Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 12

Original Article

Justifying Decisions
Making Choices for Others Enhances Preferences
for Impoverished Options
Jingyi Lu,1 Yiming Liang,2,3,1 and Hebing Duan1
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

1
School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai, PR China
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

2
Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, PR China
3
Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, PR China

Abstract: Prior research has found that people prefer impoverished over enriched options. However, individuals make decisions either for
themselves or for others every day. The present research investigates how and why the decision target (self or other) influences preferences for
impoverished and enriched options. We hypothesized and found that participants who made choices for others preferred impoverished over
enriched options more than those who made choices for themselves (Studies 1 and 2) because the former group believed that they should
justify their decisions to others more than the latter group (Study 2). Overall, the current research sheds light on self–other differences in the
trade-off between impoverished and enriched options, as well as the role of justification in this effect.

Keywords: self–other decision making, decision target, enriched option, impoverished option, justification

Perfect options are desirable but seldom available. In most 2010; Lu, Xie, & Xu, 2013; Polman, 2012a, 2012b). In the
cases, individuals have to make choices among imperfect current research, we investigate the effect of the decision
alternatives, such as enriched and impoverished options. target (self or other) on preferences for impoverished and
An enriched option has extremely high and low values for enriched options.
different dimensions, whereas an impoverished option has
moderate values for all dimensions (Ganzach, 1995; Shafir,
1993). For example, an enriched restaurant offers extre-
mely appetizing but expensive food. By contrast, an impov- Trade-Offs Between Enriched and
erished restaurant offers average tasting but fairly priced
food. Similarly, an enriched job candidate performs well
Impoverished Options
in certain aspects of a job but poorly in other aspects,
Comparisons Between Attributes
whereas an impoverished candidate performs averagely in
all aspects. Consider the following scenario. Option A scores XA on
Research shows that decision makers generally prefer Attribute X and YA on Attribute Y, that is, A (XA, YA),
impoverished to enriched options (Shafir, Simonson, & whereas Option B scores XB on Attribute X and YB on
Tversky, 1993; Wang & Murnighan, 2013). However, some Attribute Y, that is, B (XB, YB). An “economic man” bases
factors, including cultural orientation (Briley, Morris, & his preferences on the comparison between the scores for
Simonson, 2000), self-construal (Zhang & Mittal, 2007), each attribute of these two options (Tversky & Kahneman,
and need for cognition (Drolet, Luce, & Simonson, 2009), 1986). Assuming Attributes X and Y are of equal impor-
moderate this effect. tance, people should prefer Option A over Option B if
Notably, individuals frequently make choices for them- (XA  XB)  (YB  YA) > 0. By contrast, they should prefer
selves or on behalf of another in daily life. For example, Option B over Option A if (XA  XB)  (YB  YA) < 0.
consumers choose a vacation spot for themselves, whereas In addition, if (XA  XB)  (YB  YA) = 0, then people
travel agencies sometimes choose a vacation spot for should be indifferent between the two options.
customers. Similarly, investors select stocks for themselves, In a typical case (Shafir, 1993), enriched options
whereas consultants make investment decisions for their score higher than average on some attributes (positive
clients. Self–other differences are consistently found in attributes, P) but lower than average on other attributes
decision making (Danziger, Montal, & Barkan, 2012; Laran, (negative attributes, N) compared with impoverished

Social Psychology (2017), 48(2), 92–103 Ó 2017 Hogrefe Publishing


DOI: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000302
J. Lu et al., Justifying Decisions 93

options (e.g., enriched option [80, 20]; impoverished option choices for others than for themselves (Lerner & Tetlock,
[50, 50]). That is, 1999).
ðPenriched option  Pimpoverished option Þ The relationship between the decision target and justifi-
 ðN impoverished option  N enriched option Þ ¼ 0: ð1Þ cation was partially supported by Hong and Chang (2015)
who investigated the effect of self-construal on justification
Therefore, neither option stands out. seeking. Self-construal refers to the way by which people
view the self in relation to others. Individuals with an inde-
pendent self-construal define themselves by their internal
Negativity Bias and Preferences
attributes, whereas those with an interdependent self-
for Impoverished Options construal define themselves by their relation to others.
Descriptive behaviors frequently violate normative Moreover, the latter group is more mindful of others than
principles (Tversky & Kahneman, 1986). Normatively, indi- the former group when making decisions (Markus &
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

viduals should weigh negative and positive stimuli equally. Kitayama, 1991). Hong and Chang (2015) showed that indi-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

However, negativity bias is prevalent such that people are viduals with an interdependent self-construal sought more
more sensitive to negativity than to positivity (Baumeister, justifications than those with an independent self-construal,
Bratslavsky, Finkenauer, & Vohs, 2001; Rozin & Royzman, suggesting that a greater focus on others caused more
2001). Prospect theory, which proposes that losses loom justification-seeking behaviors. More direct evidence was
larger than gains (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979), provides provided by Polman (2012a) and Polman and Vohs
evidence for negativity bias. In summary, people weigh (2016). They found that people who made decisions for
negative attributes more than positive attributes. others thought about the reasons for their decisions more
Let κ and λ denote the relative weights attached to than those who made decisions for themselves. Thus,
positive and negative attributes, respectively. Considering self–other differences exist in justification.
negativity bias, κ < λ. Hence,
κðPenriched option  Pimpoverished option Þ
 λðN impoverished option  N enriched option Þ < 0: ð2Þ Justification, Negativity Bias,
When this logic is followed, impoverished options are and Preferences for Impoverished
preferred over enriched options.
Preferences for impoverished options are supported by
Options
both the compromise effect and the generalist bias. The Negativity bias is stronger when people believe that they
former refers to the phenomenon in which a middle option need to justify their decisions to others. Tetlock and Boett-
is selected more than an extreme option (Shafir et al., 1993; ger (1994) found that when decision makers believed that
Simonson, 1989). The latter refers to the tendency in which they should justify decisions to those who would evaluate
generalists who perform averagely in all aspects are them, they would be more concerned about potential
preferred to specialists who are good in a limited number negative outcomes to avoid blame. Hence, when the deci-
of tasks (Custódio, Ferreira, & Matos, 2013; Wang & sion target shifts from self to others, κ (i.e., the relative
Murnighan, 2013). Both middle options and generalists weight attached to positive attributes) decreases, whereas
perform moderately, and thus, they can be regarded as λ (i.e., the relative weight attached to negative attributes)
impoverished options to a certain extent. Therefore, these increases. Therefore,
effects suggest preferences for impoverished alternatives.
κ justification ðPenriched option  Pimpoverished option Þ
 λjustification ðN impoverished option  N enriched option Þ
Decision Target and Justification < κ no justification ðPenriched option  Pimpoverished option Þ
 λ no justification ðNimpoverished option  Nenriched option Þ; ð3Þ
In reality, people make choices not only for themselves, but
also for others. Although the decision problem itself is held thereby leading to amplified preferences for impoverished
constant, a shift in the decision target leads to different options.
perceptions of the problem (Lu & Xie, 2014; Polman, The above prediction was supported by several
2012b). One of the differences lies on perceived justification. researches. For example, Simonson (1989) observed that
Decisions for another person are more public than those for seeking justifications enhanced preferences for middle
oneself because the decision outcome will be seen and options. Simonson and Nowlis (2000) showed amplified
evaluated by others. Therefore, individuals are more preferences for all-average options when people were more
concerned with justifying their decisions when making concerned with the evaluations of others. Similarly,

Ó 2017 Hogrefe Publishing Social Psychology (2017), 48(2), 92–103


94 J. Lu et al., Justifying Decisions

Maimaran and Simonson (2011) found that decision makers Table 1. Information of vacation spots (Study 1)
opted more for impoverished over enriched options when Attributes Spot A Spot B
they knew their decisions would be seen by others than Version 1
when they did not. Quality of beaches 71 51
On the basis of the relationship between the decision Quality of restaurants 29 49
target and justification as well as the relationship between Quality of environment 73 53
justification and preferences for impoverished options, Cost 27 47
we hypothesize that people who make choices for others General weather conditions 72 52
prefer impoverished over enriched options more than those Transportation facilities 28 48
who make choices for themselves because the former Overall average 50 50
perceives that they should justify their decisions to others Version 2
more than the latter. Previous research provides initial Quality of beaches 51 71
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

evidence for our reasoning. For example, one study showed Quality of restaurants 49 29
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

that people who made decisions for others tended to Quality of environment 53 73
choose a compromise option more than those who made Cost 47 27
decisions for themselves (Chang, Chuang, Cheng, & General weather conditions 52 72
Huang, 2011). Transportation facilities 48 28
Overall average 50 50
Notes. The attribute values ranged from 0 to 100. Zero indicated worst, 50
indicated average, and 100 indicated best.
The Present Research
The current research aims to investigate the effect of the in the study. They were randomly assigned to one condition
decision target on preferences for enriched and impover- in a 2 (decision target: self or other)  2 (option: enriched or
ished options. We hypothesize that people who decide for impoverished) mixed design, where the decision target was
others prefer impoverished over enriched options more a between-participant variable and option was a within-
than those who decide for themselves. Moreover, a differ- participant variable.
ence in justification is responsible for the aforementioned
self–other differences. In particular, people who make Procedure and Materials
choices for others believe that they should justify their deci- The cover story told to the participants was that the study
sions to others more than those who make choices for was aimed to investigate decision-making habits. First, the
themselves. Study 1 tests whether self–other differences participants in the self condition wrote down their own sur-
exist in preferences for enriched and impoverished options. name, whereas those in the other condition wrote down
Study 2 examines the role of justification. the surname of a friend. This task reminded all the partici-
pants of a specific person (Lu, Jia, Xie, & Wang, 2016).
Subsequently, the participants in the self condition con-
sidered that they were planning a vacation between two
Study 1: Vacation Spots candidate spots. Their performances are provided in Table 1
(adapted from Zhang & Mittal, 2007). The attribute values
In this study, we aimed to explore self–other differences ranged from 0 to 100 (0 = worst, 50 = average, 100 = best).
regarding preferences for enriched and impoverished The order of the two options was counterbalanced across
options. The participants chose between two candidate participants. In the other condition, the participants imag-
vacation spots, either for themselves or for others. One spot ined that their friend whose surname they had written
performed well in some dimensions but poorly in others, down was planning a vacation. The friend was not sure
whereas the other spot performed averagely in all dimen- which spot to choose, and thus, asked the participants to
sions. We expected amplified preferences for impoverished make the decision for him or her.
over enriched options in the other condition than in the self After reading the scenario, the participants indicated who
condition. planned to go on a vacation in the scenario. This question
served as a manipulation check. Then, the participants
rated the likelihood that they would choose each option
Method on a 9-point scale (1 = unlikely, 9 = most likely). Thereafter,
Participants and Design they chose a vacation spot.
A total of 120 university students (28 men, 92 women; Previous research indicated an effect of self-construal
Mage = 21.57 years, SD = 4.53) volunteered to participate on preferences for enriched and impoverished options

Social Psychology (2017), 48(2), 92–103 Ó 2017 Hogrefe Publishing


J. Lu et al., Justifying Decisions 95

(Zhang & Mittal, 2007). People with an independent self-


construal prefer enriched options over impoverished options
than those with an interdependent self-construal. To elimi-
nate such influence, we asked the participants to complete
a self-construal scale which was used to assess their disposi-
tional self-construal (Singelis, 1994). In this scale, 12 items
measured interdependent self-construal (e.g., “I have
respect for the authority figures with whom I interact”;
“It is important for me to maintain harmony within my
group”; α = .78), whereas another 12 items measured inde-
pendent self-construal (e.g., “I’d rather say ‘No’ directly,
than risk being misunderstood”; “Speaking up during a class
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

is not a problem for me”; α = .64). The participants indicated


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

their agreement with these items on a 7-point scale


(1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). Then, information Figure 1. Likelihood ratings as a function of the decision target and
option (Study 1). The error bars indicate the standard errors of means.
regarding gender, age, and monthly consumption was col- Likelihood ratings indicate the likelihood that the participants choose
lected. Finally, the participants were thanked and debriefed. an option.

Results and Discussion p = .002, ηp2 = .15. The participants in the other condition
Manipulation Check and Control Variables preferred impoverished (M = 6.48, SD = 1.74) over enriched
The raw data for Study 1 are provided as Electronic Supple- (M = 3.91, SD = 1.93) options, F(1, 55) = 40.59, p < .001,
mentary Material, ESM 1. Three participants who failed the ηp2 = .43, as well. From another perspective, the likelihood
manipulation check were excluded from the subsequent ratings for enriched option were higher in the self condition
analysis. Meanwhile, the participants in the self and other than in the other condition, F(1, 115) = 6.90, p = .010,
conditions did not differ in interdependent self-construal ηp2 = .06. However, the likelihood ratings for impoverished
and monthly consumption, ps > .147. Although the two option between the two conditions did not have any differ-
groups differed in independent self-construal (Mself = 4.71, ence, F(1, 115) = 0.99, p = .322, ηp2 = .01. Notably, this inter-
SD = 0.55; Mother = 4.48, SD = 0.68), F(1, 115) = 3.98, action was crossover and nonremovable according to the
p = .049, ηp2 = .03, neither independent nor interdependent classification by Wagenmakers, Krypotps, Criss, and
self-construal was correlated with the likelihood ratings for Iverson (2012). Consistent with our hypothesis, these
enriched and impoverished options, ps > .234. Therefore, results indicated that the participants who made choices
self-construal was not considered in the subsequent statisti- for others preferred impoverished over enriched options
cal analysis. more than those who made choices for themselves.
Furthermore, to test whether order would influence self–
Decision Target and Likelihood Ratings other differences in the trade-off between impoverished
Likelihood ratings were submitted to a 2 (decision target: and enriched options, we conducted a 2 (decision target:
self or other)  2 (option: enriched or impoverished) self or other)  2 (option: enriched or impoverished) 
mixed-design analysis of variance (ANOVA), with the deci- 2 (order: enriched option first or impoverished option first)
sion target as a between-participant variable and option as a mixed-design ANOVA on likelihood ratings. The results
within-participant variable. Consequently, the main effect showed that the three-way interaction was not significant,
for the decision target was not significant (Mself = 5.48, F(1, 113) = 0.38, p = .537, ηp2 < .01, suggesting that self–
SD = 2.07; Mother = 5.20, SD = 2.24), F(1, 115) = 1.96, other differences in likelihood ratings were not determined
p = .164, ηp2 = .02. By contrast, the main effect for option by presenting order.
was significant, F(1, 115) = 45.37, p < .001, ηp2 = .28, such
that the participants preferred impoverished (M = 6.30, Decision Target and Choice
SD = 1.91) over enriched (M = 4.39, SD = 1.95) options A chi-square test showed that the decision target had a
regardless of the decision target. marginal effect on choice, w2(1, N = 117) = 2.97, p = .085.
We also observed an interaction between the decision In particular, in the self condition, 18 out of 61 participants
target and option (Figure 1), F(1, 115) = 4.94, p = .028, (29.51%) chose enriched option, whereas 43 (70.49%)
ηp2 = .04. The participants in the self condition were more chose impoverished option. In the other condition, 9 out
likely to choose impoverished (M = 6.13, SD = 2.05) than of 56 (16.07%) chose enriched option, whereas
enriched (M = 4.84, SD = 1.88) options, F(1, 60) = 10.15, 47 (83.93%) chose impoverished option. These results

Ó 2017 Hogrefe Publishing Social Psychology (2017), 48(2), 92–103


96 J. Lu et al., Justifying Decisions

exhibited the trend that making decisions for others facili- Study 1, a 2 (decision target: self or other)  2 (option:
tated preferences for impoverished options yet attenuated enriched or impoverished) mixed design was adopted,
preferences for enriched options. However, these results wherein the decision target was a between-participant
should be treated with caution because of the marginal variable and option was a within-participant variable.
significant effect.
In summary, Study 1 showed that people who made Procedure and Materials
choices for others opted for impoverished over enriched The procedure was similar to that in Study 1 with three
options more than those who made choices for themselves. exceptions. First, the participants imagined that they were
Nevertheless, limitations remained. To begin with, both choosing a hotel for a trip. The performances of two candi-
options scored higher on some attributes (i.e., quality of date hotels on six dimensions (i.e., cost, transportation,
beaches, quality of environment, and general weather room size, bed size, distance from spots, and check-in
conditions) than others (i.e., quality of restaurants, cost, service speed) are provided in Table 2. The order of the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

and transportation facilities) in the decision scenario. The two options was counterbalanced across participants.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

observed self–other differences might be caused by varying In addition, half of the participants read the information
perceived importance attached to these attributes between that both hotels had higher values in terms of cost, room
personal decision makers and agents. People who made size, and distance from spots, whereas another half read
choices for themselves might weigh beach quality over the information that both hotels had higher values in terms
restaurant quality. By contrast, those who decided for others of transportation, bed size, and check-in service speed. This
might think that restaurant quality was more important than manipulation excluded the potential effect of varying
beach quality. Although no literature, to our knowledge, perceived importance attached to different attributes,
supported this argument, attribute values were counterbal- therefore remedying the limitation in Study 1.
anced across participants in the next study. Second, to Second, after completing the manipulation check and
eliminate the effects of potential confounding variables on likelihood ratings, and then choosing a hotel, the partici-
preferences for enriched and impoverished options, pants rated the extent to which they believed they should
additional control variables, such as perceived responsibil- justify their decisions to other people (“To what extent do
ity, regret, and task difficulty, were included in Study 2. you believe you should justify your decisions to other
Moreover, Study 2 was designed to test the reasons that people?” 1 = unnecessary, 9 = extremely necessary).
underlay the self–other differences demonstrated in Study 1. Third, the participants did not complete a self-construal
scale because self-construal did not influence the likelihood
ratings in Study 1. Instead, they rated task difficulty (“How
Study 2: Hotels difficult is the decision task?” 1 = easy, 9 = difficult), their
regret (“Will you feel regretful if your decision is not
The aim of Study 2 was twofold. First, we sought to repli- good?” 1 = not regretful, 9 = very regretful), and perceived
cate the self–other differences found in Study 1 using a responsibility (‘‘Do you feel responsible for the decision
different decision scenario. Second, the role of justification outcome?” 1 = not responsible, 9 = highly responsible) as in
in self–other differences regarding preferences for enriched Lu et al. (2016). Perceived difficulty may differ between
and impoverished options was explored. tasks of making decisions for the self and for others
The participants chose between two hotels, either for because people are more frequently to make choices for
themselves or for others. One hotel was an enriched option, the self than for others in daily life. Therefore, task diffi-
whereas the other was an impoverished option. Further- culty was considered as a control variable. The level of
more, the participants rated the extent to which they should regret was also controlled because research showed that
justify their decisions to other people. We anticipated that making decisions for others (vs. self) could trigger more
the participants in the other condition would believe that anticipatory regret (Beattie, Baron, Hershey, & Spranca,
they should justify their decisions to other people more than 1994). Perceived responsibility was controlled because
those in the self condition. Therefore, the former group self–other difference regarding this variable was found in
would opt for impoverished over enriched options more previous studies (Lu & Xie, 2014; Lu et al., 2013). More-
than the latter group. over, dialectical thinking was included as a control variable
because higher dialectical thinking may be associated with
more tolerance of enriched options (“Every coin has two
Method
sides”; “To pursue the advantages of an item, I can accept
Participants and Design its disadvantages”; 1 = totally disagree, 9 = totally agree).
A total of 119 university students (37 men, 82 women; Mage = The average value of the two items reflected dialectical
23.13 years, SD = 3.36) participated in Study 2. Identical to thinking (α = .62).

Social Psychology (2017), 48(2), 92–103 Ó 2017 Hogrefe Publishing


J. Lu et al., Justifying Decisions 97

Table 2. Information of hotels (Study 2)


Attributes Hotel A Hotel B More black stars
Version 1
Cost wwwIIIII wIIIIIII Lower
Transportation wwwwwIII wwwwwwwI More convenient
Room size wwwIIIII wIIIIIII Larger
Bed size wwwwwIII wwwwwwwI Larger
Distance from spots wwwIIIII wIIIIIII Closer
Check-in service speed wwwwwIII wwwwwwwI Faster
Version 2
Cost wIIIIIII wwwIIIII Lower
Transportation wwwwwwwI wwwwwIII More convenient
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Room size wIIIIIII wwwIIIII Larger


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Bed size wwwwwwwI wwwwwIII Larger


Distance from spots wIIIIIII wwwIIIII Closer
Check-in service speed wwwwwwwI wwwwwIII Faster
Version 3
Transportation wwwIIIII wIIIIIII More convenient
Cost wwwwwIII wwwwwwwI Lower
Bed size wwwIIIII wIIIIIII Larger
Room size wwwwwIII wwwwwwwI Larger
Check-in service speed wwwIIIII wIIIIIII Faster
Distance from spots wwwwwIII wwwwwwwI Closer
Version 4
Transportation wIIIIIII wwwIIIII More convenient
Cost wwwwwwwI wwwwwIII Lower
Bed size wIIIIIII wwwIIIII Larger
Room size wwwwwwwI wwwwwIII Larger
Check-in service speed wIIIIIII wwwIIIII Faster
Distance from spots wwwwwwwI wwwwwIII Closer
Note. Overall, more black stars indicated better performances.

Results and Discussion ηp2 < .01. The main effect for option was significant,
F(1, 115) = 41.94, p < .001, ηp2 = .27, such that the partici-
Manipulation Check and Control Variables pants preferred impoverished (M = 6.58, SD = 1.97) over
The raw data for Study 2 are provided as Electronic Supple- enriched (M = 4.61, SD = 2.15) options.
mentary Material, ESM 2. Two participants failed the Similar to Study 1, the interaction between the decision
manipulation check. Therefore, their data were excluded. target and option was significant (Figure 2), F(1, 115) =
The participants in the self and the other conditions did 15.54, p < .001, ηp2 = .12. The participants in the other
not differ in ratings for task difficulty, regret, perceived condition were more likely to choose impoverished
responsibility, dialectical thinking, and monthly consump- (M = 7.16, SD = 1.67) over enriched (M = 4.10, SD = 1.99)
tion, ps > .093. Therefore, these variables were not consid- options, F(1, 61) = 64.37, p < .001, ηp2 = .51. However, the
ered in the subsequent analysis. likelihood ratings between two options did not have any
difference in the self condition (Menriched option = 5.18,
Decision Target and Likelihood Ratings SD = 2.20; Mimpoverished option = 5.93, SD = 2.09),
A 2 (decision target: self or other)  2 (option: enriched or F(1, 54) = 2.71, p = .105, ηp2 = .05. Moreover, the likelihood
impoverished) mixed-design ANOVA was conducted on ratings for enriched option were higher in the self condition
likelihood ratings, with the decision target as a between- than in the other condition, F(1, 115) = 7.84, p = .006,
participant variable and option as a within-participant ηp2 = .06. By contrast, the likelihood ratings for impover-
variable. The results showed that the effect for the decision ished option were higher in the other condition than in the
target was not significant (Mself = 5.55, SD = 2.17; self condition, F(1, 115) = 12.57, p = .001, ηp2 = .10. This inter-
Mother = 5.63, SD = 2.39), F(1, 115) = 0.11, p = .738, action was nonremovable (Wagenmakers et al., 2012).

Ó 2017 Hogrefe Publishing Social Psychology (2017), 48(2), 92–103


98 J. Lu et al., Justifying Decisions

the other condition (M = 7.27, SD = 1.76) believed that they


need to justify their decisions to others more than those in
the self condition (M = 5.58, SD = 2.22), F(1, 115) = 21.16,
p < .001, ηp2 = .16.

Mediation Analysis of Justification


We found self–other differences regarding justification and
preferences for impoverished and enriched options. In this
section, we analyzed if justification played a mediating role
between the decision target and preferences for impover-
ished and enriched options. First, we tested a mediation
model with the decision target as the independent variable,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

justification as a potential mediator, and likelihood ratings


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

for impoverished option as the dependent variable.


Figure 2. Likelihood ratings as a function of the decision target and The total effect of the decision target on the likelihood rat-
option (Study 2). The error bars indicate the standard errors of means.
Likelihood ratings indicate the likelihood that the participants choose
ings for impoverished option was significant, β = .31,
an option. p < .001. However, when both the decision target and jus-
tification were included in the regression model to predict
In addition, to test whether self–other differences in the the likelihood ratings for impoverished option, the effect
trade-off between impoverished and enriched options would of the decision target became less significant, β = .23,
be moderated by order, a 2 (decision target: self or other)  p = .018 (Figure 3A). A Sobel test revealed a significant
2 (option: enriched or impoverished)  2 (order: enriched mediating effect (Z = 2.06, p = .040). These results indi-
option first or impoverished option first) mixed-design cated that making decisions for others (vs. self) increased
ANOVA on likelihood ratings was conducted. The results perceived justification, which in turn, increased the likeli-
showed that the three-way interaction was not significant, hood of choosing impoverished options.
F(1, 113) = 0.38, p = .537, ηp2 < .01. Similarly, to test whether Next, we tested a similar mediation model with the deci-
self–other differences would be moderated by attribute sion target as the independent variable, justification as a
value, we conducted a 2 (decision target: self or other)  potential mediator, and the likelihood ratings for enriched
2 (option: enriched or impoverished)  2 (attribute value: option as the dependent variable. The total effect of the
lower cost or higher cost) mixed-design ANOVA on likeli- decision target on the likelihood ratings for enriched option
hood ratings. Consequently, the three-way interaction was was significant, β = .25, p = .006. When both the decision
not significant, F(1, 113) = 0.74, p = .392, ηp2 < .01. These target and justification were included in the regression
results indicated that neither order nor attribute value influ- model to predict the likelihood ratings for enriched option,
enced self–other differences in likelihood ratings. the effect of the decision target remained significant,
Taken together, consistent with our hypothesis, the β = .29, p = .004 (Figure 3B). A Sobel test showed that jus-
participants who made decisions for others preferred tification did not mediate the effect of the decision target
impoverished over enriched options more than those who on the likelihood of choosing enriched option (Z = 1.00,
made decisions for themselves. p = .319).
We intentionally measured the potential mediator (justi-
Decision Target and Choice fication) after the dependent variable (likelihood ratings) in
A chi-square test showed that choice was influenced by the this study to avoid demand effects. Measuring the mediator
decision target, w2(1, N = 117) = 7.28, p = .007. In the self before the dependent variable will remind participants of
condition, 21 out of 55 participants (38.18%) chose enriched the mediator, therefore artificially amplifying the influence
option, whereas 34 (61.82%) chose impoverished option. of the mediator on the dependent variable (O’Brien &
In the other condition, 10 out of 62 participants (16.13%) Kardas, 2016). However, the ratings of the mediator might
chose enriched option, whereas 52 (83.87%) chose impover- be influenced by the ratings of the dependent variable in
ished option. These results demonstrated that making such a measuring order. To exclude this possibility, we
decisions for others facilitated preferences for impoverished examined two alternative reverse mediation models where
options yet attenuated preferences for enriched options. the likelihood ratings mediated justification.
The decision target was treated as the independent vari-
Decision Target and Justification able, the likelihood ratings for impoverished option as a
A one-way ANOVA test showed that justification ratings potential mediator, and justification as the dependent vari-
were influenced by the decision target. The participants in able. The total effect of the decision target on justification

Social Psychology (2017), 48(2), 92–103 Ó 2017 Hogrefe Publishing


J. Lu et al., Justifying Decisions 99

(A) Figure 3. Mediation model (Study


2). Likelihood ratings indicate the
likelihood that the participants
choose an option (A–D). Standard-
ized coefficients and their signifi-
cances are reported. The total
effects of the independent variable
on the dependent variable are
reported in parentheses, and the
standardized coefficients when the
mediator is included in the model
are reported above the arrow.
(B) ***p < .001; **p < .01; *p < .05.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

(C)

(D)

was significant, β = .39, p < .001. However, when both the be justified more than choosing an impoverished one
decision target and likelihood ratings for impoverished (Simonson & Nowlis, 2000).
option were included in the regression model to predict Finally, we tested a mediation model with the decision
justification, the effect of the decision target remained sig- target as the independent variable, the likelihood ratings
nificant, β = .33, p < .001 (Figure 3C). A Sobel test showed for enriched option as a potential mediator, and justification
that this mediating effect was not significant, Z = 1.93, as the dependent variable. The total effect of the decision
p = .054. Statistically, the p value (.054) suggested the target on justification was significant, β = .39, p < .001.
possibility that the likelihood ratings for impoverished When both the decision target and likelihood ratings for
option mediated the effect of the decision target on justifi- enriched option were included in the regression model,
cation. However, logically, it is impossible that a participant the effect of the decision target remained significant,
believed he or she should justify his or her decision when β = .42, p < .001 (Figure 3D). The mediating effect was
he or she had chosen an impoverished option more than not significantly revealed by a Sobel test, Z = 0.96, p = .337.
when he or she had chosen an enriched option. Instead, Overall, these results indicated that the decision target
choosing an enriched option is perceived as something to influenced the likelihood ratings for impoverished option

Ó 2017 Hogrefe Publishing Social Psychology (2017), 48(2), 92–103


100 J. Lu et al., Justifying Decisions

through justification. Making decisions for others (vs. self) An interesting question is whether the effect of the
promoted perceived justification, hence causing more decision target on extreme aversion can be generalized to
preferences for impoverished option. In addition, because the case with only one option (either an extreme or average
the participants in the self and other conditions did not differ option). In other words, whether there will be a difference
in ratings for task difficulty, regret, perceived responsibility, between joint and separate evaluations (Hsee, 1996).
and dialectical thinking, as reported in the section of We conducted a post-study to answer this question.
“manipulation check and control variables,” suggesting that We invited 232 university students to participate in the
these variables were not influenced by the decision target, study. They were randomly assigned into one condition in
we did not test their mediation roles in the relationship a 2 (decision target: self or other)  2 (option: enriched
between the decision target and preferences for impover- or impoverished) between-participant design. The materials
ished and enriched options. were similar to those in Study 2, except that the participants
were presented with only one option (Table 3). The manip-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ulation check question asked who planned to go on a trip in


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

the scenario. Next, the participants indicated the likelihood


General Discussion
that they would choose this option from 1 (= unlikely) to
Previous research has demonstrated preferences for impov- 9 (= most likely). The raw data for post-study are provided
erished over enriched options (Shafir et al., 1993; Simonson, as Electronic Supplementary Material, ESM 3. As a result,
1989) because people attach more weight to negativity than six participants who missed the manipulation check ques-
to positivity (Baumeister et al., 2001; Rozin & Royzman, tion and 16 who failed the manipulation check were
2001). That is, individuals tend to avoid negative outcomes excluded from the subsequent analysis. This failure rate
brought by enriched options. The present research provides was acceptable (Steffel, Williams, & Perrmann-Graham,
evidence for such preference. Individuals opt for impover- 2016). The results showed a main effect for option,
ished over enriched alternatives regardless of the decision F(1, 206) = 7.79, p = .006, ηp2 = .04. The participants pre-
target. ferred impoverished (M = 4.83, SD = 1.68) over enriched
Moreover, we shed light on the role of the decision options (M = 4.10, SD = 2.26). The interaction between
target, which is a factor that can increase the attractiveness the decision target and option was also significant,
of impoverished options. People who make decisions for F(1, 206) = 4.18, p = .042, ηp2 = .02. The participants in
others prefer impoverished over enriched options more the other condition preferred impoverished over enriched
than those who make decisions for themselves. The self– options (Mimpoverished option = 5.25, SD = 1.81; Menriched option =
other difference in justification accounts for the aforemen- 3.94, SD = 2.25) more than those in the self condition
tioned effect. People deciding for others believe that they (Mimpoverished option = 4.46, SD = 1.48; Menriched option = 4.25,
should justify the reasons for their decisions more than SD = 2.29). Therefore, our work extends extremeness
those deciding for themselves. Therefore, people who aversion in the case of making decisions for others not
decide for others tend to choose impoverished options, only from three options to two options, but also from two
which are easier to justify than enriched options. options to one option.

Extremeness Aversion Ease of Justification


Extremeness aversion is a prevalent phenomenon that The results of the mediation analysis indicate that people
refers to the tendency that people prefer middle options who make decisions for others believe that they should
over extreme options (Neumann, Böckenholt, & Sinha, explain the reasons for their decisions to others; therefore,
2016). For example, Options A and B have both advantages they choose impoverished options. These results are in line
and disadvantages. If Option C is added to make Option A with those of previous findings, thereby suggesting that
the middle option, then individuals are more inclined to impoverished options are perceived as easier to justify than
choose Option A over Option B or C. Nevertheless, the enriched options (Simonson, 1989).
decision target serves as a moderator. In particular, making Notably, we provide a new explanation for this effect.
choices for others amplifies extremeness aversion (Chang Prior research has shown that justifying options with
et al., 2011). The current research shows that the effect of average attributes is easy because such options are safe
the decision target on extremeness aversion is not limited (Simonson, 1989). Thus, these options are chosen to cope
to cases with three options. Even in cases with two options with uncertainty. We believe that people perceive that the
(i.e., one is extreme and the other is average), people who disadvantages of enriched options cannot be offset by their
choose for others demonstrate higher extremeness aversion advantages because of negativity bias. Thus, choosing
than those who choose for themselves. impoverished options is easier to justify.

Social Psychology (2017), 48(2), 92–103 Ó 2017 Hogrefe Publishing


J. Lu et al., Justifying Decisions 101

Table 3. Information of hotels (post-study) with an enriched option because both have a higher
Attributes Hotel More black stars variance. Similarly, a riskless option is similar to an impov-
Version 1 erished option to a certain extent because both have a lower
Cost wwwIIIII Lower variance (Maimaran & Simonson, 2011).
Transportation wwwwwIII More convenient Our results show that people who make choices for
Room size wwwIIIII Larger others opt for impoverished over enriched options more
Bed size wwwwwIII Larger than those who make choices for themselves because the
Distance from spots wwwIIIII Closer former group prefers lower variance more than the latter
Check-in service speed wwwwwIII Faster group. From this logic, riskless options with lower variance
Version 2 should be more attractive for people who make decisions
Cost wIIIIIII Lower for others than to those who make decisions for them-
Transportation wwwwwwwI More convenient selves. In summary, the commonality that underlies self–
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Room size wIIIIIII Larger other differences in preferences for impoverished options
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Bed size wwwwwwwI Larger and risk preferences is the self–other difference in the
Distance from spots wIIIIIII Closer perceived attractiveness of the outcome variance.
Check-in service speed wwwwwwwI Faster
Version 3
Transportation wwwIIIII More convenient
Self–Other Decision Making
Cost wwwwwIII Lower and Invariance Violations
Bed size wwwIIIII Larger
Economists assert that invariance is a fundamental princi-
Room size wwwwwIII Larger
ple of rationality. Invariance refers to that preferences will
Check-in service speed wwwIIIII Faster
remain stable if the decision problem itself remains
Distance from spots wwwwwIII Closer
unchanged. However, this principle is severely challenged
Version 4
(Tversky & Kahneman, 1986). People violate the principle
Transportation wIIIIIII More convenient
of invariance because of cognitive limitations and environ-
Cost wwwwwwwI Lower
ment structures (Simon, 1990).
Bed size wIIIIIII Larger
Self–other differences in the trade-off between enriched
Room size wwwwwwwI Larger
and impoverished options observed in the current research
Check-in service speed wIIIIIII Faster
demonstrate invariance violations. The decision problem
Distance from spots wwwwwwwI Closer
itself is held constant (i.e., choosing between enriched
Note. Overall, more black stars indicated better performances.
and impoverished options). However, preferences change
with the decision target. The decision target reflects the
structures of the environment which cause preference
changes. Therefore, self–other differences in decision
New Perspective on Understanding making provide evidence for invariance violations.
Decisions Under Risk
Our findings are also pertinent to research on self–other
differences in risk preferences. Although the results are
Limitations and Future Directions
inconsistent to a certain extent, some studies have indi- Overall, the results of the two studies are consistent.
cated higher risk aversion for people who make choices Compared with the participants in the self condition, those
for others than those who make choices for themselves in the other condition tended to choose impoverished over
(Dore, Stone, & Buchanan, 2014; Stone, Choi, de Bruin, enriched options. However, differences still existed.
& Mandel, 2013). Our research provides a new perspective In Study 1, the likelihood ratings for enriched option
on understanding why making decisions for others facili- differed between two conditions, whereas the likelihood
tates risk aversion. ratings for impoverished option did not differ. Nevertheless,
Scholars define risk through outcome variance (Mishra, the likelihood ratings for both enriched and impoverished
2014). An option with a higher variance (e.g., gain $100 options differed between the two conditions in Study 2.
with a probability of .5; otherwise, gain nothing) is riskier Such divergence might be caused by two reasons. First,
than an option with a lower variance (e.g., gain $75 with the relatively small sample sizes in both studies did not
a probability of .5; otherwise, gain $25), assuming that both cover the individual differences in preferences. Second,
options have the same expected value (Mishra, Barclay, & the varying decision scenarios between the two studies
Lalumière, 2014). A risky option has something in common might cause the inconsistency. Future research should use

Ó 2017 Hogrefe Publishing Social Psychology (2017), 48(2), 92–103


102 J. Lu et al., Justifying Decisions

multiple decision scenarios to test the robustness of our However, large organizations should ask their own recruit-
results. ing departments to select employees because these
Moreover, our results show that individuals who make departments, who make decisions for themselves, will
decisions for others believe that they should justify their prefer specialists.
reasons to other people. We infer that perceived justifica-
tion promotes weight attached to negative aspects more Acknowledgments
than positive aspects. However, we did not provide empir-
Jingyi Lu and Yiming Liang contributed equally to the paper.
ical evidence for this inference. Hence, we call for further
This research was financially funded by the programs of
research to measure cognitive focus on negative and
National Natural Science Foundation of China (71501073)
positive aspects by eye-tracking technique.
and “Chenguang Program” supported by Shanghai Educa-
In addition, future research may manipulate perceived
tion Development Foundation and Shanghai Municipal
justification to provide stronger evidence for the mediator
Education Commission (16CG29). We extend our sincere
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

of justification. In particular, participants should be


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

gratitude to Tian-Yi Hu, Huiyuan Jia, and Qingzhou Sun


explicitly asked to justify their decisions to others or not.
for their comments on an earlier version. We are also grate-
If self–other differences in preferences for impoverished
ful to Rong Dong for helping collect data.
or enriched options diminish, then the role of justification
will be further verified. Another way to further test the role
Electronic Supplementary Materials
of perceived justification is to make impoverished options
The electronic supplementary material is available with the
harder to justify and to make enriched options easier to jus-
online version of the article at http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/
tify. If perceived justification indeed mediates the effect of
1864-9335/a000302
the decision target on preferences for impoverished and
enriched options, self–other differences in preferences ESM 1. Data (.sav).
should diminish because enriched options are more Raw data for Study 1.
justifiable in this condition. ESM 2. Data (.sav).
Last, although people who make choices for themselves Raw data for Study 2.
do not need to provide reasons to others in some cases, ESM 3. Data (.sav).
they should still justify their choices to themselves. Raw data for the Post-Study.
Therefore, the difference between justifying to oneself
and to another in preferences for impoverished options is
a topic worth investigating. References
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D.
(2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychol-
Practical Implications ogy, 5, 323–370. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323
The results presented in the current research are valuable Beattie, J., Baron, J., Hershey, J. C., & Spranca, M. (1994).
Psychological determinants of decision attitude. Journal of
for both marketers and managers. For example, clothes Behavioral Decision Making, 7, 129–144. doi: 10.1002/bdm.
with extraordinary styles (i.e., enriched options) should be 3960070206
targeted at consumers who purchase clothes for themselves Briley, D. A., Morris, M. W., & Simonson, I. (2000). Reasons as
because such options are distinctive, but not accepted by carriers of culture: Dynamic versus dispositional models of
cultural influence on decision making. Journal of Consumer
everyone. By contrast, clothes with conservative styles Research, 27, 157–178. doi: 10.1086/314318
(i.e., impoverished options) should be targeted at con- Chang, C.-C., Chuang, S.-C., Cheng, Y.-H., & Huang, T.-Y. (2011).
sumers who select clothes for other people because these The compromise effect in choosing for others. Journal of
options, although common, are easily accepted by most Behavioral Decision Making, 25, 109–122. doi: 10.1002/
bdm.720
people. That is, marketers should pay attention to the Custódio, C., Ferreira, M. A., & Matos, P. (2013). Generalists
decision targets of their customers to promote sales. versus specialists: Lifetime work experience and chief execu-
Furthermore, in the job market, some job seekers are tive officer pay. Journal of Financial Economic, 108, 471–492.
generalists who perform averagely in various tasks, whereas doi: 10.1016/j.jfineco.2013.01.001
Danziger, S., Montal, R., & Barkan, R. (2012). Idealistic advice
others are specialists who do well in certain tasks but and pragmatic choice: A psychological distance account.
poorly in others. Research has found that small companies Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 1105–1117.
benefit from generalists, whereas large companies benefit doi: 10.1037/a0027013
from specialists (Pugh, Hickson, Hinings, & Turner, Dore, R. A., Stone, E. R., & Buchanan, C. M. (2014). A social values
analysis of parental decision making. The Journal of Psychol-
1968). Our results indicate that small organizations may ogy, 148, 477–504. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2013.808603
outsource recruitment to headhunters because head- Drolet, A., Luce, M. F., & Simonson, I. (2009). When does choice
hunters, who make decisions for others, prefer generalists. reveal preference? Moderators of heuristic versus goal-based

Social Psychology (2017), 48(2), 92–103 Ó 2017 Hogrefe Publishing


J. Lu et al., Justifying Decisions 103

choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 36, 137–147. Pugh, D., Hickson, D. J., Hinings, C. R., & Turner, C. (1968).
doi: 10.1086/596305 Dimensions of organization structure. Administrative Science
Ganzach, Y. (1995). Attribute scatter and decision outcome: Quarterly, 13, 65–105. doi: 10.2307/2391262
Judgment versus choice. Organizational Behavior and Human Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity
Decision Processes, 62, 113–122. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1995.1036 dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology
Hong, J., & Chang, H. H. (2015). “I” follow my heart and “we” rely Review, 5, 296–320. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0504_2
on reasons: The impact of self-construal on reliance on feelings Shafir, E. (1993). Choosing versus rejecting: Why some options are
versus reasons in decision making. Journal of Consumer both better and worse than others. Memory and Cognition, 21,
Research, 41, 1392–1411. doi: 10.1086/680082 546–556. doi: 10.3758/BF03197186
Hsee, C. K. (1996). The evaluability hypothesis: An explanation for Shafir, E., Simonson, I., & Tversky, A. (1993). Reason-based choice.
preference reversals between joint and separate evaluations of Cognition, 49, 11–36. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(93)90034-S
alternatives. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Simon, H. A. (1990). Invariants of human behavior. Annual Review
Processes, 67, 247–257. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1996.0077 of Psychology, 41, 1–19. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.41.020190.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis 000245
of decision under risk. Econometrica: Journal of the Econo- Simonson, I. (1989). Choice based on reasons: The case of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

metric Society, 47, 263–291. doi: 10.2307/1914185 attraction and compromise effects. Journal of Consumer
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Laran, J. (2010). Goal management in sequential choices: Research, 16, 158–174. doi: 10.1086/209205
Consumer choices for others are more indulgent than personal Simonson, I., & Nowlis, S. M. (2000). The role of explanations and
choices. Journal of Consumer Research, 37, 304–314. need for uniqueness in consumer decision making: Unconven-
doi: 10.1086/652193 tional choices based on reasons. Journal of Consumer
Lerner, J. S., & Tetlock, P. E. (1999). Accounting for the effects Research, 27, 49–68. doi: 10.1086/314308
of accountability. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 255–257. Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and
doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.255 interdependent self-construals. Personality and Social Psy-
Lu, J., Jia, H., Xie, X., & Wang, Q. (2016). Missing the best chology Bulletin, 20, 580–591. doi: 10.1177/0146167294205014
opportunity; who can seize the next one? Agents show less Steffel, M., Williams, E. F., & Perrmann-Graham, J. (2016).
inaction inertia than personal decision makers. Journal of Passing the buck: Delegating choices to others to avoid
Economic Psychology, 54, 100–112. doi: 10.1016/j.joep. responsibility and blame. Organizational Behavior and Human
2016.03.004 Decision Processes, 135, 32–44. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2016.
Lu, J., & Xie, X. (2014). To change or not to change: A matter of 04.006
decision maker’s role. Organizational Behavior and Human Stone, E. R., Choi, Y., de Bruin, W. B., & Mandel, D. R. (2013). I can
Decision Processes, 124, 47–55. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp. take the risk, but you should be safe: Self-other differences
2013.12.001 in situations involving physical safety. Judgment and Decision
Lu, J., Xie, X., & Xu, J. (2013). Desirability or feasibility: Self–other Making, 8, 250–267.
decision-making differences. Personality and Social Psychology Tetlock, P. E., & Boettger, R. (1994). Accountability amplifies the
Bulletin, 39, 144–155. doi: 10.1177/0146167212470146 status quo effect when change creates victims. Journal of
Maimaran, M., & Simonson, I. (2011). Multiple routes to self- Behavioral Decision Making, 7, 1–23. doi: 10.1002/bdm.
versus other-expression in consumer choice. Journal of 3960070102
Marketing Research, 48, 755–766. doi: 10.1509/jmkr.48.4.755 Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1986). Rational choice and
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: the framing of decisions. Journal of Business, 59, 251–278.
Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psycholog- doi: 10.1086/296365
ical Review, 98, 224–253. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224 Wagenmakers, E.-J., Krypotps, A.-M., Criss, A. H., & Iverson, G.
Mishra, S. (2014). Decision-making under risk: Integrating per- (2012). On the interpretation of removable interations: A survey
spectives from biology, economics, and psychology. Personality of the field 33 years after Loftus. Memory and Cognition, 40,
and Social Psychology Review, 18, 280–307. doi: 10.1177/ 145–160. doi: 10.3758/s13421-011-0158-0
1088868314530517 Wang, L., & Murnighan, J. K. (2013). The generalist bias. Organi-
Mishra, S., Barclay, P., & Lalumière, M. L. (2014). Competitive zational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 120, 47–61.
disadvantage facilitates risk taking. Evolution and Human doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2012.09.001
Behavior, 35, 126–132. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav. Zhang, Y., & Mittal, V. (2007). The attractiveness of enriched and
2013.11.006 impoverished options: Culture, self-construal, and regulatory
Neumann, N., Böckenholt, U., & Sinha, A. (2016). A meta-analysis focus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 588–598.
of extremeness aversion. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 26, doi: 10.1177/0146167206296954
193–212. doi: 10.1016/j.jcps.2015.05.005
O’Brien, E., & Kardas, M. (2016). The implicit meaning of (my)
Received July 12, 2016
change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111,
Revision received October 27, 2016
882–894. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000073
Accepted October 31, 2016
Polman, E. (2012a). Effects of self–other decision making on
Published online April 28, 2017
regulatory focus and choice overload. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 102, 980–993. doi: 10.1037/a0026966
Polman, E. (2012b). Self–other decision making and loss aversion. Jingyi Lu
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 119, School of Psychology and Cognitive Science
141–150. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2012.06.005 East China Normal University
Polman, E., & Vohs, K. D. (2016). Decision fatigue, choosing for 200062 Shanghai
others, and self-construal. Social Psychological and Personality PR China
Science, 7, 471–478. doi: 10.1177/1948550616639648 jylu@psy.ecnu.edu.cn

Ó 2017 Hogrefe Publishing Social Psychology (2017), 48(2), 92–103

You might also like