Influencing Consumers' Selection of Domestic Versus Imported Products Implications For Marketing Based On A Model of Helping Behavior

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

Influencing Consumers' Selection

of Domestic Versus Imported Products:


Implications for Marketing Based
on a Model of Helping Behavior

Janeen E. Olsen
Louisiana State University

Kent L. Granzin
University of Utah

Abhijit Biswas
Louisiana State University

Recent marketing campaigns have urged American consum- business and many nations' adoption of freer trade policies
ers to "Buy American." Marketers can improve the success (Terpstra and Sarathy 1991). In the United States, the clear
of their campaigns if they understand the network of influ- majority of American-made products face direct foreign
ences that lead American consumers to help threatened do- competition. Companies are responding to the challenges
mestic workers. Consumers' cooperation in purchasing do- brought by foreign competition in a number of ways, such
mestic products may be viewed as a form of help for as improving the quality of their products, pressing for
American workers whose jobs are threatened by the success tighter import restrictions, and developing promotional
of imported products. This study presents a model designed campaigns to motivate consumers to buy domestic products
to explain consumers' willingness to help these workers. (Hirsch and Milbank 1992; Stemquist and Phillips 1991).
Survey data were subjected to structural equation analysis Concurrently, other interested parties such as labor unions
to test the model. Results confirmed willingness to help is and industry trade associations are sponsoring their own
influenced by the salience of the problem, identification "Buy American" campaigns, and are finding some success
with the workers, inequity of the situation, felt similarity (Dillin 1992).
with the workers, empathy with the workers, and the costs Some scholars point to the detrimental aspects of such
of helping. These findings suggest ways to market the Buy campaigns for both consumers (McGee 1989) and the
American theme. threatened industries themselves (Rugman and Gestrin
1991). Nonetheless, the increasing frequency of these mar-
keting campaigns suggests marketing scholars should inves-
tigate the nature of the process by which consumers decide
to respond favorably to the Buy American appeal (Ettenson,
American consumers now have more opportunity to Wagner, and Gaeth 1988). This study considers one aspect
choose between domestic and imported products than ever of the process by characterizing consumers' support for
before, given the trend toward increased globalization of threatened workers as "helping." The context of this re-
search is the American textile industry, where industry
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science
Volume 21, Number 4, pages 307-321.
groups have been currently using television campaigns to
Copyright 9 1993 by Academy of Marketing Science. promote Buy American attitudes (Peterson 1991). Their ad-
All rights of reproduction in any form reserved. vertisements depict scenes of plant closings and unemploy-
ISSN 0092-0703. ment caused by foreign competition, asking consumers to

JAMS 307 FALL, 1993


INFLUENCING CONSUMERS' SELECTION OF DOMESTIC OLSEN, GRANZIN, AND BISWAS
VERSUS IMPORTED PRODUCTS

help threatened workers by purchasing apparel made in the particular threat to American workers that comes from im-
United States. ported clothing. This report further suggests implications of
Research has suggested that consumers show a bias in the empirical findings for sponsors of Buy American cam-
favor of products produced in their home country (Bilkey paigns.
and Nes 1982). Three types of effects, representing three As a cautionary note, the extent to which such Buy Amer-
different theoretical approaches, have been identified to ican campaigns benefit the nation and its consumers re-
explain this country-of-origin bias (Obermiller and Span- mains a controversial subject in some quarters, in the vein
genberg 1989). The first approach is the one most of- of such debatable issues as distribution of cigarettes, censor-
ten encountered in the marketing literature and considers ship of cable TV, and the speed of making new drugs avail-
country-of-origin effects from a cognitive, information pro- able to the market. Controversy aside, this study concerns
cessing perspective. Country-of-origin serves as a cue to the marketing issue of promoting a certain type of purchase
inform consumers about a product's quality (Dickerson behavior to consumers, not its social, economic, ethical, or
1982; Narayana 1981), risk (Lumpkin, Crawford, and Kim developmental implications for the United States and its
1985), performance (Han and Terpstra 1988), or other im- individual citizens.
portant characteristics. Consumers tend to rely on country-
of-origin cues most when the information about the product
is lacking, but as consumers acquire the information they CONCEPTUALIZATION OF A HELPING MODEL
need to make a purchase decision, perhaps through product FOR EXPLAINING CONSUMERS' WILLINGNESS
trial or advertising, and become more familiar with a partic- TO HELP WORKERS IN DOMESTIC INDUSTRIES
ular product, country-of-origin is relied on less (Johansson,
Douglas, and Nonaka 1985; Ofir and Lehmann 1986). In This section presents a helping model that portrays a set
some cases, knowing a product's country-of-origin may mo- of proposed influences on American consumers' willingness
tivate consumers to acquire more information on which to to help American workers by buying domestic products.
base a purchase decision (Hong and Wyer 1989). The model integrates various contributions from the litera-
The second source of country-of-origin bias considers ture involving helping behavior, which is alternatively re-
overall affective responses consumers may have toward par- ferred to as altruism or prosocial behavior. In addition, the
ticular countries and their effect on purchase. For example, conceptualization integrates an additional behavioral con-
consumers who hold negative impressions of China, per- cept found important in scholarly marketing research into
haps because of reading about human rights abuses, may the present context, the concept of the ethnocentric or, alter-
decide to avoid Chinese products, or consumers of French natively, the ethno-national affinity of the consumer.
ancestry may show special fondness for products from The model characterizes a helping situation as involving
France. Research has shown that consumers tend to prefer a victim and a (potential) helper who may provide assis-
products coming from countries with cultures, levels of de- tance if sufficiently motivated. In the present case, the vic-
velopment, and political systems similar to their own tim is American industry and its workers, whose livelihood
(Crawford and Lamb 1981; Wang and Lamb 1983). is currently threatened by growing imports of foreign prod-
The normative processes that affect consumer choice of ucts (Konrad, Mitchell, and Zinn 1992). The (potential)
domestic products are the third theoretical approach taken to helper is the American consumer, who can choose to aid the
study country-of-origin bias (Obermiller and Spangenberg workers by buying domestically produced products.
1989). In this approach county-of-origin reflects the norma- Figure 1 presents a graphical portrayal of the model,
tive pressures a consumer feels to buy domestic products, where the flow of influence runs from salience through
regardless of the product or country in question. Consumers willingness to help. The mode of presentation in this section
may feel that they should buy American products in support will be to discuss each of the eight constructs of the model
of their country and fellow workers; these feelings have in turn and, for each endogenous construct taken as an
been labeled consumer ethnocentrism (Shimp and Sharma effect, justify influences from antecedent constructs proposed
1987). It is the source of these beliefs that is the focus of this to cause that construct. These proposed relationships will then
study. be formally stated as hypotheses for empirical testing.
The purpose of this study is 1) to propose a model that
features a carefully selected set of influences on consumers' Salience
willingness to choose American-made over imported prod-
ucts, 2) to test this model empirically, and 3) to draw mar- Salience is an exogenous construct that connotes the ex-
keting implications from the findings. This research consid- tent to which the need for help is recognized as important by
ers consumers' willingness to buy domestic products as a the (potential) helper. In helping research, salience has
form of "helping behavior;" that is, as action taken to assist taken such various forms as the importance, seriousness,
workers who are victims of foreign competition. To develop severity, clarity, and closeness of the situation to the helper
a model for subsequent empirical investigation, the concep- (Piliavin et al. 1981). To illustrate, salience may reflect the
tualization drew from the extensive literature in the behav- perceived extent of the victim's need for help (Bar-Tal
ioral sciences that deals with helping behavior. The concep- 1976). In the present context, salience reflects the perceived
tualization adapted numerous findings reported in this threat to American industry and its textile workers, who
literature to the present marketing context to create a model thus become victims needing help. As an exogenous vari-
proposed to explain consumers' willingness to purchase able, salience of the situation has no antecedent relation-
American products. The empirical test then focused on that ships.

JAMS 308 FALL, 1993


INFLUENCING CONSUMERS' SELECTION OF DOMESTIC OLSEN, GRANZIN, AND BISWAS
VERSUS IMPORTED PRODUCTS

FIGURE 1
A Model of the Process Leading to Consumer Help for American Workers

ETHNO-
NATIONAL
IDENTITY

+ / I / ~ / // ~ 1 ~ +

+/-

1.ENCE ~ +]- bE PERCEIVED ~, ~ COSTS WILUNGNESS


TO HELP

Ethno-national Identity is heightened by the helper's sensitivity to the needs of the


victim. Piliavin et al. (1981) showed we-ness increases in
American consumers' identification with domestic work- the face of a perceived threat. Kramer and Brewer (1984)
ers can be viewed in various ways. Shimp and Sharma found people are more prosocial when they are given salient
(1987, p. 280) used the term "consumer enthnocentrism" to information that portrays them as part of a larger (superordi-
refer to "the beliefs held by American consumers about the nate) group that shares a scarce resource. Thus, the model
appropriateness, indeed morality, of purchasing foreign- proposes that salience exerts a positive influence on a con-
made products." They stated that ethnocentric American sumer's ethnocentric identification with, and expression of
consumers reject foreign products because they believe norms relevant to, American workers. Here, this group
these imports harm the U.S. economy and its workers. Con- identity will be termed Ethno-national Identity, to concep-
versely, nonethnocentric consumers may evaluate foreign tually integrate the construct into the evolving model of
products on their worth in an objective fashion, or they may helping behavior.
even prefer them for their foreign heritage.
Consumer enthnocentrism therefore reflects a normative HI: Greater salience engenders greater ethno-national
sense of the group identity that can motivate American con- identity.
sumers to Buy American. Waheeduzzaman and Marks
(1989) further clarified the nature of consumers' propensity Perceived Equity
to identify with their own society, using the term "ethno-
national affinity" to refer to a person's attachment to a refer- According to Walster, Walster, and Ber~cheid (1978), a
ent group, such as the domestic society. They emphasized state of equity exists when the participants in a situation
that this identification provides a force toward compliance receive gains that are equal in consideration of (i.e., propor-
with national norms and standards in a consumption-related tionate to) their contributions, or conversely, when they
situation. make contributions commensurate with their gains. The per-
This concept of identification with a referent group trans- ception of equity reveals the nature of the situation (Piliavin
lates directly into the context of helping behavior. The im- et al. 1981), the nature of the perceiver (Hatfield, Walster,
portance of recognizing a helper's felt association with oth- and Piliavin 1978), and the social influences on the per-
ers has been substantiated by helping research, where group ceiver (McClelland and Rohrbaugh 1978; Morgan and Saw-
identity has been characterized in various forms, including yer 1976). In this study, perceived equity involves whether
mutuality, we-feeling, we-ness, and common fate. Re- consumers feel American workers are making a reasonable
searchers have found group identity to reflect the salience of contribution to resolve their unfavorable situation; that is,
the situation. Dovidio and Morris (1975) showed we-feeling whether they are doing their share to gain success in the

JAMS 309 FALL, 1993


INFLUENCING CONSUMERS' SELECTION OF DOMESTIC OLSEN, GRANZIN, AND BISWAS
VERSUS IMPORTED PRODUCTS

international marketplace. If consumers perceive workers helper and victim. Research has found the positive influ-
are not making sufficient effort to help themselves, equity ence of perceived similarity on helping is heightened by the
exists; that is, workers are gaining their just due. If consum- salience of the issue (Piliavin et al. 1981). People are more
ers perceive workers are making sufficient effort, inequity stimulated to consider whether they are kindred to the vic-
exists because they are unfairly disadvantaged. In the latter tim in helping situations important to them, and they are
case, consumers will be motivated to act to create equity more aware of attributes common to those in distress when
(Walster, Walster, and Berscheid 1978). the circumstances are of nontrivial importance (Stotland
Previous helping research shows people tend to be more 1969).
aroused physiologically, cognitively, and emotionally to re-
spond to the need of others when they perceive the situation H3: Greater salience heightens one's feelings of sim-
to be more salient (Geer and Jarmecky 1973; Hatfield, Wal- ilarity to the victim.
ster, and Piliavin 1978; Krebs and Russell'1981; Piliavin et
al. 1981). The implication for the present study is that when Emp~hy
consumers find the situation to be salient, they will be stim-
ulated to assess the equity of the situation, which is a cogni- Hoover, Wood, and Knowles (1983) presented empathy
tive response (Krebs and Russell 1981). That is, salience in a helping situation as a form of awareness that is "the
stimulates a consumer to perceive either greater equity or ability to understand how a situation appears to another
greater inequity; which assessment occurs depends on fac- person and how that person is reacting cognitively and emo-
tors not examined in this research; for example, the con- tionally to the situation." Piliavin et al. (1981) argued that
sumer's internalized norms of equity (Krebs and Russell severity of the situation (here, salience) accentuates this
1981; Rushton 1981). In contrast, consumers will be cog- feeling of empathy.
nitively indifferent to a situation that is not salient to them. The influence of salience also acts indirectly through sim-
Researchers stress that social influences affect the percep- ilarity by intensifying empathic feelings for other persons
tion of equity; in fact, equity theory requires the perceiver to who are perceived to be similar to the helper (Krebs 1975).
recognize a relationship with another for equity theory to Supporting a direct link between similarity and empathy,
apply at all (Hatfield, Walster, and Piliavin 1978). Specifi- previous research indicates empathy increases when the
cally, social influences can influence an individual to per- helper perceives the recipient to be similar to him/her
sonally accept reduced utility (here, less desirable apparel) (Krebs 1975; Piliavin et al. 1981). Piliavin et al. (1981)
to obtain a more even distribution of utility to others (he.re, reported people are more likely to be empathic toward those
continued employment) (McClelland and Rohrbaugh 1978). with whom they have ties; that is, when group identity
In fact, the standard for equitable outcomes that one im- exists.
poses depends on the nature of this relationship; for exam- Equity theory suggests empathy increases for those placed
ple, whether friends or nonfriends are involved (Morgan in a position felt to be inequitable (Walster, Walster, and
and Sawyer 1976). The issue here is one of group identity. Berscheid 1978). Piliavin et al. (1981) summarized previous
Jasso (1983) says a person's sense of distributive justice research by stating that empathy results from the perception
(equity) is interdependent with the group with which he/she of inequity, which creates commensurate distress (e.g.,
identifies. Lerner and Meindl (1981) state that when operat- guilt, empathy) for the perceiver. Therefore, in the obverse
ing within the context of assessing and creating justice, a sense, perceived equity decreases empathy.
person emphasizes his/her identification with the recipient
of help and tends to favor his/her own kind rather than H4: Greater empathy results from greater (a) sa-
"them." Specifically, people are more disposed to give to lience, (b) ethno-national identity, and (c) sim-
friends than to strangers (Krebs and Russell 1981). ilarity, but from (d) lesser perceived equity.
Here, the pertinent form of identification is ethno-
national identity, as introduced in the preceding section. Responsibility
The studies cited above indicate that people give more fa-
vorable consideration to in-group members. Therefore, it In a helping context, responsibility is the acceptance or
appears a consumer with stronger ethno-national identity rejection of an obligation to alleviate the distressful situa-
will evoke an equity norm more favorable to the workers; tion. The responsibility construct represents a continuum
the helper will be more likely to perceive the unfavorable ranging from completely internalized with no assignment of
condition of inequity when he/she holds a higher sense of responsibility to someone else to completely externalized
ethno-national identity. with no acceptance of responsibility (Weiner 1980; Reis-
enzein 1986; Schmidt and Weiner 1988). A potential helper
1-12: (a) Salience influences perceived equity, but no may internalize responsibility by accepting an obligation to
direction of influence can be proposed, and (b) help or may externalize it by assigning responsibility else-
greater ethno-national identity brings a decrease where; for example, to another person or institution. Mayer
in perceived equity (i.e., it accentuates the per- et al. (1985) found greater salience increases the develop-
ception of inequity). ment of felt (internalized) responsibility. In an obverse
Similarity sense, externalization of responsibility may occur because
the helper seeks to reduce salience by diffusing the obliga-
Similarity is the helper's perception of sharing charac- tion (Latane and Darley 1976) or dissolving it (Batson et al.
teristics with the victim; it reflects perceived likeness of 1986; Piliavin et al. 1981); for example, through recogniz-

JAMS 310 FALL, 1993


INFLUENCINGCONSUMERS' SELECTIONOF DOMESTIC OLSEN, GRANZIN, AND BISWAS
VERSUS IMPORTED PRODUCTS

ing the presence of other potential helpers and/or assuming empathy, and (f) greater externalized respon-
that others have accepted the obligation to help. sibility.
The arguments advanced on the basis of equity theory
(Hatfield, Walster, and Piliavin 1978; Walster, Walster, and
Willingness to Help
Berscheid 1978) suggest internalization of responsibility to
help is an intermediate stage in the process of helping to
correct a state of inequity. One's feeling of distress also Previous helping research has used various operationatiz-
affects acceptance of the obligation to help; research has ations of helping, ranging from ascriptions of deservingness
found internalized responsibility increases with forms of of help to actual physical assistance. Given that Buy Ameri-
empathic concern (Amato, Ho, and Partridge 1984; Batson can campaigns attempt to generate a predisposition to help
et al. 1986; Duval, Duval, and Neely 1979). workers, willingness to help is the appropriate construct
here.
H5: Greater (externalized) responsibility is associated Much previous research has examined the direct links
with (a) salience, without a direction of influence between antecedent constructs and various forms of helping
being proposed, (b) greater perceived equity, and behavior. This research has shown helping to be affected by
(c) lesser empathy. all other constructs in the model presented here. First, stud-
ies have linked salience, in such alternative formulations as
Perceived Costs of Helping severity and perceived need, positively with helping (Bar-
Tal 1976; Piliavin et al. 1981; Shotland and Stebbins 1983;
Potential helpers weigh the costs associated with helping Taormina and Messick 1983). Other work showed helping
when deciding whether to help the victim (Bleda et al. increases for those victims who are more needful of help
1976; Latane and Darley 1976). Piliavin et al. (1981) con- (Batson et al. 1978), such as dependent elderly parents (Cic-
solidated numerous studies in providing a rationale for the irelli 1983). Researchers also found salience to work in
constructs held to influence costs. They argued that greater interaction with greater internal responsibility to influence
severity and lesser need (here, salience) increase psycho- helping (Geer and Jarmecky 1973; Mayer et al. 1985;
logical costs when no help is given, whereas diffusion and Rogers et al. 1982). In sum, the more that helpers are keen-
dissolution (externalization) of responsibility decrease psy- ly aware of the importance of the need for help, that is, find
chological costs of not helping. Thus, salience should affect it salient, the more they will help.
the perception of costs of helping, but, because the nature of Second, previous studies involving various forms of
relevant costs in the present context may take forms other group identity indicate helping increases when individuals
than psychological (e.g., economic), the direction of influ- perceive they are members of a group (Kramer and Brewer
ence from salience cannot be specified. 1984) or they share a common fate with others (Hayden,
The perception of a "we-group" can decrease the per- Jackson, and Guydish 1984). One explanation, offered by
ceived personal costs of helping a victim, both because it is Batson et al. (1979), is that the helper possibly views help-
easier to know how to help a group member and because ing as a way to ensure future reciprocity. Alternatively, a
well-intentioned failure would be less embarrassing. Any we-feeling may make the helper more sensitive to the vic-
action that maintains group rapport is a benefit (i.e., a re- tim's need (Dovidio and Morris 1975). In the broader sense
duction in the perceived cost) (Bleda et al. 1976). Equity of the present study, ethno-national identity should motivate
theory suggests help increases as the helper anticipates low one to help other Americans.
costs from helping to correct inequity (Hatfield, Walster, Third, work involving equity suggests helping is a means
and Piliavin 1978; Walster, Walster, and Berscheid 1978). of correcting perceived inequity (Hatfield, Walster, and Pil-
In aggregate, the more the perception of inequity lowers the iavin 1978; Piliavin et al. 1981; Walster, Walster, and Ber-
costs of helping, the greater is the motivation to help (Hat- scheid 1978). Conversely, when the potential helper judges
field, Walster, and Piliavin 1978). the situation to be equitable, the motivation to help should
Piliavin et al. (1981) stated a person may help someone be low.
perceived as similar because it is presumably easier to judge Fourth, support for a path from similarity to helping has
whether help is actually needed, and the expected costs of come from many quarters (e.g., Batson et al. 1986; Krebs
an error in judgment about how much help should be given t975; Piliavin et al. 1981; Taormina and Messick 1983).
would thus be lower. Also, researchers have found those Taormina and Messick (1983) provided one rationale for
who empathize with the victim should find more rewards in this link by claiming that similarity implies a cooperative
helping and lowered costs of distress (Piliavin et al. 1981; goal orientation rather than a competitive one. Thus, per-
Schroeder et al. 1988). Piliavin et al. (1981) asserted that ceived similarity leads to helping because it promotes mutu-
when responsibility is focused on a potential helper, that al support rather than competitiveness.
person incurs higher personal costs of not helping, and help- Fifth, with respect to empathy, affective reactions to a
ing increases. distressful situation in general are important influences on
helping (Meyer and Mulherin 1980; Reisenzein 1986;
H6: (a) Salience influences perceived costs, but the Schmidt and Weiner 1988). More specifically, Batson et al.
direction of influence cannot be specified. Per- (1986) associated greater helping with higher empathic con-
ceived costs of helping should decrease with: (b) cern. Numerous other studies provide further support for a
greater ethno-national identity, (c) lesser per- link between empathy and helping in one form or another
ceived equity, (d) greater similarity, (e) greater (Amato 1986; Carlson and Miller 1987; Krebs 1975). In

JAMS 311 FALL, 1993


INFLUENCING CONSUMERS' SELECTION OF DOMESTIC OLSEN, GRANZIN, AND BISWAS
VERSUS IMPORTED PRODUCTS

sum, when the helper empathizes or otherwise takes the part were adaptations of measures created during previous re-
of the victim, helping more likely follows. search involving helping behavior by the authors in two
Sixth, research has associated helping with a respon- other marketing research contexts (Granzin and Olsen 1991;
sibility norm (Batson et al. 1978) and with greater (internal) Olsen and Granzin 1992); this previous research involved
ascription of responsibility (Batson et al. 1986). Numerous sequential revisions of the measures of helping constructs to
other studies linked helping directly to higher internalized improve their reliability and validity. Ethno-national identi-
responsibility (Geer and Jarmecky 1973; Piliavin et al. ty was measured using the full battery of seventeen items in
1981; Schwartz 1973) or to its interaction with higher sa- the CETSCALE developed by Shimp and Sharma (1987)
lience (Mayer et al. 1985; Rogers et al. 1982). In symmetri- and subsequently validated through applications in multiple
cal fashion, other research showed helping decreases when countries (Netemeyer, Durvasula, and Lichtenstein 1991)
the helper externalizes responsibility by assigning it to oth- and using multiple products in the U.S. (Herche 1992).
ers (Latane and Darley 1976). Therefore, personally assum- The tests of hypotheses were performed using LISREL
ing the obligation increases helping, whereas passing it to analysis. The analysis followed the two-stage method sug-
others decreases helping. gested by Herting and Costner (1985) and formalized by
Seventh, studies found helping decreases with greater Anderson and Gerbing (1988). The first stage involved es-
perceived costs (Bleda et al. 1976; Cialdini, Baumann, and tablishing the measurement model. Here, Cronbach's alpha,
Kendrick 1981; Krebs 1975; Latane and Darley 1976). Sup- exploratory factor analysis, and confirmatory factor analy-
porting the influence of costs, research showed helping re- sis were used to select the indicators to be used in the
flects an evaluation of both costs of not helping and costs of subsequent analysis; in essence, the process jointly purified
helping, wherein the least cost alternative is selected (Shot- the "scales" for the set of eight constructs. This purification
land and Stebbins 1983). In the first case, helping is greater process first used alpha coefficients and the structure of the
when the victim's cost of not receiving help is high (Amato eight principal components (Churchill 1979) to eliminate
1986). For example, Batson et al. (1986) showed helping those items making lesser contributions to the measurement
increases when it is difficult (i.e., costly) for helpers to not model. Selecting a set of measures having intra-construct
help by escaping a distressful situation. In the second case, reliability and inter-construct validity in this fashion is the
helping also increases when the helper's cost of helping is multidimensional counterpart of the common process of
low, for example, because of social support or special com- using alphas to indicate which items to delete from a single,
petence of the helper (Amato 1986; Piliavin et al. 1981); unidimensional, multi-item scale.
here, altruism dominates egoism (Schroeder et al. 1988). Subsequently, the LISREL confirmatory factor analysis
process was used to determine the final set of measures. As
H7: Willingness to help increases with (a) greater sa- recommended by Anderson and Gerbing (1988), additional
lience, (b) greater ethno-national identity, (c) items of lesser usefulness to the measurement model were
lesser perceived equity, (d) greater similarity, (e) deleted. (Note, however, that their alternative recommenda-
greater empathy, (f) lesser externalized respon- tion of assigning indicators to different constructs where
sibility, and (h) lesser costs of helping. useful was not adopted.) Given the complexity of handling
a large number of indicators in structural equation analysis,
two indicators were selected for all constructs except the
METHOD output construct, where four indicators were used to mea-
sure willingness to help. The Appendix presents the ques-
Data Collection tionnaire items corresponding to these indicators, as well as
symbols useful for reference to Table 1, the matrix of cor-
A quota sample of 243 adults was surveyed in a large relations among indicators. Figure 2 presents the measure-
southern metropolitan area. To represent the general popu- ment model with the final set of measures.
lation in terms of age and gender, the sample was based on At the second stage of the LISREL analysis, only after
the most recent census of the MSA, and various sections of the set of items for the measurement model was specified,
the area were covered to gain appropriate representation the analysis turned to an investigation of the proposed struc-
of the population with respect to socioeconomic char- tural relationships among the constructs (Anderson and
acteristics. Respondents were asked to complete a self- Gerbing 1988; Herting and Costner 1985). The resulting in-
administered questionnaire in the presence of student inter- ferential tests of hypothesized relationships were thus con-
viewers who were aware of the nature of the study and able ducted with a measurement model that was independently
to resolve any difficulties with the instructions that might purified of items having lesser reliability and validity.
arise.

Measurement and Analysis RESULTS

Each of the eight constructs proposed to constitute the Test of the Measurement Model
components of the model were measured using six-point
Likert-type items anchored by "strongly agree" and "strong- Table 2 presents the standardized pattern coefficients and
ly disagree." Multiple items for each construct were used, the test statistics that indicate the quality of the measure-
41 in total, ranging in number from three to nine for each of ment model. The pattern coefficients are relatively high.
the constructs except ethno-national identity. These items Because one pattern coefficient for each latent construct was

JAMS 312 FALL, 1993


INFLUENCING CONSUMERS' SELECTION OF DOMESTIC OLSEN, GRANZIN, AND BISWAS
VERSUS IMPORTED PRODUCTS

TABLE 1
Correlations Between the Operational Measures
Measure xl x2 Yl Y2 Y3 Y4 Y5 Y6 Y7 Y8 Y9 Ylo YH Y12 Y13 Y14 Yls Y16
Salience, xt 1.000
Salience, x2 .465 1.000
Identity, Yl .408 .323 1.000
Identity, Y2 .437 .310 .738 1.000
Equity, Y3 -.026 -.049 -.109 -.067 1.000
Equity, Y4 -.063 -.154 -.192 -.154 .546 1.000
Similarity, y~ .271 .168 .117 .179 .056 .112 1.000
Similarity, Y6 .166 .025 .070 .058 .099 .132 .491 1,000
Empathy, Y7 .334 .419 .363 .327 -.209 -.187 .126 .023 1.000
Empathy, Ya .280 .395 .420 .338 -.150 -.149 .122 .054 .666 1.000
Responsibility, Y9 -.185 -.126 -.324 -.325 .338 .344 - . I 14 .021 -.449 -.343 1.000
Responsibility, Yl0 -.127 -.113 -.136 -.102 .227 .163 -.015 .006 -.333 -.318 .492 1.000
Costs, Yll .018 -.075 -.032 -.029 .343 .149 -.133 -.073 -.232 -.149 .182 .226 1.000
Costs, Y12 .082 -.054 -.063 -.074 .242 .096 -.074 -.075 - . 142 -.101 .064 .132 .518 1.000
Help, Yt3 .331 .326 .550 ,498 -.153 -.185 .234 .154 .370 ,427 -.301 -.233 -.289 -.246 1.000
Help, Yt4 .345 .309 .489 .453 -.109 -.087 .263 .136 .445 .433 -.338 -.219 -.241 -.276 .632 1.000
Help, Yt5 .285 .239 .524 .466 -.141 -.t27 .218 .205 .310 9308 -.239 -.109 -.208 -.244 .539 .430 1.0~
Help, Yl6 .233 .214 .300 .346 - . 112 -.044 .247 .147 .274 9237 -.204 -.160 -.206 -.178 .477 ,524 .376 1.000

set at 1.0 to establish the scale for that construct, this coeffi- are "t-values" in LISREL parlance, ranging from 4.21 to
cient could not be tested for significance. However, the 13.65. The eight values for construct reliability ranged from
coefficient selected to be fixed was the one found highest in .644 to. 851. Thus, examination of the measurement model
magnitude by preliminary examination; the remaining shows the set of operational measures is appropriate for an
smaller, free coefficients were all highly significant, with investigation of the proposed structural linkages among the
critical ratios or z-values (Bollen 1989; Long 1983a), which constructs.

FIGURE 2
A Measurement Model Featuring Eight Helping Constructs
E1 E2 E7 El

E13
Es if4 ffli ff12

~ E14

ffl8

ffs ~o ff.I ffla

JAMS 313 FALL, 1993


I N F L U E N C I N G C O N S U M E R S ' SELECTION OF D O M E S T I C O L S E N , G R A N Z I N , A N D BISWAS
VERSUS IMPORTED PRODUCTS

TABLE 2
Pattern Coefficients, z Statistics, and Rellabilities for the Measurement Model
Standardized Indicator Construct
Construct/Indicator Coefficient z Reliability Reliability
Salience .644
Workers t o m o r r o w ' s unemployed .737 a .543
Foreign competn, causes damage .640 7.30 b .409

Ethnocentric Group Identity .851


Americans shouldn't buy foreign .891 __a .794
Manufr. tries to dictate .829 13.65 b .687

Perceived Equity .719


Are workers putting effort .847 --" .718
Workers could improve situation .643 6.29 b .413

Similarity .688
Can happen to me tomorrow. .863 --~ .745
I am no different from worker .568 4.21 b .324

Empathy .800
M a k e s me feel bad to see .855 --" .732
It is a shame workers suffering .776 11.07 b .602

Responsibility .677
Should improve own situation .824 --" .680
T h e y should take care problems .597 6.73 b .356

Perceived Costs .692


Do not make clothes as stylish .796 --a .625
W o u l d have to sacrifice style .665 7.01b .438

Willingness to Help .803


Will shop first retail stores .816 __a .661
Will try to buy U.S. brands .774 12.58 b .596
Willing to b u y American made .656 10.36 b .429
Willing to look at labels .592 9.21 ~ .350

QCoefficient of leading indicator for each construct was set to 1.0 to establish scale for the construct.
~p < .001.

Test of the Model as a Whole structs in the model provided further support for the model.
The values of R 2 for these equations were: ethno-national
Analysis of the full structural model, which included both identity, .38; perceived equity, .04; similarity,. 12; empathy,
the measurement model and the proposed structural link- .44; responsibility, .51; costs, .35; and willingness to help,
ages, produced a chi-square value of 150.33 (d.f. = 111, p .79. In particular, the high degree of explanation for the
= .008). Although this test indicated a discrepancy between output or criterion construct of the helping model was espe-
the matrix of covariances based on observations and the ma- cially reassuring, as it showed the constructs proposed to
trix imputed by the model, such discrepancies are common, explain willingness to help performed their explanatory role
especially for samples of this size. The goodness of fit index well. In addition, the coefficient of determination for the
was .937, and the index adjusted for degrees of freedom was set of structural equations taken jointly was .58. In sum,
.903, both of which indicated a good fit between model and these statistics provided general support for the structural
data. Further, the normed fit index of .906 (Bender and linkages proposed by the conceptualization underlying this
Bonett 1980; Byrne 1989) provided additional evidence of research.
the substantial improvement over a null model made by the Table 3 presents evidence concerning the hypotheses in-
specified measurement model and structural linkages. The volving individual links between pairs of constructs. Again,
root mean square residual was .045. The parsimonious z-tests were used to assess the statistical significance of the
goodness-of-fit index proposed by Mulaik et al. (1989), parameter estimates for the structural linkages (Bollen
PGFI(1), was .608. Collectively, these statistics provide sup- 1989; Long 1983b), with one-tailed tests used for direction-
port for the proposed structural equation model as a whole. al hypotheses and two-tailed tests used for nondirectional
tests, as indicated in the table. For the tests of direct links
Test of the Structural Linkages between constructs, 14 of the 24 hypothesized relationships
were supported at p < .05. Using the standard errors pro-
Turning to the structural equations, the squared multiple vided by LISREL to perform z-tests on the total effects, 19
correlations for the equations for the seven endogenous con- of the 26 possible links, and 18 of the 24 hypothesized

JAMS 314 FALL, 1993


INFLUENCING CONSUMERS' SELECTION OF DOMESTIC OLSEN, GRANZIN, AND BISWAS
VERSUS IMPORTED PRODUCTS

TABLE 3
Magnitude and Significance of Hypothesized Structural Relationships
Following~Leading Hypothesized Standard. Direct (Total)
Constructs Sign Parameter Effect Effect z Values c
Group identity
Salience + ~/~l .617 .747- (.747 ~) 6.33 (6.33)

Perceived equity
Salience +/- ~/21 .069 .079 (-.078) .54 (.75)
Group identity - 132~ -.222 -.211 s (-.211~) 1.90 (1.90)

Similarity
Salience + ~/3J .352 .412~ (.412 '~) 3.88 (3.89)

Empathy
Salience + "/41 .472 .548~ (.698") 3.53 (5.97)
Group identity + 134~ .195 .187" (.232-) 1.85 (2.32)
Perceived equity - 1342 -.210 -.212~ (-.212-) 2,72 (2.72)
Similarity + 1343 -.015 -.015 (-.015) ,20 (.19)

Responsibility
Salience +/- "/51 .036 .040 ( - . 3 5 1 b) ,32 (3.31)
Group identity . . . . ( - . 195 h) -- (2.67)
Perceived equity + 1332 .359 .349" (.4604) 3,96 (4.84),
Similarity . . . . (.008) -- (.20)
Empathy - 1354 -.541 -.521 a (-.521-) 4,59 (4.57)

Costs
Salience +/- "/6~ .313 .338 (.010) 1.86 (.10)
Group identity - 136~ .028 .025 (-.124) .24 (1.14)
Perceived equity + 1362 .435 .408 a (.435',) 3.48 (4.53)
Similarity - 1363 -- .269 -- .248~ (-- .243") 2.50 (2.45)
Empathy - 1364 --.386 --.359 a (--.304a) 2.41 (2.55)
Responsibility + 136.~ - . 111 - . 107 ( - . 107) .75 (.75)

Willingness to help
Salience + ~/Tt .106 .117 (.639 a) .90 (5.81)
Group identity + 1371 .559 .512" (.578 o) 6.42 (6.72)
Perceived equity - 1372 .111 - . 107 ( - . 125 a) 1.28 (2.05)
Similarity + 1373 .134 .127" (--.221 a) 1.80 (2.80)
Empathy + 1374 .105 .100 (.2640) .91 (3.07)
Responsibility - 1375 -.085 .084 (-.042) .86 (.42)
Costs - 1376 - .387 - .396 ~ ( - .396 ~) 3.96 (3.96)

-p < .05 in sole direction of directional hypothesis using one-tailed test.


bp < .05 using two-tailed test to reflect no hypothesis of direction for this effect.
"The first figure in this column is the z value for direct effect; the second figure in parentheses is the z value for total effect.

links, were supported. As a whole, the individual hypothe- salience and ethno-national identity and negatively by per-
ses were therefore well supported. ceived equity. Similarity reflects the positive impact of sa-
lience and perceived equity shows the negative influence of
Impact of the Direct Effects ethno-national identity. Finally, ethno-national identity was
positively affected by salience.
The tests of direct effects provided a simple, straightfor- The pervasiveness of the statistical support for the model
ward means of assessing whether the proposed links be- can be seen in the finding that each of the seven endogenous
tween pairs of constructs were supposed by the data. Sig- constructs was the endpoint for at least one significant direct
nificance tests for direct effects leading to the output linkage. Further, all of the constructs preceding willingness
criterion, willingness to help, indicated it reflects positive to help, with the sole exception of responsibility, signifi-
direct influences from ethno-national identity and similarity, cantly affected at least one other construct.
and a negative direct influence from costs of helping. Mov-
ing backward in the chain of influence, costs of helping Impact of the Total Effects
reflects the positive direct impact of perceived equity and
the negative impact of similarity and empathy. In turn, ex- One benefit of structural equation analysis is its ability to
ternalized responsibility showed a positive direct influence test the relationships between pairs of constructs at a higher
from perceived equity and a negative one from empathy. Its level of complexity than their direct links. The total effects
predecessor construct, empathy, was affected positively by computed by LISREL include the indirect effects that sup-

JAMS 315 FALL, 1993


INFLUENCING CONSUMERS' SELECTION OF DOMESTIC OLSEN, GRANZIN, AND BISWAS
VERSUS IMPORTED PRODUCTS

plement the direct effects. These indirect effects occur when threatened textile workers. All constructs but responsibility
additional, alternative paths of influence lead through other were thus linked with the criterion construct; the general
constructs intervening between the pair in question. Thus, implications of these influences and their applicability for a
the total effects give a more comprehensive indication of the marketing campaign to fight foreign products are consid-
impact of one construct on another (Bollen 1989). In this ered here. First, the positive influence of salience indicates
analysis, indirect effects helped create five significant paths consumers are more willing to help workers regarding is-
of influence for pairs of constructs where either no direct sues they find important, specifically when they view for-
paths were proposed or where the direct links were nonsig- eign competition as harmful to domestic industry and its
nificant. Thus, the output construct, willingness to help, workers. Thus, those designing Buy American campaigns
was affected by indirect paths in addition to the proposed must raise the consciousness of their target audience; Amer-
direct paths such that it received positive influences from icans must learn of the detrimental economic effects of
salience and empathy, and a negative influence from per- foreigners' penetration into the domestic market, and par-
ceived equity. Responsibility was affected by negative total ticularly of the many jobs being lost by their fellow Ameri-
paths from salience and ethno-national identity, the latter cans.
where no direct path was proposed. Second, the positive relationship with ethno-national
It is sometimes instructive to examine the nature of the identity indicates that a greater identification with threat-
indirect paths that substantially augment direct paths in this ened workers increases a consumer's predisposition to help
manner. As an example of indirect influence, note the (non- them. The earlier conceptualization held such identification
standardized) direct effect of empathy on willingness to help to generate norms supportive of the consumer's own cul-
was .100, whereas the total effect was .264. Thus, the ture; here, norms are protective of American business and
additional paths through responsibility and especially costs its workers' employment. To capitalize on this culturally
of helping played a substantial part in linking empathy to based support, the campaign should therefore portray Amer-
the output construct. As an additional example, the direct ican workers as cultural allies, patriotic Americans who are
nonstandardized effect of salience on responsibility was a just as loyal to their flag as are the consumers. Workers
weak .040, whereas the indirect effect was - . 3 9 1 , giving a should be shown as working to keep America a viable force
strong total influence of - . 3 5 1 . One part of the indirect in various areas of international competition. The threat-
impact of salience on responsibility acted straight through ened workers can be portrayed in the workplace as helping
perceived equity, whereas another acted through ethno- make America competitive in the global economy; they can
national identity and thence through both perceived equity be shown in their personal lives as solid citizens contribut-
and empathy to responsibility. Of course, these indirect ef- ing to a society that maintains generally accepted American
fects became successively more attenuated as they included cultural values. The appropriate message would be that to
additional constructs that were themselves less than perfect preserve the America these consumers love, they should
covariates of one another and of responsibility. Nonethe- only purchase foreign goods out of necessity.
less, a pairwise effect can be notably improved by recogniz- Third, the statistically inverse association with perceived
ing indirect effects, as shown here. equity indicates consumers who feel textile workers are not
trying hard enough (that is, who find the current situation is
equitable for workers) are less willing to help. Conversely,
DISCUSSION those who find it inequitable are more willing to help. Con-
sequently, the campaign should reinforce the beliefs of
These findings provide overall support for a model that those who feel the situation is inequitable and demonstrate
uses eight helping constructs and their interrelationships to to the others that it is not equitable. Toward this end, con-
portray the influences on consumers' predisposition to help sumers should be shown a situation that is beyond the con-
American workers whose livelihood is threatened by im- trol of helpless workers who are unable to stem the flow of
ports. This discussion begins by treating the empirical sup- imported goods without assistance. The message can fea-
port for influences on the criterion construct of willingness ture terms like "unfair' and "unjust" to portray advantages
to help. It then progresses backward through the model to given foreign industries v i s a vis American ones. American
examine the flows of influence into the other six endoge- workers should be portrayed as contributing considerable
nous constructs. The operational measures will be explicitly effort in the workplace, while a hard day's work nonetheless
mentioned as a vehicle for providing relevant substantive brings rewards that inequitably fall short of the workers'
content to this discussion (just as they provide statistical contributions.
content as indicators in the measurement model). Fourth, the positive link with similarity suggests the cam-
paign should make the target consumer feel as much as
Influences on Willingness to Help possible "just like" the threatened workers, especially with
respect to losing her/his job. Of course, the empirical find-
Willingness to help was operationalized in this study as a ings here do not signal how to portray workers to communi-
consumer's support for checking the labels to learn where cate their similarity to the target consumer (e.g., whether to
clothing is produced, buying American-made clothes and show them as the typical next-door neighbor, to personify
American brands, and shopping at stores that stock Ameri- them as "All-American" citizens, or to present them as
can products. In support of multi-part Hypothesis 7, the members of the subculture that constitutes the audience for
data revealed three direct sources and three additional total the promotion). The findings indicate that similarity be-
sources of influence on consumers' predisposition to help tween the target consumer and the displaced workers can be

JAMS 316 FALL, 1993


INFLUENCINGCONSUMERS"SELECTIONOF DOMESTIC OLSEN, GRANZIN, AND BISWAS
VERSUS IMPORTEDPRODUCTS

communicated by showing that the current situation for dis- For Hypothesis 6 involving influences on costs of help-
placed workers could easily be the future situation for the ing, three of the six hypothesized relationships were sup-
consumer and/or the consumer's friends and relatives. ported empirically. These mixed results are not surprising,
Fifth, the positive association with empathy indicates the given the complex nature of the construct, wherein costs
campaign should have a strong emotional content and em- may be incurred for both helping and not helping. Consum-
phasize the suffering and difficulties faced by workers. Pre- ers will find the costs of helping lower to the extent they are
vious helping research provides solid evidence for the im- led to put themselves in the place of Americans who are like
portant part played by emotional arousal in generating a themselves, persons who are being treated unfairly by for-
helping response (Amato 1986; Amato, Ho, and Partridge eign firms. For Hypothesis 5, although the responsibility
1984; Batson and Coke 1981; Coke, Batson, and McDavis construct did not affect helping, it was itself affected by four
1978; Hoover, Wood, and Knowles 1983; Krebs 1975; Pil- antecedents, three hypothesized and one nonhypothesized.
iavin et al. 1981; Schroeder et al. 1988). Jn fact, the com- Potential helpers are less likely to externalize an obligation
prehensive model advanced by Weiner (1980, 1985) holds for helping by assigning it to workers they consider to be
that emotional reactions provide the direction and the motor treated inequitably, workers with whom they identify and
for such behavior. Consumers should therefore be induced also empathize regarding an issue held salient.
to vicariously share the suffering of the unfortunate workers Empathy, which influenced willingness to help and costs
who have been displaced into a dreadful and possibly hope- of helping as hypothesized, was influenced by three of the
less situation. Specifically, the campaign should urge and four constructs proposed to affect it by Hypothesis 4. Con-
facilitate the target consumer to place him/herself in the sumers become more empathic in the case of salient prob-
place of the disadvantaged workers; for example, "How lems that affect workers with whom they identify, problems
would you like to have foreigners take your job away?" or that place these victims in an inequitable situation. Support-
"How would you feel if your fellow Americans made it easy ing the only proposed path to similarity in Hypothesis 3,
for foreigners to put you out of work?" consumers see themselves as more similar to threatened
Sixth, those who perceive the costs of helping to be high workers when they find the issue more salient. As stated in
are less willing to help. While the empirical results for costs Hypothesis 2, consumers find the situation more inequitable
of helping stem from two allied, narrow measures of cost when they feel greater identification with threatened work-
pertaining to the attributes of the product, the implications ers. The influence of salience posited by Hypothesis 1 was
can easily be generalized. Consumers must be convinced supported; the more important the problem, the greater con-
the costs of helping are minimal and the costs of not helping sumers' identification with the workers.
are maximal. In the first case, consumers should be shown
there is little or no loss from purchasing products that fea- Implications for Future Research
ture attributes most important to them; that is, whatever
their cost, American products provide considerable value The specific helping model developed for this study,
for the money spent. which incorporates the contributions of many varied studies
In the second case, consumers should be shown that eco- in the behavioral sciences, was well supported by the empir-
nomic decline is costly, not only for the nation but also for ical test. Nonetheless, considerable work remains in the
individual consumers. To illustrate, an impressionistic, development of a broadly applicable model of helping be-
graphic portrayal could depict an America of the future with havior that can be used in specific contexts, such as Buy
widespread poverty, lack of economic and social oppor- American campaigns.
tunity, and reduced purchase and consumption opportunities Future research can profitably concentrate on exploring
for the very people who supported foreign suppliers in the the nature of the costs and the benefits that impact most
1990s. In complementary fashion, the benefits from sup- heavily on helping behavior. This research program should
porting American industry in this decade could be portrayed identify a comprehensive list of the relevant costs and bene-
in parallel terms, showing prosperity, broad economic and fits. Here, the costs and benefits of both helping and not
social opportunity, and a wide selection of (American) prod- helping should be investigated. To give them the broadest
ucts to fill every consumption need in the America of the application, they should be developed first within the spe-
future. cific context of this study, and then later developed across
various contexts so they apply to American products in
Influences on Antecedent Constructs general. They should be carefully laid out, perhaps using a
matrix with marginal categories for costs and, alternatively,
All seven constructs antecedent to the criterion construct benefits of: I) helping versus not helping, 2) personal ver-
enter significant relationships with other constructs in the sus nonpersonal involvement, and 3) short-term versus
network of influences leading to willingness to help; as long-term implication.
mentioned, all but responsibility affect the criterion con- The concept of equity needs further development. As it
struct. To provide a fuller understanding of the contributions appears in behavioral science research, the perception of
of these constructs, this section examines the influences on equity typically reflects a balancing of contributions made
the antecedent, intervening variables in the network of in- and rewards received by an individual. An important task
fluences. The rationale for this discussion is that to better for marketing research is to determine the dimensions con-
understand how marketing can impact costs of helping, for sumers use in performing this tradeoff. Thus, future re-
example, it is necessary to see how the cost of helping is search should investigate the nature of the rewards (e.g.,
determined by other constructs. pay, status, security) American consumers consider to con-

JAMS 317 FALL, 1993


INFLUENCING CONSUMERS' SELECTION OF DOMESTIC OLSEN, GRANZIN, AND BISWAS
VERSUS IMPORTED PRODUCTS

stitute the relevant retums for threatened workers. It should discriminant validity may be understood by recognizing that
also establish the dimensions of the contributions made by equity represents the consumer's perception that the cost to
these workers, going beyond "effort" to include such inputs the two parties should be commensurate with their outputs.
as the time and abilities furnished and the sacrifice from not If equity is perceived, the cost to workers (here, their effort
performing alternative occupations, including leisure time, to help themselves) is insufficient to justify the consumer's
etc. Using the relevant dimensions would facilitate measur- absorbing an unjust cost of helping. Thus, future research
ing consumers' assessment of the equity/inequity of the should proceed to refine the measures of equity as well as
workers' situation. those of the other constructs, through development of high-
The nature of consumers' identification with the workers ly reliable batteries of items for representing each of the
must be clarified. Behavioral scientists have provided vari- constructs. Ideally, these measures will be sufficiently broad
ous allied conceptions of group identity, including common in their applicability that they can used in adaptations of
fate and common threat. Work in the field o f marketing by helping behavior to other marketing contexts.
Waheeduzzaman and Marks (1989) suggests ethnocentrism
and affiliated constructs must be further refined concep-
tually and empirically before marketing researchers can find APPENDIX
a common ground for study and interpretation. It will be Indicators for Eight Constructs
profitable to further clarify the treatment of cultural identi- in the Helping Model
fication and group identification as research continues to Indicator
investigate the impact of consumers' identifications on help- Construct Symbol Item from Questionnaire
ing. The findings of the present study suggest that the no-
Salience x~ Because of foreign imports, today's
tion of common fate (e.g., "We Americans are all in it textile workers are going to be
together") or common threat (e.g., "A threat to one of our tomorrow's unemployed.
workers is a threat to all of us") would be a viable beginning x2 Foreign competitors cause severe
point for this future research. damage to our textile industry.
Identity y~ Americans should not buy foreign prod-
Responsibility was the only construct without a signifi- ucts, because this hurts American
cant impact on willingness to help, despite its demonstrated business and causes unemployment.
usefulness in previous helping research. Thus, future re- y: There should be very little trading or
search should strengthen the adaptation and measurement of purchasing of goods from other coun-
this construct. The present study considered externalized tries unless out of necessity.
Equity Y3 I can't help wonder, are textile workers
obligation; specifically, assigning responsibility for correct- putting enough effort into fighting
ing the situation to the supposed victims themselves. One foreign competition to justify our
obvious modification would instead use internalized respon- help?
sibility in the context of this study. Alternatively, a future 3'4 If textile workers would make some ef-
fort, I am sure they could improve
application might implement a bipolar construct that incor- their situation.
porates a continuum based on both externalized and inter- Similarity Ys What's happening to textile workers
nalized responsibility. today can happen to me tomorrow.
Similar suggestions can be made regarding the other con- Y6 I am no different than the average tex-
structs in the helping model. To illustrate, similarity might tile worker when it comes to losing
my job from foreign competition.
be examined to determine whether it is most effectively Empathy Y7 It makes me feel bad to see the diffi-
represented as similarity with respect to the workers' job culties textile workers are facing
situation or whether other characteristics of the workers because of imports.
should be used. Salience might focus on the importance of Y8 It is a shame that textile workers are
suffering because of imports.
the issue in an alternative fashion; for example, in terms of Responsibility Y9 Textile workers should improve their
its impact on the consumers themselves, on the workers own situation without any special help
themselves, or on the future of the entire nation. Regarding from me.
empathy, the nature of empathic arousal might be measured Y~o Imports are textile workers' problems,
using a stimulus that is more graphic. For example, survey so they are the ones who should take
care of them.
participants might be asked to assume they had just learned Costs y~ ~ The textile workers should not expect us
there was a 5 0 - 5 0 chance they would be laid off at the end to buy only clothes made here be-
of the month because their employer was suffering large cause they do not make clothes that
losses to foreign competition (e.g., in the case of workers in are as stylish as those made abroad.
Y~2 I would have to sacrifice style if I only
tangible goods industries, due to lost sales; for government bought clothes made in the U.S.
workers, due to reduced tax revenues; or for service work- Help Y13 I will shop first at retail stores that make
ers, due to a general economic downturn). a special effort to sell American made
Finally, because the quality of structural equation analy- products.
sis is particularly vulnerable to operational measures that Yt4 When I buy clothing I will try as much
as I can to buy U.S. brands.
provide insufficiently divergent measures for the differing Yl5 I am willing to always buy American
constructs in the model, future effort must deal with mea- made clothes.
surement. Here, the modification index for the loading of Yl6 I am willing to take the time to look on
one indicator of equity (Y4) on the costs of helping construct labels so I know where the clothes I
wear were made.
is considerably higher than desirable. It appears this lack of

JAMS 318 FALL, t993


INFLUENCING CONSUMERS" SELECTION OF DOMESTIC OLSEN, GRANZIN, AND BISWAS
VERSUS IMPORTED PRODUCTS

SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS minants of Reactions to Dishonesty." Journal of Experimental Social


Psychology 12 (January): 9-25.
Bollen, Kenneth A. 1989. Structural Equations with Latent Variables.
This study presented a model based on seven behavioral New York: Wiley.
constructs proposed to explain consumers' willingness to Byme, Barbara M. 1989. A Primer of LISREL. New York: Springer-
help American workers threatened by foreign imported Verlag.
products. The model performed well empirically in this Carlson, Michael and Norman Miller. 1987. "Explanation of the Relation
marketing context. Consumers' predisposition to help dis- Between Negative Mood and Helping." Psychological Bulletin 102
(July): 91-101.
advantaged American workers received significant direct Churchill, Gilbert A., Jr. 1979. "A Paradigm for Developing Better Mea-
explanation (in the structural sense) from constructs repre- sures of Marketing Constructs." Journal of Marketing Research 16 (Feb-
senting consumers' identification with their culture, as ex- mary): 64-73.
pressed in terms of norms supporting the workers, consum- Cialdini, Robert B., Donald J. Baumann, and Douglas T. Kenrick. 1981.
"Insight from Sadness: A Three-Step Model of the Development of Al-
ers' perceived similarity with the workers, and lower truism as Hedonism." Developmental Review l (September): 207-223.
perceived costs of buying American products. The criterion Cicirelli, Victor G. 1983. "Adult Children's Attachment and Helping Be-
construct also received significant total explanation (as the havior to Elderly Parents: A Path Model." Journal of Marriage and the
sum of direct plus indirect structural influences) from con- Family 45 (November): 815-825.
sumers' belief the issue is important, their feeling of empa- Coke, Jay S., C. Daniel Batson, and Katherine McDavis. 1978. "Empathic
Mediation of Helping: A Two-Stage Model." Journal of Personality and
thy for the workers, and their not feeling any disadvantage Social Psychology 36 (July): 752-766.
suffered by the workers is nonetheless equitable. Crawford, John C. and Charles W. Lamb. 1981. "Source Preferences for
Additional linkages among these constructs and consum- Imported Products."' Journal of Purchasing and Material Management
ers' assignment of responsibility to the workers for improv- 17 (Winter): 28-33.
Dickerson, Kitty G. 1982. "Imported Versus U.S.-Produced Apparel: Con-
ing their own plight demonstrate that the constructs and sumer Views and Buying Patterns." Home Economics Research Journal
their interrelationships form a complex network. This net- 10 (March): 241-252.
work of influences provides a useful basis for marketing Dillin, John. 1992. "Top Retailer Wal-Mart a Target of Strong 'Buy Ameri-
programs designed to win support among Americans for can' Push." The Christian Science Monitor 84 (June 5): 3.
American industry, its workers, and its products. By under- Dovidio, John F. and William N. Morris. 1975. "Effects of Stress and
Commonality of Fate on Helping Behavior." Journal of Personality and
standing this network of influences, marketers are better Social Psychology 31 (February): 145-149.
prepared to influence consumers' perceptions and beliefs at Duval, Shelley, Virginia H. Duval, and Robert Neely. 1979. "Self-Focus,
appropriate points in the process leading to helping. Felt Responsibility, and Helping Behavior." Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology 37 (October): 1769-1778.
Ettenson, Richard, Janet Wagner, and Gary Gaeth. 1988. "Evaluating the
REFERENCES Effect of Country of Origin and the Made in the U.S.A. Campaign: A
Conjoint Analysis." Journal of Retailing 64 (Spring): 85-99.
Amato, Paul R. 1986. "Emotional Arousal and Helping Behavior in a Geer, James H. and Lynn Jarmecky. 1973. "The Effect of Being Responsi-
Real-Life Emergency." Journal of Applied Social Psychology 16 (7): ble for Reducing Another's Pain on Subjects' Response and Arousal."
633-641. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 26 (May): 232-237.
Amato, Paul R., Robert Ho, and Sonia Partridge. 1984. "'Responsibility Granzin, Kent L. and Janeen E. Olsen. 1991. "Characterizing Participants
Attribution and Helping Behaviour in the Ash Wednesday Bushflres." in Activities Protecting the Environment: A Focus on Donating, Recyc-
Australian Journal of Psychology 36 (August): 191-203. ling and Conservation Behaviors." Journal of Public Policy & Marketing
Anderson, James C. and David W. Gerbing. 1988. "Structural Equation 10 (Fall): 1-27.
Modeling in Practice: A Review and Recommended Two-Step Ap- Hun, C. Min and Vern Terpstra. 1988. "Country of Origin Effects for Uni-
proach." Psychological Bulletin I03 (May): 411-423. National and Bi-National Products." Journal of International Business
Bar-Tal, Daniel. 1976. Prosocial Behavior: Theory and Research. New Studies 18 (Summer): 235-254.
York: Halsted Press. Hatfield, Elaine, G. William Walster, and Jane A. Piliavin. 1978. "Equity
Batson, C. Daniel, Michelle H. Bolen, Julie A. Cross, and Helen E. Theory and Helping Relationships." In Altruism, Sympathy, and Help-
Neufinger-Benefiel. 1986. "Where is the Altruism in the Altruistic Per- ing: Psychological and Sociological Principles. Ed. Lauren Wispe. New
sonality?" Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 50 (January): York: Academic Press, 115-139.
212-220. Hayden, Shelly R., Thomas T. Jackson, and J. Guydish. 1984. "Helping
Batson, C. Daniel, Pamela J. Cochran, Marshall F. Biederman, James L. Behavior of Females: Effects of Stress and Commonality of Fate.'" Jour-
Blosser, Maurice J. Ryan, and Bruce Vogt. 1978. "Failure to Help When nal of Psychology t 17 (July): 233-237.
in a Hurry: Callousness or Conflict?" Personality and Social Psychology Herche, Joel. 1992. "A Note on the Predictive Validity of the CET-
Bulletin 4 (Winter): 97-101. SCALE." Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 20 (Summer):
Batson, C. Daniel and Jay S. Coke. 1981. "Empathy: A Source of Altruis- 261-264.
tic Motivation for Helping?" In Altruism and Helping Behavior: Social, Herting, Jerald R. and Herbert L. Costner. 1985. "Respeciflcation in Mul-
Personality, and Developmental Perspectives. Eds. J. Philippe Rushton tiple Indicator Models." In Causal Models in the Social Sciences. Ed.
and Richard M. Sorrentino. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 157- Hubert M. Blaylock. New York: Aldine, 321-393.
187. Hirsch, James S. and Dana Milbank. 1992. "Buy American is Easier Said
Batson, C. Daniel, Suzanne Pate, Harold Lawless, Paul Sp~kman, Scott than Done." Wall Street Journal 219 (January 28): BI,B5.
Lambers, and Bradley Worman. 1979. "Helping Under Conditions of Hong, Sung-Tai and Robert S. Wyer. 1989. "Effects of Country-of-Origin
Common Threat: Increased 'We-Feeling' or Ensuring Reciprocity?" So- and Product Attribute Information on Product Evaluation: An Informa-
cial Psychology Quarterly 42 (December): 410-414. tion Processing Perspective." Journal of Consumer Research 16 (Sep-
Bentler, Peter M. and Douglas G. Bonett. 1980. "Significance Tests and tember): 175-187.
Goodness of Fit in the Analysis of Covariance Structures." Psychologi- Hoover, Claudia W., Elizabeth E. Wood, and Eric S. Knowles. 1983.
cal Bulletin 88 (November): 588-606. "Forms of Social Awareness and Helping." Journal of Experimental
Bilkey, Warren J. and Erik Nes. 1982. "Country-of-Origin Effects on Prod- Social Psychology 19 (November): 577-590.
uct Evaluations.'" Journal of International Business Studies 13 Jasso, Guillermina. 1983. "Social Consequences of the Sense of Distribu-
(Spring/Summer): 89-99. tive Justice: Small-group Applications." In Equity Theory: Psychologi-
Bleda, Paul R., Sharon E. Bleda, Donn Byrne, and Leonard A. White. cal and Sociological Perspectives. Eds. David M. Messick and Karen S.
1976. "When a Bystander Becomes an Accomplice: Situational Deter- Cook. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers, 243-294.

JAMS 319 FALL, 1993


INFLUENCING CONSUMERS' SELECTION OF DOMESTIC OLSEN, GRANZIN, AND BISWAS
VERSUS IMPORTED PRODUCTS

Johansson, Johny K., Susan P. Douglas, and Ikujiro Nonaka. 1985. "As- Attribution-Affect Model of Helping Behavior." Journal of Personality
sessing the Impact of Country of Origin on Product Evaluations: A New and Social Psychology 50 (June): 1123-1133.
Methodological Perspective." Journal of Marketing Research 22 (No- Rogers, Marian, Norman Miller, F. Stephan Mayer, and Shelley Duval.
vember): 388-396. 1982. "Personal Responsibility and Salience of the Request for Help:
Konrad, Walecia, Russell Mitchell, and Laura Zinn. 1992. "Why Leslie Determinants of the Relation Between Negative Affect and Helping
Weruer Shops Overseas." Business Week No. 3250 (February 3): 30. Behavior." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 43 (Novem-
Kramer, Roderick M. and Marilynn B. Brewer. 1984. "Effects of Group ber): 956-970.
Identity on Resource Use in a Simulated Commons Dilemma.'" Journal Rugman, Alan M. and Michael V. Gestrin. 1991. "U.S. Trade Laws as
of Personality and Social Psychology 46 (May): 1044-1057. Barriers to Globalization." The World Economy 14 (September): 335-
Krebs, Dennis. 1975. "Empathy and Altruism." Journal of Personality and 352.
Social Psychology 32 (December): 1134-1146. Rushton, J. Philippe. 1981. "The Altruistic Personality." In Altruism and
Krebs, Dennis and Cristine Russell. 1981. "Role-Taking and Altruism: Helping Behavior: Social, Personality and Development Perspectives.
When You Put Yourself in the Shoes of Another, Will They Take You to Eds. J. Philippe Rushton and Richard M. Sorrentino. Hillsdale, NJ:
Their Owner's Aid?" In Altruism and Helping Behavior: Social, Person- Lawrence Erlbaum, 251-266.
ality and Development Perspectives. Eds. J. Philippe Rushton and Rich- Schmidt, Greg and Bernard Weiner. 1988. "An Attribution-Affect-Action
ard M. Sorrentino. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 137-165. Theory of Behavior: Replications of Judgments of Help-Giving." Per-
Latane, Bibb and John M. Darley. 1976. "Help in a Crisis: Bystander sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin 14 (September): 610-621.
Response to an Emergency." In Contemporary Topics in Social Psychol- Schroeder, David A., John F. Dovidio, Mark E. Sibicky, and Linda L.
ogy. Eds. John W. Thibaut, Janet T. Spence, and Robert C. Carson. Matthews. 1988. "Empathic Concern and Helping Behavior: Egoism or
Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press, 309-332. Altruism?" Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 24 (July): 333-
Lerner, Melvin H. and Hames R. Meindl. 1981. "Justice and Altruism." In 353.
Altruism and Helping Behavior: Social, Personality and Development Schwartz, Shalom H. 1973. "Normative Explanations of Helping Behav-
Perspectives. Eds. J. Philippe Rushton and Richard M. Sorrentino. ior: A Critique, Proposal, and Empirical Test." Journal of Experimental
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 137-165. Social Psychology 9 (July): 349-364.
Long, J. Scott. 1983a. Confirmatory Factor Analysis: A Preface to LIS- Shimp, Terence A. and Subhash Sharma. 1987. "'Consumer Ethnocentr-
REL. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications. ism: Construction and Validation of the CETSCALE." Journal of Mar-
- - . 1983b. Covariance Structure Models: An Introduction to LISREL. keting Research 24 (August): 280-289.
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications. Shotland, R. Lance and Charles A. Stebbins. 1983. "Emergency and Cost
Lumpkin, James R., John C. Crawford, and Gap Kim. 1985. "Perceived as Determinants of Helping Behavior and the Slow Accumulation of
Risk as a Factor in Buying Foreign Clothes." International Journal of Social Psychological Knowledge." Social Psychology Quarterly 46
Advertising 4 (Number 2): 157-171. (March): 36-46.
Mayer, Stephan F., Shelley Duval, Rolf Holtz, and Craig Bowman. 1985. Sternquist, Brenda and Lisa A. Phillips. 1991. "Hong Kong, U.S. Retail
"Self-Focus, Helping Request Salience, Felt Responsibility, and Helping Buyers Increase Purchase of Imports." Marketing News 25 (Oct. 14):
Behavior." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 11 (June): 133- 7,9.
144. Stotland, Ezra. 1969. "Exploratory Investigations of Empathy." In Ad-
McClelland, Gary and John Rohrbaugh. 1978. "Who Accepts the Pareto vances in Experimental Social Psychology. Volume 4. Ed. Leonard
Axiom? The Role of Utility and Equity in Arbitration Decisions." Be- Berkowitz. New York: Academic Press, 271-314.
havioral Science 23 (November): 446-456. Taormina, Robert J. and David M. Messick. 1983. "Deservingness for
McGee, Robert W. 1989. "The Protectionist Mentality: An Economic and Foreign Aid: Effects of Need, Similarity, and Estimated Effectiveness."
Philosophical Analysis, Part II." The Mid-Atlantic Journal of Business Journal of Applied Social Psychology 13 (September/October): 371-
26 (Fall): 81-104. 391.
Meyer, John P. and Anne Mulherin. 1980. "From Attribution to Helping: Terpstra, Vern and Ravi Sarathy. 1991. International Marketing. Fifth
An Analysis of the Mediating Effects of Affect and Expectancy." Jour- Edition. Chicago: The Dryden Press.
nal of Personality and Social Psychology 39 (August): 201-210. Waheeduzzaman, A. N. M. and Lawrence J. Marks. 1989. "Halo Effects,
Morgan, William R. and Jack Sawyer. 1976. "Bargaining, Expectations, Consumer Etho-national Affinity, and Behavioral Intentions: An Exten-
and the Preference for Equality over Equity." Journal of Personality and sion of the Multi-Attribute Model of Country of Origin Effects." In
Social Psychology 6 (June): 139-149. Proceedings of the American Marketing Association Educators' Confer-
Mulaik, Stanley A., Larry R. James, Judith Van Alstine, Nathan Bennett, ence. Ed. Paul Bloom. Chicago: American Marketing Association,
Sherri Lind, and C. Dean Stilawell. 1989. "Evaluation of Goodness-of- 252-257.
Fit Indices for Structural Equation Models." Psychological Bulletin 105 Walster, Elaine, G. William Walster, and Ellen Berscheid. 1978. Equity:
(May): 430-445. Theory and Research. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Narayana, Chem. 1981. "Aggregate Images of American and Japanese Wang, Chih-Kang and Charles Lamb, Jr. 1983. "The Impact of Selected
Products: Implications for International Marketing." Columbia Journal Environmental Forces Upon Consumers' Willingness to Buy Foreign
of World Business 16 (Summer): 3-33. Products." Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 11 (Winter):
Netemeyer, Richard G., Srinivas Durvasula, and Donald R. Lichtenstein. 71-84.
1991. "A Cross-National Assessment of the Reliability and Validity of Weiner, Bernard. 1980. "A Cognitive (Attribution)-Emotion-Action Model
the CETSCALE." Journal of Marketing Research 28 (August): 320- of Motivated Behavior: An Analysis of Judgments of Help-Giving."
327. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 39 (August): 186-200.
Obermiller, Carl and Eric Spangenberg. 1989. "Exploring the Effects of . 1985. "An Attributional Theory of Achievement Motivation and
Country of Origin Labels: An Information Processing Framework." In Emotion." Psychological Review 92 (October): 548-573.
Advances in Consumer Research. Ed. Thomas K. Srull. Provo, UT:
Association for Consumer Research, 454-459.
Ofir, Chezy and Donald R. Lehmann. 1986. "Measuring Images of For- ABOUT THE AUTHORS
eign Products." Columbia Journal of Worm Business 21 (Summer):
105-108.
Olsen, Janeen E. and Kent L. Granzin. 1992. "Gaining Retailers' Assis- Janeen E. Olsen is Associate Professor of marketing at
tance in Fighting Counterfeiting: Conceptualization and Empirical Test Louisiana State University. She received her Ph.D. from the
of a Helping Model." Journal of Retailing 68 (Spring): 90-109. University of Utah. Her research interests include interna-
Peterson, Laurie. 1991. "Buy American, But Only Sometimes."Adweek's tional marketing and channels of distribution. Her work has
Marketing News 36 (September 2): 9.
Piliavin, Jane A., John F. Dovidio, Samuel L. Gaertner, and Russell D.
appeared in the Journal of Business Research, Journal of
Clark, III. 1981. Emergency Intervention. New York: Academic Press. Advertising, Journal of Retailing, and other marketing jour-
Reisenzein, Rainer. 1986. "A Structural Equation Analysis of Weiner's nals.

JAMS 320 FALL, 1993


INFLUENCING CONSUMERS" SELECTION OF DOMESTIC OLSEN, GRANZIN, AND BISWAS
VERSUS IMPORTED PRODUCTS

Kent L. G r a n z i n is Professor of marketing at the Univer- A b h i j i t Biswas is Assistant Professor of marketing at Loui-
sity of Utah. He received his Ph.D. from the University siana State University. He received his Ph.D. from the Uni-
of Illinois. His research interests include the fitness mar- versity of Houston. Dr. Biswas's work has been published
ket, consumer logistics, helping behavior, and marketing in the Journal of Marketing, Journal of the Academy of
channels. His research findings have been reported in the Marketing Science, Journal of Macromarketing, Journal of
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science and in vari- Business Research, Journal of Advertising, Journal of Con-
ous other business and social science journals and proceed- sumer Affairs, Psychology and Marketing, and Journalism
ings. Quarterly, as well as other refereed journals.

JAMS 321 FALL, 1993

You might also like