Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Main Article

Ger J Exerc Sport Res 2021 · 51:102–111 Dennis Murr1 · Paul Larkin2 · Oliver Höner1
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12662-020-00687-2 1
Institute of Sports Science, Eberhard Karls University, Tübingen, Germany
Received: 14 August 2019 2
Institute for Health and Sport, Victoria University, Melbourne, Australia
Accepted: 15 October 2020
Published online: 26 November 2020
© The Author(s) 2020
Decision-making skills of high-
performance youth soccer
players
Validating a video-based diagnostic
instrument with a soccer-specific motor
response

Introduction knowledge about a (current) situation players (e.g., Ruiz Pérez et al., 2014) re-
to select an appropriate decision, based search is scarce within talent promotion
In team sports like soccer, a multidimen- on one’s perceived ability to execute programs (e.g., regional association or
sional spectrum of performance factors is a context-specific motor skill. From youth national teams; youth academies)
required to perform at the elite level. This a sporting perspective, the ability to with high-performance level (e.g., elite)
has been acknowledged by Williams and make the correct decision during com- players. This statement is emphasized in
Reilly (2000) who developed a heuristic plex game situations, under high game a recent meta-analysis which explored
model for the categorization of soccer pressure and time constraints is a key cognitive functions measurements with
talent predictors. The model identifies component of in-game performance performance level as the moderator vari-
potential talent predictors across four (Höner, Larkin, Leber, & Feichtinger, able (Scharfen & Memmert, 2019). The
core areas of sport science, including 2020). Thus, decision-making has been results indicated, high-performance level
physical, physiological, psychological shown to be an important skill, with athletes had superior performance, with
and sociological characteristics. While several cross-sectional studies assess- a small to medium effect size, compared
there seems to be an emphasis on phys- ing decision-making performance and to low-performance level athletes. While
iological and physical characteristics in demonstrating that decision-making there is a plethora of studies examining
research and practice (Johnston, Wattie, skills discriminate between skilled and known group differences, a potential
Schorer, & Baker, 2018; Wilson et al., less-skilled players in team sports (e.g., reason for the lack of research examin-
2016), recently, there has been increased Diaz, Gonzalez, Garcia, & Mitchell, ing homogenous samples could lie in the
interest in the psychological attributes, 2011; Lorains, Ball, & MacMahon, 2013; fact that it is more difficult to find large
such as perceptual-cognitive factors Woods, Raynor, Bruce, & McDonald, effect sizes for discriminating athletes
(Mann, Dehghansai, & Baker, 2017). 2016). With respect to soccer, Ruiz of a similar ability (Bergkamp, Niessen,
Researchers have highlighted the im- Pérez et al. (2014) found Spanish club den Hartigh, Frencken, & Meijer, 2019).
portance of perceptual-cognitive factors players with international experience Despite the fact researchers have used
for skilled performance, with findings demonstrated better decision-making cross-sectional approaches to discrimi-
showing highly skilled players possess performance in comparison to local nate between high- and low-performance
superior decision-making, anticipation level players. Further, Höner (2005) decision-makers, Murr, Feichtinger,
and situational probability proficiency found youth national players had su- Larkin, O’Connor, and Höner (2018)
compared to their less-skilled coun- perior decision-making skills compared highlighted in their systematic review
terparts (e.g., Lex, Essig, Knoblauch, to local youth players, and additionally that there is a significant gap in the
& Schack, 2015; Ward, Ericsson, & older players (i.e., U17 age group) had literature concerning empirical evidence
Williams, 2013). a significant decision-making perfor- related to the predictive value of decision-
The focus of the current study is mance advantage over younger players making assessments. According to this
decision-making as the cognitive perfor- (i.e., U15 age group). While researchers review, only one investigation has used
mance factor. Causer and Ford (2014) have used an expertise approach to a video-based assessment to examine
define decision-making in sports as highlight superior performance of ex- the predictive ability of decision-making
a cognitive process in which one uses pert/elite players over novice/nonelite skills in soccer. O’Connor, Larkin, and

102 German Journal of Exercise and Sport Research 1 · 2021


Williams (2016) demonstrated a large Traditional diagnostic instruments with the ball while watching the video
significant effect in discriminating be- used to examine decision-making, such stimuli and then execute their decision
tween selected and nonselected players as video-based tests, provide advantages by passing to a player in the video).
within an Australian talent development in test execution or methodological con- Travassos et al. (2013) reaffirmed this
program. However, it should be noted trol and have demonstrated the ability issue and emphasized that an expertise
the prognostic period for this study to discriminate skilled and less-skilled effect is more consistent if participants
was very short (i.e., the selection at the players (e.g., Roca, Williams, & Ford, have to execute sport-specific actions in
conclusion of the data collection), with 2012). However, a limitation of previous experimental studies. Hadlow, Panchuk,
further research required to investigate investigations is the video presenta- Mann, Portus, and Abernethy (2018)
longer prognostic periods. Despite this tion stimuli which is often presented address these topics in a new classifi-
positive result, more research is needed to from a third-person perspective (i.e., cation framework to modify perceptual
develop valid and reliable measures that television broadcast perspective; e.g., training in sport and emphasize the im-
have a strong predictive value to assist O’Connor et al., 2016). It has been sug- portance of high sport-specific stimulus
the identification of high-performance gested however, when developing sport- and response correspondence.
level talent. specific assessments, that researchers
In addition to the lack of assessment consider increasing the representative- Present study
within high-performance level groups, ness of a task (Bonney, Berry, Ball, &
a further restriction of the current de- Larkin, 2019). Therefore, there could be With respect to their meta-analysis,
cision-making literature is the limited a benefit of presenting the video stimulus Travassos et al. (2013) suggested fur-
understanding of the potential per- from a first-person perspective which ther research is required to develop and
formance differences between playing represents a more realistic perceptual examine the impact of decision-mak-
positions. From a physical perspective, environment compared to broadcast ing instruments which integrate sport-
researchers have found physiological footage. specific perceptual (e.g., first-person
and anthropometric differences across A further limitation of the current perspective) task constraints and sport-
playing positions. For example, Rago, diagnostic instruments is the method of specific (e.g., passing or shooting) skill
Pizzuto, and Raiola (2017) revealed mid- responding to the video presentation, executions. Therefore, this study aims to
fielders and defenders completed more with participants providing a verbal, develop and evaluate a valid first-person
high-intensity running than forwards, written or button response (e.g., Larkin, perspective video-based diagnostic in-
and Boone, Vaeyens, Steyaert, Vanden O’Connor, & Williams, 2016; Ward strument which incorporates a soccer-
Bossche, and Bourgois (2012) found cen- & Williams, 2003). Currently, it re- specific motor response to the decision
tral defenders are taller and heavier than mains unclear if these nonsport-specific process and, thus, to provide a more
midfielders and wing defenders. There- responses correspond to real game situa- representative task.
fore, it is possible that due to specific tions and performance (van Maarseveen, To address this aim, three objectives
game-related roles of players in different Oudejans, Mann, & Savelsbergh, 2016). were pursued: In order to ensure a scien-
positions (e.g., make goals, organize Overall, the extant research lacks studies tific sound assessment, the reliability of
the build-up), in addition to physical examining decision-making competence the diagnostic instrument was evaluated
capabilities, playing position may also on video-based measures which incor- (Objective I). The focus of this study
influence decision-making ability (De- porate a sport-specific motor response. was on the criterion-related validity of
prez et al., 2015; Pocock, Dicks, Thelwell, To address this limitation, Hagemann, the diagnostic instrument (Objective II).
Chapman, & Barker, 2019). An initial Lotz, and Cañal-Bruland (2008) utilized Here, both diagnostic and prognostic
investigation by Höner (2005) attempted a video-based decision-making train- validation methods were used to ex-
to address this issue and demonstrated ing tool with a soccer-specific motor amine positive relationships between
that midfielders make better decisions response. Using a similar visual-mo- the diagnostics’ results and appropriate
compared to defenders and forwards. tor response (i.e., players had to pass performance factors (i.e., age groups
However, the results could be attributed the ball against different targets in the in middle to late adolescence, playing
to the situations used in the video scenes video), Frýbort, Kokštejn, Musálek, and time in official matches of the current
which presented more offensive deci- Süss (2016) investigated the influence of season, future draft in youth national
sions specific to midfield performance. varying exercise intensity on decision- team) resulting in three hypotheses:
Therefore, research is needed to address making time and accuracy. Although
this limitation by attempting to measure these investigations provide the foun- Diagnostic validity:
decision-making performance in dif- dation for incorporating soccer-specific
ferent game contexts, such as build-up responses within laboratory-based de- H1 Decision-making skills improve
(i.e., wing and central defense situations) cision-making training, to date no di- across adolescent age groups (i.e., U16;
and offensive (i.e., midfield and forward agnostic instruments assess decision- U17; U19) Specifically:
situations) game-based decisions. making skills using a more represen-
tative task (e.g., participants dribble

German Journal of Exercise and Sport Research 1 · 2021 103


Abstract

4 H1a: U17 soccer players demonstrate Ger J Exerc Sport Res 2021 · 51:102–111 https://doi.org/10.1007/s12662-020-00687-2
better decision-making skills than © The Author(s) 2020
U16 players
4 H1b: U19 soccer players demonstrate D. Murr · P. Larkin · O. Höner
better decision-making skills than Decision-making skills of high-performance youth soccer players.
U16 players Validating a video-based diagnostic instrument with a soccer-
4 H1c: U19 soccer players demonstrate specific motor response
better decision-making skills than
U17 players Abstract
Objectives. This study aimed to develop players (U17 > U16 in SCbu: Φ = 0.24 and SCoff:
a valid video-based diagnostic instrument Φ = 0.39, p < 0.01; U19 > U16 in SCbu: Φ = 0.41
H2 Players who played more minutes that assesses decision-making with a sport- and SCoff: Φ = 0.46, p < 0.01); however, there
in official matches of the current sea- specific motor response. was no difference between U17 and U19
son demonstrate better decision-making Methods. A total of 86 German youth aca- players. Furthermore, the predictive value of
skills than players with less minutes. demy players (16.7 ± 0.9 years) viewed game the test indicates that future youth national
situations projected on a large video screen team players make better decisions with
and were required to make a decision by respect to the build-up category (SCbu:
Prognostic validity: dribbling and passing to one of three targets Φ = 0.20; p < 0.05), whereas playing position
(representing different decision options). did not significantly influence decision-
H3 Future youth national team play- The test included 48 clips separated into making competence.
ers demonstrate better decision-making two categories: build-up (bu) and offensive Conclusion. Results indicate the video-based
decisions (off). Criterion-related validity was decision-making diagnostic instrument can
skills than nonselected players.
tested based on age (i.e., U16, U17, and discriminate decision-making competence
Finally, in an explorative analysis it U19), playing status (i.e., minutes played in within a high-performance youth group.
was examined whether decision-making official matches of the current season) and The outcomes associated with national
performance is influenced by playing po- in a prospective approach relating to future youth team participation demonstrate the
sition (Objective III). youth national team status (i.e., selected or predictive value of the diagnostic instrument.
nonselected). Finally, it was investigated This study provides initial evidence to suggest
whether decision-making competence was a new video-based diagnostic instrument
Methods influenced by playing position (i.e., defenders with a soccer-specific motor response can be
vs. midfielders vs. forwards). used within a talent identification process to
Sample and design Results. Instrumental reliability demonstrated assist with assessment of decision-making
satisfactory values for SCbu (r = 0.72), and performance.
The study sample consisted of 86 youth lower for SCoff (r = 0.56). Results showed
the diagnostic instrument is suitable for Keywords
academy players, born between 1996 and discriminating between playing status (SCbu: Football · Talent identification · Adolescence ·
2001, from a professional German soc- Φ = 0.22, p < 0.01; SCoff: Φ = 0.14, p < 0.05) Perceptual cognitive factors · Athletic
cer club. The players competed in the and between younger (U16) and older performance
highest German youth league, and thus
belong to the top 1% of German players
for their age groups (i.e., U16, U17, and
U19). At the first measurement point the dent variable age group (H1) were ex- of all 86 players were captured. Play-
players were 15–19 years of age (median amined splitting the sample into three ers who participated at least in one Ger-
[Mage] = 16.7 years, standard deviation age categories (U16: N = 41, U17: N = 55 man youth national team training course
[SDage] = 0.96) and were tested over a 3- and U19: N = 44; H1a–H1c). A further in subsequent seasons (i.e., 2015/2016;
year period (i.e., near the end of seasons analysis of the criterion-related validity 2016/2017; 2017/2018) were identified as
2014/15 to 2016/17), resulting in three was conducted on the independent vari- selected (N = 16), with all other players
measurement points (T2015, T2016, and able playing status (H2) that was deter- identified as nonselected (N = 70). Fur-
T2017). Over the 3-year period, a total mined based on minutes played in of- thermore, with respect to the explorative
of 140 data points were collected. Due to ficial matches (i.e., U19/U17 German analysis (Objective III), players were sep-
the nature of professional youth academy Youth Bundesliga and U16 Oberliga). arated by playing positions as defined by
selection processes (i.e., some players are Median split procedure was utilized to their respective coaches (i.e., defenders
deselected, and new players are recruited separate players into two categories: first [DF, N = 55]; midfielders [MF, N = 61];
to the academy) which resulted in not ev- team regular (i.e., who play more minutes and forwards [FW, N = 24]).
ery player being tested at all three time than median; N = 70) and reserve play-
points (. Table 1). ers (i.e., who play less minutes than the Decision-making diagnostic
To examine the three hypothesizes, median; N = 68). Further, to examine the instrument
the study sample was separated into three predictive value of the decision-making Stimulus materials Decision-making
subsamples. To assess diagnostic crite- diagnostic instrument relative to future competence was assessed using a newly
rion-related validity (Objective II), po- youth national team status (H3), data of developed soccer-specific video-based
tential differences between the indepen- the respective first measurement point diagnostic instrument. To create the

104 German Journal of Exercise and Sport Research 1 · 2021


Table 1 Datapointscollectedwithrespect two passing options and one option to Only one correct option was determined
to age group and measurement time point shoot at goal. Between each trial, a black by the expert panel for each scenario.
Age Measurement point Total screen remained for 6 s. Each clip com- Any trial where the participant missed
group T2015 T2016 T2017 menced with the first frame of the video a target or did not decide before the
U16 13 12 16 41 frozen for 1.5 s, to orientate the partici- scene ended was also coded as a zero.
U17 21 17 17 55 pants to the situation. Subsequently, participants’ decisions
from 48 testing video situations were
U19 15 14 15 44
Procedure Each participant individually used for calculating offensive (SCoff; i.e.,
Total 49 43 48 140
completed the test in the same indoor mean value of 24 corresponding scenes)
room at the youth academy over a 6- and build-up (SCbu; i.e., mean value of
diagnostic instrument, more than 300 week period at each measurement point 24 corresponding scenes) decision accu-
dynamic on-ball decision-making situ- (i.e., T2015; T2016; T2017). At the com- racy scores. Both scores (i.e., SCoff and
ations, recorded from the first-person mencement of each test, the participant SCbu) were converted to a percentage of
perspective of the ball carrier at different was provided with a standardized in- correct decisions. As the primary aim
positions on the pitch (i.e., forward, struction (e.g., “In each scene you start of the study was to determine decision-
midfield, wing defense, central defense; to dribble from one of five positions.”; making accuracy, we did not calculate or
. Fig. 1) were designed and filmed as “A short freeze frame at the start of each assess speed of decision-making within
part of a pilot study (Dieze, 2015). video will help you to orientate to the cur- the study.
The footage of each situation moved like rent game situation.”; “During the drib-
a player in possession of the ball and were bling your decision is based on passing Statistical analyses
reviewed by a panel of expert coaches the ball to one of three targets that rep-
(i.e., one UEFA pro-level, one UEFA A- resent the position of your teammate.”). Data were analyzed using SPSS ver-
level and two UEFA B-level). During the Prior to each testing block, three famil- sion 24. Diagnostics’ instrumental relia-
review process, a round table forum was iarization video scenes were presented bility (Objective I) was examined using
held, whereby the panel discussed the for participants to become accustomed a split-half procedure (odd-even-method
outcome of each clip (i.e., best correct to the procedures and the respective cat- corrected by Spearman–Brown formula;
decision), until 100% agreement was egory situation. Participants were then Lienert & Raatz, 2011) for players’ first
reached. Following this detailed evalua- able to ask any questions they had in assessment results.
tion by the expert panel, 30 video scenes relation to the video or response pro- An examination of distributional
were selected as the most realistic game cedures. Following the familiarization properties revealed the decision-making
situations and were thus used for the videos, the 12 testing videos were pre- accuracy scores SCoff and SCbu were not
study (24 testing scenes; 6 familiarization sented. For each trial, participants were normally distributed. A Mann–Whit-
scenes). positioned with the ball on a starting ney-U-test (as one-tailed hypothesis) was
To ensure a more comprehensive di- position which represented the location used to determine differences in SCoff
agnostic instrument, the 24 testing video on the pitch. As the video commenced and SCbu between age groups, playing
scenes were mirrored (i.e., the same scene the participants started to dribble with status and future youth national team
presented from the opposite direction), the ball while watching the evolving game status (Objective II, H1–H3). To ex-
creating a final diagnostic instrument of situation on the screen. Prior to overstep- plore mean differences between playing
48 game situations. Each situation was ping a limiting line orthe videoscene end- positions, nonparametric Kruskal–Wal-
then classified as either offensive deci- ing the participant provided a response lis tests were conducted with post hoc
sions (i.e., situations in forward and mid- by passing to one of three possible tar- pairwise comparisons (Objective III).
field positions where players create scor- gets (i.e., supporting teammates in the As the number of measurement points
ing opportunities) or decisions that occur video, . Fig. 2). For each trial, the first may influence decision-making scores
in the build-up of a game (i.e., situations author recorded the target (i.e., decision) and consequently the results in regards to
in defensive positions such as wing and the participants passed the ball to, while the diagnostics’ validity (due to memo-
central defense where players start offen- a research assistant placed a ball on the rable or habituation effects), a priori anal-
sive moves). For testing, the video scenes next starting position. ysis was conducted to determine whether
were projected on a 2.76 m wide × 1.50 m the number of measurement points cor-
high video screen and were presented Dependent measures The decision- related with the independent variables
in four video blocks of 12 videos (i.e., making accuracy for each video situa- playing status, age group, future youth
forward; midfield; wing defense; central tion was assessed using a coding criterion national team status, and playing posi-
defense) completed by all participants. of one for a correct decision (i.e., passing tion. The variables age group (rs = 0.18;
During the test, each video scene was or shooting to the best option identified p < 0.05) and playing position (rs = 0.22;
5–6 s in duration and had three possible by the expert panel) and zero for an p < 0.01) correlated with the number of
passing options; however, in the forward incorrect decision (i.e., passing or shoot- measurement points. Therefore, in a sec-
category players had the choice between ing to an option not rated the best). ond step and as an additional analysis for

German Journal of Exercise and Sport Research 1 · 2021 105


Main Article

Fig. 1 8 Example of a midfield scene used within the first-person perspec-


tive video-based decision-making diagnostic instrument

the effects of age groups and playing posi-


tion, the number of measurement points
served as a manifest covariate. This was
to assess whether the number of mea-
surement points influenced the result pat-
tern and statistical decisions with respect
to age groups. As there is no statisti-
cal procedure controlling for a covariate
in nonnormal distributional properties,
ANCOVAs (analyses of covariance) were
conducted in addition to Mann–Whit-
ney-U-tests (post hoc analysis) to assess
for possible influences of the number of
measurement points on the results. Fig. 2 8 The structure of the first-person perspective video-based decision-
Effect sizes ω and Φ for nonparamet- making diagnostic instrument experimental set up
ric tests were calculated and classified in
accordance to Cohen (1992). To deter- As H2 and H3 investigated a group tus (H3; Objective II). Mann–Whitney-
mine the size of a possible population prediction, additional stepwise logistic U-tests identified significant differences
effect (within objective II and III), sensi- regressions were conducted. The overall (each p < 0.01) in decision-making com-
tivity was calculated by post hoc power model fit was examined with the like- petence between U17 and U16 (H1a;
analyses using G*Power version 3.1.9.7. lihood ratio chi-square (X2) test. The U17 > U16 in SCbu: Z = 2.35, Φ = 0.24
G*power provides sensitivity calcula- SCoff and SCbu regression coefficients as and in SCoff: Z = 3.82, Φ = 0.39), as
tions for mean comparisons between well as the odds ratios eβ and their 95% well as between U19 and U16 players
two groups only based on Cohen’s d. confidence intervals were calculated with (H1b; U19 > U16, SCbu: Φ = 0.41 and
The analyses determined the sensitivity reference to the respective selection cri- SCoff: Φ = 0.46). However, no significant
for objective II (α = 0.05, 1 – β = 0.85, teria (i.e., “reserve player” for H2; “non- differences were found between the com-
one-tailed), whereas H1 ranged from selected” for H3). Finally, individual se- parison of U19 and U17 players (H1c;
0.56 ≤ d ≤ 0.60, with H2 equal to d = 0.47, lection probabilities were determined for U17 > U19 in SCbu: Z = 0.04, p = 0.48 and
and H3 equal to d = 0.77. Regarding ob- playing status and future youth national U19 > U17 in SCoff: Z = 0.69, p = 0.244).
jective III, the power analyses (α = 0.05, team status on the basis of a player’s SCoff In relation to playing status (H2) first-
1 – β = 0.85, two-tailed) provided a range and SCbu. team regular players performed signif-
of 0.57 ≤ d ≤ 0.76. Therefore, medium icantly better than reserve players in
effect sizes could be detected within the Results both build-up and offensive situations,
present study. As the present study uti- with low to moderate effect sizes (SCbu:
lized Φ as an effect size, this corresponds Instrumental reliability demonstrated Z = 2.57, Φ = 0.22, p < 0.01 and SCoff:
to a range of Φ between 0.30 and 0.50. satisfactory values for build-up scenes Z = 1.69, Φ = 0.14, p < 0.05). With re-
Due to the fact that only three measure- (r = 0.72), and lower for offensive scenes spect to the stepwise logistic regression,
ment points are available for a very small (r = 0.56; Objective I). . Table 2 presents only build-up situations showed signif-
number of players, a longitudinal study an overview of the decision-making icance and therefore remained in the
was omitted. scores relative to age (H1) playing status model (χ2(1) = 8.00, p < 0.01). The odds
(H2) and future youth national team sta- ratios e β from the logistic regression

106 German Journal of Exercise and Sport Research 1 · 2021


Table 2 Descriptive and inferential statistics for criterion-related validity with regards to age group (Objective II, H1), playing status (Objective II, H2),
future youth national team players (Objective II, H3), and the explorative analysis of playing position (Objective III)
Variables Decision Descriptive Effect size
accuracy statistics Post hoc analysis/Mann–Whitney U-test Kruskal–Wallis
score (%) (M ± SD) test
Age group U16 U17 U19 U16 vs. U17 U16 vs. U19 U17 vs. U19
(N = 41) (N = 55) (N = 44)
Φ
SCbu 66 ± 14 73 ± 14 71 ± 18 0.24** 0.41** 0.00
SCoff 63 ± 11 74 ± 13 75 ± 14 0.39** 0.46** 0.00
Playing status FTRP RP FTRP vs. RP
(N = 70) (N = 68)
Φ
SCbu 74 ± 13 67 ± 17 0.22**
SCoff 73 ± 13 69 ± 15 0.14*
Youth national team Selected Nonselected Selected vs.
(N = 16) (N = 70) nonselected

Φ
SCbu 73 ± 11 65 ± 16 0.20*
SCoff 69 ± 11 66 ± 13 0.08
Playing positiona DF MF FW DF vs. MF DF vs. FW MF vs. FW
(N = 55) (N = 61) (N = 24)
Φ ω
SCbu 71 ± 16 72 ± 13 63 ± 17 0.00 0.21 0.25* 0.44#
SCoff 70 ± 16 73 ± 13 69 ± 9 0.07 0.07 0.15 0.15
FTRP First team regular player, RP Reserve player, DF Defenders, MF Midfielders, FW Forwards, SCbu Decision accuracy score for build-up, SCoff Decision
accuracy score for offensive
**p < 0.01, *p < 0.05; # p < 0.10
a
Playing position correlated significantly with the number of measurement points. However, ANCOVAs (analysis of covariance) with the main factor playing
position and the covariate number of measurement points demonstrated no changes in the statistical decisions regarding the multiple group comparison.
Thus, the results pattern for playing position is independent of the number of measurement points

model indicated that a one standard up score improved the chance of being tical decisions regarding the multiple
deviation (SD = 0.153) increase in build- selected for a future youth national team group comparisons of playing position.
up score, improved the chance of being by a factor of 1.89 (= (eβ)0.153 = 64.200.153).
a first-team regular player by a factor of Regarding playing position (Objec- Discussion
1.65 (= (e β )0.153 = 26.690.153). tive III), the results for the different
With respect to the prognostic va- subsamples (i.e. DF, MF, FW) in both The aim of this study was to develop
lidity (H3), Mann–Whitney-U-test build-up and offensive decision category a valid video-based decision-making
demonstrated that selected players ranged in mean from 69–72% with only diagnostic instrument which presented
have a higher decision-making accu- one exception (i.e., the result of 63% footage from a first-person perspective
racy than nonselected players in build- from FW deviated in their nonposition- and incorporated a soccer-specific mo-
up (Z = 1.82, Φ = 0.20, p < 0.05) and of- specific build-up category compared to tor response. A further strength of the
fensive (Z = 0.78, p = 0.217) situations; other playing positions distinctly). De- study was the video stimuli were not
however, only build-up situations pro- spite these facts, the Kruskal–Wallis tests limited to one game situation, but rather
vided a significant difference. Analyzing did not reveal significant differences in different situations (i.e., build-up and of-
the prediction of future youth national any decision-making competence (SCbu: fensive decisions). While there were no
team status with regard to the logistic H(2) = 5.26, p = 0.072; SCoff: H(2) = 1.81, differences between playing positions,
regression models led to the same re- p = 0.404). In terms of possible influ- the study does provide a foundation
sult as for the playing status variable. ences caused by correlations between for future research. In particular, the
Only build-up score demonstrated sig- the manifest variable, the number of diagnostic instrument was developed as
nificance and therefore remained in the measurement points, and the indepen- a measure to discriminate decision-mak-
model (χ2(1) = 4.15, p < 0.05). A one dent variable playing position, ANCOVA ing competence within a group of youth
standard deviation increases in build- demonstrated no changes in the statis- high-performance level players. Results

German Journal of Exercise and Sport Research 1 · 2021 107


Main Article

showed the diagnostic instrument, which these situations by exchanging current enough to differentiate performance of
included realistic soccer video-scenes in lower reliability clips with newly devel- high-performing late adolescent athletes
combination with a soccer-specific mo- oped higher reliability clips. Also adding requires further investigation. A possi-
tor response, is a suitable instrument more items to the diagnostic instrument ble explanation as to why there was no
for discriminating playing status and could be an approach for improving significant difference between the U17
partially for age (U19 and U17 > U16). the reliability and even reduce the need and U19 players (H1c) could be due to
Further, it provides a more representa- for large sample sizes (Schweizer et al., at least two possible reasons. First, from
tive assessment compared to previous 2020). the perspective of the U19 team, the
studies where the diagnostic instrument This study advances current sport- best players from the squad had already
was limited by a lack of a soccer-specific based decision-making literature by played for the senior professional team
response (e.g., Bennett, Novak, Pluss, demonstrating the ability to develop (i.e., in either the first or reserve/second
Coutts, & Fransen, 2019). An additional a decision-making diagnostic instru- team) and therefore were not able to
aim was to examine the predictive value ment in which the process of visual participate in the investigation. In rela-
of the new video-based decision-making perception of in-game video-scenes (i.e., tion to the U17 group, two of the three
diagnostic instrument, with the findings first person perspective) is more chal- U17 cohort squads in this study were
indicating that in parts of the study (i.e., lenging by an additional soccer-specific very successful and participated in the
in build-up categories) future youth na- motor action (i.e., dribbling) and re- finals of the German U17 championship
tional team players perform better in the sponse (i.e. passing). Traditional video- (i.e., the highest level of competition at
decision-making diagnostic instrument. based instruments which assess decision- this age). Therefore, it seems feasible
Using reliable diagnostic instruments making performance generally present that despite the age difference with re-
is fundamental to scientific work. The footage from a broadcast (i.e., television spect to U19 group, both groups were of
implications associated with using a di- broadcast) or third person perspective a very similar performance level (limit-
agnostic instrument with a lack of, or associated with nonsoccer-specific de- ing the studies’ internal reliability with
unknown, reliability is whether the par- cision response (i.e., written, verbal or regard to discriminating between age
ticipant performance differences are due button response). In this context, Mann, groups). Second, this finding supports
to random test error or actual per- Farrow, Shuttleworth, Hopwood, and other studies to investigate age-related
formance changes of the participants MacMahon (2009) indicated using a di- soccer-specific performance skill differ-
(Gadotti, Vieira, & Magee, 2006). In agnostic instrument from first person- ences. For example, Huijgen, Elferink-
comparison to the assessment of manifest perspective is more realistic. However, Gemser, Post, and Visscher (2010) high-
variables (e.g., time, height, distance), it was noted this may be more difficult lighted there were no improvements
the measurement of latent constructs from a decision-making perspective than from ages 16 to 19 in dribbling and
such as decision-making competence using footage from an aerial perspective, sprinting, which the authors attributed
poses a much larger challenge. While and therefore may provide more robust to the end of puberty. Furthermore,
researchers have highlighted the limited diagnostic instruments. Furthermore, it Beavan et al. (2019) revealed in a battery
reporting of reliability for new diagnos- has been suggested the lack of sport-spe- of cognitive function tests (e.g., Vienna
tic instruments (Hadlow et al., 2018; cific responses may limit the correlation Test System, which measures inhibition
Schweizer, Furley, Rost, & Barth, 2020), between video-based tests and actual and cognitive flexibility) that there were
a key aim of the current study was to in-game decision-making performance no differences between U19 and U17 age
measure the reliability of the latent con- (e.g., van Maarseveen et al., 2016). This groups. At this stage, further research
struct decision-making (Objective I). is further supported by Travassos et al. is needed to explore whether there are
Here, an adequate level of reliability (2013) who indicated that perceptual- peak developmental phases for cognitive
for the build-up situations was found; cognitive assessments need to consider factors such as decision-making.
however, the lower reliability for offen- the task representativeness when de- A strength of this study is that the
sive situations has to be considered as veloping sport-specific diagnostic in- sample consisted of participants from
a larger limitation. A potential reason struments. By developing assessments, one of the most successful youth soccer
for the lower reliability of the offensive which consider representative task con- academies in Germany, thus, result-
situations could be due to the restric- straints, such as presenting decision- ing in a very homogeneous high-level
tion of range phenomenon within the making situations from a first-person group. This is not a common approach
high-performance level group (i.e., ho- perspective, combined with a sport- used by researchers investigating soccer-
mogeneous expert samples) the study specific motor response, enhances the specific decision-making skills as they
was conducted (Schweizer et al., 2020). validity of the assessment measure. have generally conducted cross-sectional
Despite this limitation, the current study While the current study demonstrated studies comparing and identifying skill
does provide a method for establishing the ability to differentiate U16 and older differences between groups of high-
the reliability of the offensive decision- adolescent athletes (i.e., U17 and U19, performance and low-performance level
making situations. In addition, future objective II), developing a decision- participants (i.e., sub-elite; intermediate;
studies may improve the reliability of making diagnostic instrument sensitive novice participants; Diaz et al., 2011;

108 German Journal of Exercise and Sport Research 1 · 2021


jective III). However, while assump-
tions were based on previous investiga-
tions which have demonstrated differ-
ences based on playing positions (e.g.,
Höner, 2005), the current findings did not
reveal a significant difference. Despite
this finding, the novel approach used in
the study (i.e., provide the players video-
scenes from different playing positions
Fig. 3 9 Selection on the pitch) may enable more detailed
probabilities ob-
tained from the lo- comparisons between playing positions
gistic regression in future. Hence, research should divide
for reaching first playing positions in a stricter manner,
team regular player for instance when considering the posi-
(FTRP) and for draft- tion of a forward, there may be specific
ing youth national
team (YNT) status differences between central forwards and
wide forwards (i.e., wingers).
As this is one of the first studies to
Roca et al., 2012; Scharfen & Memmert, course) by a factor of 1.89. The probabil- develop a decision-making diagnostic in-
2019). Therefore, while the findings of ity curve (. Fig. 3) increased more rapidly strument which links visual perception of
the current study indicated only a small for higher SCbu values and therefore re- first-person video scenes and soccer-spe-
significant effect size for discriminat- sults proved the diagnostics’ prognostic cific motor responses in the assessment,
ing playing status (H2), the possible relevance, as participants who perform the findings should be considered with
reason for this may be attributed to the better on the assessment are also more respect to several limitations. First, the
high sensitivity when comparing athletes likely to be selected. This finding sup- execution-time of decision-making was
within a homogeneous high-level group. ports other video-based research which not measured directly. Decision-making
This supports previous findings by van has demonstrated the ability to predict se- was only restricted by the length of the
Maarseveen et al. (2016) who also failed lectionintohighperforming youthsoccer video scenes and a marked limiting line.
to detect significant differences in per- programs (O’Connor et al., 2016). This Currently, there are different opinions
ceptual-cognitive skills between talented indicates the possibility for a video-based about the importance of execution time
female soccer players of a comparable diagnostic instrument with a soccer-spe- with respect to decision-making. There
highly skilled performance level (i.e., cific motor response to be used within seems to be the belief that experienced
national soccer talent team). Never- the talent identification process to assist players make quicker and more accurate
theless, the logistic regression models with the assessment of players’ decision- decisions compared to less skilled play-
demonstrated that players with good making performance. The nonparamet- ers (Vaeyens, Lenoir, Williams, Mazyn, &
decision accuracy on the diagnostic in- ric analysis demonstrated a small signifi- Philippaerts, 2007). Nevertheless, a fur-
strument will have a higher probability cant effect size for SCbu and no significant ther development of the diagnostic in-
of being a first team regular player (SCbu: difference for SCoff between selected and strument could include the measurement
Odds Ratio = 1.65; . Fig. 3). Therefore, nonselected future youth national team of execution-time to determine whether
the results provide initial evidence to players. However, this finding may have highly skilled players decide faster than
suggest the new diagnostic instrument been limited by the relatively low num- less skilled players or whether they wait
can potentially identify more skilled ber of participants selected into national even longer for the perfect moment to ex-
individuals within a high-performance youth programs (N = 16). Therefore, fu- ecute the appropriate response. Second,
level youth group. ture research should consider increasing the current sample was limited by the re-
Further, in comparison to previous the number of national youth players in strictions of testing within a professional
investigations which only assessed per- the sample to fully understand the po- sporting club environment. As such, sev-
formance using a cross-sectional design tential decision-making skill differences eral high-performing U19 athletes were
(e.g., Höner, 2005), the current investi- between these high performing players not included in the sample due to pro-
gation implemented a prospective design (i.e., selected; nonselected for youth na- fessional club commitments. As a result,
to allow for a greater understanding of tional team; Ackerman, 2014). it may be possible that the lack of signif-
potential future soccer success (H3). The The present study extended the cur- icant differences between U17 and U19
logistic regression model for SCbu indi- rent research knowledge related decision- players in the current sample may not
cated that players who performed well on making assessments by developing a di- fully describe the potential age-related
the assessment improved the chance of agnostic instrument which incorporated differences between these two groups.
being selected (i.e., selected to participate decision situations from defense, mid- Therefore, future investigations should
in a German youth national team training field, and forward playing positions (Ob- aim to sample all athletes from within an

German Journal of Exercise and Sport Research 1 · 2021 109


Main Article

age group to ensure a true representation formance youth group, in particular for Funding. Open Access funding enabled and orga-
nized by Projekt DEAL.
of the performance group. Finally, sim- playing status and to some extent age.
ilar to other studies (e.g., Höner, 2005), Although the reliability of the diagnostic
this study limited the number of options instrument is satisfied for the build-up Compliance with ethical
(i.e., three option per situation) and the category, future research should aim for guidelines
sport-specific motor response (i.e., play- higher reliability for all measured vari-
ers only had the options to pass or shoot). ables (e.g., offensive decision), by using Conflict of interest. D. Murr, P. Larkin and O. Höner
Future studies should also consider the the guidelines proposed, for example, declare that they have no competing interests.
development of diagnostic instruments by Schweizer et al. (2020). Further, the
All procedures performed in studies involving human
which include other decision-making op- results demonstrate the predictive value participants were in accordance with the ethical stan-
tions, such as dribbling. While observa- of the new video-based decision-making dards of the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later
tional studies (e.g., recording decision- diagnostic instrument, especially the amendments or comparable ethical standards. The
ethics department of the Faculty of Economics and
making skills in small-sided games) such build-up situation for national youth Social Sciences at the University of Tübingen and the
as Romeas, Guldner, and Faubert (2016) team selection. However, future investi- youth academy of the professional soccer club ap-
have attempted to assess the whole spec- gations with a longer prognostic period proved the implementation of this study. All players
and legal guardians (i.e., parents) provided informed
trum of decision-making alternatives, it (e.g., up to senior level) is of interest to consent prior to participation in the study.
is a challenge developing such situations strengthen the results. With respect to
within a video stimuli. A possible alter- playing positions, no differences were Open Access. This article is licensed under a Creative
Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which
native maybe developing diagnostic in- found between positions; however, fur- permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and re-
struments that provide more graduations ther examination of playing position on production in any medium or format, as long as you
of decisions (i.e., best option; second best decision-making skills is warranted (e.g., give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and
the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons li-
option; etc.), rather than current binary are midfielder better decision-makers re- cence, and indicate if changes were made. The images
forms (e.g., Bennett et al., 2019). Further, gardless on which area of the pitch the or other third party material in this article are included
off-the-ball decision-making in defensive decision-making skill is required?). in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless in-
dicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If
game situations such as positioning close Considering these results, this study material is not included in the article’s Creative Com-
or far from the ball, which has not been provides initial evidence to suggest a soc- mons licence and your intended use is not permitted
assessed in the literature, should also be cer-specific video-based diagnostic in- by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use,
you will need to obtain permission directly from the
considered in future research. strument can be used within talent identi- copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit
fication processes to assist with the assess- http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
Conclusion ment of players’ decision-making perfor-
mance. Finally, future decision-making
Sport-based decision-making is a cogni- assessment studies can use similar proce- References
tive process in which athletes use their dures to those employed in the current in-
knowledge about a (current) situation to vestigation to develop a valid and reliable Ackerman, P. L. (2014). Nonsense, common sense,
and science of expert performance: talent and
select an appropriate decision based on video-based decision-making diagnostic individual differences. Intelligence, 45, 6–17.
their perceived ability to execute a sport- instrument in other sports/domains. In https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.04.009.
specific motor skill response (Causer & addition, further exploration of the inte- Beavan, A. F., Spielmann, J., Mayer, J., Skorski, S.,
Meyer, T., & Fransen, J. (2019). Age-related
Ford, 2014). While decision-making is gration of motor specific responses with differences in executive functions within high-
defined by the ability to perceive appro- the video footage to create an even more level youth soccer players. Brazilian Journal of
priate stimuli and execute a sport-specific realistic diagnostic instrument may be Motor Behavior, 13(2), 64–75.
Bennett, K. J. M., Novak, A. R., Pluss, M. A., Coutts, A. J.,
skill response, traditional video-based possible. One approach would be to use & Fransen, J. (2019). Assessing the validity of
instruments assess decision-making us- virtual reality technology to allow body a video-based decision-making assessment for
ing nonsport-specific responses such as parts (e.g., foot) interacting with the stim- talent identification in youth soccer. Journal of
Science and Medicine in Sport, 22(6), 729–734.
verbal, written, or button responses (e.g., ulus material or to measure a player’s in https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2018.12.011.
Roca et al., 2012). This study addressed situ task constraints. Bergkamp, T. L. G., Niessen, A. S. M., den Hartigh, R. J. R.,
this limitation by developing a valid Frencken, W. G. P., & Meijer, R. R. (2019). Method-
ological issues in soccer talent identification
first-person perspective video-based Corresponding address research. Sports Medicine. https://doi.org/10.
diagnostic instrument which requires 1007/s40279-019-01113-w.
a soccer-specific motor response (i.e., Dennis Murr Bonney, N., Berry, J., Ball, K., & Larkin, P. (2019).
Institute of Sports Science, Australian football skill-based assessments: a
participants dribble with the ball while proposed model for future research. Frontiers
Eberhard Karls University
watching a video stimulus and then exe- Tübingen, Germany in Psychology, 10, 429–429. https://doi.org/10.
cute their decision by passing to a player 3389/fpsyg.2019.00429.
dennis.murr@uni-
Boone, J., Vaeyens, R., Steyaert, A., Vanden Bossche,
in the video). The results indicate the tuebingen.de L., & Bourgois, J. (2012). Physical fitness of
© Mario
video-based decision-making diagnostic elite Belgian soccer players by player position.
Heilemann Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research,
instrument can discriminate decision-
making competence within a high-per-

110 German Journal of Exercise and Sport Research 1 · 2021


26(8), 2051–2057. https://doi.org/10.1519/JSC. soccer? Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, a meta-analytic review. Applied Cognitive
0b013e318239f84f. 28(2), 129–138. https://doi.org/10.1080/ Psychology, 33(5), 843–860.
Causer, J., & Ford, P. R. (2014). ‘Decisions, decisions, 10413200.2015.1085922. Schweizer, G., Furley, P., Rost, N., & Barth, K.
decisions’: transfer and specificity of decision- Lex, H., Essig, K., Knoblauch, A., & Schack, T. (2020). Reliable measurement in sport
making skill between sports. Cognitive (2015). Cognitive representations and cognitive psychology: The case of performance outcome
Processing, 15(3), 385–389. https://doi.org/10. processing of team-specific tactics in soccer. measures. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 48,
1007/s10339-014-0598-0. PLoS One, 10(2), e118219. https://doi.org/10. 101663. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.
Cohen, J. (1992). Statistical power analysis. Current 1371/journal.pone.0118219. 2020.101663.
Directions in Psychological Science, 1(3), 98–101. Lienert, G. A., & Raatz, U. (2011). Testaufbau Travassos, B., Araujo, D., Davids, K., O’hara, K., Leitão,
Deprez,D.,Fransen,J.,Boone,J.,Lenoir,M.,Philippaerts, und Testanalyse [Setup and analysis of tests]. J., & Cortinhas, A. (2013). Expertise effects
R., & Vaeyens, R. (2015). Characteristics of high- Weinheim: Beltz. on decision-making in sport are constrained
level youth soccer players: variation by playing Lorains, M., Ball, K., & MacMahon, C. (2013). Expertise by requisite response behaviours—A meta-
position. Journal of Sports Sciences, 33(3), differences in a video decision-making task: analysis. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 14(2),
243–254. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414. speed influences on performance. Psychology of 211–219.
2014.934707. Sport and Exercise, 14(2), 293–297. https://doi. Vaeyens, R., Lenoir, M., Williams, A. M., Mazyn, L.,
Diaz, D. C. D., Gonzalez, V. S., Garcia, L. L., & Mitchell, org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2012.11.004. & Philippaerts, R. M. (2007). The effects of
S. (2011). Differences in decision-making van Maarseveen, M. J. J., Oudejans, R. R. D., Mann, task constraints on visual search behavior and
development between expert and novice D. L., & Savelsbergh, G. J. P. (2016). Perceptual- decision-making skill in youth soccer players.
invasion game players. Perceptual and motor cognitive skill and the in situ performance Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 29(2),
skills, 112(3), 871. of soccer players. The Quarterly Journal of 147–169.
Dieze, S. (2015). Positionsspezifische Diagnostik der Experimental Psychology. https://doi.org/10. Ward, P., & Williams, A. M. (2003). Perceptual and
Entscheidungskompetenz im Fußball: Entwick- 1080/17470218.2016.1255236. cognitive skill development in soccer: the
lung und erste Validierung einer Videotestbatterie Mann, D., Dehghansai, N., & Baker, J. (2017). multidimensional nature of expert performance.
[Position-specific diagnostics of decision-making Searching for the elusive gift: advances in Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 25(1),
in soccer: developing and validation of a video- talent identification in sport. Current Opinion in 93–111.
based test battery]. Tübingen: Eberhard Karls Psychology, 16(SupplementC), 128–133. https:// Ward, P., Ericsson, K. A., & Williams, A. M. (2013).
Universität Tübingen. doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.04.016. Complex perceptual-cognitive expertise in
Frýbort, P., Kokštejn, J., Musálek, M., & Süss, V. (2016). Mann, D., Farrow, D., Shuttleworth, R., Hopwood, a simulated task environment. Journal of
Does physical loading affect the speed and M., & MacMahon, C. (2009). The influence of Cognitive Engineering and Decision Making, 7(3),
accuracy of tactical decision-making in elite viewing perspective on decision-making and 231–254.
junior soccer players? Journal of Sports Science & visual search behaviour in an invasive sport. Williams, A. M., & Reilly, T. (2000). Talent identification
Medicine, 15(2), 320–326. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 40(4), and development in soccer. Journal of Sports
Gadotti, I., Vieira, E., & Magee, D. (2006). Importance 546–564. Sciences, 18(9), 657–667.
and clarification of measurement properties Murr, D., Feichtinger, P., Larkin, P., O’Connor, D., Wilson, R. S., James, R. S., David, G., Hermann, E.,
in rehabilitation. Brazilian Journal of Physical & Höner, O. (2018). Psychological talent Morgan, O. J., Niehaus, A. C., et al. (2016).
Therapy, 10(2), 137–146. predictors in youth soccer: a systematic review Multivariate analyses of individual variation in
Hadlow, S. M., Panchuk, D., Mann, D. L., Portus, M. R., of the prognostic relevance of psychomotor, soccer skill as a tool for talent identification and
& Abernethy, B. (2018). Modified perceptual perceptual-cognitive and personality-related development: utilising evolutionary theory in
training in sport: a new classification framework. factors. PLoS One, 13(10), e205337. https://doi. sportsscience. Journal of Sports Sciences. https://
Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 21(9), org/10.1371/journal.pone.0205337. doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2016.1151544.
950–958. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsams.2018. O’Connor, D., Larkin, P., & Williams, A. M. (2016). Woods, C. T., Raynor, A. J., Bruce, L., & McDonald,
01.011. Talent identification and selection in elite youth Z. (2016). Discriminating talent-identified
Hagemann, N., Lotz, S., & Cañal-Bruland, R. (2008). football: an Australian context. European Journal junior Australian football players using a video
Wahrnehmungs-Handlungs-Kopplung beim of Sport Science, 16(7), 837–844. https://doi.org/ decision-making task. Journal of Sports Sciences,
taktischen Entscheidungstraining: Eine ex- 10.1080/17461391.2016.1151945. 34(4), 342–347. https://doi.org/10.1080/
ploratorische Studie [Perceptual-action-cou- Pocock, C., Dicks, M., Thelwell, R. C., Chapman, 02640414.2015.1053512.
pling in tactical decision-making training: an M., & Barker, J. B. (2019). Using an imagery
explorative study]. E-Journal Bewegung und intervention to train visual exploratoryactivityin
Training, 2, 17–27. eliteacademyfootballplayers. Journal of Applied
Höner, O. (2005). Entscheidungshandeln im Sportspiel Sport Psychology, 31(2), 218–234.
Fußball: Eine Analyse im Lichte der Rubikontheo- Rago, V., Pizzuto, F., & Raiola, G. (2017). Relationship
rie [Decision making in football—An analysis in between intermittent endurance capacity and
the context of the Rubicon theory]. Schorndorf: match performance according to the playing
Hofmann. position in sub-19 professional male football
Höner, O., Larkin, P., Leber, P., & Feichtinger, P. players: preliminary results. Journal of Physical
(2020). Talentauswahl und -entwicklung Education and Sport, 17(2), 688.
im Sport. In J. Schüler, M. Wegner & Roca, A., Williams, A. M., & Ford, P. R. (2012).
H. Plessner (Eds.), Sportpsychologie: Grundlagen Developmental activities and the acquisition of
und Anwendung [Textbook of sports psychology: superior anticipation and decision making in
basics and application] (pp. 499–530). Berlin, soccer players. Journal of Sports Sciences, 30(15),
Heidelberg, New York: Springer. 1643–1652. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.
Huijgen, B. C. H., Elferink-Gemser, M. T., Post, W., & 2012.701761.
Visscher, C. (2010). Development of dribbling in Romeas, T., Guldner, A., & Faubert, J. (2016). 3D-
talented youth soccer players aged 12–19 years: Multiple Object Tracking training task improves
a longitudinal study. Journal of Sports Sciences, passing decision-making accuracy in soccer
28(7), 689–698. https://doi.org/10.1080/ players. Psychology of Sport & Exercise, 22, 1–9.
02640411003645679. Ruiz Pérez, L. M., Palomo Nieto, M., García Coll, V.,
Johnston, K., Wattie, N., Schorer, J., & Baker, J. (2018). Navia Manzano, J. A., Miñano Espín, J., & Psotta,
Talent identification in sport: a systematic R. (2014). Self-perceptions of decision making
review. Sports Medicine, 48(1), 97–109. https:// competence in Spanish football players. Acta
doi.org/10.1007/s40279-017-0803-2. Gymnica, 44(2), 77–83.
Larkin, P., O’Connor, D., & Williams, A. M. (2016). Does Scharfen, H. E., & Memmert, D. (2019). Measurement of
grit influence sport-specific engagement and cognitive functions in experts and elite athletes:
perceptual-cognitive expertise in elite youth

German Journal of Exercise and Sport Research 1 · 2021 111

You might also like