Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/51979871

Relationship Between Postural Control and Fine Motor Skills in Preterm


Infants at 6 and 12 Months Adjusted Age

Article  in  The American journal of occupational therapy.: official publication of the American Occupational Therapy Association · November 2011
DOI: 10.5014/ajot.2011.001503 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

16 213

4 authors:

Tien-Ni Wang Tsu-Hsin Howe


National Taiwan University New York University
29 PUBLICATIONS   475 CITATIONS    42 PUBLICATIONS   568 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Jim Hinojosa Sharon L. Weinberg


New York University New York University
102 PUBLICATIONS   1,421 CITATIONS    72 PUBLICATIONS   934 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

STATA Textbook View project

SPSS Textbook View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Sharon L. Weinberg on 01 December 2020.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Relationship Between Postural Control and Fine Motor
Skills in Preterm Infants at 6 and 12 Months
Adjusted Age

Tien-Ni Wang, Tsu-Hsin Howe, Jim Hinojosa, Sharon L. Weinberg

KEY WORDS We examined the relationship between postural control and fine motor skills of preterm infants at 6 and 12 mo
 child development adjusted age. The Alberta Infant Motor Scale was used to measure postural control, and the Peabody De-
velopmental Motor Scales II was used to measure fine motor skills. The data analyzed were taken from 105
 infant
medical records from a preterm infant follow-up clinic at an urban academic medical center in south Taiwan.
 infant, premature Using multiple regression analyses, we found that the development of postural control is related to the
 motor skills development of fine motor skills, especially in the group of preterm infants with delayed postural control.
 postural balance This finding supports the theoretical assumption of proximal–distal development used by many occupational
therapists to guide intervention. Further research is suggested to corroborate findings.

Wang, T.-N., Howe, T.-H., Hinojosa, J., & Weinberg, S. L. (2011). Relationship between postural control and fine motor
skills in preterm infants at 6 and 12 months adjusted age. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 65, 695–
701. doi: 10.5014/ajot.2011.001503

Tien-Ni Wang, PhD, OT, is Assistant Professor, School


of Occupational Therapy, College of Medicine, National
Taiwan University, 17 Syujhou Road 4F, Taipei City 100,
A dvances in medical technology have contributed to a significant increase in
the survival rates of preterm infants, who are at high risk for long-term
neurological, cognitive, and behavioral complications (Aylward, 2005). Among
Taiwan, Republic of China; tnwang@ntu.edu.tw
these high-risk areas, motor development is reported to be more affected by
Tsu-Hsin Howe, PhD, OTR, is Assistant Professor, premature birth than are other developmental domains (Lorenz, Wooliever,
Steinhardt School of Culture, Education, and Human
Jetton, & Paneth, 1998) and is therefore an important area to monitor in
Development, New York University, New York.
preterm infants.
Jim Hinojosa, PhD, OT, FAOTA, is Professor, The relation between postural control and fine motor skills is the primary
Steinhardt School of Culture, Education, and Human focus of therapy when clinicians are working on young children’s motor ac-
Development, New York University, New York.
quisition. Clinicians often base their therapeutic interventions on the proximal–
Sharon L. Weinberg, PhD, is Professor, Steinhardt distal principle of normal motor development (Case-Smith, Fisher, & Bauer,
School of Culture, Education, and Human Development, 1989; Rosenblum & Josman, 2003). This principle suggests that motor
New York University, New York.
development initially occurs proximally (i.e., in the head and trunk) and
thereafter progresses outwardly toward the distal parts of the body (i.e., the
hands and feet; Case-Smith et al., 1989; Loria, 1980; Skinner, 1979). Postural
control, the ability to maintain the center body mass or a body part over a stable
or moving base of support (Massion, 1998; Shumway-Cook & Woollacott,
1995), is an example of a proximal motor function. Despite widespread ac-
ceptance of the proximal–distal principle, the evidence supporting this as-
sumption is limited (Case-Smith et al., 1989).
Both clinical and laboratory studies have examined the relationship between
proximal motor function (e.g., postural control) and distal motor function (e.g.,
fine motor skills) in the typically developing population at different age ranges
(Case-Smith et al., 1989; Fallang, Saugstad, & Hadders-Algra, 2000; Rochat,
1992; Rosenblum & Josman, 2003; Thelen & Spencer, 1998). These studies
collectively do not lend strong support for the proximal–distal principle,

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 695


however. Fallang and colleagues (2000) found a strong The first purpose of our study was to examine the
association between the kinematics of reaching activities general relationship between postural control and fine
as an indication of fine motor skills and postural control motor skills in preterm infants at 6 and 12 mo adjusted
in a laboratory setting using healthy infants ages 4 and 6 age. In addition, we examined the relationship between
mo. Yet, in another sample of healthy infants ages 2–6 postural control and fine motor skills, comparing groups
mo, Case-Smith et al. (1989) found a moderately low, with different adjusted ages and groups with and without
albeit statistically significant, correlation between postural motor delays separately.
control and fine motor skills (rs 5 .31–.35, ps < .05) in
a clinical setting. In still another study of older children, Method
Rosenblum and Josman (2003) investigated the re-
lationship between postural control and fine motor skills Research Design
in 47 typically developing children at ages 5–6 yr and We used a cross-sectional design (Portney & Watkins,
found low to moderate, but statistically significant, cor- 2009) to investigate the differences in postural control
relations between postural control and fine manual dex- and fine motor skills between preterm and full-term in-
terity (rs 5 2.31 to 2.47, ps < .05). The findings of fants at the adjusted ages of 6 and 12 mo. The institutional
these studies have low correlations that provide limited review board of the National Cheng Kung University
information about the relationship between postural Hospital and New York University Human Subjects Re-
control and fine motor skills. view Board reviewed and approved the study procedures.
The relationship between postural control and fine
motor skills also has been investigated in the preterm Participants
infant population (de Groot, 2000; Fallang, Saugstad, &
Hadders-Algra, 2003; Plantinga, Perdock, & de Groot, National Cheng Kung University Hospital in Taiwan
1997; Samsom & de Groot, 2000); in these studies, provided data from infants’ 6- and 12-mo preemie follow-
however, only a single component of postural control was up visits between August 2008 and April 2009. All the
investigated. Past studies of postural control in premature medical records reviewed met the following criteria: (1) the
infants (de Vries & de Groot, 2002; Fallang et al., 2003; preterm infant’s birth weight was £1,500 g; (2) the infant’s
Plantinga et al., 1997) examined only specific positions in gestational age was <37 wk; (3) the infant had no marked
the realm of possible postural control positions. For ex- neurological impairments, such as Grade III or IV in-
ample, when studying the relationship between postural traventricular hemorrhage or periventricular leukomalacia;
control and fine motor skills, Plantinga et al. (1997) and (4) the infant had no congenital impairment.
examined only the sitting position, de Vries and de Groot Tien-Ni Wang reviewed 133 medical records from the
(2002) examined only trunk rotation, and Fallang et al. preemie follow-up program recorded at National Cheng
(2003) focused only on reaching in the supine position. Kung University Hospital in Taiwan. The preemie follow-
Because these researchers measured postural control dif- up team evaluated 71 boys and 62 girls between August
ferently, using different dimensions of postural control as 2008 and April 2009. On the basis of the inclusion criteria,
indicators, drawing comparisons across studies and arriv- 12 preterm infants (8 boys and 4 girls) who had in-
ing at a conclusion about the relationship between postural traventricular hemorrhage, Grade III and IV, or periven-
control, in general, and fine motor skills is difficult. tricular leukomalacia were excluded. Because the data
In addition, previous studies of the relationship be- collection period spanned longer than 6 mo, 16 preterm
tween postural control and fine motor skills have been infants (9 boys and 7 girls) were tested at both 6 and 12 mo
limited in terms of the variety of preterm populations adjusted age. One of the two sets of data collected on these
studied (Plantinga et al., 1997; Samsom & de Groot, 16 preterm infants was eliminated to be consistent with
2000). The results of these studies cannot be generalized to a cross-sectional design. Moreover, one set of data collected
other preterm infant populations. Samsom and de Groot at 6 mo adjusted age was eliminated to balance the sample
(2000), for example, investigated the relationship between sizes for the two age groups. The final sample consisted of
postural control and fine motor skills in high-risk preterm 105 participants: 54 preterm infants at 6 mo adjusted age
infants. They defined high-risk infants as those <32 wk and 51 preterm infants at 12 mo adjusted age.
gestational age, birth weight <1,500 g, and with at least
one medical complication. They did not include healthy Instruments
preterm infants without medical complications or preterm We used two standardized assessments. Postural control
infants between 33 and 36 wk gestational age. was measured with the Alberta Infant Motor Scale (AIMS;

696 November/December 2011, Volume 65, Number 6


Piper & Darrah, 1994), and fine motor skills were for PDMS–V for a group of infants between ages 2
measured by the Fine Motor scale of the Peabody and 17 mo were .90–.95. The interrater reliability (r 5
Developmental Motor Scales, 2nd edition (PDMS–2; .98) and test–retest reliability (ICC 5 .97) of the
Folio & Fewell, 2000). The PDMS–2 measures in- PDMS–2 are both high (Folio & Fewell, 2000). The
cluded the Composite Fine Motor score and the scores concurrent validities of the Fine Motor Skill subscale of
of two subscales: Grasping and Visual–Motor Integration. the Bayley Scales of Infant Development II (Bayley,
Alberta Infant Motor Scale. In this study, we used the 1969) and PDMS–2 in infants at 12 mo were reported
AIMS (Piper & Darrah, 1994), a standardized norm- to be between .78 and .90 (Provost et al., 2004). In
referenced observation- and performance-based test, to this study, we used the standard score of the PDMS–2
measure postural control. The AIMS evaluates the sponta- with a mean (M) 5 10 and a standard deviation (SD) 5
neous movement repertoires of infants from birth through 3 to represent the performance of preterm infants’ fine
independent walking. It consists of 58 items assessing all motor skills.
components of postural control, including antigravity
movements, body alignment, trunk rotation, and weight Procedures
shifting in different positions (i.e., supine, prone, sitting,
Infants with birth weight £1,500 g from six different
standing). The total raw score (sum of the positional item
hospitals in the southern region of Taiwan are automat-
scores) ranges from 0 to 58. The higher the score is, the
ically enrolled in the centralized preterm follow-up pro-
better the infant’s postural control.
gram at National Cheng Kung University Hospital. A
Interrater reliability of the AIMS is reported to be
follow-up team evaluates all premature infants at the
higher than .96, and test–retest reliability ranges from .86
adjusted ages of 6 and 12 mo with a series of neuro-
to .99 at different ages (Liao & Campbell, 2004). Good
logical, cognitive, and motor development tests. The
to excellent concurrent validity was found with other
evaluation team administers the AIMS and PDMS–2
standardized motor evaluations, including the PDMS–2
as part of the motor tests. Tien-Ni Wang, who was not
and the Bayley Scales of Infant Development I and II
a member of the evaluation team, retrieved the infants’
(rs 5 .74–.89; Almeida, Dutra, Mello, Reis, & Martins,
demographic information, birth history, and results
2008; Piper & Darrah, 1994). The AIMS also demon-
from the motor skills assessments from the existing
strated good intrarater and interrater reliabilities (intra-
medical records of the follow-up clinics. All members of
class correlation coefficients [ICCs] 5 .97–.99) as well as
the evaluation team were blind to the purpose of this
excellent concurrent validity with the Bayley Scales of
study.
Infant Development II (.78 at 6 mo and .90 at 12 mo)
when used in the Taiwanese preterm population (Jeng,
Yau, Chen, & Hsiao, 2000). Data Analysis
Peabody Developmental Motor Scales–2. The PDMS–2 We analyzed the data using SPSS-PC for Windows
is a standardized and norm-referenced test that consists of Release 17.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago). Initially, we used
a Gross Motor and a Fine Motor scale, each of which is descriptive statistics to examine the tenability of the
divided into skill categories. In this study, we used the underlying assumptions of the tests of inference used
Fine Motor scale to measure fine motor skills. The Fine to answer the questions addressed. The underlying as-
Motor scale consists of test items measuring (1) con- sumptions for these tests, including linearity, normality,
trolled use of the fingers and hands under the general and homoscedasticity, were found to be tenable. Ac-
heading of Grasping (PDMS–G) and (2) Visual–Motor cordingly, we conducted three regression analyses, one
Integration (PDMS–V) using test items such as reaching on each of the three Fine Motor subscales of the PDMS–2
for and grasping an object, building with blocks, and as dependent measures variables: the PDMS–G, the
copying designs. Test items reflect typical motor tasks PDMS–V, and the Composite Fine Motor score (PDMS–
for each age, including drawing and manual dexterity F). Finally, we investigated the relationship between
activities. postural control and fine motor skills for two groups of
The test manual reported good reliability and validity, preterm infants, those with and without motor delays at
as have other studies (Folio & Fewell, 2000; Provost et al., 6 and 12 mo adjusted age. In this study, we identified
2004). Test–retest reliability values for the total motor preterm infants as being delayed in postural control if
quotient for a group of infants between ages 2 and 17 mo their AIMS score was lower than the fifth percentile
were .89–.96, those for PDMS–G for a group of infants ranking of the norm in their respective age group (Campos
between ages 2 and 17 mo were .82–.91, and those et al., 2006; Piper & Darrah, 1994).

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 697


Results Table 1. Correlation Matrix Between All Pairs of Variables
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Demographic Characteristics 1. BW —
The final sample consisted of 105 preterm infants (54 boys, 2. GA .578** —
3. Gender .155 2.067 —
51 girls). A total of 54 infants were at 6 mo adjusted age,
4. AIMS .058 2.088 2.166 —
and 51 were at 12 mo adjusted age. Birth weight of the 105 5. PDMS–G .268** .146 2.048 .431** —
participants ranged from 720 g to 1,496 g (M 5 1,178.50, 6. PDMS–V .200* .139 2.089 .355** .653** —
SD 5215.47), and gestational age ranged from 24 wk to 7. PDMS–F .265** .157 2.069 .440** .950** .856** —
36 wk (M 5 29.67, SD 5 2.57). We found no significant Note. N 5 105. — 5 not applicable; AIMS 5 Alberta Infant Motor Scale;
group differences between the 6- and 12-mo adjusted age BW 5 birth weight; GA 5 gestational age; PDMS 5 Peabody Developmental
Motor Scale–2; PDMS–F 5 PDMS Composite Fine Motor score; PDMS–G 5
groups in terms of birth weight (t 5 20.376, df 5 103, PDMS Grasping subscale; PDMS–V 5 PDMS Visual–Motor Integration
p 5 .708), gestational age (t 5 1.163, df 5 103, p 5 subscale.
*p 5 .05. **p 5 .01.
.247), or gender (x2 5 1.591, df 5 2, N 5 105, p 5
.244). Relationship Between Postural Control and
Fine Motor Skills After Controlling for
Postural Control and Fine Motor Skills in Demographic Variables
Preterm Infants
Because birth weight, gender, gestational age, and adjusted
To make scores comparable between different ages and age are known to correlate with fine motor skills, we
tests, we transformed all AIMS and PDMS–2 raw scores examined the relationship between postural control and
into standard scores. We used the standard scores printed fine motor skills after controlling for these demographics.
in the AIMS test manual, which are z scores (M 5 0, In this way, we could examine the unique contribution of
SD 5 1) and contingent on age (Piper & Darrah, 1994). postural control on fine motor skills over and above these
The z scores are based on a normative group of full-term demographic variables. The particular set of dependent
infants, which explains why the M for the AIMS has variables included the PDMS–G, the PDMS–V, and the
a negative value for this preterm population (M 5 20.56, PDMS–F, and the particular set of independent variables
SD 5 1.08). Fine motor skills included the PDMS–G, included postural control (as measured by the AIMS) and
PDMS–V, and PDMS–F standard scores with M 5 10 the demographic control variables gender, gestational age,
and SD 5 3. As with the AIMS, the standard scores of the birth weight, and adjusted age.
PDMS–G and PDMS–V are obtained from the PDMS–2 The findings showed that after accounting for the
test manual and are contingent on age (Folio & Fewell, effects of the control variables, postural control contributes
2000). The PDMS–F standard score is a summary value to fine motor skills as measured by the PDMS–F, PDMS–
of the PDMS–G and PDMS–V standard scores. These G, and PDMS–V (Table 2). The R2 values in the final
standard scores are based on a normative group of full- model of equations indicate that after controlling for
term infants, which again explains why the means for the gender, birth weight, gestational age, and adjusted age,
PDMS–G (M 5 9.44, SD 51.47) and PDMS–V (M 5 postural control uniquely explained 25.6% of the vari-
9.74, SD 5 0.89) are all less than the full-term mean ance in fine motor skills, 24.8% of the variance in
of 10. grasping fine motor skills, and 16.7% of the variance in
Table 1 presents the matrix of correlations between visual–motor integration skills.
all pairs of variables, including demographic variables
(i.e., birth weight, gestational age, and gender), pos- Table 2. Multiple Linear Regression Analyses Testing the Effects
tural control as measured by the AIMS, and fine motor of Postural Control on Fine Motor Skills in Preterm Infants
skills as measured by the PDMS–G, PDMS–V, and Control Variable Predictor
PDMS–F. On the basis of the correlational analyses, we Equation Intercept GA BW Gender AIMS F(4, 104) R2
found statistically significant associations between the
1. PDMS–F 15.414 .066 .002 2.097 .867** 8.606** .256**
fine motor skills variables and postural control (rs 5 2. PDMS–G 7.633 .036 .001 2.015 .580** 8.223** .248**
.355–.440, p < .05) when the effect of age was con- 3. PDMS–V 8.381 .030 .001 2.082 .287** 5.007** .167**
trolled. These results indicate that fine motor skills in Note. N 5 105. AIMS 5 Alberta Infant Motor Scale; BW 5 birth weight; GA 5
terms of grasping, visual–motor integration, and com- gestational age; PDMS 5 Peabody Developmental Motor Scale–2; PDMS–F 5
PDMS Composite Fine Motor score; PDMS–G 5 PDMS Grasping subscale;
posite scores of fine motor skills are associated with PDMS–V 5 PDMS Visual–Motor Integration subscale.
postural control. *p 5 .05. **p 5 .01.

698 November/December 2011, Volume 65, Number 6


Relationship Between Postural Control and Fine to have a stronger association between postural control
Motor Skills in Different Age and Motor Subgroups and fine motor skills than those at 12 mo adjusted age
We further examined the relationship between postural (r 5 .415). Moreover, we found a strong, significant
control and fine motor skills at two different adjusted ages: relationship between postural control and fine motor
6 mo (n 5 54) and 12 mo (n 5 51); we also looked at the skills in preterm infants with delayed postural control
relationship for those with delayed postural control (n 5 (r 5 .671, p 5 .017), but not in those without delayed
12) and those without delayed postural control (n 5 93). postural control (r 5 .139, p 5 .185).
With respect to the demographics, we found no signifi- The results of this study demonstrated a significant
cant differences in birth weight (p > .05) and gestational association between postural control and fine motor skills.
age (p > .05) between the groups of infants with and In this study, a higher percentage of variance in the fine
without delayed postural control or between the groups motor scores was explained by the scores of postural
of infants at 6 and 12 mo adjusted age (see Table 3). control (16.7%–25.6%) than was explained in Case-
Table 3 shows the correlation between postural Smith et al.’s (1989) study (12%), which was based on
control as measured by the AIMS and fine motor skills as the typically developing population. These different
measured by the PDMS–F in the different subgroups findings are likely the result of the different populations
defined by adjusted age and status of postural control. studied. This study’s results indicate that postural control
Although the relationship between postural control and and fine motor skills are more strongly associated in
fine motor skills is stronger in preterm infants at 6 mo preterm infants than in typically developing infants.
adjusted age than in those at 12 mo adjusted age, the When we examined preterm infants in different age
difference in the correlations is not large (.503 vs. .415). groups, the study results indicated that descriptively
When examining the correlation between postural con- speaking, preterm infants at 6 mo adjusted age have
trol and fine motor skills in the groups with and without a stronger association between postural control and fine
delayed postural control, we found that infants without motor skills (r 5 .503) than those at 12 mo adjusted age
delayed postural control did not have a significant asso- (r 5 .415). Van der Fits, Otten, Klip, Van Eykern, and
ciation between postural control (AIMS) and fine motor Hadders-Algra (1999) offered an explanation of why the
skills (PDMS–F; r 5 .139, p 5 .185), whereas those with relationship between postural control and fine motor
delayed postural control did (r 5 .671, p 5 .017). skills decreases with age. Van der Fits and his coauthors
investigated the developmental changes in postural con-
trol accompanying reaching movements in healthy in-
Discussion fants from 6 to 18 mo old. Their findings indicated that
The findings of this study support postural control’s as- once an infant is successful in reaching, postural control
sociation with fine motor skills in preterm infants. The and reaching develop relatively independently.
relationships between postural control and all fine motor A unique aspect of this study was our examination of
skills variables were low to moderately significantly pos- postural control as a whole. The results of this study
itive (rs 5 .355–.440, p < .05). Analyses demonstrated expand the findings of previous studies that focused on
that preterm infants at 6 mo adjusted age (r 5 .503) tend only a single component or position (de Vries & de
Groot, 2002; Fallang et al., 2003; Plantinga et al.,
1997). These findings provide further evidence to sup-
Table 3. Birth Weight, Gestational Age, and Relationship port the relationship between general postural control
Between Postural Control and Fine Motor Skills in Preterm
Infants in Different Subgroups
and fine motor skills, including all of its components in
different positions. This result is especially important for
Birth Weight Gestational Age Relationship
infant populations, because they develop their fine mo-
Group M (SD) M (SD) Pearson r n
tor skills in different positions using various postural
Infants with DPC 1,197.6 (210.0) 30.4 (3.3) .671* 12
components.
Infants without DPC 1,176.0 (217.1) 29.6 (2.5) .139 93
6 mo AA 1,202.2 (210.8) 29.6 (2.2) .503** 54
Moreover, we included a wider population of preterm
12 mo AA 1,153.4 (219.6) 29.8 (2.9) .415** 51 infants than did prior studies. This study’s population was
Total 1,178.5 (215.5) 29.7 (2.6) .440** 105 preterm infants born between 32 and 36 wk gestational
Note. Postural control was measured by the Alberta Infant Motor Scale, and age and free of medical complications. During the past 2
fine motor skills were measured by the composite fine motor score of the decades, the number of preterm infants with younger
Peabody Developmental Motor Scale–2. AA 5 adjusted age; DPC 5 delayed
postural control; M 5 mean; SD 5 standard deviation.
gestational age and lower birth weight has increased
*p 5 .05. **p 5 .01. dramatically; the results add to our theoretical knowledge

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 699


of the general relationship between postural control and Msall, 2007), the performance of postural control and
fine motor skills in this population. fine motor skills and the relationship between them is
worth investigating in a wider age range. Moreover, in-
Clinical Implications vestigating the developmental motor trajectory of preterm
Infants’ motor development is a major area of focus for infants using a longitudinal design is needed to verify our
therapists working with children who have developmental findings. Retrospective data collection also has some in-
disabilities. To fulfill the need for practical applications, herent limitations, such as data integrity and the issue of
we further analyzed the relationship between postural quality. For example, in our study, we did not establish
control and fine motor skills. We compared the differ- interrater reliability between two occupational therapists
ences between two groups: (1) a group of preterm infants on the team before administering AIMS and PDMS–2.
without delayed postural control and (2) a group of
preterm infants with delayed postural control. The results Conclusion
indicated that the relationship between postural control
We investigated the relationship between postural control
and fine motor skills is strong and significant for preterm
and fine motor skills in preterm infants at 6 and 12 mo
infants with delayed postural control (r 5 .671, p 5
adjusted age. We found that the development of postural
.017), although it is not significant for preterm infants
control is related to the development of fine motor skills,
without delayed postural control. These findings further
especially in the group of preterm infants with delayed
echoed Van der Fits and colleagues’ (1999) conclusion
postural control. Understanding that the relationship
that postural control and fine motor skills are two in-
between postural control and fine motor skills in infants
terdependent systems when the motor system is not
with delayed postural control is strong might offer insight
mature or is delayed.
into helpful intervention strategies for preterm infants. s
On the basis of our findings, we argue that the
proximal–distal principle might be appropriately applied
to infants with motor delay. These results, however, are Acknowledgments
different from Case-Smith’s (1989) study, in which she This article is based on research conducted by Tien-Ni
found a low but significant distal–proximal relationship Wang in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the
in typically developing infants. Case-Smith suggested that Doctor of Philosophy in Occupational Therapy degree at
the proximal–distal principle may not be a valid postulate the Steinhardt School of Culture, Education, and Human
on which to design treatment programs for infants. The Development, New York University. We thank the staffs
different population could also explain this conflict with of the National Cheng Kung University Hospital preemie
our own findings. follow-up clinic and the Premature Baby Foundation of
The results of this study should help therapists un- Taiwan for their assistance in data collection.
derstand the general relationship between postural control
and fine motor skills in preterm infants. We demonstrated References
a strong relationship between postural control and fine
Almeida, K. M., Dutra, M. V., Mello, R. R., Reis, A. B., &
motor skills in preterm infants with motor delay. We also
Martins, P. S. (2008). Concurrent validity and reliability
found a stronger relationship between postural control and of the Alberta Infant Motor Scale in premature infants.
fine motor skills in younger preterm infants at 6 mo ad- Jornal de Pediatria, 84, 442–448. doi: 10.2223/JPED.
justed age than at 12 mo adjusted age. This finding suggests 1836
that clinicians should consider postural control and fine Aylward, G. P. (2005). Neurodevelopmental outcomes of
motor skills as two interdependent systems when treating infants born prematurely. Journal of Developmental and
Behavioral Pediatrics, 26, 427–440. doi: 10.1097/00004703-
infants at a younger age or infants with motor delay. In
200512000-00008
addition, we propose that therapists should treat infants’ Bayley, N. (1969). Bayley Scales of Mental and Motor Develop-
difficulties in postural control and fine motor skills con- ment. New York: Psychological Corp.
currently until they have mastered postural control. Campos, D., Santos, D. C. C., Gonçalves, V. M. G., Goto,
M. M., Arias, A. V., Brianeze, A. C., et al. (2006). Agree-
Limitations and Further Studies ment between scales for screening and diagnosis of motor
development at 6 months. Jornal de Pediatria, 82, 470–474.
This cross-sectional study focused on preterm infants age
doi: 10.2223/JPED.1567
1 yr or younger. Because delays in motor skills consis- Case-Smith, J. (1989). Reliability and validity of the posture
tently appear at a later age (Churcher et al., 1993; Darlow, and fine motor assessment of infants. OTJR: Occupation,
Horwood, Mogridge, & Clemett, 1997; Sullivan & Participation and Health, 9, 259–272.

700 November/December 2011, Volume 65, Number 6


Case-Smith, J., Fisher, A. G., & Bauer, D. (1989). An analysis Massion, J. (1998). Postural control systems in developmental
of the relationship between proximal and distal motor perspective. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 22,
control. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 43, 465–472. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(97)00031-6
657–662. Piper, M. C., & Darrah, J. (1994). Motor assessment of the
Churcher, E., Egan, M., Walop, W., Huang, P. P., Booth, A., developing infant. Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders.
& Roseman, G. (1993). Fine motor development of Plantinga, Y., Perdock, J., & de Groot, L. (1997). Hand func-
high-risk infants at 3, 6, 12 and 24 months. Physical and tion in low-risk preterm infants: Its relation to muscle power
Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 13, 19–37. doi: 10. regulation. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology,
1080/J006v13n01_02 39, 6–11. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1997.tb08197.x
Darlow, B. A., Horwood, L. J., Mogridge, N., & Clemett, Portney, L. G., & Watkins, M. P. (2009). Foundations of
R. S. (1997). Prospective study of New Zealand very clinical research: Applications to practice (3rd ed.). Upper
low birthweight infants: Outcome at 7–8 years. Journal Saddle River, NJ: Pearson/Prentice Hall.
of Paediatrics and Child Health, 33, 47–51. doi: 10.1111/ Provost, B., Heimerl, S., McClain, C., Kim, N. H., Lopez,
j.1440-1754.1997.tb00990.x B. R., & Kodituwakku, P. (2004). Concurrent validity
de Groot, L. (2000). Posture and motility in preterm infants of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development II Motor Scale
[Review]. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, and the Peabody Developmental Motor Scales–2 in chil-
42, 65–68. doi: 10.1017/S0012162200000128 dren with developmental delays. Pediatric Physical Therapy,
de Vries, A. M., & de Groot, L. (2002). Transient dystonias 16, 149–156. doi: 10.1097/01.PEP.0000136005.41585.FE
revisited: A comparative study of preterm and term chil- Rochat, P. (1992). Self-sitting and reaching in 5- to 8-month-
dren at 2 1/2 years of age. Developmental Medicine and old infants: The impact of posture and its development on
Child Neurology, 44, 415–421. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749. early eye–hand coordination. Journal of Motor Behavior,
2002.tb00836.x 24, 210–220. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1992.9941616
Fallang, B., Saugstad, O. D., & Hadders-Algra, M. (2000). Rosenblum, S., & Josman, N. (2003). The relationship be-
Goal directed reaching and postural control in supine po- tween postural control and fine manual dexterity. Physical
sition in healthy infants. Behavioural Brain Research, 115, and Occupational Therapy in Pediatrics, 23, 47–60. doi:
9–18. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(00)00231-X 10.1300/J006v23n04_04
Fallang, B., Saugstad, O. D., & Hadders-Algra, M. (2003). Samsom, J. F., & de Groot, L. (2000). The influence of postural
Postural adjustments in preterm infants at 4 and 6 months control on motility and hand function in a group of ‘‘high
post-term during voluntary reaching in supine position. risk’’ preterm infants at 1 year of age. Early Human Develop-
Pediatric Research, 54, 826–833. doi: 10.1203/01.PDR. ment, 60, 101–113. doi: 10.1016/S0378-3782(00)00107-9
0000088072.64794.F3 Shumway-Cook, A., & Woollacott, M. H. (1995). Motor con-
Folio, M. R., & Fewell, R. R. (2000). Peabody Developmental trol: Theory and practical applications. Baltimore: Williams
Motor Scales (2nd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. & Wilkins.
Jeng, S. F., Yau, K. I., Chen, L. C., & Hsiao, S. F. (2000). Skinner, L. (1979). Motor development in the preschool years.
Alberta Infant Motor Scale: Reliability and validity when Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.
used on preterm infants in Taiwan. Physical Therapy, 80, Sullivan, M. C., & Msall, M. E. (2007). Functional perfor-
168–178. mance of preterm children at age 4. Journal of Pediatric
Liao, P. M., & Campbell, S. K. (2004). Examination of the Nursing, 22, 297–309. doi: 10.1016/j.pedn.2007.02.001
item structure of the Alberta Infant Motor Scale. Pedi- Thelen, E., & Spencer, J. P. (1998). Postural control during
atric Physical Therapy, 16, 31–38. doi: 10.1097/01.PEP. reaching in young infants: A dynamic systems approach.
0000114843.92102.98 Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 22, 507–514. doi:
Lorenz, J. M., Wooliever, D. E., Jetton, J. R., & Paneth, N. 10.1016/S0149-7634(97)00037-7
(1998). A quantitative review of mortality and developmental Van der Fits, I. B., Otten, E., Klip, A. W., Van Eykern, L. A.,
disability in extremely premature newborns. Archives of Pedi- & Hadders-Algra, M. (1999). The development of pos-
atrics and Adolescent Medicine, 152, 425–435. tural adjustments during reaching in 6- to 18-month-old
Loria, C. (1980). Relationship of proximal and distal function infants: Evidence for two transitions. Experimental Brain
in motor development. Physical Therapy, 60, 167–172. Research, 126, 517–528. doi: 10.1007/s002210050760

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 701


View publication stats

You might also like