the passive training group produced the majority of their utter- or "old" information, and the agent-adjunct was "new" informa-
ances containing English verbs in response to agent-focused ques- tion. Overall, in children's full passives, patient-subjects were
tions, whereas children in the active training group produced most pronouns, or were omitted altogether, 86% of the time, and their
of their English verb utterances in response to patient-focused agent-adjuncts were pronouns only 4% of the time. Utterances
questions. In addition, there was a marginally significant interac- with content nouns expressing both agent and patient roles—as
tion of training group with age, F(l, 52) = 3.66, p = .06, which had been modeled for the children by the adult on almost every
was due to the younger children of the passive training condition occasion—accounted for only 13% of children's full passive ut-
using English verbs more than the other groups. terances. It is interesting to note that almost all of the utterances in
In general, the children resorted to using familiar English verbs which children used a full noun as patient-subject were in re-
when the constructions in which the novel verbs were introduced sponse to neutral questions. This makes discourse sense, as ques-
did not match the discourse demands of the question posed. This tions such as "What did Ernie do?" and "What happened to the
strategy resulted in the active-trained children using English verbs car?" use a full nominal to identify the participant that the child
mostly to answer patient-focused questions, whereas the passive- needs to use as subject in the reply: "He meeked the car" or "It got
trained children used English verbs primarily to answer agent- meeked by Ernie." In neutral questions, no participants are explic-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

focused questions. The younger children of the passive training itly mentioned at all, and so it is reasonable for the child to indicate
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

group were the most prolific in adopting English substitutes for the them both with full nominals.
novel verbs, presumably because they often could not find a The same general pattern held for children's truncated passives.
satisfactory way to use the novel verbs. The older passive-trained The patient-subject was expressed with a pronoun or was omitted
children relied less often on English verbs, presumably because in 88% of all passive utterances (with a pronoun in 67%, and an
they were better able to productively use the experimental verbs in omission in 21%). As in the case of full passives, truncated
the active transitive construction. All children in the active training passives with a full noun designating the sentence subject came
group relied infrequently on English verbs, most likely because the mostly in response to neutral questions (79%). Three truncated
active transitive construction was not necessarily a poor choice for passives produced by 1 older child in response to agent-focused
answering a patient-focused question (i.e., in answering the ques- questions ("What's Big Bird gonna do?") are worthy of further
tion "What happened to the ball?" one could appropriately say, for comment. These complex utterances ("He's gonna let that get
instance, "Big Bird meeked it" without changing the construction meeked," "It made him get meeked," and "He's gonna want that
in which the verb occurred). get meeked") clearly satisfied the discourse demands to place the
Use of pronouns. Children used pronouns somewhat differ- agent in subject position, while at the same time following the
ently in their passive and active transitive constructions, although passive models in which the child heard the experimental verbs
there was at least one very important similarity. Table 5 presents used. This child never used the experimental verbs in any con-
the percentages of various types of passive utterances produced by struction other than the passive, exhibiting marked conservative-
children in the passive training group only (because active-trained ness with the experimental verbs despite her clear linguistic
children produced very few passives), summed across age (be- sophistication.
cause the two age groups performed very similarly). First, we Children's use of pronouns in their active transitive utterances
present the results for children's full passive utterances. The most was more variable. Table 6 presents the percentages of each of
striking result is that fully 83% of children's full passives con- several types of active transitive utterances for each training group,
tained either a pronominal patient-subject and a nominal agent- summed across age (because the two ages were highly similar).
adjunct (as in "He got meeked by Ernie"; 58%) or else an omitted The most striking finding is that children in the passive training
subject and a nominal object (as in "Got meeked by Bert"; 25%). group expressed the agent-subject with a pronoun, or else omitted
In conformity to the typical use of the full passive construction, it entirely, in 90% of their active transitive utterances. This is
children were clearly assuming that the patient-subject was shared broadly congruent with their tendency to use pronouns, or to omit

Table 4
Average Numbers of Utterances Containing English Verbs and Percentage of Children in the
Passive and Active Training Groups Who Produced Them in Study 1

Question type

Neutral Patient Agent Total

Group M % M % M % M %

Passive training
Younger 0.80 30 0.90 45 2.10 70 3.80 70
Older 0.05 5 0.10 10 0.60 35 0.75 35
Active training
Younger 0.12 12 0.50 25 0.12 12 0.75 25
Older 0.12 12 0.75 50 0.00 0 0.88 50

Note, n = 20 in the passive group at each age; n = 8 in the active group at each age. Percentages indicate
the percentage of children who produced at least one utterance of a given type.
36 BROOKS AND TOMASELLO

Table 5
Percentages of Full and Truncated Passives of Each Subtype Produced by Children in the Passive Training Group
of Study 1 (TV = 40) ^ ^ ^

Subtype Example Neutral Patient Agent Combined

Full passive % of all full passives


1. P got V by A "The car got meeked by the frog" 10.8 1.8 0.7 13.3
2. P got V by pronoun "The car got meeked by that" 0.4 0.0 0.0 0.4
3. Pronoun got V by A "It got meeked by the frog" 19.4 30.9 7.9 58.3
4. Pronoun got V by pronoun "It got meeked by that" 0.4 0.0 0.4 0.7
5. Pronoun got V by [omitted] "It got meeked by the..." 0.4 0.4 0.4 1.1
6. Got V by A "Got meeked by the frog" 10.1 12.9 1.4 24.5
7. Got V by pronoun "Got meeked by that" 0.0 1.8 0.0 1.8

Truncated passive % of all truncated passives


"The car got meeked" 9.1 0.8 1.7 11.6
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

1. P got V
"It got meeked" 26.4 24.8 15.7 66.9
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

2. Pronoun got V
3. Got V "Got meeked" 7.4 10.7 3.3 21.5

Note. P = patient (full noun); V = verb; A = agent (full noun).

the subjects of their passive sentences as well. Passive-trained focused questions, which again indicates discourse sensitivity be-
children expressed the patient-object with a pronoun in 57% of cause an agent-focused question ("What did Ernie do?") pulls
their active transitive utterances, however, clearly deviating from strongly for a pronominal subject in response.
their strong tendency with passives to express the postverbal Overall, then, children's use of pronouns in this experiment
argument with a full nominal. Over half of passive-trained chil- simply underscores their sensitivity to the discourse demands of
dren's active transitive utterances had only pronouns as indicators both the constructions they are using and the questions they are
of the participants. answering. The most striking overall pattern is children's extensive
In contrast to children in the passive training group, children in use of what Chafe (1995) called the light subject constraint. Adults
the active training group used pronouns less often to indicate overwhelmingly express shared information in subject position in
participants. Active-trained children expressed the agent-subject pronominal form (sometimes using presentational sentences to
with a pronoun, or else omitted it, in 68% of their active transitive establish a topic and using a pronoun thereafter), and it seems that
utterances. In contrast, these children used pronouns to express our 2- and 3-year-olds did this an overwhelming majority of the
patient-objects only 18% of the time. The major difference with time as well. The finding that children pronominalized subjects
the passive training group was that children in the active training most often when using the passive or when creating a novel active
group used full nouns in both agent-subject and patient-object transitive utterance (having been trained with passive utterances)
positions—thus copying the adult model exactly—over 31% of the suggests the possibility that something like processing load may be
time. They did this mostly in response to either neutral or patient- at work as well, because it is most efficient to use full nouns with

Table 6
Percentages of Active Transitive Utterances of Each Subtype Produced by Children in the Passive and Active Training Groups
of Study 1

% of all active transitive utterances

Active transitive subtype Example Neutral Patient Agent Combined

Passive training group


1. A V P "The frog meeked the car" 5.0 0.0 0.0 5.0
2. A V pronoun "The frog meeked it" 3.3 1.7 0.0 5.0
3. Pronoun V P "He meeked the car" 0.0 0.0 18.3 18.3
4. Pronoun V pronoun "He meeked it" 8.3 3.3 28.3 40.0
5. V P "Meeked the car" 0.0 0.0 20.0 20.0
6. V pronoun "Meeked it" 1.7 1.7 8.3 11.7
Active training group
1. A V P "The frog meeked the car" 13.3 14.5 3.5 31.4
2. A V pronoun "The frog meeked it" 0.0 0.4 0.4 0.8
3. Pronoun V P "He meeked the car" 11.0 5.9 9.8 26.7
4. Pronoun V pronoun "He meeked it" 3.1 2.7 2.0 7.8
5. V P "Meeked the car" 5.5 3.5 14.5 23.5
6. V pronoun "Meeked it" 2.4 2.4 5.1 9.8

Note, n = 40 for the passive training group; n = 16 for the active training group. A = agent (full noun); V = verb; P = patient (full noun).
YOUNG CHILDREN LEARN TO PRODUCE PASSIVES 37
a fully mastered construction and pronouns when using less fa- duced many fewer passive utterances in response to agent-focused
miliar constructions (Bloom, 1991; Chafe, 1995). questions than they did to neutral or patient-focused questions
(about one third the number). Children also made reversal errors
Discussion relatively frequently in which they expressed the agent in the
preverbal position and the patient in the fry-phrase. Thus, in re-
In the present study, we successfully taught English-speaking sponse to discourse pressure to have the agent be the sentence
children ranging in age from 33 to 44 months to produce passive subject, 45% of the younger children and 40% of the older children
and active transitive utterances with two nonce verbs. Ninety varied word order without changing the form in which the verb
percent of the passive-trained children learned to use the experi- occurred. In addition, when asked the mismatching agent-focused
mental verbs with the passive construction, using mostly full rather questions, the younger passive-trained children often used an En-
than truncated passives, and 100% of the active-trained children glish verb, avoiding use of the experimental verb altogether. It is
learned to use the verbs with the active transitive construction. clear that many of the younger children were in a quandary about
Children accomplished this after only 1 hr of training, and they how to simultaneously be discourse appropriate (by making the
produced many examples of these two constructions during the agent the subject of their reply) and still use the syntactic marking
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

elicitation period at the end of the hour: Over 10 per child for pattern they had heard the adult use with the experimental verb.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

passives and over 15 per child for actives. The older children in the passive training group relied on the same
Children were equally proficient in learning the two experimen- strategies when asked the mismatching agent-focused questions,
tal verbs. It was somewhat surprising that children did not find the but many of them were able to draw on a wider range of syntactic
nonce verb tarn harder than meek, because meek was prototypical options. Thus, 55% of the older passive-trained children simply
in calling for an animate agent and an inanimate patient, whereas used the active transitive construction to reply—an option appar-
tarn involved inanimate agents and animate patients. However, it ently not available to the majority of the younger children. Several
should be noted that our manipulation of animacy was a rather of the older children also used a periphrastic causative construc-
weak one because the animate objects involved were toys as tion, either with a truncated passive or intransitive usage of the
opposed to real animate agents or patients. Although some of the verb (e.g., "He made it get meeked" or "He made it meek"). The
toys were used to represent animate objects and some represented overall picture is thus one in which children of both ages know that
inanimate objects, none of them could get up and move on their the subject of their reply to an agent-focused question should have
own. the agent as subject, but they have different syntactic resources for
Some children showed an understanding of the active transitive responding to this discourse demand.
construction that was clearly verb-general. That is, their represen- Children's use of pronouns in the experiment also demonstrated
tation of the active transitive construction was abstract enough to their sensitivity to discourse pressure. The most general finding
support productive transfer to newly learned verbs that the child was that they used pronouns as the sentence subject of the over-
had never heard used in this construction. Thus, 20% of the whelming majority of their replies to the adult's questions, even
younger children and 55% of the older children who were trained though they almost never heard the experimental verbs used with
to use the nonce verbs in the passive construction were able to pronouns. This was especially noteworthy in the passive training
produce an active transitive utterance with those verbs when they group in which children's most common pattern for replying to
were put under discourse pressure to do so, even though they had neutral and patient-focused questions was to use a pronoun for the
never heard the verbs used in that way. This argues that these patient-subject and a full noun for the agent in the by-phrase,
children had an abstract schema for the active transitive construc- whereas they did all kinds of things to avoid the passive in reply
tion to which they assimilated their newly learned verbs as they to agent-focused questions (as discussed earlier). In their use of the
attempted to answer the agent-focused questions. On the other active transitive construction, the children used subject pronouns
hand, few children of either age showed similar command of the especially often when they were asked the matching agent-focused
passive construction. Only 1 younger child and 1 older child (12%) question, as would be discourse appropriate.
who were trained to use the nonce verbs in the active transitive In some ways, the most surprising findings of Study 1 concerned
construction produced a passive utterance when they were put children's limited productivity in varying the constructions in
under discourse pressure to do so (and they did this very infre- which the novel verbs were used. It is possible, however, that some
quently). This suggests that most of the children did not have a children may have been capable of using the novel verbs in the
verb-general schema for the passive construction to which they "opposite" construction, but task demands interfered with their
could assimilate their newly learned verbs as they attempted to performance. There are two main possibilities. First, some children
answer the patient-focused questions. In general, then, the results may have construed the experimental situation as one in which
of this study suggest that very few English-speaking children their task was to talk the way the experimenter was talking, and for
below 3 years of age can productively extend usage of passive and any given child that involved one construction, either passive or
active transitive constructions to verbs they have never heard used active transitive, for both of the novel verbs. Children may thus
with these constructions. However, another possibility is that the have been primed to talk in only one way in the experimental
conditions of Study 1 were not sufficient to draw out verb-general situation. Second, within each of the verbs, children were encour-
knowledge of passive and active transitive constructions in chil- aged to conceptualize the event from only one perspective, that is,
dren below 3 years of age. This possibility is addressed in Study 2. from the agent's perspective in the active training group and from
Children in Study 1 showed a great deal of sensitivity to the the patient's perspective in the passive training group. Although
discourse demands of the questions posed, especially the children the discourse demands of the mismatching questions invited chil-
in the passive training group. The passive-trained children pro- dren to take the opposite perspective, this may not have been
38 BROOKS AND TOMASELLO

sufficiently powerful to overcome all of the previous training. We identical to those of Study 1 with one main exception. To encourage
designed Study 2 to test these two possibilities by priming both children to conceptualize each event from both active and passive dis-
course perspectives, we introduced "mismatching" comments for each verb
active and passive constructions within a single session, and by
during training. When the novel verb was presented in a passive construc-
encouraging children to adopt both agent-focused and patient- tion, agent comments (e.g., "Look at what [agent] is doing!") were made.
focused perspectives on the events. When the novel verb was presented in the active transitive construction,
patient comments ("Look what's happening to [patient]!") were made.
Study 2 Mismatching comments were provided in the 6th through 10th sequences
of models of Session 1 and the 2 sequences of Session 2. Elicited produc-
Study 2 was similar to Study 1, with two modifications corre- tion, consisting of agent-focused, patient-focused, and neutral questions
sponding to the two concerns expressed earlier. First, instead of presented in a randomized order, took place at the end of the second
being trained in the same way for both of their experimental verbs, session, as in Study 1.
as in Study 1, children in the second experiment were trained with Observational and analytic procedures. Children's responses in the
the passive construction for one of their verbs and with the active elicited production task were recorded and tabulated as in Study 1. Fre-
transitive construction for the other. Second, instead of training quencies of passive, active transitive, and intransitive utterances containing
solely from one perspective for a given event, as in Study 1, we the novel verbs, as well as utterances containing English verbs, were
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

analyzed using ANOVAs for repeated measures. Because each child ex-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

also provided children with some discourse about the event from
perienced one nonce verb in the passive construction and the other in the
the "opposite" perspective using the common English verbs do and
active transitive construction, training condition was a within-subjects
happen. That is, if the active transitive construction was being variable in this study (in contrast to Study 1 in which it was a between-
trained using sentences such as "Big Bird is meeking the car," we subjects variable). As in Study 1, children had three chances to answer each
also sometimes said things such as "Look what happened to the of the three types of questions for each verb. Because most children, at
car!" Conversely, when the passive construction was being trained least once or twice, produced more than one utterance in response to a
using such sentences as "The frog got tammed by the stove," we question, the actual number of utterances observed for a given question
also sometimes said things such as "Look what the stove is doing." type and exposure condition sometimes exceeded three.
These two modifications were made to afford children every
opportunity to be productive with their newly learned verbs as
adults put them under discourse pressure to use them in the Results
opposite construction.
As in Study 1, we look first at children's passive utterances, then
at their active transitive utterances, then at their intransitive utter-
Method ances, then at their utterances with English verbs, and finally at
their use of pronouns in the two target constructions.
Participants. Twenty children (mean age = 34.0 months, range =
32-37 months; 10 boys and 10 girls) were recruited from the same sources Passives. Table 7 presents the average numbers of full and
as Study 1. Two girls did not use the novel verbs during the course of the truncated passive utterances produced in the elicited production
experiment and were replaced. The children in Study 2 were of the same task. These data were analyzed in a mixed-design ANOVA with
basic age as the younger children in Study 1. passive type (full vs. truncated), question type (neutral, patient,
Materials and design. The novel verbs and actions were identical to agent), and training condition (passive vs. active) as within-
those of Study 1. Additional puppets and objects were used for both subjects variables. First, children's tendency to use the novel verbs
meeking and tamming to make it possible for either an animate or inani- in the passive construction varied greatly as a function of training
mate puppet (e.g., fish, squirrel, airplane, grapes) to act on either an condition, F(l, 19) = 36.46,p < .001. Fifteen children (75%) used
animate or inanimate object (e.g., Mickey Mouse, dinosaur, apple, car).
the verb they experienced in the passive construction in passive
Each child heard one verb used only in the active transitive construction
sentences (averaging just under five utterances per child), whereas
and the other verb only in the passive construction, with training in both
cases proceeding generally as in Study 1. The assignment of verbs to only 8 children (40%) used the verb they experienced in the active
constructions was counterbalanced across participants. transitive construction in a passive sentence (averaging less than
Training and elicitation procedures. The training procedures were one utterance per child). Frequencies of passives also varied as a

Table 7
Average Numbers of Full and Truncated Passive Utterances and Percentage of Children Who Produced Them
in the Two Experimental Conditions of Study 2 (N = 20)

Question type

Neutral Patient Agent Total

Full Truncated Full Truncated Full Truncated Full Truncated Combined

Condition M M M M M M M M M

Passive introduction 1.40 65 0.40 25 1.75 75 0.70 35 0.35 30 0.25 15 3.50 75 1.35 35 4.85 75
Active introduction 0.10 5 0.10 5 0.30 25 0.30 20 0.00 0 0.00 0 0.40 25 0.40 20 0.80 40

Note. Percentages indicate the percentage of children who produced at least one utterance of a given type.
YOUNG CHILDREN LEARN TO PRODUCE PASSIVES 39
function of the question posed, with the vast majority of children's incorrect nominal in an otherwise correct passive sentence (6% of
passives coming in response to neutral or patient-focused ques- total).
tions (89%). This effect was highly significant, F(2, 38) = 20.92, Active transitive utterances. Table 8 presents the average
p < .001, with planned comparisons indicating that children pro- numbers of active transitive utterances produced in the elicited
duced more passives in response to patient-focused questions than production task. These data were analyzed in a mixed-design
for neutral or agent-focused questions, and more passives in re- ANOVA with question type (neutral, patient, agent) and training
sponse to neutral questions than agent-focused ones, Fs(l, 38) s condition (passive vs. active) as within-subjects variables. First,
5.33.ps < .05. There was also a significant interaction between the children's tendency to use the novel verbs in the active transitive
variables of training condition and question type, F(2, 38) = construction varied greatly as a function of training condition, F(l,
12.24, p < .05, which was due to the fact that children produced 19) = 40.73, p < .001. Eighteen children (90%) used the verb they
few passive utterances with the active-introduced verb, irrespec- experienced in the active transitive construction in active transitive
tive of what question was posed, whereas for the passive- sentences (averaging just over five utterances per child), whereas
introduced verb, they produced over four times as many passive only 7 children (35%) used the verb they experienced in the
utterances in response to patient-focused questions than agent- passive construction in an active transitive sentence (averaging
focused questions. Also, children produced more full than trun-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

less than one utterance per child). Two of these children used the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

cated passives, F(l, 19) = 5.45, p < .05, especially with the passive-introduced novel verb in the active transitive construction
passive-introduced verb, resulting in an interaction of training only, never once using the passive construction for the verb they
condition and passive type, F(2, 38) = 7.62, p < .05. Lastly, the heard used in this way. In the ANOVA there was also a main effect
three-way interaction of training condition, passive type, and ques- of question type, F(2, 38) = 18.45, p < .001: Children produced
tion type reached significance, F(2, 38) = 4.19, p < .05, presum- more active transitive utterances in response to agent-focused
ably a consequence of floor effects for the active-introduced verb. questions than for neutral or patient-focused questions, and more
As in Study 1, children sometimes made errors with their for neutral questions than for patient-focused ones, Fs(l, 38) ==
passive utterances. Children produced an average of 2.4 passive 6.11, ps < .05. In addition to the main effects, the interaction of
errors each, with 75% of the children producing at least one training condition and question type was significant, F(l, 38) =
passive error. Reversal errors were the most common and ac- 8.78, p < .001, because the effect of question type was less
counted for 50% of all passive errors. Twelve children (60%) pronounced for the passive-introduced novel verb (as a result of a
produced at least one reversal error, with 75% of reversals in floor effect).
response to the agent-focused questions. The second most frequent
Children produced a total of 18 errors involving the active
type of passive error, comprising over 27% of all the passive
transitive construction, with 40% of the children producing at least
errors, involved usage of the wrong experimental verb. These
one error. Reversals, with the patient nominal in the preverbal
errors were produced by 3 children (15%) who on numerous
position and the agent postverbal, accounted for 13 of the chil-
occasions incorrectly used the passive-introduced nonce verb in a
dren's errors and were produced by 25% of the children. As
passive construction to answer the neutral and patient-focused
expected, the majority (71%) of reversal errors occurred in re-
questions for the activity associated with the active-introduced
sponse to patient-focused questions. The remaining five errors
verb. For example, while correctly saying "Big Bird meeked the
involved usage of the wrong nonce verb or nominal.
ball" in answering the agent-focused questions, the children
Intransitives. Matching the results of Study 1, children in
switched to the opposite verb and construction, for instance, by
Study 2 rarely used the novel verbs in intransitive utterances. Of
saying "It got tammed by Big Bird," when responding to neutral or
the 16 intransitive utterances produced by 9 children (45%), 14
patient-focused questions. These errors indicate another means by
were referentially ambiguous. In one utterance the agent was
which children showed sensitivity to the discourse demands of the
expressed as the subject, and in one the patient was subject.
questions, while being conservative with the verb-argument struc-
Frequencies of intransitive utterances were analyzed in a repeated-
ture they heard for each of the experimental verbs. The remaining
measures ANOVA with within-subjects variables of training con-
errors were similar to those of Study 1: Three children made the
dition and question type. No significant effects were found. The
error of omitting the verb in an otherwise correct passive construc-
low frequency of intransitive usages of the novel verbs here and in
tion (17% of passive errors overall), and 3 children used an
Study 1 provides additional confirmation of children's conserva-

Table 8
Average Numbers of Active Transitive Utterances and Percentage of Children Who Produced
Them in the Two Experimental Conditions of Study 2 (N = 20)

Question type

Neutral Patient Agent Total

Condition M % M % M % M %

Passive introduction 0.20 15 0.10 5 0.50 30 0.80 35


Active introduction 1.65 70 0.85 55 2.65 85 5.15 90

Note. Percentages indicate the percentage of children who produced at least one utterance of a given type.
40 BROOKS AND TOMASELLO

tism with respect to using the nonce verbs in a novel construction. percent of the children learned to use a nonce verb with the passive
It should be noted, however, that the percentage of children in construction, producing mostly full rather than truncated passives
Study 2 who produced at least one novel intransitive utterance with (averaging over five passive utterances per child), and 95% of the
an experimental verb was more than double the percentage ob- children learned to use a nonce verb with the active transitive
served in Study 1. construction (averaging over five active transitive utterances per
English verbs. English verbs were used in place of one or both child). These values are only slightly lower than those for the
of the experimental verbs by 13 children (65%). Children produced younger group of Study 1, even though in Study 2 the children
a total of 62 utterances containing familiar English verbs, with an experienced the two experimental verbs in two different construc-
average of 1.65 English verb substitutions per child for the tions. (Although there were the same number of models per verb
passive-introduced novel verb, and 1.45 English verb substitutions in the two studies, in Study 1 a given child experienced twice as
per child for the active-introduced verb. In all of these utterances, many utterances that were either passive or active because they
English verbs occurred in the active voice, either in transitive or experienced both verbs in the same construction.)
intransitive constructions. Usages of English verbs were rather In comparison with Study 1, more children in Study 2 demon-
evenly distributed across question types, and a repeated-measures strated an understanding of the active transitive construction that
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ANOVA revealed no significant effects. was clearly verb-general. In Study 1, only 20% of the younger
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Use of pronouns. As in Study 1, children's utterances were children who were trained to use the nonce verbs in the passive
categorized with respect to whether the two main nominals were construction ever produced an active transitive utterance with a
expressed with full nouns or pronouns or were omitted altogether. passive-introduced verb. In the present study, that value increased
Table 9 presents the frequencies of each subtype of passive and to 35%, presumably on account of the two experimental modifi-
active transitive utterance with respect to children's use of pro- cations we made: (a) having children learn one verb in each of two
nouns and full nouns. In their full passive utterances, children used constructions and (b) helping children to see each event from both
a full noun to express the agent in the Zry-phrase 92% of the time. the agent's and patient's perspective during training.
In contrast, they used a full noun to express the patient-subject in
With respect to the productivity of the passive, the value was
only 36% of their full passive utterances and in only 6% of their
considerably higher in this study as well. In the first study, only 1
truncated passives. In their active transitive utterances, children
younger child (12%) who was trained to use the nonce verbs in the
used full nouns for the agent-subject 29% of the time and for the
active transitive construction produced a passive utterance. The
patient-object 85% of the time. Overall, pronoun use was very
similar to that found in Study 1. major finding of Study 2 was that, with our new procedures, 40%
of the children produced at least one passive sentence with their
active-trained verb. Five of these 8 children produced full passives,
Discussion with the others producing truncated passives only, usually with
As in Study 1, in the present study we successfully taught pronouns (e.g., "It got meeked"). Finally, of the sample of 20
English-speaking children below 3 years of age to produce passive children in the second study, 4 were productive with both the
and active transitive utterances with nonce verbs. Seventy-five passive and active transitive constructions.

Table 9
Percentages of Passive and Active Transitive Utterances of Each Subtype Produced by Children in Experiment 2 (N = 20)

Subtype Example Neutral Patient Agent Combined

Full passive % of all full passives


1. P got V by A "The car got meeked by the frog" 19.2 12.8 2.6 34.6
2. P got V by pronoun "The car got meeked by that" 0.0 0.0 1.3 1.3
3. Pronoun got V by A "It got meeked by the frog" 10.3 17.9 1.3 29.5
4. Pronoun got V by pronoun "It got meeked by that" 0.0 3.8 0.0 3.8
5. Pronoun got V by [omitted] "It got meeked by the..." 0.0 0.0 1.3 1.3
6. Got V by A "Got meeked by the frog" 7.7 16.7 3.8 28.2
7. Got V by pronoun "Got meeked by that" 1.3 0.0 0.0 1.3

Truncated passive '•> of all truncated passives


1. Pgot V "The car got meeked" 0.0 0.0 5.7 5.7
2. Pronoun got V "It got meeked" 17.1 51.4 5.7 74.3
3. Got V "Got meeked" 11.4 5.7 2.9 20.0

Active transitive % of all active transitive utterances


1. A V P "The frog meeked the car" 13.4 6.7 5.9 26.0
2. A V pronoun "The frog meeked it" 0.8 0.8 0.8 2.5
3. Pronoun V P "He meeked the car" 10.9 3.4 21.8 36.1
4. Pronoun V pronoun "He meeked it" 1.7 2.5 4.2 8.4
5. V P "Meeked the car" 4.2 1.7 16.8 22.7
6. V pronoun "Meeked it" 0.0 0.8 3.4 4.2

Note. P = patient (full noun); V = verb; A = agent (full noun).


YOUNG CHILDREN LEARN TO PRODUCE PASSIVES 41

The improved performance of the almost 3-year-old children in General Discussion


Study 2 can be attributed, in part, to the effect of priming both
constructions. That is, repeatedly hearing the verbs used in passive The results of these two studies demonstrate that English-
and active transitive constructions encouraged children to use both speaking children under 3 years of age can learn to use the passive
of these constructions in their subsequent utterances. This finding construction relatively easily. Although the passive construction,
bears a striking resemblance to the syntactic priming phenomenon especially the full passive, is demonstrably more complex than the
studied extensively by Bock and her colleagues with adults (e.g., active transitive construction, young children can learn to produce
Bock, 1986, 1989; Bock & Loebell, 1990; Bock, Loebell, & it with a nonce verb in less than 1 hr. This fact suggests that the
Morey, 1992). Bock and colleagues have consistently found that main reason for the relative paucity of full passive utterances in the
adults tend to repeat the syntactic constructions that have been language of young English-speaking children is that they have
primed in their experiments. For instance, after repeating a passive very few of the kinds of learning experiences with which we
description of an event such as Five people were carried by the provided them in the present experiments. It is certainly not the
boat, adults tend to describe a new picture with a passive sentence. case that taking a discourse perspective on an event opposite to the
Likewise, active priming sentences tend to be followed by active- one that they are used to taking (i.e., from the perspective of the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

voice picture descriptions (Bock et al., 1992). In the present study, patient rather than the agent) presents them with any serious
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

children heard approximately 100 active sentences and 100 passive difficulties. Young children also seem to appreciate many of the
sentences with the novel verbs. By allowing children to hear one discourse properties of passives, both in the sense that they can
nonce verb in an active transitive construction and another in a identify the kinds of discourse situations in which it is used most
passive construction (rather than two verbs in the same construc- appropriately and in the sense that they can use pronouns in the
tion), we almost doubled the percentage of children who produc- construction in discourse-appropriate ways.
tively used the active transitive construction, and more than tripled Some of the children (12% in Study 1 and 40% in Study 2) were
the percentage of children who productively used the passive productive with the passive construction in the sense that they
construction with a newly learned verb. Furthermore, this im- were able to use it with a nonce verb that they had experienced
provement was in spite of the fact that the children in Study 2 had only in an active transitive construction. Indeed, 25% of the
only half as many opportunities to use an active-introduced verb in children in Study 2 produced a full passive sentence with the nonce
a passive construction or a passive-introduced verb in an active verb they had never heard in that construction. It should be noted
transitive construction. Overall, of the younger children in Stud- that the verbs children passivized productively in the present
ies 1 and 2, only 18% in Study 1 exhibited productivity with either studies were all highly prototypical transitive verbs. It is likely that
the active transitive or passive construction, as compared with 55% they would have had considerably more trouble with less proto-
of the children in Study 2. This difference is statistically reliable, typically transitive verbs (e.g., experiential verbs such as see)
^ ( 1 , N = 48) = 7.24, p < .01. It is possible that still more direct because young children use these in passives less often in their
spontaneous speech (Gordon & Chafetz, 1990) and have poor
priming would increase the percentage of children productively
comprehension of passives with experiential verbs (Sudhalter &
using the active transitive and passive constructions even further.
Braine, 1985).
We should also note that the majority of children, even those
Even though less than half of the children used an active-
who were not productive with the passive and active transitive
introduced verb in the passive construction, virtually all of the
constructions, showed sensitivity to the focus of the agent and children demonstrated an understanding that certain kinds of ques-
patient-focused questions in the same general ways as in Study 1. tions call for a response in which the real-world patient of the
Children produced the greatest number of active transitive utter- target action should be the sentence subject of their reply. Thus,
ances in response to agent-focused questions with the fewest in when they heard sentences of the type "Ernie is tamming the boat,"
response to patient-focused questions. Conversely, they most often (and all sentences with tarn were of this type) and then were asked
used the passive construction when answering patient-focused "What happened to the boat?," children did things such as use a
questions and least often in response to agent-focused questions. known English verb that described the action of the patient (e.g.,
When children did use a full passive in response to an agent- fall down); make reversal errors in which they kept the active
focused question, they frequently made reversal errors by putting voice they had heard with tamming while reversing the agent and
the patient in thefey-phrase.On occasion, they used the appropriate patient inappropriately; or, in a few cases in the second study,
construction to answer a question, but they did so with the wrong actually use the nonce verb that they had just learned in the passive
verb. As in Study 1, children were clearly searching for ways to even though it was clearly semantically inappropriate. Altogether,
balance discourse demands and use of the verb-argument struc- 60% of the almost 3-year-olds in Study 2 either used the active-
tures they had heard used with particular verbs. introduced verb in a passive construction or else made a reversal
In contrast to the results of Study 1, the results of Study 2 error involving the active transitive construction; thus, the majority
suggest that quite a few English-speaking children below 3 years of the children actively varied word order in response to discourse
of age have constructed abstract schema for the passive and pressure to make the patient be the sentence subject.
active-transitive constructions to which they can assimilate newly Children were also able to learn nonce verbs in the active
learned verbs relatively quickly. Children at this age are aware that transitive construction quite readily. In Study 1, over half of older
particular types of questions make discourse demands for placing children (around 3.5 years) used this construction productively
agents and patients in particular syntactic positions in any linguis- with a passive-introduced verb, and some of the younger children
tic response that might be made, and they use a variety of syntactic (20% in Study 1 and 35% in Study 2) showed productive transfer
strategies to meet such discourse demands. as well. However, the younger children were no more productive
42 BROOKS AND TOMASELLO

with the active transitive construction than they were with the velopment (Braine, 1963). It should be noted that children
passive (and only 20% of the children in Study 2 were productive around 3 years of age do sometimes produce utterances in which
with both- constructions). It should be emphasized that when the they use verbs with constructions in a productive manner, as
verb was introduced in a passive construction, children used a documented by Bowerman (1982, 1988), among others. These are
variety of syntactic strategies to answer the mismatching agent- such things as "Don't giggle me" and "He get died." But perusal
focused questions. Indeed, in Study 2, 85% of the almost 3-year- of the relevant studies finds that these are extremely rare for
olds either used the passive-introduced verb in an active transitive children under 3 years of age, which accords with the findings of
construction or else reversed the positions of the agent and patient the present study that only some very young children have suffi-
within the passive construction (resulting in a passive reversal cient flexibility with the basic syntactic constructions to produce
error). Hence, the overwhelming majority of children in Study 2 novel verb-construction combinations with newly learned verbs.
productively varied word order in response to discourse pressure to Although the discourse circumstances of this type of overgeneral-
produce a sentence with the agent as subject. ization error are not always reported in full, it is typically the case
The finding that only a minority of the almost 3-year-olds used that they occur under the kinds of discourse pressure we have used
a passive-introduced verb in an active transitive construction may in this study, and so, like adults under certain kinds of discourse
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

seem surprising because presumably, they have been producing a pressure (e.g., sports announcers), the children produce "one-shot
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

variety of active transitive utterances for some months. A plausible innovations" with the limited resources at their disposal (Braine &
interpretation is that the children may have a generalized active Brooks, 1995).
transitive construction, but they cannot get to it from hearing a In all of the studies in which we and other researchers have
word used in a construction that is not a central component of their given 2-year-old children nonce verbs in various syntactic con-
productive grammatical system. Indeed, the children might have structions, the consistent finding is that children of this age are
abstract passive and active transitive constructions, but it may be extremely conservative with new verbs, using them almost exclu-
difficult for them to get from one to the other if they are not sively in the syntactic constructions in which they have heard
connected pieces of knowledge. The current findings fit well with others use them (although they have the ability to substitute
other recent studies in which children have been taught nonce nominals freely). Thus, even at the earliest stages of language
verbs in other constructions and then are put in situations that call acquisition, children seem to keep track of how particular verbs are
for an active transitive utterance. For example, Olguin and Toma- used in particular sentence types. Learning which verbs may occur
sello (1993) could not get young 2-year-olds to produce fully in which constructions is a necessary component of attaining
transitive utterances with any of several nonce verbs taught to mastery of a language, and recent constraint-based models of
them in various ways. Akhtar and Tomasello (1997) taught 2- and language comprehension (e.g., MacDonald, 1994; MacDonald,
3-year-old children nonce verbs in a presentational format ("This Pearlmutter, & Seidenberg, 1994; Trueswell, Tanenhaus, & Garn-
is called dacking") and then tried several different strategies for sey, 1994; Trueswell, Tanenhaus, & Kello, 1993) have shown how
eliciting active transitive utterances. They had no success with adults' knowledge of the frequency with which particular verbs
children younger than 3.5 years of age (3.5-year-olds were very occur in particular constructions affects their on-line syntactic
productive, however). They also found that children who were decision making (e.g., when resolving syntactic ambiguities).
almost 3 years old could not even comprehend reversible active Speakers of a language seem to store knowledge of how verbs are
transitive sentences with nonce verbs; that is, after having heard used at several levels of generality simultaneously—the most
"This is called dacking" they could not act out appropriately the important of these being verbs as lexical items, with their idiosyn-
sentence "Big Bird is dacking Cookie Monster." Indeed, in the cratic patterns of syntactic marking, and syntactic constructions as
present study more children at around 3 years of age productively meaning-bearing elements in their own right (Tomasello &
created active transitive utterances than in Akhtar and Tomasello Brooks, in press). The present findings mesh well with this mul-
(1997). Children may have had greater success in the present study tilevel approach by demonstrating that very young children are
because they were asked to produce an active transitive utterance highly attuned to how individual verbs are used in sentence con-
with a verb they had heard used with the passive, which contained structions, and they vary their usage of verbs by closely following
both of the arguments to be used in the active transitive (so that adult patterns of usage. Many of the children also provided evi-
they just had to rearrange them), whereas in Akhtar and Tomasello dence of abstract syntactic constructions by using the novel verbs
(1997), children had to generate new argument roles that they had in ways in which they had never before heard them used.
not heard used with the new verb. We know little about how children come to have abstract
Our results are consistent with the view that children's earliest syntactic constructions, and, to our knowledge, the process has not
syntactic constructions are structured by the particular verbs or yet been systematically investigated in children's early language
predicative terms they occur with (Tomasello, 1992). The abstract- development. We do know that children have a strong preference
ness of children's early constructions, and thus the source of their to use their newly learned verbs in the same constructions in which
earliest productivity, appears to come primarily from their argu- they have heard others use them. Our hypothesis is that the process
ment slots. Thus, from their very first word combinations, young of constructing abstract syntactic constructions is very similar to
children can learn a nonce noun in one semantic role (or in a the process by which children come to have other kinds of abstract
presentational format) and then use it productively in other seman- linguistic structures such as past-tense markers (cf. Braine, 1971,
tic roles with other verbs they have never heard it used with 1988; MacWhinney, 1978; Plunkett & Marchman, 1991). The
(Tomasello, Akhtar, Dodson, & Rekau, 1997; Tomasello & Ol- main idea is that children register linguistic features and patterns in
guin, 1993). This leads to such things as the famous "All gone memory as a function of the frequency with which these linguistic
sticky" and other productive combinations early in language de- entities are encountered. The key is to view syntactic constructions
YOUNG CHILDREN LEARN TO PRODUCE PASSIVES 43
such as the passive and the active transitive as wholistic linguistic References
entities that may themselves be categorized (Braine & Brooks,
1995; Goldberg, 1995; Tomasello & Brooks, in press). With this Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1997). Young children's productivity with
perspective, the proposal is simply that children's initial construc- word order and verb morphology. Developmental Psychology, 33, 952-
tions are organized around individual verbs, and on the basis of 965.
some kind of similarity (perhaps "structure mapping" of the type Allen, S. E. M , & Crago, M. B. (1996). Early passive acquisition in
Inuktitut. Journal of Child Language, 23, 129-156.
proposed by Gentner & Markman, 1997), they go on to form more
Bloom, L. (1991). Language development from two to three. New York:
abstract and verb-general constructions. Precisely how they do this Cambridge University Press.
is not known at this time, but it is clear that children have to be able Bock, J. K. (1986). Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive
to recognize when the same construction is occurring with a Psychology, 18, 355-387.
number of different verbs. Bock, J. K. (1989). Closed-class immanence in sentence production. Cog-
A child might start by noting semantic similarities among the nition, 31, 163-186.
different verbs that appear in the same construction while at the Bock, J. K., & Loebell, H. (1990). Framing sentences. Cognition, 35, 1-39.
same time noting similarities in the thematic roles of the particular Bock, J. K., Loebell, H., & Morey, R. (1992). From conceptual roles to
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

nominals involved and their positions with respect to the verb. The structural relations: Bridging the syntactic cleft. Psychological Re-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

view, 99, 150-171.


child might identify regularities in how familiar verbs are syntac-
Bowerman, M. (1982). Reorganizational processes in lexical and syntactic
tically marked, and may register on an item-by-item basis whether
development. In E. Wanner & L. R. Gleitman (Eds.), Language acqui-
a new verb fits the emerging pattern. At this point, the construction sition: The state of the art (pp. 319-346). New York: Cambridge
may exist in a state that does not support productive transfer, but University Press.
nonetheless makes it easier to assimilate a new verb to that Bowerman, M. (1988). The "no negative evidence" problem: How do
construction than if there were no regularities noted at all. That is, children avoid constructing an overgeneral grammar? In J. A. Hawkins
in the present study, some of the children may have had construc- (Ed.), Explaining linguistic universals (pp. 73-101). Oxford, England:
tions that were abstract enough to facilitate their acquisition of the Basil Blackwell.
syntactic marking of a new verb even if they never used the verb Braine, M. D. S. (1963). The ontogeny of English phrase structure: The
in a construction that was different from the one in which it was first phase. Language, 39, 1-14.
Braine, M. D. S. (1971). On two types of models of the internalization of
introduced. Thus, at the earliest stages of schema formation, the
grammars. In D. Slobin (Ed.), The ontogenesis of grammar: A theoret-
child may identify constructions as recurrent syntactic patterns,
ical symposium (pp. 153-186). New York: Academic Press.
and still may exhibit conservative behavior with respect to using Braine, M. D. S. (1988). Modeling the acquisition of linguistic structure. In
individual verbs only in the particular constructions in which they Y. Levy, I. Schlesinger, & M. D. S. Braine (Eds.), Categories and
have heard others use them. Possibly some notion of "critical processes in language acquisition (pp. 217-259). Hillsdale, NJ: Erl-
mass" is involved in which a certain number of verbs of a certain baum.
type must be individually learned in a certain construction before Braine, M. D. S., & Brooks, P. J. (1995). Verb argument structure and the
the child is willing to generate novel verb-construction combina- problem of avoiding an overgeneral grammar. In M. Tomasello & W. E.
tions (Marchman & Bates, 1994). It is at this point, and only at this Merriman (Eds.), Beyond names for things: Young children's acquisition
point, that problems of overgeneralization become an issue—with of verbs (pp. 353-376). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Brooks, P. J., & Tomasello, M. (1998). How children avoid overgeneral-
the precise age being different for different constructions. At this
ization errors when acquiring transitive and intransitive verbs. In E.
point, children need to learn how to constrain the productivity of Clark (Ed.), Proceedings of the 29th Annual Child Language Research
their constructions in conventional ways, either by identifying Forum (pp. 171-179). Stanford, CA: Center for the Study of Language
semantic criteria associated with small subclasses of verbs (Brooks and Information.
& Tomasello, 1998; Pinker, 1989) or else by attending to some Brooks, P. J., Tomasello, M., Dodson, K., & Lewis, L. B. (in press). Young
dimensions of adult use of the construction (Braine & Brooks, children's overgeneralizations with fixed transitivity verbs. Child De-
1995; Brooks & Tomasello, 1998; Brooks, Tomasello, Dodson, & velopment.
Lewis, in press; Goldberg, 1995). Budwig, N. (1990). The linguistic marking of nonprototypical agency: An
exploration into children's use of passives. Linguistics, 28, 1221-1252.
In this study, we have shown that young children can learn to
Chafe, W. (1995). Discourse, consciousness, and time: The flow and
use a nonce verb in the full passive construction fairly readily. displacement of conscious experience in speaking and writing. Chicago:
They appreciate many of the discourse properties of passives in the University of Chicago Press.
sense that they use passives primarily in discourse-appropriate Clark, E. (1982). The young word maker: A case study of innovation in the
situations, and they use pronouns in the construction in discourse- child's lexicon. In E. Wanner & L. R. Gleitman (Eds.), Language
appropriate ways. By 3 years of age, some children have an acquisition: The state of the art (pp. 390-425). New York: Cambridge
understanding of the passive construction that is general enough to University Press.
support their production of passives with verbs that they have Demuth, K. (1989). Maturation and the acquisition of the Sesotho passive.
never heard used in the passive construction, and is abstract Language, 65, 56-80.
Demuth, K. (1990). Subject, topic, and Sesotho passive. Journal of Child
enough that it can be primed (Bock, 1986). However, not all
Language, 17, 67-84.
children in the present study displayed such flexibility in their use Gentner, D., & Markman, A. (1997). Structure mapping in analogy and
of the passive construction. The processes by means of which similarity. American Psychologist, 52, 45-56.
young children begin to both construct and constrain each of their Goldberg, A. E. (1995). Constructions: A construction grammar approach
verb-general syntactic constructions remain a topic for future to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
investigation. Gordon, P., & Chafetz, J. (1990). Verb-based versus class-based accounts
44 BROOKS AND TOMASELLO

of actionality effects in children's comprehension of passives. Cogni- Quiche Mayan. Papers and Reports on Child Language Develop-
tion, 36, 227-254. ment, 27, 71-80.
Harris, F. N., & Flora, J. A. (1982). Children's use of get passives. Journal Shibitani, M. (1985). Passives and related constructions: A prototype
of Psycholinguists Research, 11, 297-311. analysis. Language, 61, 821-848.
Horgan, D. (1978). The development of the full passive. Journal of Child Sudhalter, V., & Braine, M. D. S. (1985). How does comprehension of
Language, 5, 65-80. passives develop? A comparison of actional experiential verbs. Journal
MacDonald, M. C. (1994). Probabilistic constraints and syntactic ambigu- of Child Language, 12, 455-470.
ity resolution. Language and Cognitive Processes, 9, 157-201. Suzman, S. M. (1985). Learning the passive in Zulu. Papers and Reports
MacDonald, M. C , Pearlmutter, M. J., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1994). on Child Language Development, 24, 131-137.
Lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review, Tomasello, M. (1992). First verbs: A case study of early grammatical
101, 676-703. development. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
MacWhinney, B. (1978). The acquisition of morphophonology. Mono- Tomasello, M., Akhtar, N., Dodson, K., & Rekau, L. (1997). Differential
graphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 43 (1-2, Serial productivity in young children's use of nouns and verbs. Journal of
No. 174). Child Language, 24, 373-387.
Maratsos, M., Fox, D. E. C , Becker, J. A., & Chalkley, M. A. (1985).
Tomasello, M., & Brooks, P. J. (in press). Early syntactic development: A
Semantic restrictions on children's passives. Cognition, 19, 167-191.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

construction grammar approach. In M. Barrett (Ed.), The development of


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Marchman, V. A., & Bates, E. (1994). Continuity in lexical and morpho-


language. London: UCL Press.
logical development: A test of the critical mass hypothesis. Journal of
Tomasello, M., & Olguin, R. (1993). Twenty-three-month-old children
Child Language, 21, 339-366.
have a grammatical category of noun. Cognitive Development, 8, 451—
Marchman, V. A., Bates, E., Burkhardt, A., & Good, A. B. (1991).
464.
Functional constraints of the acquisition of the passive: Toward a model
of the competence to perform. First Language, 11, 65-92. Trueswell, J. C , Tanenhaus, M. K., & Garnsey, S. M. (1994). Semantic
Olguin, R., & Tomasello, M. (1993). Two-year-olds do not have a gram- influences on parsing: Use of thematic role information in syntactic
matical category of verb. Cognitive Development, 8, 245-272. disambiguation. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 285-318.
Pinker, S. (1989). Learnability and cognition: The acquisition of argument Trueswell, J. C , Tanenhaus, M. K., & Kello, C. (1993). Verb-specific
structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. constraints in sentence processing: Separating effects of lexical prefer-
Pinker, S., Lebeaux, D. S., & Frost, L. A. (1987). Productivity and ence from garden-paths. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
constraints in the acquisition of the passive. Cognition, 26, 195— Memory, and Cognition, 19, 528-553.
267.
Plunkett, K., & Marchman, V. (1991). U-shaped learning and frequency
effects in a multi-layered perceptron: Implications for child language Received November 20, 1997
acquisition. Cognition, 38, 43-102. Revision received May 5, 1998
Pye, C , & Quixtan Poz, P. (1988). Precocious passives and antipassives in Accepted May 5, 1998

You might also like