Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 6

Forgetting Trauma Stimuli

Author(s): Anne P. DePrince and Jennifer J. Freyd


Source: Psychological Science, Vol. 15, No. 7 (Jul., 2004), pp. 488-492
Published by: Sage Publications, Inc. on behalf of the Association for Psychological Science
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/40064137 .
Accessed: 17/10/2011 10:11

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Sage Publications, Inc. and Association for Psychological Science are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize,
preserve and extend access to Psychological Science.

http://www.jstor.org
PSYCHOLOGICALSCIENCE

Research Article

Trauma
Forgetting Stimuli
Anne P. DePrince1 and Jennifer J. Freyd2
1 2
University of Denver and University of Oregon

ABSTRACT- Previous work reported in this journal suggested encode the material), impaired retrieval processes (e.g., material is
that the cognitive capacities of high dissociators are impaired encoded, but cannot be retrieved; see McNally, Clancy, & Schacter,
under conditions of focused (selective) attention, but not under 2001), or both. Directed-forgetting paradigms have been used to in-
conditions of divided attention, compared with the cognitive vestigate avoidant-processing hypotheses for negative stimuli (e.g.,
capacities of low dissociators. Using a directed-forgetting Cloitre, Cancienne, Brodsky, Dulit, & Perry, 1996) and trauma-related
paradigm, the current study demonstrated that under divided- stimuli (e.g., McNally, Metzger, Lasko, Clancy, & Pitman, 1998;
attention demands, high dissociators have impaired memoryfor Moulds & Bryant, 2002).
words associated with trauma (e.g., incest) but not for neutral In a directed-forgetting task, participants are presented with items
words, as compared with low dissociators. In addition, high and instructed after each item (or list of items) to remember or forget
dissociators reported significantly more trauma history and the material (MacLeod, 1999). Memory is tested for both to-be-
significantly more betrayal trauma (abuse by a caregiver) than forgotten and to-be-remembered items. The directed-forgetting task
low dissociators. These results are consistent with the proposal has been employed in two forms. In the item method, words appear one
that dissociation may aid individuals with histories of betrayal at a time with a memory instruction following each word. In the list
traumas to keep threatening information out of awareness. method, participants receive the memory instruction at the end of a
list of words. When the item method is used, participants likely se-
lectively rehearse to-be-remembered words, whereas when the list
Traumatic exposure- particularly exposure to chronic and repeated method is used, participants likely inhibit to-be-forgotten words (e.g.,
trauma- has been associated with many deleterious consequences, Basden, Basden, & Gargano, 1993; MacLeod, 1999).
such as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), alterations in neu- McNally et al. (1998) found that women diagnosed with PTSD who
roendocrine function, and depression. Although there is little dispute reported a history of childhood abuse did not demonstrate poorer
that traumatic exposure is often associated with negative con- recall for trauma-related stimuli compared with control groups, re-
sequences, controversy has surrounded the extent to which traumatic gardless of remember or forget instruction. Further, the women diag-
exposure has an impact on basic cognitive processes of attention and nosed with PTSD showed impaired recall for negative and positive
memory. The controversy has been most intense regarding memory for words they were instructed to remember. The authors argued that
trauma: Are some types of trauma associated with impaired memory these findings are inconsistent with an avoidant-encoding hypothesis.
for the event and avoidant encoding of trauma-related information? In subsequent work, McNally et al. (2001) tested women who re-
At the same time that there has been controversy about memory for ported repressed or recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse
trauma, researchers have increasingly applied cognitive psychology to and women who denied any history of childhood sexual abuse. Using
the study of various forms of posttraumatic distress, including acute the same methodology as in the previous study (McNally et al., 1998),
stress disorder (e.g., Moulds & Bryant, 2002), PTSD (e.g., Brewin, the authors did not find evidence that the repressed- or recovered-
Dalgleish, & Joseph, 1996), and dissociation (e.g., DePrince & Freyd, memory groups were engaging in avoidant processing of trauma-
1999). Dissociation - the breakdown of normally connected processes related information.
of consciousness and memory- is of particular interest to both trauma Directed forgetting, in its standard form, implicitly requires focused
researchers and cognitive scientists. Dissociation has been associated attention. However, recent research suggests that attentional context
with trauma exposure (see Putnam, 1997) and involves the alteration may play a critical role in the conditions under which recall of trauma-
of fundamental cognitive functions. Betrayal-trauma theory predicts related information is impaired. Freyd, Martorello, Alvarado, Hayes,
that dissociating information from awareness is mediated by the threat and Christman (1998) found that high dissociators showed more
that the information poses to the individual's system of attachment Stroop interference than low dissociators under selective-attention
(Freyd, 1994, 1996, 2001). demands, suggesting that high dissociators have disruptions in con-
Theorists have proposed that memory impairment for trauma- sciously controlled attentional abilities. We (DePrince & Freyd, 1999)
related information involves avoidant processing (e.g., people may found that although high dissociators performed worse (more Stroop
disengage attention from threatening information and thus fail to interference) than low dissociators under selective-attention condi-
tions, they performed better (less Stroop interference) than low dis-
Address correspondence to Anne P. DePrince, Department of Psy- sociators under divided-attention conditions. A free-recall task
chology, University of Denver, 2155 S. Race St., Denver, CO 80208; revealed that high dissociators recalled more neutral and fewer
e-mail: adeprinc@du.edu. trauma-related words than low dissociators, supporting the argument

488 Copyright© 2004 AmericanPsychologicalSociety Volume15- Number7


Anne P. DePrince and Jennifer J. Freyd

that dissociation may help to keep threateninginformationfrom The DES is a self-reportmeasure of dissociation (Bernstein &
awareness.1 Putnam,1986). Participantsindicatethe percentageof time that they
These studiessuggestthathigh dissociatorsmightbe at a cognitive experienceeach of 28 items (e.g., "Somepeople have the experience
advantagein tasks that require divided attention. We predicted, of feeling that other people, objects, and the worldaroundthem are
therefore,thatdivided-attentioncontextswouldhelp high dissociators not real"). DES score was calculated by averagingresponses across
to keep threateninginformationfromawareness.In a previousstudy the 28 items.
(DePrince& Freyd,2001), we tested high and low dissociatorsusing The BriefBetrayalTraumaSurvey(BBTS;Goldberg& Freyd,2003)
an item-methoddirected-forgetting task underselective- and divided- is a 12-item, behaviorallydefined, self-reportmeasure.Items assess
attention conditions. Like McNally et al. (1998), we found no noninterpersonal(e.g., naturaldisasters)and interpersonal(e.g., as-
difference in free recall of trauma-relateditems presented under sault)traumasbeforeand afterage 18. Interpersonaltraumaticevents
selective-attentionconditions;however,underdivided-attentioncon- distinguishbetweenthose perpetratedby someonerelationallyclose to
ditions, high dissociatorsrecalled fewer trauma-relatedand more the victim and those perpetratedby someonenot close to the victim.
neutralto-be-remembered wordsthan did low dissociators. Freyd and Goldberg(2003) categorizedthe items as reflectingthree
The item methodof the directed-forgetting task likely drives par- levels of betrayaldependingon whetherthe event endorsedwas per-
ticipants to selectively rehearse words; this presumablyenhances petratedby someonerelationallyclose to the victim. An exampleof a
encodingand does not invoke inhibition.Recent researchpoints to high-betrayalitem is "madeto have some formof sexual contact,such
inhibitionas a possiblemechanismin memoryimpairmentfortrauma- as touchingor penetration,by someonewithwhomyou wereveryclose
relatedinformation(e.g., Anderson,2001; Anderson& Green,2001). (such as a parentor lover)."An exampleof a medium-betrayal item is
Wewere thereforeinterestedin how high and low dissociatorswould "witnessedsomeonewith whomyou were very close deliberatelyat-
performin a directed-forgetting task using the list method,which is tack anotherfamilymemberso severelyas to resultin marks,bruises,
believed to invoke inhibitoryprocesses. We predicted results con- blood, broken bones, or broken teeth." And an example of a low-
sistent with those of our previousstudies (DePrince& Freyd, 1999, betrayalitem is "beenin a majorearthquake,fire, flood,hurricane,or
2001). Specifically,we expected that under divided-attentioncondi- tornadothat resulted in significantloss of personalproperty,serious
tions,high dissociatorswoulddemonstrateimpairedrecall fortrauma- injuryto yourselfor a significantother,the deathof a significantother,
relatedbut not neutralstimuli, relative to low dissociators.We also or the fear of your own death."Constructvalidity has been demon-
predicted that high dissociators would report significantly more stratedbased on agreementbetweentraumaticevents endorsedon the
trauma(includingbetrayaltraumasforwhichthe reportedperpetrator BBTSand an existing traumainventory(DePrince,2001).
was describedas very close) than low dissociators.
Procedure
METHOD Participantswere tested individuallywith an experimenterpresent.
They saw wordsappearone at a time on the computerscreen in four
Participants blocks (two selective- and two divided-attentionblocks); each word
Participantswere24 low dissociators(meanage= 19.0; 16 female)and was presented for 6 s. In each attention condition, one block was
21 high dissociators(meanage = 19.0; 14 female)enrolledin an in- followed by rememberinstructionsand the other by forget instruc-
troductorypsychologycourse at the Universityof Oregon;they re- tions. Each block comprisedsix neutraland six trauma-related words.
ceived partialcredit towarda researchrequirementfor participating. During the selective-attentionblocks, words appeared in black on a
Usingthe samemethodologyas in ourpreviousstudies(e.g., DePrince white background.During divided-attentionconditions, the stimuli
& Freyd,1999, 2001), we recruitedparticipantswho scoredabove20 changed in color from red to blue at randomintervals.Participants
andbelow10 on the DissociativeExperiencesScale (DES;Bernstein& were instructedto press a key every time the color changed (as in
Putnam,1986) for the high- and low-dissociatorgroups,respectively. DePrince& Freyd,2001). Block order(dividedvs. selective attention
MeanDESscorewas 28.9 {SD= 13.4) forthe high-DESgroupand 5.8 and forgetvs. rememberinstructions)was randomizedfor each par-
(SD= 2.5) forthe low-DESgroup.Recognitiondatafor 1 high-DESand ticipant.A blockof countrynameswas presentedat the beginningand
1 low-DESparticipantwere deleted because of computererror. end of the experiment.
Recall was assessed by giving participants5 min to writedownall
of the wordsthey could rememberfromthe lists presented,regardless
Materials of the rememberor forget instruction.For the recognitiontest, par-
Stimuli presented during the directed-forgettingtask were neutral ticipants saw items presentedone at a time and pressed one key to
(e.g., stairs, curtain) and trauma-related (e.g., rape, assault, incest) indicatethey recognizedthe wordfromthe lists viewedpreviouslyand
wordstaken fromMcNallyet al. (1998). Wordsmatchedfor neutral anotherkey to indicatethey did not recognizethe wordfromthe lists.
(e.g., chair, light) and trauma-related(e.g., attack, violate) meaning Test items included words from the experimentallists previously
were addedto the lists for use in the recognitiontask. presentedand 24 wordsnot previouslyviewed. Finally,participants
completedthe BBTS.
Alternatively,high dissociatorsmay be more familiarwith traumawords
thanlow dissociatorsare, makingthe wordsless memorable.Weaddressedthis RESULTS
issue in the currentresearch.In a sampleof 286 undergraduateparticipants,
dissociationlevel was not related to familiarityratings for the traumaand Trauma History
neutralwordsused in the currentstudy,suggestingthat differencesin recall Reported
cannotbe explainedby simple differencesin familiaritywith traumastimuli The total number of traumaticevents reportedon the BBTS was
betweengroups. computed for each participant(possible scores rangedfrom0 to 24).

Volume 7
15- Number 489
Forgetting Trauma Stimuli

TABLE 1
Mean Number of Traumas by Dissociation Group

Dissociationgroup Difference
Low DES betweengroups
High DES
Traumacategory (n = 24) (n = 21) t p
Total 2.6(2.5) 6.2(3.9) -3.7 .001
High betrayal 0.5(0.7) 1.5(1.6) -2.7 <.01
Mediumbetrayal 1.2(1.5) 2.9(1.9) -3.2 <.01
Lowbetrayal 0.9(0.9) 1.9(1.4) -2.8 <.01

Note. Standard deviations are in parentheses. For each participant, sums


were calculated for the total number of traumas reported (range: 0-24), as
well as the number of low-betrayal events (range: 0-6 events), medium-
betrayal events (range: 0-12 events), and high-betrayalevents (range:0-6).
The t tests were two-tailed.DES = DissociativeExperiencesScale.

To moreclosely examinethe type of traumasreported,we also calcu-


lated totalsforthe numberof high-, medium-,and low-betrayalevents.
High dissociatorsreportedsignificantlymoretraumaticevents overall Fig. 1. Percentage correct recall of to-be-remembered neutral and
and morehigh-betrayalevents than low dissociators(see Table 1). trauma-related words presented under divided-attention conditions.
Results are shown separately for participants with high (above 20) and
low (below 10) scores on the Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES).
Memory Findings
The totalnumberof itemscorrectlyrecalledand recognizedwas tallied vided into high-DES(n = 13) and low-DES(n = 10) groups;the same
(see Table2). A significantDESGroupx WordCategoryinteractionfor significantinteractionof DES groupand wordcategoryfor to-be-re-
to-be-rememberedstimuli in the divided-attentioncondition,F(l, 43) memberedwordsunderdivided-attention conditionswas present,F(l,
= 15.313, p < .001, =
r\p .26, indicatedthat as in previousstudies, = <
21) 8.704, p .01, r\p2 = .29. There was no significantinteractionof
high dissociators recalled more neutraland fewer traumawordsthan DES groupby wordcategoryor main effect of dissociationfor to-be-
low dissociators(see Fig. 1). Main effects for DES group and word
forgottenwordspresentedunderdivided-attentionconditions,nor for
categorywere not significantin this analysis. In a follow-upanalysis, to-be-remembered or to-be-forgotten wordspresentedunderselective-
those participantswho reportedat least one betrayaltraumawere di- attentionconditions.
Analysesof the recognitiondatarevealeda trendforthe interaction
of DES group and word category for the to-be-remembereditems
TABLE 2 = 2.939,
presented under divided-attentionconditions, F(l, 41)
Mean Number of Words Correctly Recalled and Recognized =
p .09, r\p2 = .07. No other effects for these items were significant
underdividedattention.The DES-group-by-word-category interaction
Traumawords Neutralwords
was significantfor the to-be-forgottenitems under divided-attention
Remember Forget Remember Forget
conditions,F(l, 41) = 4.447, p < .05, r\p2= .10, such that the high-
Test and condition instruction instruction instruction instruction
DES grouprecognizedmore neutraland fewer trauma-relateditems
Low-DESgroup than the low-DESgroup.The main effect for wordcategorywas sig-
Recall nificant, F(l, 41) = 14.521, p < .001, r\p2= .26; participantsrec-
Selective attention 3.1 (1.1) 1.5 (1.1) 2.0 (1.6) 2.3 (1.6) ognizedmoretraumathan neutralwords.Therewas no maineffect of
Divided attention 1.7 (1.4) 1.5 (1.2) 1.1 (0.9) 0.7 (1.0) DES group.The interactionbetween DES groupand wordcategory
Recognition was not significantunderselective-attentiondemands.
Selective attention 5.1 (1.1) 5.0 (1.1) 4.7 (1.4) 4.9 (1.2)
Divided attention 4.8 (1.3) 4.6 (1.4) 4.3 (1.1) 2.9 (1.5)
DISCUSSION
High-DESgroup
Recall Amongthe to-be-rememberedwordspresentedunder divided-atten-
Selective attention 3.6 (1.1) 1.4 (1.2) 2.3 (1.2) 2.5 (1.1) tion conditions,high dissociatorsrecalled fewer trauma-relatedand
Divided attention 1.0 (1.0) 1.5 (1.3) 2.0 (1.2) 0.8 (0.8) more neutral words compared with low dissociators. Among the
Recognition to-be-forgottenwords presented under divided-attentionconditions,
Selective attention 5.3 (0.9) 5.1 (0.9) 5.1 (0.8) 5.3 (0.9)
high dissociatorsrecognizedmore neutraland fewer trauma-related
Divided attention 4.3 (1.2) 4.3 (1.4) 4.4 (1.2) 3.8 (1.7)
wordscomparedwith low dissociators.Takentogetherwith previous
Note. Standard deviations are in parentheses. DES = Dissociative Experi- findings(DePrince& Freyd, 1999, 2001; Freyd et al., 1998), these
ences Scale. findingspoint to the importanceof attentionalcontext in identifying

490 Volume15- Number7


AnneP. DePrinceand JenniferJ. Freyd

the conditions under which high dissociators forget trauma-related cally identifies attentional context as a critical aspect of the conditions
information. under which individuals may experience memory impairment for
The high-dissociator group reported significantly more trauma, in- trauma-related information. Future investigation is needed to evaluate
cluding high-betrayal events, than low dissociators. Further, the dis- the proposition that the divided-attention condition more closely re-
sociation-group-by-word-category interaction was present when we flects task demands in complex social situations than does the typical
looked at only those participants who reported a history of betrayal laboratory situation. If this is indeed the case, researchers may dis-
trauma. This pattern is consistent with betrayal-trauma theory's pre- cover that keeping betrayal-trauma stimuli out of awareness and
diction that individuals who experience events high in betrayal will memory is fairly common for individuals with dissociative tendencies.
use dissociation to keep threatening information from awareness.
We did not find interactions between dissociation group and re-
member-forget instruction, suggesting that inhibitory mechanisms did Acknowledgments - This research was supported in part by the
not play a role in the high dissociators' memory impairment for Trauma and Oppression Research Fund at the University of Oregon
trauma-related versus neutral information. Presumably, the effect of Foundation. We thank Lew Goldberg, Kathy Pezdek, James Cutting,
semantic content occurred at initial encoding, resulting in the inter- Jennifer Labus, Eileen Zurbriggen, Bertram Malle, and an anonymous
action of dissociation group and word category for to-be-remembered reviewer for helpful feedback.
items; an effect at encoding is consistent with the prediction that high
dissociators will engage in avoidant processing of threatening in-
formation under certain attention conditions. This does not mean that REFERENCES
inhibitory mechanisms are not a viable route to memory impairment
for trauma-related information (to the contrary, Anderson and col- Anderson,M.C.(2001). Active forgetting:Evidenceforfunctionalinhibitionas
a source of memory failure. Journal of Aggression,Maltreatment&
leagues, e.g., Anderson, 2001, and Anderson & Green, 2001, have
Trauma,4, 185-211.
presented models of inhibitory mechanisms that might account for Anderson,M.C, & Green,C. (2001). Suppressingunwantedmemoriesby ex-
some memory impairment of trauma-related information); rather, the ecutive control.Nature,410, 131-134.
participants in this study appear to have failed to encode the trauma- Basden, B.H., Basden, D.R., & Gargano,G.J. (1993). Directed forgettingin
related information under divided-attention conditions. implicitand explicit memorytests:A comparisonof the methods.Journal
Research with preschool-age children suggests a similar effect. of ExperimentalPsychology: Learning, Memory,and Cognition, 19,
603-616.
Children in a community sample saw neutral and emotionally charged
Becker-Blease,K.A., Freyd,J.J., & Pears, K.C. (2004). Preschoolers'memory
pictures under selective- and divided-attention conditions (Becker- for threateninginformationdepends on traumahistory and attentional
Blease, Freyd, & Pears, 2004). Children who had trauma histories and context: Implicationsfor the developmentof dissociation. Journal of
who were highly dissociative recognized fewer charged pictures rel- Trauma& Dissociation,5, 113-131.
ative to nontraumatized children under divided-attention conditions; Bernstein,E.M.,& Putnam,F.W.(1986). Development,reliability,and validity
no group differences were found under selective-attention conditions. of a dissociation scale. Journal of Nervousand Mental Disease, 1 74,
727-735.
The current study is an important addition to correlational research
Brewin,C.R., Andrews,B., & Gotlib,I.H. (1993). Psychopathologyand early
examining the relation between exposure to trauma and memory im- experience:A reappraisalof retrospectivereports.PsychologicalBulle-
pairment. For example, previously we found that reports of abuse tin, 113, 82-98.
perpetrated by caregivers were related to higher degrees of reported Brewin,C.R., Dalgleish,T., & Joseph,S. (1996). A dual representationtheory
of posttraumaticstress disorder.PsychologicalReview,103, 670-686.
memory impairment than was abuse by noncaregivers (Freyd, De-
Cloitre,M.,Cancienne,J., Brodsky,B., Dulit, R., & Perry,S.W.(1996). Memory
Prince, & Zurbriggen, 2001). Such self-report research is inherently
performanceamong women with parental abuse histories: Enhanced
limited by its correlational nature and the potential confounds asso- directedforgettingor directed remembering?Journalof AbnormalPsy-
ciated with self-report of memory persistence (e.g., Schooler, 2001). chology,105, 204-211.
The current study demonstrated differences in experimental measures DePrince,A.P.(2001). Traumaand posttraumaticresponses: An examinationof
of memory for trauma-related stimuli without relying solely on self- fear and betrayal.Unpublisheddoctoraldissertation,Universityof Ore-
reports of memory persistence. gon, Eugene.
DePrince,A.P., & Freyd,J.J. (1999). Dissociative tendencies, attention,and
There are several limitations to the current work. This study used
memory.PsychologicalScience,10, 449-452.
an undergraduate sample, which likely does not reflect the more se- DePrince,A.P, & Freyd,J.J. (2001). Memoryand dissociativetendencies:The
vere distress observed in clinical groups. The self-reported trauma roles of attentionalcontext and word meaning.Journalof Traumaand
histories are limited in that they were not corroborated, though there is Dissociation,2, 67-82.
evidence that retrospective reports of early childhood events are Femina, D.D., Yeager,C.A., & Lewis, D.O. (1990). Child abuse: Adolescent
recordsvs. adult recall. ChildAbuse& Neglect, 14, 227-231.
reasonably reliable (Brewin, Andrews, & Gotlib, 1993). However,
Fergusson,D.M., Horwood,L.J., & Woodward,L.J. (2000). The stability of
empirical reports indicate that abuse rates based on a single report are child abuse reports:A longitudinalstudy of the reportingbehaviourof
likely to significantly underestimate true prevalence (Femina, Yeager, youngadults. PsychologicalMedicine,30, 529-544.
& Lewis, 1990; Fergusson, Horwood, & Woodward, 2000; Martin, Freyd, JJ. (1994). Betrayal-trauma: Traumaticamnesia as an adaptive re-
Anderson, Romans, & Mullen, 1993; Sjoberg & Lindblad, 2002). Such sponse to childhoodabuse. Ethics& Behavior,4, 307-329.
false negatives would likely decrease the strength of the observed Freyd,JJ. (1996). Betrayaltrauma:The logic of forgetting childhoodabuse.
correlation between dissociation and trauma. Cambridge,MA:HarvardUniversityPress.
Freyd, J.J. (2001). Memoryand dimensions of trauma:Terrormay be *all-
The current study adds to understanding of impaired memory for too-well remembered'and betrayalburied. In J.R. Conte (Ed.), Critical
trauma stimuli by examining memory for trauma-related information issuesin child sexual abuse:Historical,legal, and psychologicalperspec-
under experimental conditions. The research reported here specifi- tives (pp. 139-173). ThousandOaks, CA: Sage.

Volume 7
IS- Number 491
Forgetting Trauma Stimuli

Freyd,J.J., DePrince,A.P., & Zurbriggen,E.L. (2001). Self-reportedmemory McNally,R.J., Clancy,S.A., & Schacter,D.L. (2001). Directedforgettingof
for abuse depends upon victim-perpetratorrelationship. Journal of traumacues in adults reportingrepressed or recoveredmemoriesof
Trauma& Dissociation,2, 5-17. childhoodsexual abuse.Journalof AbnormalPsychology,110, 151-156.
Freyd,J.J., & Goldberg,L.R. (2003). Suggestedcategorizationof BBTS items McNally,R.J., Metzger,L.J.,Lasko,N.B., Clancy,S.A., & Pitman,R.K.(1998).
into A) high, medium,and low betrayalor B) more betrayaland less Directedforgettingof traumacues in adultsurvivorsof childhoodsexual
betrayal. Retrieved April 1, 2004, from http://dynamic.uoregon.edu/ abuse with and withoutposttraumaticstress disorder.Journalof Abnor-
~ jjf/bbts/categories.html mal Psychology,107, 596-601.
Freyd,J.J., Martorello,S.R., Alvarado,J.S., Hayes, A.E., & Christman,J.C. Moulds,M.L.,& Bryant,R.A. (2002). Directedforgettingin acute stress dis-
(1998). Cognitive environmentsand dissociative tendencies: Perfor- order.Journalof AbnormalPsychology,111, 175-179.
mance on the standardStroop task for high versus low dissociators. Putnam, F.W. (1997). Dissociationin children and adolescents.New York:
AppliedCognitivePsychology,12, S91-S103. GuilfordPress.
Goldberg, L.R., & Freyd, J.J. (2003). The Brief Betrayal TraumaSurvey Schooler,J.W. (2001). Discoveringmemoriesof abuse in the light of meta-
(BBTS). Retrieved April 1, 2004, from http://dynamic.uoregon.edu/ awareness.Journalof Aggression,Maltreatment& Trauma,4, 105-136.
~jjfybbts/ Sjoberg, R., & Lindblad,F. (2002). Limited disclosure of sexual abuse in
MacLeod,C. (1999). The item and list methodsof directed forgetting:Test children whose experiences were documentedby videotape.American
differencesand the role of demandcharacteristics.PsychonomicBulletin Journalof Psychiatry,159, 312-314.
& Review,6, 123-129.
Martin,J., Anderson,J., Romans,S., & Mullen,P. (1993). Askingaboutchild
sexual abuse: Methodologicalimplicationsof a two stage survey.Child
Abuse& Neglect,17, 383-392. (Received 4/17/03; Revisionaccepted 6/3/03)

492 Volume15- Number


7

You might also like