Aggression and Violent Behavior: $Jjuhvvlrqdqg9Lrohqw%Hkdylru

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

$JJUHVVLRQDQG9LROHQW%HKDYLRU  ²

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Aggression and Violent Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/aggviobeh

A meta-analysis of the association between psychopathy and sadism in 7


forensic samples☆

Debra O'Connell , David K. Marcus
Department of Psychology, Washington State University, PO Box 644820, Johnson Tower 233, Pullman, WA 99164-4820, USA

A B S T R A C T

This meta-analysis examined the association between psychopathic personality traits as assessed by the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R; Hare, 1991), and
sadism. The PCL-R yields a total score as well as two factor scores. Factor 1 encompasses the interpersonal/affective components of psychopathy such as glibness, and
callousness. Factor 2 captures the antisocial behavior aspects of psychopathy including impulsivity, and poor planning abilities. The meta-analysis included 19
independent adult, male forensic samples from 16 articles that included a total of 5161 participants. The average r across all studies for PCL-R total score and sadism
was 0.24 (p < .001). Both PCL-R Factor 1 (r = 0.25, p < .01) and PCL-R Factor 2 (r = 0.26, p < .01), yielded similar correlations with sadism. Sadism mea-
surement method and type of sadism assessed did not significantly moderate the association between psychopathy and sadism. However, there was low power in the
moderator analyses. These results support previous research that found both the interpersonal/affective component of psychopathy and the antisocial behavior
component of psychopathy are associated with sadism.

Psychopathic personality traits include characteristics such as su- explain this shared variance.
perficial charm, irresponsibility, fearlessness, conning behavior/ma- Within forensic settings, the association between psychopathy and
nipulation, and lack of empathy (Hare, 1970). Sadism is defined as sadism has only been studied in male samples, and researchers have
deriving enjoyment from the suffering of others (Baumeister & primarily utilized the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R; Hare,
Campbell, 1999). Although individuals who exhibit each of these per- 1991) to measure psychopathy. The PCL-R yields a total score as well as
sonality traits often hurt others, the reasons for doing so seem to be two correlated factors (Hare, 1991). Factor 1 encompasses the inter-
driven by different motivations. Psychopathic individuals will hurt personal/affective components of psychopathy such as glibness,
others in order to obtain something they want, and will not care that shallow affect, and callousness. Factor 2 captures the antisocial beha-
they have caused harm to someone (Hare & Neumann, 2008). Sadistic vior aspects of psychopathy including impulsivity, parasitic lifestyle,
individuals will hurt others simply for the pleasure they experience and poor planning abilities. Measurement method for sadism in the
from doing so (Paulhus, 2014). Studies have consistently found an as- literature has varied, and includes both self-report as well as clinician-
sociation between psychopathic personality traits and sadism, with ratings of behavior, personality traits, or a combination of both. Pre-
offenders who are both psychopathic and sadistic being more violent vious literature reviews on psychopathy and sadism have focused on
and likely to recidivate (Meloy, 2000; Porter, Woodworth, Earle, forensic subsamples such as homicide offenders (e.g., James & Proulx,
Drugge, & Boer, 2003) compared to individuals who are either psy- 2014; Kirsch & Becker, 2007). To date, no meta-analyses have been
chopathic or sadistic. In fact, a diagnosis of psychopathy and/or sadism conducted to examine the association between psychopathy and sadism
is often used to justify longer sentences or deny parole (e.g., Levenson, among offenders. By synthesizing the available findings across different
2004; Lloyd, Clark, & Forth, 2010). Still, the precise nature of how types of forensic samples (e.g., homicide offenders, sex offenders, sub-
psychopathy and sadism are related remains ambiguous, both in terms stance abuse offenders) via meta-analysis, it will be possible to quantify
of the magnitude of the association and in terms of what components the strength of the overall association between psychopathy and


This work was based on Debra O'Connell's preliminary exam project at Washington State University.

Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, Washington State University, Pullman, WA 99164-4820, USA.
E-mail addresses: debra.oconnell@wsu.edu (D. O'Connell), david.marcus@wsu.edu (D.K. Marcus).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2019.02.013
Received 15 May 2018; Received in revised form 2 October 2018; Accepted 18 February 2019
$YDLODEOHRQOLQH)HEUXDU\
‹(OVHYLHU/WG$OOULJKWVUHVHUYHG
D. O'Connell and D.K. Marcus $JJUHVVLRQDQG9LROHQW%HKDYLRU  ²

sadism. adulthood. Mokros et al. (2011) found that the PCL-R Affective and
The current meta-analysis examined the overall association between Antisocial Facets were both correlated with sexual sadism, whereas the
psychopathy and sadism as well as the associations between the inter- PCL-R Interpersonal and Lifestyle Facets were not. These findings sug-
personal/affective component of psychopathy and sadism and the an- gest that some traits from both PCL-R Factor 1 and Factor 2 are asso-
tisocial behavior component of psychopathy and sadism. Only studies ciated with sadism.
utilizing the PCL-R were included in the meta-analysis. An additional
advantage of limiting the meta-analysis to PCL-R studies is that, unlike 1.2. Psychopathy and sadistic personality
some other measures of psychopathy, none of the items on the PCL-R
address sadistic pleasure at another's pain.1 Sadistic Personality Disorder (SPD) first appeared in the appendix of
the DSM-III-R (American Psychiatric Association, 1987) as a personality
1. Psychopathy and sadism disorder to be considered for future editions of the DSM. To meet di-
agnostic criteria for SPD, an individual must have engaged in “a per-
Sadism as a construct has changed over time and debate continues vasive pattern of cruel, demeaning, and aggressive behavior, beginning
as to whether sadism is best conceptualized as a personality dysfunc- by early adulthood” (American Psychiatric Association, 1987, p. 371)
tion, a type of antisocial behavior, or a combination of both (e.g., for the purpose of amusement or obtaining pleasure from the suffering
Krueger, 2010). Additionally, there has been debate over whether of others. Ultimately, SPD was not included in later editions of the DSM,
sexual sadism is a separate construct from other forms of sadism (e.g., but these DSM criteria have been used in some studies that have ex-
sadism as a personality disorder, and sadism as a personality trait; amined the association between psychopathy and sadism (e.g.,
Kirsch & Becker, 2007; Krueger, 2010). Because of continued debate Delescluse & Pham, 2005).
over the definition of sadism, a review of the association between Holt, Meloy, and Strack (1999) examined the association between
psychopathy and sadism will be organized by type of sadism assessed psychopathy and sadism in a sample of 41 offenders classified as either
and include a brief overview of each type of sadism. violent, or sexually violent. Psychopathy was measured using the two-
factor model of the PCL-R. Sadism was measured with the sadism scale
1.1. Psychopathy and sexual sadism of the Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory-II (MCMI-II; Millon, 1987),
the sadism items from Personality Disorder Examination (PDE;
The severity of sexually sadistic behavior can vary across a spectrum Loranger, 1988), and the diagnostic criteria for sexual sadism from the
of behavior from adults engaged in consensual acts of sadism and ma- DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) Holt et al. (1999)
sochism to torture and homicide of non-consenting victims. Empirical found significant, positive correlations between the PCL-R total score,
research on the association between psychopathy and sexual sadism has F1, and F2 with both the MCMI-II sadism scale and the PDE. The cor-
exclusively examined male forensic samples, particularly sex offenders. relations ranged from r = 0.48–0.64. Additionally, individuals classi-
Across studies, PCL-R total scores are consistently and positively cor- fied as “psychopaths,” based on the PCL-R recommended score of 30 or
related with sexual sadism (Firestone, Bradford, Greenberg, Larose, & higher, scored significantly higher on the MCMI-II sadism scale and the
Curry, 1998; Langevin, 2003; Porter et al., 2003). However, the asso- sadism items on the PDE compared to non-psychopathic offenders. Of
ciations between sexual sadism and the two factors of the PCL-R have note, the psychopathic sexually violent offenders did not differ sig-
been inconsistent. Porter et al. (2003) found an association between the nificantly from the psychopathic violent offenders in their MCMI-II
interpersonal/affective components of psychopathy (PCL-R, F1) and scores or their PDE scores. There were no significant correlations be-
sexual sadism, but not the antisocial behavior components of psycho- tween DSM-IV sexual sadism criteria and psychopathy, although the
pathy (PCL-R, F2) and sexual sadism. Similarly, homicidal offenders researchers noted that one limitation was the small number of partici-
had higher F1 scores and diagnoses of sexual sadism compared to non- pants meeting diagnostic criteria for DSM-IV sexual sadism in their
homicidal offenders (Firestone et al., 1998). In contrast, sexually sa- sample (n = 3).
distic sex offenders had significantly higher F2 scores compared to In summary, a majority of the research on the association between
other sex offenders, but sadistic sex offenders did not differ from other psychopathy and sadism in forensic settings has assessed sexual sadism.
sex offenders on F1 (Barbaree, Seto, Serin, Amos, & Preston, 1994). Occasionally, sadism has been more broadly defined as a general en-
Mokros, Osterheider, Hucker, and Nitschke (2011) examined the joyment of hurting others. Although there is clear evidence that there is
association between psychopathy and sexual sadism using the hospital an association between PCL-R total scores and sadism (e.g., Firestone
records of 100 sex offenders, using the four facet scoring model of the et al., 1998; Holt et al., 1999; Langevin, 2003; Porter et al., 2003), the
PCL-R. The four-facet model of the PCL-R separates F1 into Inter- findings have been less consistent regarding how each PCL-R factor is
personal and Affective facets, and F2 into Lifestyle, and Antisocial fa- related to sadism.
cets. The Interpersonal Facet is characterized by such traits as manip-
ulation, and pathological lying, and the Affective Facet captures such 2. The present study
items as lack of remorse, callousness, and lack of empathy. The Lifestyle
Facet encompasses impulsivity, and irresponsibility, whereas the Anti- The present meta-analysis assessed the association between psy-
social Facet primarily refers to criminal activity during childhood and chopathic personality traits as measured by the Psychopathy Checklist-
Revised (PCL-R; Hare, 1991) and all types of sadism (i.e., sexual sadism,
personality models of sadism, and mixed violent/sexual sadism) in
1
The original criteria for the meta-analysis included any measure of psy- forensic samples. The meta-analysis was limited to studies utilizing the
chopathy; however, all of the research conducted with non-forensic samples PCL-R because the literature search yielded only one study that did not
(mostly college students) utilized self-report measures of psychopathy and use the PCL-R in a forensic sample.
sadism. Therefore, including the non-forensic samples would have confounded Consistent with previous research on psychopathy and sadism
psychopathy measure, sample type, and method variance. Additionally, the
(e.g.,Firestone et al., 1998; Holt et al., 1999; Langevin, 2003; Mokros
studies with non-forensic samples utilized the “dark triad,” and “dark tetrad”
et al., 2011), it was hypothesized that PCL-R total scores, Factor 1 and
models of psychopathy and everyday sadism that are conceptually different
from PCL-R defined psychopathy, and sadism measured in the forensic samples. Factor 2 will all positively correlate with measures of sadism. Because
Finally, by limiting the meta-analysis to offender/forensic samples, we ensured of shared method variance, we also expected that the association be-
that the data were collected from individuals who had seriously harmed others. tween psychopathy and sadism would be stronger when sadism was
Following a preliminary review of relevant studies, we decided to limit the measured by clinician-ratings compared to self-report. Additionally, we
inclusion criteria to PCL-R studies of forensic samples. examined whether type of sadism (i.e., sexual or other) moderated the


D. O'Connell and D.K. Marcus $JJUHVVLRQDQG9LROHQW%HKDYLRU  ²

association between psychopathy and sadism. effect sizes were calculated, and then the Z-values were converted back
to r for reporting. The effect sizes were weighted by sample size, and the
3. Method inverse variance weight used was the sample size minus three (Lipsey &
Wilson, 2001, p. 64).
3.1. Identification of studies When multiple measures of sadism were included in a study, the
effect sizes from each measure were averaged. However, if a study used
The first author conducted a search of PsycINFO using the term both a dichotomous (e.g., DSM-III Sadistic Personality Disorder diag-
“psychopathy” and variants (i.e., “psychopath,” and “psychopathic”) nosis) and a continuous measure (e.g., Sadistic Attitudes and Behavior
combined with the term “sadism” and variants (i.e., “sadist,” and “sa- Scale) of sadism, the effect size from the continuous measure was in-
distic”). These search terms initially yielded 694 results, and filters cluded in the meta-analysis to maximize variance and power.
were applied to only include empirical, peer-reviewed articles which All analyses were conducted using a maximum-likelihood random
narrowed the results to 92. Additional searches were conducted in effects model with Lipsey and Wilson's (2001) statistical program for
PubMed and ISI Web of Science. After cross-referencing the PubMed SPSS. A Q test of homogeneity was used to determine whether there
search results with the articles returned from the PsycInfo search, was significant heterogeneity among the effect sizes. I2 was calculated
PubMed yielded five additional articles (k = 97). All search results from to determine the percentage of variance attributable to heterogeneity
ISI Web of Science were accounted for in either the PsycINFO or across studies (Higgins, Thompson, Deeks, & Altman, 2003). I2 values
PubMed search. To be included in the meta-analysis, studies had to (a) range from 0 to 100. Values of 25%, 50%, and 75% have been proposed
provide codable statistics in the form of a correlation coefficient or the as anchor points for low, moderate, and high I2 values (Higgins et al.,
necessary information to calculate one; (b) measure psychopathy using 2003). The regression test for funnel plot asymmetry (Egger, Smith,
the PCL-R (Hare, 1991); (c) include a measure of sadism or a descrip- Schneider, & Minder, 1997), the trim and fill procedure (Duval &
tion of diagnostic criteria (e.g., DSM-III sadistic personality disorder Tweedie, 2000), and the leave-one-out analysis were conducted using
diagnosis); and (d) have researchers who have direct contact with the metafor package in R (Viechtbauer, 2010) to examine possible
subjects or access to criminal or psychological records.2 publication bias or outlier effects. The meta-analytic analogue to the
Sixteen of the 97 articles met the inclusion criteria and provided analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to test for the categorical
data for 19 independent samples. The 76 excluded references were moderators (Hedges, 1982; Lipsey & Wilson, 2001).
excluded because they (a) did not include details about how psycho-
pathy and sadism were measured (13); (b) were commentaries, theory 4. Results
papers, or review articles which did not report new data (44); (c)
measured Antisocial Personality Disorder, but not psychopathy (6); (d) 4.1. Primary analyses
had a sample overlapping with another study (k = 3); (d) used a psy-
chopathy measure other than the PCL-R (5); or (e) did not provide The mean effect size for PCL-R total score and sadism was r = 0.24,
usable data to calculate a correlation coefficient (10). See Fig. 1 for a which was statistically significant (k = 19; 95% CI = 0.16–0.34;
flowchart of study selection. Z = 5.34, p < .001). There was considerable heterogeneity among the
In cases where usable data were not provided, but all other inclu- studies Q (18) = 109.40, p < .001, I2 = 84.5, indicating that these
sion criteria were met, the author listed to receive correspondence was studies do not all derive from the same population. The funnel plot
contacted for further information. Out of 12 authors emailed to request indicated that most of the effect sizes fell within the expected dis-
data, two authors provided the location of usable data that had been tribution (Fig. 2), and the regression test for funnel plot asymmetry
overlooked during coding, one author provided a reference to a recently using the standard error of the observed values as a predictor variable
published article, four reported that they no longer had the data files, was not significant (z = 1.70, p = .08). Additionally, the trim and fill
and there was no response from the remaining six. The reference sec- procedure did not impute any additional studies to the funnel plot. The
tions of the included articles were then used to locate additional re- leave-one-out procedure indicated that no single study was skewing the
ferences to potentially include in the meta-analysis, which did not yield mean effect size, with rs ranging from 0.22 to 0.27, and all p va-
any additional usable additional samples/studies. Thus, the meta-ana- lues < .001.
lysis included 19 independent samples from 16 studies that included Eight studies reported the associations between the PCL-R factor
5161 participants. All studies included in the meta-analysis are listed in scores and sadism. The mean effect size for the association between
Table 1. PCL-R F1 and sadism was r = 0.25, which was statistically significant
(95% CI = 0.09–0.44; Z = 3.01, p < .01). There was moderate het-
erogeneity among the studies Q(7) = 15.44, p = .03, I2 = 54.7, in-
3.2. Procedure dicating that these studies do not all derive from the same population.
The funnel plot generated from the data indicated that all but one study
The studies were coded for region, sample size, percentage fell within the expected distribution (Fig. 3). The regression test for
Caucasian, IQ, age group, type of sadism (sexual, mixed sexual/violent, funnel plot asymmetry with standard error selected as the predictor was
and mixed sexual/personality), sadism measure used, and sadism not significant (z = 0.32, p = .74). The trim and fill procedure did not
measure method (clinician rating, self-report). All included studies used suggest imputing any additional studies. The leave-one-out analysis
exclusively male samples. When reported, separate effect sizes were indicated even when the study with the largest effect size was elimi-
calculated for the association between PCL-R F1 and sadism, and PCL-R nated, the average effect remained significant at p < .01, with rs
F2 and sadism. Any articles that did not report Pearson's product-mo- ranging from 0.16 to 0.29, and all p values ≤ .01.
ment correlation coefficients, but provided the requisite data to calcu- The mean effect size for the association between PCL-R Factor 2 and
late an effect size were converted using an online effect-size calculator sadism was r = 0.26, which was statistically significant (95%
(Wilson, 2001). To correct for bias, Pearson's rs were then converted CI = 0.08–0.46; Z = 2.82, p < .01). There was a moderate amount of
with Fisher's Zr transformation (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001, p.107), mean heterogeneity among the studies Q(7) = 18.52, p < .01, I2 = 62.2,
indicating that these studies do not all derive from the same population.
2
Despite applying filters to the search terms to only yield empirical, peer- Examination of the funnel plot indicated that all but two studies fell
reviewed articles, there were many articles returned that involved clinical within the expected distribution (Fig. 4). The regression test for funnel
opinions of celebrities, people in documentary films, fictional characters, and plot asymmetry was not significant (z = 0.65, p = .51). Additionally,
unclear specifications of level of contact with the subjects. the trim and fill analysis indicated there were no missing studies in the


D. O'Connell and D.K. Marcus $JJUHVVLRQDQG9LROHQW%HKDYLRU  ²

Records identified through searches of Records identified through other


PsycInfo, PubMed, and ISI Web of sources*
Science of the terms “psychopathy” and
variants combined with the term (1)
“sadism” and variants.

(699)

Records screened after limiting the results Full-text articles excluded, with reasons
to empirical, peer-reviewed articles.
- no usable data (10)
(97)
- no direct contact with those assessed or used
inadequate records (e.g., analyzing book
characters, using documentary films; 44)

- no specification of the diagnostic criteria (13)

- measured Antisocial Personality Disorder,


Articles included in the meta- but not psychopathy (6)
analysis -overlapping sample with another study (3)
(16; k = 19)** -used a measure other than the PCL-R (5)

Fig. 1. Flow chart of study selection. *In response to an email to clarify whether the sample of one study overlapped with another, the author provided a reference to
a recently published article that met the inclusion criteria. **3 studies provided data on two independent samples each, and were coded as two separate studies
thereby adding 3 more samples to the analyses.

funnel plot. Finally, re-running the analyses leaving out one study at a association between PCL-R scores and self-reported sadism did not
time indicated that even when the study with the largest effect size was differ significantly from the association between PCL-R scores and
eliminated, the average effect remained significant at p < .05 with rs clinician-ratings of sadism, QB(1) = 2.54, p = .11; QW(17) = 19.54,
ranging from 0.20 to 0.31, and all p values < .05. p = .29. The average effect size for the three studies of PCL-R total
scores and self-report measures of sadism was 0.38 (95%
CI = 0.19–0.62; Z = 3.71, p < .001), and the average effect size for
4.2. Moderation analyses the 16 studies of PCL-R total score and clinician-ratings of sadism was
0.21 (95% CI = 0.12–0.31; Z = 4.47, p < .001).
Measurement method used to assess sadism (i.e., self-report vs. Fifteen of the 19 samples assessing the association between PCL-R
clinician-rating), and type of sadism were tested as potential mod- total scores and sadism measured sexual sadism (k = 15). The other
erators of the relationship between PCL-R total scores and sadism. The four samples assessed mixed violent and sexual sadism (k = 2), and
minimum number of studies in each category for a variable to be in- personality/character sadism (k = 2). There were no significant dif-
cluded as a moderator was k = 3, which precluded moderator analyses ferences between the studies that specifically assessed sexual sadism
for PCL-R factor scores (i.e., of the studies that reported associations and the other four studies, QB(1) = 1.79, p = .18; QW(17) = 18.45,
with PCL-R factor scores, only two used self-report measures of sadism, p = .36. The average effect size for the 15 samples that assessed the
and only one study examined a form of sadism other than sexual association between psychopathy total scores and sexual sadism was
sadism). 0.22 (95% CI = 0.12–0.32; Z = 4.24, p < .001). The average effect
A random effects model analogous to the ANOVA (Hedges, 1982) size for the four studies that assessed the association between psycho-
was conducted to test whether sadism measurement method moderated pathy total scores and other types of sadism was 0.36 (95%
the association between psychopathy and sadism. More specifically, we CI = 0.17–0.58; Z = 3.57, p = .001).
investigated whether the variation among the effect sizes for psycho-
pathy total score and sadism was attributed to differences in how
sadism was measured (e.g., self-report vs. clinician-rating). The


D. O'Connell and D.K. Marcus $JJUHVVLRQDQG9LROHQW%HKDYLRU  ²

Table 1
Studies included in the meta-analysis.
Reference N Type of sadism Sadism measure method Effect (r)-Ptotal Effect (r)-PF1 Effect (r)-PF2

Barbaree et al. (1994) 23 S CR 0.35 0.20 0.38


Briken, Habermann, Kafka, Berner, and Hill (2006)a 56 S CR 0.59
Briken et al. (2006)b 29 S CR 0.41
Delescluse and Pham (2005) 76 M CR 0.04 0.09 −0.03
Eher et al. (2015) 992 S CR 0.15
Harenski, Thornton, Harenski, Decety, and Kiehl (2012) 15 S CR 0.12 0.07 0.19
Hill, Habermann, Berner, and Briken (2007) 166 S CR 0.20
Holt et al. (1999) 41 M CR 0.64 0.64 0.57
Langevin and Curnoe (2015)a 2414 S CR 0.00
Langevin and Curnoe (2015)b 62 S CR −0.04
Looman, Dickie, and Maillet (2008) 18 S CR 0.23 0.34 0.25
Mokros et al. (2011) 99 S CR 0.28
Nitschke, Istrefi, Osterheider, and Mokros (2012) 35 S CR 0.16
Porter et al. (2003) 38 S SR 0.35 0.34 0.20
Richards and Jackson (2011) 78 S CR 0.06 0.06 0.07
Robertson and Knight (2014)a 314 S SR 0.35
Robertson and Knight (2014)b 599 S CR 0.16
Shine and Hobson (1997) 46 P SR 0.47 0.28 0.50
Stupperich and Strack (2016) 60 P CR 0.30

Note. Type of Sadism: S = sexual, M = mixed group of more than one type, P = Personality. Sadism Measure Method: CR = clinician-rating, SR = self-report. Effect
(r) Ptotal = Pearson's product-moment correlation coefficients for the association between sadism and psychopathy total score. Effect (r) PF1 = Pearson's product-
moment correlation coefficients for the association between sadism and the interpersonal/affective component of psychopathy. Effect (r) PF2 = Pearson's product-
moment correlation coefficients for the association between sadism and the impulsivity/antisocial behavior component of psychopathy. Superscripts a and b denote
when two different samples were included in the same article.

Fig. 2. Funnel plot of effect sizes for the association between PCL-R total score Fig. 3. Funnel plot of effect sizes for the association between PCL-R Factor 1
and sadism. and sadism.

5. Discussion association between psychopathy and sadism. The medium effect sizes
found for the association between PCL-R total scores and sadism as well
Although several studies have indicated that psychopathy and as the PCL-R factor scores and sadism indicate that psychopathy and
sadism are separate, but related constructs (e.g., James & Proulx, 2014; sadism are related, but distinct constructs. Consequently, it may be
Kirsch & Becker, 2007; Porter, Campbell, Woodworth, & Birt, 2001) no fruitful in future research to examine whether psychopathy and sadism
prior meta-analyses have examined the strength of this association. The each make independent contributions to the prediction of antisocial or
primary aims of the meta-analysis were to (a) assess the association in aggressive behavior, or to the prediction of criminal recidivism. Re-
forensic samples between psychopathy as measured by the PCL-R and searchers should also examine potential interactions between sadism
various forms of sadism, and (b) determine how each factor of the PCL- and psychopathy (e.g., is sadism especially predictive of antisocial be-
R is associated with sadism. havior at high levels of psychopathy?).
The hypothesis that PCL-R total score, as well as Factor 1 and Factor Although the method of measuring sadism did not moderate the
2 would be associated with sadism was supported. These findings are association between psychopathy and sadism, only three studies used
consistent with previous research on the association between psycho- self-report measures, which limited the power of this analysis. Although
pathy and sadism. Additionally, the finding that both PCL-R Factor 1 not significantly different, the associations between the PCL-R and self-
and Factor 2 are associated with sadism supports the model proposed reported sadism (0.38) were nearly twice as large as the association
by Porter et al. (2001) that both interpersonal/affective components of between the PCL-R and clinician-rated sadism, which was contrary to
psychopathy and the antisocial behavior component contribute to the our hypothesis. Most of the clinician-rated studies diagnosed sadism


D. O'Connell and D.K. Marcus $JJUHVVLRQDQG9LROHQW%HKDYLRU  ²

well as the association between subcomponents of psychopathy and


sadism. The present study was, however, limited in several ways. First,
the small amount of empirical research available on PCL-R psychopathy
and sadism in forensic populations limited the meta-analysis to 19
samples. Additionally, the association between PCL-R psychopathy and
sadism has exclusively been studied in male, forensic samples, and most
studies have limited their definitions of sadism to clinician-rated
sexually sadistic behaviors. Further complicating the study of the as-
sociation between components of psychopathy and sadism has been the
low reporting rate of factor scores for the PCL-R. These factors resulted
in the moderator analyses being underpowered, and precluded con-
ducting moderator analyses with PCL-R factor scores and sadism.
Future research on the association between psychopathy and sadism in
offender samples should be conducted with women, and include
broader conceptualizations of sadism and as well as different models of
psychopathy (e.g., the triarchic model; Patrick, Fowles, & Krueger,
2009) to further elucidate which components of psychopathy are as-
sociated with sadism.
In summary, both the interpersonal/affective component of psy-
Fig. 4. Funnel plot of effect sizes for the association between PCL-R Factor 2
chopathy (PCL-R Factor 1) and the antisocial behavior component of
and sadism. psychopathy (PCL-R Factor 2) are similarly associated with sadism in
forensic samples. Thus, sadism among forensic populations may involve
both a lack of empathy for others and willingness to exploit others for
based on coding case files for characteristics of criminal acts to indicate
one's pleasure or gain (Factor 1), as well as a pattern of impulsive rule-
sadistic behavior (e.g., evidence of torture, recording crimes, indicators
breaking behaviors (Factor 2). Although the moderator analyses were
of planning for the crime, etc.), or had been classified by previous
null, they were likely underpowered due to the lack of published studies
clinicians as meeting diagnostic criteria for DSM Sexual Sadism based
measuring non-sexual forms of sadism or self-reported sadism. The re-
on offense characteristics (e.g., Briken et al., 2006; Eher et al., 2015;
sults of the present study hint that the overall association between
Mokros et al., 2011; Nitschke et al., 2012). When sadism was assessed
psychopathy and sadism may be stronger when sadism is measured via
via a clinical interview (e.g., Richards & Jackson, 2011), offenders were
self-report, or when sadism is assessed more broadly than a specific
only asked about behaviors that were later classified by the researchers
focus on sexual sadism (i.e., when a broad spectrum of sadistic
as sadistic. In contrast, self-report measures of sadism may also assess
thoughts, fantasies, and behaviors are assessed).
sadistic thoughts or fantasies in addition to behaviors. For example,
Robertson and Knight (2014) used the self-report Multidimensional
Declarations of interest
Inventory of Development, Sex, and Aggression (MIDSA; Knight &
Cerce, 1999) in one of the samples in their study. The MIDSA is a
None.
computerized inventory that assesses for both sexually sadistic beha-
viors and fantasies. Interestingly, Robertson and Knight (2014) found
References3
that the association between PCL-R total scores and MIDSA scores in the
first sample was roughly twice as large as the association between PCL-
American Psychiatric Association (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of disorders (3rd
R total scores and clinician-ratings of sexually sadistic behaviors in their ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Pub.
second sample. It is possible that offenders feel more comfortable dis- American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of disorders (4th
ed. – text revision). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Pub.
closing sadistic thoughts, fantasies, and behaviors in a self-report
*Barbaree, H. E., Seto, M. C., Serin, R. C., Amos, N. L., & Preston, D. L. (1994).
format, particularly if the participants are told that their answers will Comparisons between sexual and nonsexual rapist subtypes sexual arousal to rape,
be confidential or anonymous. Still, because this moderator analysis offense precursors, and offense characteristics. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 21,
was not significant, these speculations should be treated tentatively 95–114.
Baumeister, R. F., & Campbell, W. K. (1999). The intrinsic appeal of evil: Sadism, sen-
until there are additional studies that assess sadism using self-report sational thrills, and threatened egotism. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3,
measures in forensic samples. 210–221.
The moderator analysis for type of sadism assessed was not sig- *Briken, P., Habermann, N., Kafka, M. P., Berner, W., & Hill, A. (2006). The paraphilia-
related disorders: An investigation of the relevance of the concept in sexual mur-
nificant; however, a large majority of the samples (15 out of 18) only derers. Journal of Forensic Sciences, 51, 683–688.
assessed sexual sadism, limiting the power of this analysis. Of the four *Delescluse, C., & Pham, T. H. (2005). Évaluation du trouble de la personnalité sadique en
studies that did not exclusively assess sexual sadism, the effect sizes relation avec la psychopathie auprès de patients internés en hôpital psychiatrique
sécuritaire. L'Encéphale, 31, 683–691.
ranged from almost no association (Delescluse & Pham, 2005) to a Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot–based method of
medium effect size (Hill et al., 2007; Stupperich & Strack, 2016) to a testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics, 56, 455–463.
large effect size (Holt et al., 1999). However, the Delescluse and Pham Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis de-
tected by a simple, graphical test. British Medical Journal, 315, 629–634.
(2005) study, which yielded the smallest effect, also required that in
*Eher, R., Schilling, F., Hansmann, B., Pumberger, T., Nitschke, J., Habermeyer, E., &
order for participants to be classified as sadistic, they had to meet all Mokros, A. (2015). Sadism and violent reoffending in sexual offenders. Sexual Abuse:
eight DSM-II-R criteria for Sadistic Personality Disorder, which is an A Journal of Research and Treatment, 28, 46–72.
Firestone, P., Bradford, J. M., Greenberg, D. M., Larose, M. R., & Curry, S. (1998).
unrealistically stringent standard that may have attenuated a possible
Homicidal and nonhomicidal child molesters: Psychological, phallometric, and
association between psychopathy and sadism. Additionally, both stu- criminal features. Sexual Abuse: A Journal of Research and Treatment, 10(4), 305–323.
dies that assessed multiple forms of sadism included sexual sadism Hare, R. D. (1970). Psychopathy: Theory and research. New York: Wiley.
(Delescluse & Pham, 2005; Holt et al., 1999). Therefore, it remains Hare, R. D. (1991). The Hare psychopathy checklist – Revised. Toronto, ON, Canada: Multi-
Health Systems.
unclear whether different types of sadism vary in how they are related
to psychopathy.
The meta-analysis had several strengths. It was the first meta-ana- 3
References marked with an asterisk indicate references included in the
lysis to examine the association between psychopathy and sadism, as
meta-analysis.


D. O'Connell and D.K. Marcus $JJUHVVLRQDQG9LROHQW%HKDYLRU  ²

Hare, R. D., & Neumann, C. S. (2008). Psychopathy as a clinical and empirical construct. Journal of Sexual Aggression, 14, 267–279.
Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 217–246. Loranger, A. (1988). Personality Disorder Examination (PDE). Yonkers, NY: DV
*Harenski, C. L., Thornton, D. M., Harenski, K. A., Decety, J., & Kiehl, K. A. (2012). Communications.
Increased frontotemporal activation during pain observation in sexual sadism: Meloy, J. R. (2000). The nature and dynamics of sexual homicide: An integrative review.
Preliminary findings. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69, 283–292. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 5, 1–22.
Hedges, L. V. (1982). Fitting categorical models to effect sizes from a series of experi- Millon, T. (1987). Millon clinical multiaxial inventory-II manual. Minneapolis, MN: National
ments. Journal of Educational Statistics, 7, 119–137. Computer Systems.
Higgins, J. P., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring incon- *Mokros, A., Osterheider, M., Hucker, S. J., & Nitschke, J. (2011). Psychopathy and
sistency in meta-analyses. British Medical Journal, 327, 557–560. sexual sadism. Law and Human Behavior, 35, 188–199.
*Hill, A., Habermann, N., Berner, W., & Briken, P. (2007). Psychiatric disorders in single *Nitschke, J., Istrefi, S., Osterheider, M., & Mokros, A. (2012). Empathy in sexually sa-
and multiple sexual murderers. Psychopathology, 40, 22–28. distic offenders: An experimental comparison with non-sadistic sexual offenders.
*Holt, S. E., Meloy, J. R., & Strack, S. (1999). Sadism and psychopathy in violent and International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 35, 165–167.
sexually violent offenders. The Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Patrick, C. J., Fowles, D. C., & Krueger, R. F. (2009). Triarchic conceptualization of
Law, 27, 23–32. psychopathy: Developmental origins of disinhibition, boldness, and meanness.
James, J., & Proulx, J. (2014). A psychological and developmental profile of sexual Development and Psychopathology, 21, 913–938.
murderers: A systematic review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 19, 592–607. Paulhus, D. L. (2014). Toward a taxonomy of dark personalities. Current Directions in
Kirsch, L. G., & Becker, J. V. (2007). Emotional deficits in psychopathy and sexual sadism: Psychological Science, 23, 421–426.
Implications for violent and sadistic behavior. Clinical Psychology Review, 27, Porter, S., Campbell, M. A., Woodworth, M., & Birt, A. R. (2001). A new psychological
904–922. conceptualization of the sexual psychopath. Advances in Psychology Research, 7,
Knight, R. A., & Cerce, D. D. (1999). Validation and revision of the multidimensional 21–36.
assessment of sex and aggression. Psychologica Belgica, 39, 135–161. *Porter, S., Woodworth, M., Earle, J., Drugge, J., & Boer, D. (2003). Characteristics of
Krueger, R. B. (2010). The DSM diagnostic criteria for sexual sadism. Archives of Sexual sexual homicides committed by psychopathic and nonpsychopathic offenders. Law
Behavior, 39(2), 325–345. and Human Behavior, 27, 459–470.
Langevin, R. (2003). A study of the psychosexual characteristics of sex killers: Can we *Richards, H., & Jackson, R. L. (2011). Behavioral discriminators of sexual sadism and
identify them before it is too late? International Journal of Offender Therapy and paraphilia nonconsent in a sample of civilly committed sexual offenders. International
Comparative Criminology, 47, 366–382. Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 55, 207–227.
*Langevin, R., & Curnoe, S. (2015). Are dangerous offenders different from other offen- *Robertson, C. A., & Knight, R. A. (2014). Relating sexual sadism and psychopathy to one
ders? A clinical profile. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative another, non-sexual violence, and sexual crime behaviors. Aggressive Behavior, 40,
Criminology, 58, 780–801. 12–23.
Levenson, J. S. (2004). Reliability of sexually violent predator civil commitment criteria. *Shine, J., & Hobson, J. (1997). Construct validity of the Hare Psychopathy Checklist,
Law and Human Behavior, 28, 357–369. revised, on a UK prison population. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry, 8, 546–561.
Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Vol. 49. Thousand Oaks, CA: Stupperich, A., & Strack, M. (2016). Among a German sample of forensic patients, pre-
Sage Publications. vious animal abuse mediates between psychopathy and sadistic actions. Journal of
Lloyd, C. D., Clark, H. J., & Forth, A. E. (2010). Psychopathy, expert testimony, and Forensic Sciences, 61, 699–705.
indeterminate sentences: Exploring the relationship between Psychopathy Checklist- Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package.
Revised testimony and trial outcome in Canada. Legal and Criminological Psychology, Journal of Statistical Software, 36, 1–48.
15, 323–339. Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis effect size calculator.
*Looman, J., Dickie, I., & Maillet, G. (2008). Sexual arousal among rapist subtypes.



You might also like