Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

Creativity Research Journal

ISSN: 1040-0419 (Print) 1532-6934 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hcrj20

An Empirical Review of Research Methodologies


and Methods in Creativity Studies (2003–2012)

Haiying Long

To cite this article: Haiying Long (2014) An Empirical Review of Research Methodologies and
Methods in Creativity Studies (2003–2012), Creativity Research Journal, 26:4, 427-438, DOI:
10.1080/10400419.2014.961781

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2014.961781

Copyright Haiying Long

Published online: 20 Nov 2014.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 59095

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 32 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hcrj20
CREATIVITY RESEARCH JOURNAL, 26(4), 427–438, 2014
Published with license by Taylor & Francis
ISSN: 1040-0419 print=1532-6934 online
DOI: 10.1080/10400419.2014.961781

An Empirical Review of Research Methodologies and


Methods in Creativity Studies (2003–2012)
Haiying Long
Florida International University

Based on the data collected from 5 prestigious creativity journals, research methodologies
and methods of 612 empirical studies on creativity, published between 2003 and 2012,
were reviewed and compared to those in gifted education. Major findings included:
(a) Creativity research was predominantly quantitative and psychometrics and experi-
ment were the most frequently utilized quantitative methodologies, (b) judges were
employed frequently to assess creativity and correlational techniques were utilized most
widely to analyze quantitative data, (c) case study was the most frequently used
qualitative methodology, (d) most mixed-methods studies were rooted in quantitative
methodology, and (e) both creativity and gifted education research were dominated
by quantitative methodologies, but there were less qualitative studies and slightly more
mixed-methods studies on creativity. Implications of these findings were further
discussed and future research directions were suggested.

Around 15 years ago, Mayer (1999) summarized six historiometry. Researchers using biological methodology
methodologies employed by creativity researchers: examine neurological and physiological traits of indivi-
psychometric, experimental, biographical, biological, duals during the process of creative problem solving
computational, and contextual. According to him, each and describe EEG and PET results of the brain activities.
methodology entailed a unique viewpoint and a research Individuals using computational methodology use the
procedure. Researchers who use psychometric method- principles of artificial intelligence and perceive the process
ology believe that creativity is a measurable mental trait, of creative problem solving to be a computer program.
thus they administer creativity tests or questionnaires to Finally, scholars using contextual methodology detect
assess individuals’ creativity. Experimental researchers social, cultural and evolutionary influences on creativity.
view creativity as a cognitive process and require In addition to Mayer’s (1999) synthesis, two more
participants to solve problems in controlled settings. articles also examined research methodologies of
Biographical methodologists study creativity with life creativity studies along with other aspects of research.
stories and employ methods such as case studies and One was conducted by Wehner, Csikszentmihalyi, and
Magyari-Beck (1991). This study analyzed 100 disser-
# Haiying Long
tation abstracts published in 1986 on the basis of a
This is an Open Access article. Non-commercial re-use, conceptual matrix, which incorporated three aspects of
distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original creativity (i.e., traits, processes, and products), four
work is properly attributed, cited, and is not altered, transformed, or social levels of investigation (i.e., culture, organization,
built upon in any way, is permitted. The moral rights of the named group, and individual), two research methodologies
author(s) have been asserted.
An earlier version of this article was presented at 2011 annual
(i.e., quantitative and qualitative), and the nature of
meeting of the American Psychological Association in Washington, the study (i.e., empirical and theoretical). The authors
D.C. Publication of this article was funded in part by Florida found that qualitative studies accounted for 64% of
International University Open Access Publishing Fund. I thank Linda the dissertations, which was higher than the percentage
Bliss for her constructive feedback and her help with editing. of quantitative studies (i.e., 36%). Using the same con-
Correspondence should be sent to Haiying Long, Department of
Leadership and Professional Studies, Florida International University,
ceptual matrix, Kahl, da Fonseca, and Witte (2009)
11200 SW 8th Street, Miami, FL 33199. E-mail: haiying.long@fiu.edu reviewed another 100 dissertation abstracts published
428 LONG

between 2005 and 2007. They reported that there were RESEARCH METHODOLOGY AND METHOD
slightly more quantitative (51%) than qualitative studies
(42%) in this period of time but the difference was not Although research articles were often categorized by
statistically significant. They also observed a significant methods (e.g., Dai, Swanson, & Cheng, 2011; Hart,
increase in the number of quantitative studies and a Smith, Swars, & Smith, 2009), the analysis in this review
decrease in the number of qualitative studies, compared was based on both methodology and method. For this
with the results in Wehner et al.’s (1991) study, reason, it is necessary to make a distinction between
v2(3) ¼ 10.54, p ¼ .01, e ¼ 0.05. these two concepts. Methodology refers to ‘‘the general
These reviews provided profound insights to research logic and theoretical perspective’’ (Bogdan & Biklen,
methodologies that are employed to study creativity. 2007, p. 35) of a study, whereas methods only refer to
But there are a few issues worth noting. First, these specific strategies, procedures, and techniques of analyz-
reviews focused on either the synthesis of the literature ing and interpreting data (Bogdan & Biklen, 2007;
or dissertation abstracts. For one thing, the review rely- Lincoln & Guba, 1985; Merriam, 2002). It is generally
ing on literature was not well supported by empirical agreed that there are three research methodologies:
data. For another thing, although dissertations were a quantitative, qualitative, and mixed-methods (Cohen,
good source of empirical data, they were not representa- Manion, & Morrison, 2011; Creswell, 2014). Each meth-
tive of all types of publications in a field. Second, odology reflects a set of ontological and epistemological
Mayer’s (1999) synthesis presented a thorough overview assumptions. When conducting quantitative studies,
of different ways of studying creativity; however, his researchers assume that there is a social reality external
classification of the methodologies seemed to be to the knower and knowledge is objective and tangible.
grounded in a mixture of research methodologies and Therefore, they view their role as observers and endea-
substantive contents, hence, resulting in some inconsist- vor to detect universal laws about the relationships
ency. For example, psychometric and experimental and regularities of the factors selected (i.e., variables)
methodologies are two quantitative methodologies. in their studies. In contrast, qualitative researchers
Biographical methodology focuses on people’s life assume that social reality exists independent of the
stories, but it includes case study and psychometrics, knower and knowledge is subjective and personal.
where the former one is a qualitative methodology and Therefore, they regard themselves as insiders and aim
the latter is a quantitative methodology. A single focus at interpreting individual experience in a unique social
in the classification of methodologies is needed. Third, context. Mixed-methods researchers mostly espouse
these reviews were limited in the breadth and depth of ‘‘pragmatism of the middle’’ (Johnson, Onwuegbuzi, &
the analyses. Questions such as what specific methodol- Turner, 2007, p. 125) as their primary research para-
ogies and methods are distributed in creativity studies digm. This stance is rooted in pluralism that provides
were not addressed. Fourth, mixed-methods legitimacy for combining multiple epistemological per-
research was not addressed in Wehner et al. (1991) study spectives and methods in a single study (Greene, 2007;
and was not analyzed in Kahl et al. (2009) study due to Johnson, 2012; Johnson & Onwuegbuzie, 2004).
its small number. But as a quickly growing mode of Research methodology is significant not only because
inquiry in social sciences, not considering this method- it embodies philosophical assumptions, but also because
ology fails to provide a complete picture of the develop- it guides the selection of research methods. Quantitative
ment of creativity research. Fifth, these reviews only researchers tend to employ measurement, experiment,
examined research methodologies in creativity research. and statistical analysis to answer their research ques-
Yet they were not compared with those of another tions, and qualitative researchers prefer observations,
similar field. interviews, and content analysis. Because mixed-
This review examined the distributions of research methods research represents a middle ground between
methodologies and methods in the articles published on quantitative and qualitative methodology, it ‘‘combines
five key creativity journals between 2003 and 2012, and elements of qualitative and quantitative research
further compared them with those in gifted education approaches (e.g., use of qualitative and quantitative
research, which is a closely-related field of creativity in edu- viewpoints, data collection, analysis, inference techni-
cation. The purpose of this review was primarily descrip- ques) for the broad purposes of breadth and depth of
tive. It will help creativity researchers identify the latest understanding and corroboration’’ (Johnson et al.,
trends of the development in creativity research and will 2007, p, 123). However, whether a study is quantitative,
further support the findings generated from previous qualitative, or mixed-methods depends on the method-
literature reviews with the empirical data. The comparison ology, rather than the methods. For instance, some
with another field will also assist researchers in identifying researchers employ observation and protocol analysis
the strengths and weaknesses of creativity research and lay to collect data. But if they determine operational defini-
a foundation for the future research of the field. tions of the behaviors before conducting research and
REVIEW OF RESEARCH METHODOLOGIES AND METHODS 429

TABLE 1
Methods of Data Analysis

Journal Comparative Correlational Combined Measurement Total

CRJ 79 (28%) 80 (29%) 77 (28%) 43 (15%) 279


JCB 34 (31%) 40 (36%) 33 (30%) 3 (3%) 110
GCQ 3 (21%) 2 (14%) 5 (36%) 4 (29%) 14
TSC 11 (29%) 9 (24%) 13 (34%) 5 (13%) 38
PACA 8 (13%) 24 (40%) 24 (40%) 4 (7%) 60
Total 135 (27%) 155 (31%) 152 (30%) 59 (12%) 501

Note. Percentages in parentheses are relative to the total number of studies in the last column. CRJ ¼ Creativity Research Journal. JCB ¼ Journal
of Creative Behavior. GCQ ¼ Gifted Child Quarterly. TSC ¼ Thinking Skills and Creativity. PACA ¼ Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts.

are only interested in the frequency of the behaviors, the most in the field (Long, Plucker, Yu, Ding, & Kaufman,
differences between two groups of participants, and the 2014). Gifted Child Quarterly (GCQ) was selected
effects of one variable on other variables, this study is because the journal is one of the oldest educational
just an instance of quantitative methodology, rather journals that are most likely to publish creativity studies.
than that of qualitative methodology. Furthermore, Thinking Skills and Creativity (TSC) and Psychology of
studies that use quantitative and qualitative methods Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts (PACA) began to
in the stage of data collection but later code themes or publish in 2006. As two new additions to the field, their
categories generated from qualitative methods as num- impact on the field has been increasing (Long et al.,
bers are also examples of quantitative methodology, 2014). These five journals differ in their scopes and aims.
but not an example of mixed-methods research. JCB focuses on topics in general areas of creativity,
In contrast to methodology that embodies the nature intelligence, and problem solving (Bristol, 1967). CRJ
of a study, research methods are the actual ways of con- publishes creativity articles from different perspectives,
ducting a study and the information obtained from the including behavioral, clinical, cognitive, cross-cultural,
analysis of the methods is still beneficial to understand- developmental, educational, genetic, organizational,
ing creativity research. For this reason, this study psychoanalytic, psychometric, and social (Runco, 1988).
reviewed research methods, especially those in quanti- PACA accepts empirical studies, literature reviews, and
tative methodology, and classified them into three cate- critical analyses that are related to the psychology of
gories: methods of collecting data in terms of time point aesthetics, creativity, and the arts (Smith, Smith, &
(i.e., cross-sectional vs. longitudinal), methods of asses- Kaufman, 2006). By contrast, GCQ publishes articles
sing creativity, and methods of analyzing data. With about giftedness and talent development in various
regard to methods of data analysis, quantitative data contexts, such as school and home.
were analyzed by three types of statistical techniques: The study period of time was chosen to be from 2003
one focuses on mean differences and uses comparative to 2012 because it represents the most recent 10 years
techniques, such as t-test, ANOVA, MANOVA; one and the rapid advances in research methodologies and
that emphasizes relationships between variables and methods take place in this period of time. In addition,
uses correlational techniques, such as correlation and this review only focused on empirical or evidence-based
regression; another one that examines characteristics research, which was defined as ‘‘a systematic attempt
of measurement tools. As research becomes increasingly to collect information about an identified problem or
complex, a great deal of studies employed more than one question, the analysis of that information, and the
type of techniques to analyze data, for example, ANOVA application of the evidence to confirm or refute some
and regression, they were considered combined studies prior statement(s) about the problem or question under
(see Table 1). study’’ (Callahan & Moon, 2007, p. 307).

Procedure
METHOD
There were three steps in the process of collecting data.
To start with, a coding scheme was generated based on
Data Sources
the broad categories of quantitative, qualitative, and
Five creativity journals were selected as data sources. mixed-methods studies. Psychometric, experimental,
Creativity Research Journal (CRJ) and Journal of historiometric methodologies were grouped under
Creative Behavior (JCB) were selected because they quantitative methodology, whereas case study and
published the most studies on creativity and were cited phenomenological studies were under qualitative. Next,
430 LONG

the author read each article in the five journals to reached agreement on categorizing 92% of the studies
determine whether it was an empirical study and whether reviewed. The discrepancies were further discussed until
it was about creativity. The unit of analysis was study ultimate agreement was achieved. Lastly, research meth-
(i.e., there might be more than one study in an article). odologies of creativity research were compared with
Only empirical studies on creativity were included and those in gifted education based on Dai et al.’s (2011)
other types of studies, such as literature review, review article. This article and the current study were
theoretical discussion, and commentary, and studies somewhat different in the focus, scope, and coding;
on other topics, such as, aesthetics, art, and thinking therefore, only the data that matched between two stu-
skills, were excluded. dies were used. The field of gifted education was selected
In the course of coding the first 100 articles on CRJ, due to the following three reasons. First, historically,
the coding scheme was revised for a few times. For creativity and gifted education were closely related. This
instance, meta-analysis was later included in the coding was evidenced by the fact that Gifted Child Quarterly is
scheme when it was found in the journal. Because this one of a few educational journals that published
method is different from other quantitative method- creativity studies as early as 1965. Second, theoretically,
ology in its logic and research design, it was counted analytical giftedness and creativity are two components
as another quantitative methodology. In addition, of successful intelligence (Sternberg, 1985). Creative
quasi-experimental studies employ procedures that are ability is also an important part of giftedness (Renzulli,
similar to experiments; therefore, they were coded as 1978). Third, Dai et al.’s article was one of very few
one type of experimental methodology. At last, there reviews with regard to research methodologies or
were four methodologies categorized as quantitative, methods in other psychological or educational fields.
including psychometrics, experiment, historiometry,
and meta-analysis. Furthermore, studies that did not
specifically introduce methodology but followed similar RESULTS
procedures and analyses were grouped together. For
instance, Kaufman’s (2005) article on creativity and Empirical Studies in Five Journals
mental illness was coded under historiometric method-
ology because it used procedures and analyses that were A total of 1,127 studies published in five journals from
identical to historiometry although the author did not 2003 to 2012 were reviewed (see Table 2). Excluding
use that label in the article. What’s more, studies that studies on other topics and non-empirical studies, about
used more than one quantitative methodology were half (n ¼ 612) of the studies were empirical creativity
coded based on the main methodology. For example, studies. Half of these studies were published in CRJ
participants were required to complete demographic (n ¼ 317) and 21% in JCB (n ¼ 129). Around the same
and personality questionnaires in most experimental number of studies were published in TSC (n ¼ 72) and
studies, but they were still coded as studies using experi- PACA (n ¼ 78) but only a small number (n ¼ 16) in
mental methodology. GCQ. Furthermore, over 75% of the studies in CRJ
In coding qualitative studies, it was noticed that some and JCB and 60% in TSC were empirical creativity stu-
studies only indicated the use of methods, such as, inter- dies, whereas over a third of the publications in PACA
view, observation, and text analysis, but they did not and only 7% in GCQ were the same type of studies.
describe methodology. According to Merriam (2002),
these could be categorized as basic interpretive qualitat-
Quantitative, Qualitative, and Mixed Methodologies
ive studies in which ‘‘the researcher is interested in
understanding how participants make meaning of a situ- On the whole, an overwhelming majority (n ¼ 510, 83%)
ation or phenomenon, this meaning is mediated through of the 612 empirical studies on creativity published dur-
the researcher as instrument, the strategy is inductive, ing the last 10 years were quantitative studies and 13%
and the outcome is descriptive’’ (p. 6). Therefore, it (n ¼ 80) were qualitative. Less than 5% (n ¼ 22) of the
was added as another qualitative methodology. Besides, studies employed mixed methods (see Table 2). More-
because there were very few phenomenological studies, over, in all the five journals, the predominant research
they were coded as other qualitative methodologies methodology was quantitative, followed by qualitative
along with the methodologies used infrequently. In the and mixed methods. In particular, half of the creativity
final coding scheme, qualitative methodologies included studies published in TSC were quantitative whereas
case study, basic qualitative study, grounded theory, about 80% of the studies in each of the remaining four
and other qualitative methodologies. journals were quantitative (see Table 2). Qualitative stu-
After the author completed coding, 50 studies were dies made around 10% of the total studies in CRJ, JCB,
randomly selected and coded by a graduate student in and GCQ and 20% in PACA; qualitative studies
curriculum and instruction program. The two coders accounted for a third (n ¼ 26) of all the studies published
REVIEW OF RESEARCH METHODOLOGIES AND METHODS 431

TABLE 2
Empirical Studies in Five Journals

QUAN QUAL
Studies Total MIX
Journal Year Revi. Emp. Sur. Exp. His. Met. Total CS Ba. Gro. OQ Total Total

Creativity Research Journal (CRJ) 2003– 414 317 168 97 14 5 284 10 7 2 5 24 9


2012 (90%) (7%) (3%)
Journal of Creative Behavior (JCB) 2003– 159 129 72 31 7 2 112 9 1 0 3 13 4
2012 (87%) (10%) (3%)
Gifted Child Quarterly (GCQ) 2003– 232 16 11 2 1 0 14 1 1 0 0 2 0
2012 (88%) (12%)
Thinking Skills and Creativity 2006– 118 72 27 11 0 0 38 8 9 3 6 26 8
(TSC) 2012 (53%) (36%) (11%)
Psychology of Aesthetics, 2006– 204 78 36 17 7 2 62 8 3 2 2 15 1
Creativity, 2012 (79%) (20%) (1%)
and the Arts (PACA)
Total 1,127 612 314 158 29 9 510 36 21 7 16 80 22
(62%) (31%) (6%) (2%) (83%) (45%) (26%) (9%) (20%) (13%) (4%)

Note. Percentages of the total number of studies using different quantitative and qualitative methodologies are relative to the total number of
quantitative and qualitative studies in the Total column. Percentages of total number of quantitative, qualitative, and mixed methods studies in
the parentheses are relative to the total number of empirical studies in the fourth column (Total Emp.). Studies Revi. ¼ total number of studies
reviewed; Total Emp. ¼ total number of empirical studies on creativity; QUAN ¼ quantitative methodology; QUAL ¼ qualitative methodology;

in TSC. About 10% of the studies in TSC were methodology and no studies employed meta-analysis.
mixed-methods research and only around 3% in CRJ Quantitative studies in TSC used either psychometric
and JCB. In contrast, PACA only published one or experimental methodology.
mixed-methods study and GCQ did none.
The percentages of the three types of studies were
further analyzed by year. A consistent pattern was Methods of collecting data. Only 11 longitudinal
shown in all the 10 years—far more quantitative studies studies were identified in the five journals, making 2%
were published than the other two types of studies. The of all the quantitative studies. The rest of the studies
dominance of quantitative studies was especially promi- were cross-sectional. Six of the longitudinal studies were
nent in 2004 and in the four most recent years (i.e., from published in CRJ, 4 in JCB, and 1 in PACA. These stu-
2009 to 2012). Overall, more qualitative studies were dies examined creativity in different domains, for
published than mixed-methods research in all the years instance, motor development (Pagona & Costas, 2008),
except in 2006 when the percentages of the two types visual arts (Furst, Ghisletta, & Lubart, 2012), and child
of studies were equal. Although a slight increase was development (Mullineaux & Dilalla, 2009). Interest-
observed in the percentages of qualitative studies in ingly, these studies also varied by their research pur-
2007 and 2008, this trend reversed soon in 2009. poses. Although a few aimed to map developmental
Additionally, the percentages of mixed methods were trend of children’s creativity, such as Claxton, Pannells,
especially small in 2004, 2008, and 2010. and Rhoads’ (2005) work, some explored the predictive
validity of creativity tests, such as Torrance Tests of
Creative Thinking (Runco, Millar, Acar, & Cramond,
Quantitative Methodologies 2010). Others investigated the effects of creativity train-
ing or intervention. For example, Moore and Russ (2008)
Table 2 indicated that of 510 quantitative studies, reported a follow-up study on a pretend play interven-
psychometric methodology was used most frequently tion and its effect on play, creativity, and emotional
(n ¼ 314, 62%) in the five journals, followed by experi- processes among first and second grade children.
ment (n ¼ 158, 31%). Historiometry and meta-analysis
combined only accounted for 8% (n ¼ 38) of all the
quantitative studies. Comparatively speaking, there Methods of assessing creativity. Methods of how
were more historiometric studies (n ¼ 29, 6%) than meta- creativity was assessed in quantitative studies and the
analysis (n ¼ 9, 2%). Quantitative studies published in quantitative part of mixed-methods studies (n ¼ 532)
GCQ and TSC showed fewer variations than those in are reported in this section. In over half of these studies
the other three journals. More specifically, 11 out of (n ¼ 314, 59%), researchers relied heavily on divergent
14 quantitative studies in GCQ used psychometric thinking tests and problem solving tasks or products
432 LONG

to measure creativity, particularly general creative abil- Fostering Creativity Questionnaire (Teo & Waugh,
ity. Divergent thinking tests (n ¼ 202) were the primary 2010) assessed fostering creativity in teaching students
method of assessing creativity, followed by products or of higher education; Teacher Observation Form (Peters
tasks (n ¼ 112). A few notable trends about creativity & Gates, 2010) evaluated characteristics of gifted
assessment were observed in the studies examined. and talented students, creativity included; Teachers’
First, besides relying on traditional divergent think- Conceptions of Creativity Questionnaire (Kampylis,
ing tasks, many researchers created their own tasks or Berki, & Saariluoma, 2009) examined teachers’ concep-
problems to address research questions. For example, tions of creativity.
Mumford and his colleagues asked participants to Fifth, it was not uncommon for the researchers to use
assume the role of a manager in an advertising agency multiple methods to assess creativity. For instance, to
and develop new marketing campaigns (Licuanan, examine college students’ creativity and its relationship
Dailey, & Mumford, 2007) or respond to ethical with their academic achievement, Cheung, Rudowicz,
decision-making scenarios (Mumford et al., 2010). Yue, and Kwan (2003) required participants to complete
Second, human judges and their consensus were five divergent thinking tasks, which consisted of the
widely used to score participants’ creativity. Studies Alternate Uses Test (Christensen, Guilford, Merrifield, &
employing judges account for a third (n ¼ 105) of the Wilson, 1960), two tasks adapted from Sternberg (1990)
total studies (n ¼ 314) that utilized divergent thinking and Sternberg and Lubart’s (1992) work, and two from
tests and products to assess creativity. Of the 105 studies Wallach and Kogan (1965) creativity tests. Armstrong
that relied on judges, about 60% (n ¼ 64) clearly (2012) assessed different aspects of creativity by a single
indicated the use of Consensual Assessment Technique product improvement task selected from TTCT, an
(CAT; Amabile, 1982) and 40% (n ¼ 41) only mentioned updated Remote Association Task, a creative problem
the use of judges (e.g., Friedrich & Mumford, 2009; solving task adapted from Redmond, Mumford, and
Palmiero, Cardi, & Belardinelli, 2011). CAT was also Teach (1993), and a self-report Lifetime Creativity Scales
applied to score responses to divergent thinking (LCS; Richards, Kinney, Benet, & Merzel, 1988).
tests in several studies (n ¼ 16, 8%; e.g., Dollinger,
2003; Friedman, 2009; Friedman, Forster, & Denzler, Methods of analyzing data. In examining methods of
2007; Kousoulas, 2010). data analysis, meta-analysis (n ¼ 9) was excluded because
Third, researchers reformed CAT procedure to serve all meta-analysis studies use a uniform way to analyze
their research purposes. For instance, some researchers data and there is no need for further breakdown.
(e.g., Hui & Lau, 2006; Paletz & Peng, 2009) provided As shown in Table 1, about a third (n ¼ 155) of all the stu-
rating criteria for their judges; others asked novice dies (n ¼ 501) used correlation techniques. This number
people as judges (e.g., Kaufman, Baer, & Cole, 2009; was identical to that of combined studies or studies using
Kaufman, Gentile, & Baer, 2005). Barbot, Orriols, and both comparative and correlational techniques (n ¼ 152,
Pouyade (2008–2010) computerized CAT procedure 30%) and was slightly more than that of comparative
and programmed Consensual Assessment Technique- studies (n ¼ 135, 27%). The number of measurement
Interface (CAT-i) in which products were presented in studies was relatively small (n ¼ 59, 12%). These techni-
a random order and a 7-point rating scale was provided ques were further examined in each of the five journals.
for all the judges (Botella, Zenasni, & Lubart, 2011). In CRJ, there were almost equal numbers of comparative,
Horng and Lin (2009) constructed a Creative Culinary correlational, and combined studies. In contrast, in JCB,
Product Criteria Matrix based on CAT. Cropley and slightly more correlational studies were published than
Kaufman (2012) created the Creative Solution Diag- comparative and combined ones. In GCQ and TSC, there
nosis Scale (CSDS) to measure functional creativity. were more combined studies than comparative studies,
Fourth, several new inventories (n ¼ 10) have been which in turn were more than the number of
created during the past 10 years and their reliability correlational studies. In PACA, there were equal numbers
and validity were also supported in the literature. For of correlational and combined studies, which were each
example, vDiffer scale was built on the concept of inno- much more than that of comparative studies.
vation motivation and assessed ‘‘the self-attributed In an effort to understand the usage of advanced stat-
value of behavioral variation’’ (Joy, 2004, p. 313). Cre- istical analysis methods, such as multilevel modeling,
ative Achievement Questionnaire (Carson, Peterson, & latent variable analysis, hierarchical regression, mediat-
Higgins, 2005) was a self-report questionnaire measur- ing, moderating analysis, and Rasch analysis, psycho-
ing achievement across 10 domains. In addition, four metric, experimental, and historiometric studies was
new questionnaires were constructed to measure creativ- further examined. Over a quarter (n ¼ 135, 27%) of the
ity in teaching. The Creativity Checklist (Proctor & 501 studies used these advanced techniques. Hierarchi-
Burnette, 2004) measured cognitive and dispositional cal regression and SEM were used most frequently and
characteristics of creativity among elementary students; these two methods combined accounted for 41%
REVIEW OF RESEARCH METHODOLOGIES AND METHODS 433

(n ¼ 56) of all the studies employed advanced methods Stenhouse’s (1985) multi-case educational case study
(n ¼ 135). The total number of studies with advanced (Chappell, 2007), Meador, Hunsaker, and Kearney’s
statistical analysis increased from 22% (n ¼ 43) of (1999) instrumental case study (Reilly, 2008), Yin’s
quantitative studies between 2003 and 2007 to 31% (2003) descriptive case study (Lin, 2010), and Yin’s
(n ¼ 92) between 2008 and 2012. (1984) collaborative case study (Craft et al., 2012). Third,
most studies were conducted by European scholars,
especially those from the United Kingdom.
Qualitative Methodologies
Overall, case study, basic qualitative methodology, and Mixed-Methods Research
grounded theory were three widely used quantitative
methodologies in creativity studies (see Table 2). The There were only 22 mixed-methods studies published in the
largest share of qualitative studies came from case studies five journals. Most of the studies (n ¼ 17) were in CRJ and
(n ¼ 36, 45%), which was followed by basic methodology TSC. According to Creswell and Tashakkori (2007),
(n ¼ 21, 26%). In contrast, the number of grounded theory mixed-methods research is understood from four perspec-
studies (n ¼ 7, 9%) was relatively small. Some examples tives: methods, practice, methodology, and paradigm.
of other qualitative methodologies (n ¼ 16, 20%) were Researchers taking the perspectives of methods and prac-
phenomenological studies (e.g., Trotman, 2008), critical tice focus on two separate or related research questions
visual methodology (e.g., Hall & Mitchell, 2008), and and collect two types of data, which are further interpreted
a self-study approach (e.g., Reilly, 2008). in the results. But they do not explicitly discus method-
Of the five journals, TSC published most qualitative ology or paradigm. In contrast, scholars who take the per-
studies (n ¼ 26, see Table 2). The number accounted spectives of methodology and of paradigm consider
for a third of the total number (n ¼ 80) of qualitative philosophical assumptions as well as research design.
studies reviewed. These studies published in TSC In the present review, most of the 22 mixed-methods
also demonstrated a few interesting characteristics. studies employed quantitative and qualitative research
First, unlike the qualitative studies published on other from methods and practice perspective. Although
journals, those in TSC discussed methodology as well no researchers explicitly discussed methodology or
as validity issue, reflecting more methodological rigor. paradigm, a few studies elaborated on the purposes of
For instance, in Kokotsaki’s (2011) study examining employing both quantitative and qualitative data, show-
student teachers’ conceptions of creativity in the second- ing their understanding of the logic of mixed methods
ary music classroom, the author discussed the aim of the (e.g., Barak & Mesika, 2007; Cheng, 2011; Metzl,
study, the rationale for the selection of participants, and 2009; Williamson, 2011). For instance, Barak and
the methods of collecting data. She indicated, Mesika (2007) investigated teaching methods of inven-
tive problem-solving among junior high school students
and pointed out that the purpose of using mixed-
Phenomenography has been chosen as the most
methods research was to provide complementary data
appropriate research method for the study as it provides
for their research questions. They noted, ‘‘The study
useful analytic tools for describing awareness or ways
of experiencing a particular phenomenon (Marton combined quantitative and qualitative paradigms aimed
& Booth, 1997). In line with the main premise of at collecting as much information as possible on pupils’
phenomenography, the aim of the study was to enter learning, their approaches to problem-solving and
the student teachers’ lifeworld and understand the inventiveness, and the social processes in the class (Guba
various meanings they would ascribe to the concept of & Lincoln, 1994; Patton, 1990)’’ (p. 23). In a study
creativity in its implementation in the secondary music examining the role of creative thinking in resilience after
classroom. (p. 103) Hurricane Katrina, Metzl (2009) conducted interviews
with 17 participants about their personal experiences
A few qualitative studies explicitly discussed validity in addition to providing questionnaires to 80 survivors.
(e.g., Vass, 2007), trustworthiness (e.g., Reilley, 2008), The author argued, ‘‘Interviews of willing participants
and ethics and rigor (e.g., Craft, McConnon, & would supplement quantitative measures, providing tri-
Matthews, 2012). For example, Vass (2007) stated, ‘‘In angulation and augmenting understanding of creativity
order to minimize subjective misunderstanding—such within New Orleans’ context’’ (p. 114). Likewise, Cheng
as the over-interpretation or misrepresentation of (2011) remarked in her study on students’ creativity
data—I engaged in a continuous discussion with research in Hong Kong that the use of mixed methods aims,
colleagues regarding the selection and interpretation of ‘‘to offset the weakness inherent within one method with
episodes’’ (p. 111). Second, qualitative studies in TSC the strengths of the other method (Creswell & Plano-
showed more variations in the methodologies employed. Clark, 2007). Findings in this study were validated by
There were four different types of case studies, including triangulation of these two methods’’ (p. 71).
434 LONG

Comparison With Gifted Education for its overreliance on divergent thinking tests and its
scoring criteria (Sternberg & Lubart, 1999), the psycho-
Research methodologies and methods used in creativity
metric studies reviewed were more diverse. Divergent
were compared with those in gifted education based on
thinking tests were not the only way to assess creativity
Dai et al.’s (2011) review article. Their article reviewed
and they were combined with questionnaires and creative
1,234 empirical studies on giftedness, gifted education,
tasks in many studies. Judges and their consensus
and creativity, and focused on research methods as well
became an important alternative assessment approach.
as topics. Research methods reported in our review and
What’s more, researchers also produced a good deal of
Dai et al.’s review showed similar patterns. Research in
novel questionnaires and made useful adaptations of
gifted education and creativity was both dominated by
the existent assessment methods.
quantitative methodologies and methods. However,
However, contrary to these advances, this review also
qualitative methods were used less frequently (13% vs.
provides empirical evidence for a long-standing issue in
25%) in creativity research than in gifted education
the field: creativity research is dominated by quantitative
whereas the percentages of mixed methods were very
methodology. Although gifted education researchers
close in the two fields (4% vs. 3%). With respect to
believe that ‘‘qualitative methods have made inroads’’
quantitative methodologies, surveys and experiments
(Dai et al., 2010, p.130), creativity researchers cannot
were used much more frequently in creativity research:
make the same claim given that qualitative studies
62% of quantitative studies used psychometric method-
accounted for only 13% of all the empirical studies.
ology and 31% were experimental in creativity research.
This dominance of quantitative methodology has been
In contrast, there were 15% psychometric studies
persistent in all the 10 years and in the five journals.
and 10% experimental studies in gifted education. In
Besides the prevalence of quantitative methodology
addition, there were slightly fewer longitudinal creativity
in the field, creativity researchers still relied heavily
studies than gifted education research (2% vs. 5%). Case
on psychometric and experimental methodologies and
studies and interviews were used more extensively in the
cross-sectional studies. Undoubtedly, this overreliance
two fields whereas interviews were used comparatively
on just a few methodologies will probably lead to
less often in creativity studies (38% vs. 26%).
a shallow understanding of creativity.
This situation is closely relevant to the debate of what
constitute scientifically based research in social sciences
DISCUSSION that has been going on for one decade or so. Although
some researchers believed that experiment and other
The present review examined research methodologies quantitative methodologies are the gold standards for
and methods of empirical creativity studies published science (e.g., Slavin, 2002), others (e.g., Berliner, 2002;
in five major creativity journals between 2003 and Eisenhart & Towne, 2003; Erickson & Gutierrez, 2002;
2012. It indicated that creativity researchers have been St. Pierre, 2002) contended that the dominance of quan-
keeping pace with recent developments of research titative methods is unacceptable and the use of a wide
methodologies in social sciences. For instance, meta- range of research methodologies should be encouraged.
analysis and grounded theory began to make their Creativity researchers also maintained the same point of
contributions to understanding creativity even though view. For instance, Hilscher (2009) argued,
they have not yet exerted a major influence. What’s
more, resonate with Plucker and Renzulli’s (1999) call In the realm of aesthetics, creativity, and the arts, the
for the use of advanced statistical techniques, sophisti- need to practice a balanced science is pressing because
cated statistical methods, including multilevel modeling, artistic, aesthetic and creative experiences are intimately
latent growth modeling, and Item Response Theory, tied to the development of self, but can also be defined
were used in over a quarter of the quantitative studies in terms of conditions and outcomes. Furthermore, the
published in the five journals. These methods provide use of mixed methodology is not out of context given
more powerful tools to study the aspects of creativity that ‘‘social order . . . exists independently of social scien-
tific inquiry’’ (Beach, 1990, p. 217). (p. 15)
that could not be understood otherwise. For example,
Rasche modeling analyzed rater effects in assessing
creativity performance that were mostly examined by As discussed earlier, research methodologies reflect
interrater reliability in previous studies (Hung, Chen, different social truth. The perceptions shown in qualita-
& Chen, 2012). In addition, TSC and PACA, as two tive or mixed-methods research could not be revealed by
new journals, not only published more qualitative and quantitative methodologies. As a case in point,
mixed-methods studies but also showed more methodo- Reynolds and his colleagues (i.e., Reynolds, 2003,
logical rigor in these studies. Furthermore, unlike the 2004; Reynolds, Lim, & Prior, 2008) interpreted per-
earlier psychometric methodology that was criticized sonal experiences of women living with cancer and other
REVIEW OF RESEARCH METHODOLOGIES AND METHODS 435

chronic illnesses when they were creating artwork as of creativity (Amabile, 1996). However, the field seems
leisure. The focuses of these studies were on meanings, not ready for this endeavor of convergence. How could
inspirations, and subjective significance shown in the this endeavor affect the development of the field? How
process of making the artwork, which cannot be com- could creativity studies differ from other fields if instilled
prehended only by using questionnaires and universally with more convergence? These questions definitely
defined constructs. Therefore, progress can only be deserve increasing attention.
made in the field when we achieve a balanced employ- This review also identified several new research areas
ment of diverse methodologies. and topics to explore in the future. For instance,
Although creativity research is in a great need of researchers studied creativity in new domains such as
methodological diversity, it also needs methodological culinary (e.g., Horng & Hu, 2008, 2009; Horng & Lin,
rigor, or more discussion of methodological issues. For 2009), negotiation (e.g., De Pauw, Venter, & Neethling,
instance, the authors of most qualitative studies pub- 2011), sports (e.g., Blanchette, Ramocki, O’del, &
lished in CRJ, JCB, GCQ, and PACA claimed that their Casey, 2006), dealing with trauma (e.g., Metzel, 2009;
studies were qualitative just because they used methods Thomson, Keehn, & Gumpel, 2009), and therapeutic
such as interviews or observations. However, if the intervention (e.g., Fodor & Laird, 2004). Continued
study does not aim ‘‘to make sense of, or interpret, efforts on these domains will yield more fruitful findings.
phenomena in terms of the meanings people bring to Creativity in different cultures is also an emerging topic.
them’’ (Denzin & Lincoln, 2005, p. 3), it is not qualitat- Various facets of creativity were examined in countries
ive in nature. In the same vein, little mixed-methods such as United Kingdom, Spain, Brazil, Korea, China,
research paid attention to methodological issues. In Israel, and Turkey, but creativity has seldom been stud-
addition, most mixed-methods research relied on ques- ied in other countries, especially in African and other
tionnaires or surveys through which both quantitative American countries, and cross-cultural studies were only
and qualitative data were collected, hence making them limited to comparing United States and a few Asian
lean toward quantitative methodology. Howe (2004) cri- countries, mainly China and Korea. As a popular quali-
ticized this type of mixed-methods research as ‘‘mixed tative methodology, ethnography aims to explore
methods experimentalism’’ and contended that it cultural phenomena and it will provide an in-depth
marginalized qualitative methodologies and was not understanding of creative process, individuals, and
consistent with the epistemological assumptions of environment in other cultures. Furthermore, as judges
mixed-methods research. He noted, or raters become an indispensable part in assessing
creativity, what individual characteristics affect their
evaluative ability are crucial issues. CAT was adapted
It [mixed-methods experimentalism] elevates quantitative-
experimental methods to the top of the methodological for various research purposes, but how these adaptions
hierarchy and constrains qualitative methods to a largely affect the reliability and validity of CAT is also a worthy
auxiliary role in pursuit of the technocratic aim of accumu- topic to investigate. In the future, it is also interesting to
lating knowledge of ‘‘what works’’. It is not that qualitative review the interactions among research methodologies,
methods can never be fruitfully and appropriately used in substantive topics, and other perspectives of creativity
this way, but their natural home is within an interpretivist research. The comparisons between creativity research
framework with the democratic aim of seeking to under- and other fields in psychology and education will also
stand and give voice to the insider’s perspective regarding benefit the field.
various educational policies and practices. (Emphasis in
original, pp. 53–54)

REFERENCES
In addition to the needs of methodological diversity
and rigor, creativity researchers also need to combine Amabile, T. M. (1982). The social psychology of creativity: A
diverse research efforts together so that converging Consensual Assessment Technique. Journal of Personality and
evidence of creativity is accumulated. During the past Social Psychology, 43, 997–1013.
decade, despite the existence of disagreements on many Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context: Update to the social
psychology of creativity. Boulder, CO: Westview. doi: 10.1037=0022-
key issues in the field, a great number of researchers 3514.43.5.9971
believed that creativity is best studied from multiple Armstrong, D. (2012). The contributions of creative cognition and
components, including social, cultural, and personal fac- Schizotypal symptoms to creative achievement. Creativity Research
tors. This underlying belief was a key assumption for Journal, 24, 177–190. doi: 10.1080=10400419.2012.677329
many influential approaches and theories in the field, Barak, M., & Mesika, P. (2007). Teaching methods for inventive
problem-solving in junior high school. Thinking Skills and
such as confluence approach (Sternberg & Lubart, Creativity, 2, 19–29. doi: 10.1016=j.tsc.2006.10.002
1999), systems theory (Csikszentmihalyi, 1988), contex- Barbot, B., Orriols, E., & Pouyade, H. (2008–2010). Consensual
tual methodology (Mayer, 1999), and social psychology assessment technique-interface (CAT-i). Copyrights Cat-i.org.
436 LONG

Beach, W. A. (1990). Orienting to the phenomenon. In J. Anderson 1998–2010 (April). Gifted Child Quarterly, 55, 126–138. doi:
(Ed.), Communication yearbook 13 (pp. 216–244). Newbury Park, 10.1177=0016986210397831
CA: Sage. Denzin, N. K., & Lincoln, Y. S. (2005). The Sage handbook of qualitat-
Berliner, D. (2002). Educational research: The hardest science ive research (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
of all. Educational Researcher, 31, 18–20. doi: 10.3102= De Pauw, A.-S., Venter, D., & Neethling, K. (2011). The effect
0013189X031008018 of negotiator creativity on negotiation outcomes in a bilateral
Blanchette, D. M., Ramocki, S. P., O’del, J. N., & Casey, M. S. (2005). negotiation. Creativity Research Journal, 23, 42–50. doi: 10.1080=
Aerobic exercise and creative potential: Immediate and residual 10400419.2011.545734
effects. Creativity Research Journal, 17, 257–264. doi: 10.1080= Dollinger, S. J. (2003). Need for uniqueness, need for cognition, and
10400419.2005.9651483 creativity. Journal of Creative Behavior, 37, 100–116. doi: 10.1002=
Bogdan, R. C., & Biklen, S. K. (2007). Qualitative research for edu- j.2162-6057.2003.tb00828.x
cation: An introduction to theory and methods (5th ed.). New York, Eisenhart, M., & Towne, L. (2003). Contestation and change in
NY: Pearson. national policy on ‘‘scientifically based’’ education research. Edu-
Botella, M., Zenasni, F., & Lubart, T. (2011). Alexithymia and affect cational Researcher, 32, 31–38. doi: 10.3102=0013189X032007031
intensity of art students. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and Erickson, F., & Gutierrez, K. (2002). Culture, rigor, and science in
the Arts, 5, 251–257. doi: 10.1037=a0022311 educational research. Educational Researcher, 31, 21–24. doi:
Bristol, L. H., Jr. (1967). By way of introduction. Journal of Creative 10.3102=0013189X031008021
Behavior, 1, 1–2. doi: 10.1002=j.2162-6057.1967.tb00001.x Fodor, E. M., & Laird, B. A. (2004). Therapeutic intervention, bipolar
Callahan, C. M., & Moon, T. R. (2007). Sorting the wheat from the inclination, and literary creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 16,
chaff: What makes for good evidence of effectiveness in the litera- 149–161. doi: 10.1080=10400419.2004.9651449
ture in gifted education? Gifted Child Quarterly, 51, 305–319. doi: Friedman, R. S. (2009). Reinvestigating the effects of promised reward
10.1177=0016986207306317 on creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 21, 258–264. doi:
Carson, S., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2005). Reliability, 10.1080=10400410902861380
validity and factor structure of the creative achievement question- Friedman, R. S., Forster, J., & Denzler, M. (2007). Interactive
naire. Creativity Research Journal, 17, 37–50. doi: 10.1207= effects of mood and task framing on creative generation.
s15326934crj1701_4 Creativity Research Journal, 19, 141–162. doi: 10.1080=104004
Chappell, K. (2007). The dilemmas of teaching for creativity: Insights 10701397206
from expert specialist dance teachers. Thinking Skills and Creativity, Friedrich, T. L., & Mumford, M. D. (2009). The effects of conflicting
2, 39–56. doi: 10.1016=j.tsc.2007.01.001 information on creative thought: A source of performance improve-
Cheng, V. M. Y. (2011). Infusing creativity into Eastern classrooms: ments or decrements? Creativity Research Journal, 12, 265–281. doi:
Evaluations from student perspectives. Thinking Skills and 10.1080=10400410902861430
Creativity, 6, 67–87. doi: 10.1016=j.tsc.2010.05.001 Furst, G., Ghisletta, P., & Lubart, T. (2012). The creative process in
Cheung, J. C. K., Rudowicz, E. U., Yue, X. D., & Kwan, A. S. F. visual art: A longitudinal multivariate study. Creativity Research
(2003). Creativity of university students: What is the impact of field Journal, 24, 283–295. doi: 10.1080=10400419.2012.729999
and year of study? Journal of Creative Behavior, 37, 42–63. doi: Greene, J. C. (2007). Mixed methods in social inquiry. San Francisco,
10.1002=j.2162-6057.2003.tb00825.x CA: Wiley.
Christensen, P. R., Guilford, J. P., Merrifield, P. R., & Wilson, R. C. Guba, E., & Lincoln, Y. S. (1994). Competing paradigms in qualitative
(1960). Alternative uses. Beverly Hills, CA: Sheridan Psychological research. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of
Services. qualitative research (pp. 105–117). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Claxton, A. F., Pannells, T. C., & Rhoads, P. A. (2005). Hall, J., & Mitchell, M. (2008). Exploring student midwives creative
Developmental trends in the creativity of school-age children. expression of the meaning of birth. Thinking Skills and Creativity,
Creativity Research Journal, 17, 327–335. doi: 10.1207= 3, 1–14. doi: 10.1016=j.tsc.2007.09.004
s15326934crj1704_4 Hart, L. C., Smith, S. Z., Swars, S. L., & Smith, M. E. (2009). An
Cohen, L., Manion, L., & Morrison, K. (2011). Research methods in examination of research methods in mathematics education (1995–
education (7th ed.). London, England: Routledge. 2005). Journal of Mixed Methods Research, 3, 26–41. doi: 10.1177=
Craft, A., McConnon, L., & Matthews, A. (2012). Child-initiated play 1558689808325771
and professional creativity: Enabling four-year-olds’ possibility Hilscher, M. C. (2009). History, collaboration, and methodology:
thinking. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 7, 48–61. doi: 10.1016=j.tsc. Areas for reflection as Division 10 grows. Psychology of Aesthetics,
2011.11.005 Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 14–16. doi: 10.1037=a0014920
Creswell, J. W. (2014). Research design: Qualitative, quantitative, and Horng, J.-S., & Hu, M.-L. (2008). The mystery in the kitchen: Culinary
mixed methods approaches (4th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Sage. creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 221–230. doi: 10.1080=
Creswell, J. W., & Plano-Clark, V. L. (2007). Designing and conducting 10400410802060166
mixed methods research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Horng, J.-S., & Hu, M.-L. (2009). The creative culinary
Creswell, J. W., & Tashakkori, A. (2007). Editorial: Developing process: Constructing and extending a four-component model.
publishable mixed methods manuscripts. Journal of Mixed Methods Creativity Research Journal, 21, 376–383. doi: 10.1080=
Research, 1, 107–111. doi: 10.1177=1558689806298644 10400410903297956
Cropley, D. H., & Kaufman, J. C. (2012). Measuring functional Horng, J.-S., & Lin, L. (2009). The development of a scale for
creativity: Empirical validation of the Creative Solution Diagnosis evaluating creative culinary products. Creativity Research Journal,
Scale (CSDS). Journal of Creative Behavior, 46, 119–137. doi: 21, 54–63. doi: 10.1080=10400410802633491
10.1002=jocb.9 Howe, K. (2004). A critique of experimentalism. Qualitative Inquiry,
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). Society, culture, and person: A system 10, 42–61. doi: 10.1177=1077800403259491
view of creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The nature of creativity Hui, A., & Lau, S. (2006). Drama education: A touch of the creative
(pp. 325–339). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. mind and communicative-expressive ability of elementary school
Dai, D. Y., Swanson, J. A., & Cheng, H. (2011). State of research children in Hong Kong. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 1, 34–40.
on giftedness and gifted education: A survey of empirical studies doi: 10.1016=j.tsc.2005.06.001
REVIEW OF RESEARCH METHODOLOGIES AND METHODS 437

Hung, S. -P., Chen, P.-H., & Chen, H.-C. (2012). Improving creativity Moore, M., & Russ, S. (2008). Follow-up of a pretend play
performance assessment: A rater effect examination with many facet intervention: Effects on play, creativity, and emotional processes
Rasch model. Creativity Research Journal, 24, 345–357. doi: in children. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 427–436. doi:
10.1080=10400419.2012.730331 10.1080=10400410802391892
Johnson, R. B. (2012). Dialectical pluralism and mixed research. Mullineaux, P. Y., & Dilalla, L. F. (2009). Preschool pretend
American Behavioral Scientist, 56, 751–754. doi: 10.1177= play behaviors and early adolescent creativity. Journal of Creative
0002764212442494 Behavior, 43, 41–57. doi: 10.1002=j.2162-6057.2009.tb01305.x
Johnson, R. B., & Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2004). Mixed methods Mumford, M. D., Waples, E. P., Antes, A. L., Brown, R. P., Connelly,
research: A research paradigm whose time has come. Educational S., . . . Devenport, L. D. (2010). Creativity and ethics: The relation-
Researcher, 33, 14–26. doi: 10.3102=0013189X033007014 ship of creative and ethical problem-solving. Creativity Research
Johnson, R. B., Onwuegbuzie, A. J., & Turner, L. A. (2007). Toward a Journal, 22, 74–89. doi: 10.1080/10400410903579619
definition of mixed methods research. Journal of Mixed Methods Pagona, B., & Costas, M. (2008). The development of motor creativity
Research, 1, 112–133. doi: 10.1177=1558689806298224 in elementary school children and its retention. Creativity Research
Joy, S. (2004). Innovation motivation: The need to be different. Journal, 20, 72–80. doi: 10.1080=10400410701842078
Creativity Research Journal, 16, 313–330. doi: 10.1080=10400419. Paletz, S. B. F., & Peng, K. (2009). Problem finding and contradiction:
2004.9651461 Examining the relationship between naive dialectical thinking, eth-
Kahl, C. H., da Fonseca, L. H., & Witte, E. H. (2009). Revisiting nicity, and creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 21, 139–151.
creativity research: An investigation of contemporary approaches. doi: 10.1080=10400410902858683
Creativity Research Journal, 21, 1–5. doi: 10.1080= Palmiero, M., Cardi, V., & Belardinelli, M. O. (2011). The role of viv-
10400410802633350 idness of visual mental imagery on different dimensions of
Kampylis, P., Berki, E., & Saariluoma, P. (2009). In service and creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 23, 372–375. doi: 10.1080=
prospective teachers’ conceptions of creativity. Thinking Skills and 10400419.2011.621857
Creativity, 4, 15–29. doi: 10.1016=j.tsc.2008.10.001 Patton, M. Q. (1990). Qualitative evaluation and research methods (2nd
Kaufman, J. C. (2005). The door that leads into madness: Eastern ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
European poets and mental illness. Creativity Research Journal, Peters, S. J., & Gates, J. C. (2010). The teacher observation form:
17, 99–103. doi: 10.1207=s15326934crj1701_8 Revisions and updates. Gifted Child Quarterly, 54, 179–188. doi:
Kaufman, J. C., Baer, J., & Cole, J. C. (2009). Expertise, 10.1177=0016986210369258
domains, and the Consensual Assessment Technique. Journal of Plucker, J. A., & Renzulli, J. S. (1999). Psychometric approaches to the
Creative Behavior, 43, 223–233. doi: 10.1002=j.2162-6057.2009. study of human creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of
tb01316.x creativity (pp. 35–61). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Kaufman, J. C., Gentile, C. A., & Baer, J. (2005). Do gifted students Proctor, R. M. J., & Burnett, P. C. (2004). Measuring cognitive and
writers and creative writing experts rate creativity the same dispositional characteristics of creativity in elementary students.
way? Gifted Child Quarterly, 49, 260–265. doi: 10.1177= Creativity Research Journal, 16, 421–429. doi: 10.1080=
001698620504900307 10400410409534553
Kokotsaki, D. (2011). Student teachers’ conceptions of creativity Redmond, M. R., Mumford, M., & Teach, R. J. (1993). Putting
in the secondary music classroom. Thinking Skills and Creativity, creativity to work: Effect of leader on subordinate creativity. Orga-
6, 100–113. doi: 10.1016=j.tsc.2011.04.001 nizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 55, 120–151.
Kousoulas, F. (2010). The interplay of creative behavior, divergent Retrieved from http://www.journals.elsevier.com/organizational-
thinking, and knowledge base in students’ creative expression behavior-and-human-decision-processes/
during learning activity. Creativity Research Journal, 22, 387–396. Reilly, R. C. (2008). Is expertise a necessary precondition for creativ-
doi: 10.1080=10400419.2010.523404 ity? Thinking Skills and Creativity, 3, 59–76. doi: 10.1016=
Licuanan, B. F., Dailey, L. R., & Mumford, M. D. (2007). Idea j.tsc.2008.02.002
evaluation: Error in evaluating highly original ideas. Journal of Renzulli, J. S. (1978). What makes giftedness? Reexamining
Creative Behavior, 41, 1–27. doi: 10.1002=j.2162-6057.2007.tb01279.x a definition. Phi Delta Kappan, 60, 180–184.
Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Beverly Reynolds, F. (2003). Conversations about creativity and chronic illness
Hills, CA: Sage. I: Textile artists coping with long-term health problems reflect
Lin, Y.-S. (2010). Drama and possibility thinking-Taiwanese pupils’ on the origins of their interest in art. Creativity Research Journal,
perspectives regarding creative pedagogy in drama. Thinking Skills 15, 393–407. doi: 10.1207=S15326934CRJ1504_7
and Creativity, 5, 108–119. doi: 10.1016=j.tsc.2010.09.001 Reynolds, F. (2004). Conversations about creativity and chronic illness
Long, H., Plucker, J. A., Yu, Q., Ding, Y., & Kaufman, J. C. (2014). II: Textile artists coping with long-term health problems reflect on
Research performance of the field of creativity: A bibliometric the creative process. Creativity Research Journal, 16, 79–89. doi:
analysis. Creativity Research Journal, 26, 353–360. doi: 10.1080= 10.1207=s15326934crj1601_8
10400419.2014.929425 Reynolds, F., Lim, K. H., & Prior, S. (2008). Images of resistance:
Mayer, R. E. (1999). Fifty years of creativity research. In R. J. Sternberg A qualitative enquiry into the meanings of personal artwork
(Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 449–460). New York, NY: for women living with cancer. Creativity Research Journal, 20,
Cambridge University Press. 211–220. doi: 10.1080=10400410802060059
Meador, K., Hunsaker, S., & Kearney, K. (1999). Qualitative research Richards, R., Kinney, D. K., Lunde, I., Benet, M., & Merzel, A. P. L.
methods for studying creativity. In A. Fishkin, B. Cramond, & (1988). Creativity in manic-depressives, cyclothymes, their normal rela-
P. Olszewski-Kubilius (Eds.), Investigating creativity in youth: tives and control subjects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 281–288.
Research and methods (pp. 239–261). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/abn/index.aspx
Merriam, S. B. (2002). Qualitative research in practice: Examples for Runco, M. A. (1988). Creativity research: Originality, utility, and
discussion and analysis. New York, NY: John Wiley Sons. integration. Creativity Research Journal, 1, 1–7. doi: 10.1080=
Metzl, E. S. (2009). The role of creative thinking in resilience after 10400418809534283
Hurricane Katrina. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Runco, M. A., Millar, G., Acar, S., & Cramond, B. (2010). Torrance
Arts, 3, 112–123. doi: 10.1037=a0013479 Tests of Creative Thinking as predictors of personal and public
438 LONG

achievement: A fifty-year follow-up. Creativity Research Journal, 22, Teo, L. K. C., & Waugh, R. F. (2010). A Rasch measure of fostering
361–368. doi: 10.1080=10400419.2010.523393 creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 22, 206–218. doi: 10.1080=
Slavin, R. (2002). Evidence-based educational policies: Transforming 10400419.2010.481534
educational practice and research. Educational Researcher, 31, 15–21. Thomson, P., Keehn, E. B., & Gumpel, T. P. (2009). Generators and
doi: 10.3102=0013189X031007015 interpreters in a performing arts population: Dissociation, trauma,
Smith, J., Smith, L., & Kaufman, J. (2006). Welcome. Psychology of fantasy proneness, and affective states. Creativity Research Journal,
Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, S, 1. doi: 10.1037=1931- 21, 72–91. doi: 10.1080=10400410802633533
3896.S.1.1 Trotman, D. (2008). Imagination and the adolescent lifeworld: Possi-
Stenhouse, L. (1985). Case study methods. In J. P. Keeves (Ed.), bilities and responsibilities in the national secondary review. Think-
Educational research, methodology and measurement: An inter- ing Skills and Creativity, 3, 125–133. doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2008.04.002
national handbook (pp. 61–66). Oxford, UK: Pergamon Press. Vass, E. (2007). Exploring processes of collaborative creativity—The
Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intelli- role of emotions in children’s joint creative writing. Thinking Skills
gence. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. and Creativity, 2, 107–117. doi: 10.1016=j.tsc.2007.06.001
Sternberg, R. J. (1990). Wisdom and its relation to intelligence and Wallach, M. A., & Kogan, N. (1965). Modes of thinking in young
creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Wisdom: Its nature, origins, children: A study of the creativity-intelligence distinction. New York,
and development (pp. 142–177). New York, NY: Cambridge NY: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
University Press. Wehner, L., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Magyari-Beck, I. (1991). Current
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1992). Creativity: Its nature and approaches used in studying creativity: An exploratory investi-
assessment. School Psychology International, 13, 243–253. Retrieved gation. Creativity Research Journal, 4, 261–271.
from http://spi.sagepub.com/ Williamson, P. K. (2011). The creative problem solving skills of arts
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1999). The concept of creativity: and science students—The two cultures debate revisited. Thinking
Prospects and paradigms. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Skills and Creativity, 6, 31–43. doi: 10.1016=j.tsc.2010.08.001
Handbook of creativity (pp. 3–15). New York, NY: Cambridge Yin, R. K. (1984). Case study research: Design and methods. Thousand
University Press. Oaks, CA: Sage.
St. Pierre, E. A. (2002). ‘‘Science’’ rejects postmodernism. Educational Yin, R. K. (2003). Case study research: Design and methods (3rd ed.).
Researcher, 31, 25–27. doi: 10.3102=0013189X031008025 Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

You might also like