Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Computers in Human Behavior 42 (2015) 47–56

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Computers in Human Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh

An analysis of shared leadership, diversity, and team creativity


in an e-learning environment
Dae Sung Lee a, Kun Chang Lee b,⇑, Young Wook Seo c,⇑, Do Young Choi d
a
SKKU Business School, Sungkyunkwan University, Seoul 110-745, Republic of Korea
b
SKKU Business School, Sungkyunkwan University, Seoul 110-745, Republic of Korea
c
Software Engineering Center National IT Industry Promotion Agency, Seoul 138-711, Republic of Korea
d
Solution Business Unit, LG CNS Co., Ltd., Seoul 100-725, Republic of Korea

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: In response to rapid change and fierce competition, creativity is an imperative factor to develop and
Available online 11 March 2014 implement innovation. Hence, most firms have pursued diverse strategies to promote individual and
team creativity in the workplace. Shared leadership is a voluntarily, informally emergent structure
Keywords: beyond vertical leadership. A team is composed of individual members, and shared leadership and demo-
Shared leadership graphic diversity exist within the team, influencing team creativity. In this respect, we introduced shared
Demographic diversity leadership as a social network perspective as well as diversity into a team creativity model. In sum, we
Knowledge sharing
examined the influence of shared leadership and diversity on knowledge sharing and the subsequent
Team creativity
effects on team creativity. Our results showed that role diversity directly influences team creativity, with
shared leadership and knowledge sharing positively contributing to team creativity. Thus, knowledge
sharing had a partially mediating role between shared leadership and team creativity. Apart from our
hypotheses, the present results implied that if gender diversity (as a differentiated factor) is not a minor-
ity status, knowledge sharing may have a fully mediating effect between gender diversity and team
creativity.
Ó 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction accentuate shared leadership, which is not imposed on a single


designated leader, but is distributed among team members (Car-
In order to acquire an organizational competitive advantage and son, Tesluk, & Marrone, 2007; Pearce & Conger, 2003). A single lea-
respond to rapidly changing environments, the development and der may not successfully carry out all necessary leadership
implementation of creativity and innovation is essential to today’s functions because the environment has inherent complexity and
turbulent business environment (Lapierre & Giroux, 2003). Many ambiguity (Day, Gronn, & Salas, 2004). Shared leadership repre-
organizations have selected team-based work systems to increase sents mutual influences among team members, which can over-
their responsiveness and ability to foster innovation. Such organi- come the limitation of a leadership style by a single leader.
zations need to be concerned not only with enhancing creativity Therefore, as a type of horizontal, internal team leadership, shared
and innovation among individual employees, but also with devel- leadership can contribute to team creativity. In this respect, we
oping creative, innovative teams. Relatively fewer studies have choose shared leadership as an antecedent, and examine the rela-
been conducted on ‘‘team creativity’’ as compared with individual tionship between shared leadership and team creativity with a so-
creativity. In this study, we explore the effects of shared leadership, cial network perspective by using social network density.
demographic diversity, and knowledge sharing on team creativity. In modern organizations, many workers of different back-
Generally, leadership represents itself as a single designated grounds work together, and diversity is a domain that researchers
individual. Some scholars (Pearce & Conger, 2003; Pearce & Sims, and practitioners cannot disregard. Women and minorities have
2002) have argued that leadership includes shared roles and activ- become more significant in the workforce (Loden & Rosener,
ities among members of a team. As a team property, we seek to 1991; Offerman & Gowing, 1990), and there is an enhanced need
for employees of different occupational backgrounds to work
together (Dean & Snell, 1991). Due to the importance of
⇑ Corresponding authors. Tel.: +82 760 0505. team-based approaches in organizations, our study focuses on
E-mail addresses: leeds1122@gmail.com (D.S. Lee), kunchanglee@gmail.com
heterogeneity at the group level. Diversity (interchangeable with
(K.C. Lee), seoyy123@gmail.com (Y.W. Seo), dychoi96@gmail.com (D.Y. Choi).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.10.064
0747-5632/Ó 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
48 D.S. Lee et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 42 (2015) 47–56

heterogeneity) is particularly important at the group level, where they determine one’s technical skills, expertise, knowledge, and
individuals interact more regularly than at the organizational level so on. Other researchers (Milliken & Martins, 1996; Pelled, 1996;
(Jackson et al., 1991; Parker, 1994). In this sense, we emphasize Sessa & Jackson, 1995) have similarly asserted that tenure and
demographic attributes (e.g., age, race, gender, education, func- functional background are particularly relevant to work group
tional background, and tenure) within teams, investigating the tasks.
implications of demographic diversity for team creativity.
Knowledge-based systems force the members of an organiza- 2.2. Shared leadership
tion to extend their work scopes and establish autonomy (DeNisi,
Hitt, & Jackson, 2003). Therefore, knowledge sharing may act as a With the presence of several formally appointed or emergent
mediator within our team creativity model. Additionally, we as- leaders, leadership may be considered as a shared and distributed
sume that there are moderating effects of task complexity (high-le- phenomenon (Mehra, Smith, Dixon, & Robertson, 2006). In this
vel task variety and low-level task analyzability) between shared sense, shared leadership is represented by distributed influence
leadership and knowledge sharing. Task complexity is significantly within a team and lateral influence among peers (Pearce & Sims,
related to knowledge sharing (Phang & Foong, 2005). In an e-learn- 2002). Also, shared leadership is regarded as an emergent team
ing environment, task complexity might also directly (positively or property resulting from the distribution of leadership influence
negatively) influence knowledge sharing. However, when team across multiple team members (Carson et al., 2007). It represents
members complexly perceive tasks, an interactive mechanism a condition of mutual influence embedded in the interactions of
through shared leadership might be positively related to knowl- team members that significantly improve team and organizational
edge sharing. Namely, at the high level of perceived task complex- performances (Day et al., 2004). As shared leadership is defined as
ity, team members may mutually depend on the leadership of a relational phenomenon involving mutual influence among team
other members. members, social network theory provides a natural theoretical
We arranged 40 teams consisting of four to eight members in an and analytical approach to studying the relational influence struc-
e-learning environment, identifying and examining the constructs tures of teams (Mehra et al., 2006). Although there are a few useful
that are most closely related to team creativity. We first review the self-reported ratings (Avolio, Jung, & Sivasubramaniam, 1996;
selected constructs and their relationships based on previous stud- Pearce & Sims, 2002; Pearce, Yoo, & Alavi, 2004), we measured
ies. Second, we undertake assessments of the discriminant and shared leadership using a social network approach (Carson et al.,
convergent validities of these relationships. Lastly, we examine 2007; Mayo, Meindl, & Pastor, 2003). In this measurement, shared
our hypotheses using hierarchical regression analysis. leadership is a measure (density) of the total amount of leadership
displayed by team members as perceived by others on a team.

2. Literature review and hypotheses 2.3. Knowledge sharing

2.1. Demographic diversity Knowledge sharing is defined as an interactional process that is


significantly related to group performance (Nelson, Sabatier, & Nel-
Demographic diversity refers to the degree to which a working son, 1996). Knowledge sharing occurs when an individual spreads
unit is heterogeneous regarding demographic attributes. It gener- his or her acquired knowledge to other members within an organi-
ally includes immutable characteristics such as age, gender, and zation (Ryu, Ho, & Han, 2003). Factors that influence an individual’s
ethnicity (Pelled, Eisenhardt, & Xin, 1999). In other words, it willingness to share knowledge include costs and benefits, incen-
represents individuals’ relationships with organizations, such as tive systems, extrinsic and intrinsic motivations, organizational cli-
organizational tenure or functional area. Moreover, it identifies mate, and management championship (e.g., Bock & Kim, 2002;
individuals’ positions within society, such as marital status (Law- Bock, Zmud, Kim, & Lee, 2005; Kankanhalli, Tan, & Wei, 2005; Pur-
rence, 1997). The attention of demographic diversity started off vis, Sambamurthy, & Zmud, 2001; Wasko & Faraj, 2005). Some
with both women and minorities in the workforce (Buhler, researchers have examined team characteristics and processes in
1997). The concern intensified the need for organizational relation to knowledge sharing. For example, the longer a team
strategies that consider more interaction among employees with has been together and the higher the level of team cohesiveness,
different functional backgrounds (e.g., Dean & Snell, 1991). the more likely it is that team members will share knowledge (Bak-
Diversity in regards to age, gender, and race is lowly related to ker, Leenders, Gabbay, Kratzer, & Van Engelen, 2006; Sawng, Kim,
job, but age similarity may not yield similarity in general attitudes & Han, 2006). De Vries, Van den Hooff, and De Ridder (2006) inves-
about technical work. Race involves a wide collection of experi- tigated team communication styles, agreeable and extravert styles,
ences, such as traditions, lessons from parents and teachers, and and found that they were positively associated with knowledge
treatment from social activities. Sessa and Jackson (1995) found sharing willingness and behaviors. Srivastava, Bartol, and Locke
that age, race, and gender form the context of more general social (2006) showed that empowering leadership fostered knowledge
relationships, and are less directly associated with team objectives. sharing among members in management teams of hotels. Thus,
Work experiences may only be a fraction of the total set of experi- intensified research on leadership styles and constructs at the team
ences it captures. Therefore, age, race, and gender may differ with level is justified.
tenure and team membership.
In this respect, tenure and functional background may have a 2.4. Team creativity
stronger impact on perceptions of working group tasks as com-
pared with age, race, and gender. In other words, tenure and func- Creativity is generally defined as the ability to yield works that
tional background are highly related to one’s job, representing how are both novel and useful (Lubart, 1994). While creativity at the
long one has worked for a company or has been exposed to a spe- individual level is pertinent to a situation in which a person solves
cific functional area. During the period, the experience and knowl- problems in a job or in daily life, creativity at the social level can
edge obtained are typically required for cognitive tasks performed lead to new scientific findings, movements in art, inventions, and
in organizations. Ancona and David (1992) pointed out that tenure social programs (Sternberg & Lubart, 1999). Therefore, team crea-
and functional background might be particularly important for tivity is a comprehensive concept that includes new and useful
tasks within units, such as product development teams, because ideas, processes, and procedures in an interactional working
D.S. Lee et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 42 (2015) 47–56 49

environment (Woodman, Sawyer, & Griffin, 1993). Amabile, Conti, Hunt et al., 2003). On the basis of these studies, the following
Coon, Lazenby, and Herron (1996) extended the scope of creativity hypotheses are proposed:
research from its origins at the individual level to the group or so-
cial-psychological level and, eventually, to the organizational level. Hypothesis 1a. Diversity in age is negatively related to knowledge
At the team level, an allowance should be made for situational and sharing.
structural factors as well as personal characteristics and cognitive
abilities (Sternberg & Lubart, 1995; Woodman et al., 1993). As a Hypothesis 1b. Diversity in gender is negatively related to
working unit of decision-making, a team utilizes various resources knowledge sharing.
to achieve its goal. Knowledge, technology, and expertise are in- Hypothesis 1c. Diversity in major is positively related to knowl-
cluded into these necessary resources (Brand, 1998). On these edge sharing.
grounds, we propose that demographic diversity, shared leader-
ship, and knowledge sharing influence team creativity. Hypothesis 1d. Diversity in role is positively related to knowledge
sharing.
2.5. Task variety and analyzability
2.6.2. Shared leadership and knowledge sharing
As a type of complex task classification, our study posits that
There have been many studies about the relationship between
tasks have been labeled as problem tasks because they involve find-
leadership and knowledge sharing. Contrary to empowering lead-
ing the best way to achieve a specific outcome. Thus, complexity is a
ership, autocratic leadership hinders team members from sharing
function of multiple potential paths to the desired result (Campbell,
knowledge (Yukl, 2002). In terms of team communication styles,
1988). A high degree of task complexity entails ambiguity and dif-
an agreeable style affects team members’ willingness to share their
ficulties that might require new knowledge or novel solutions (Ak-
knowledge, whereas an extraverted communication style within a
gun, Byrne, Keskin, Lynn, & Imamoglu, 2005). Moreover, more
team is positively related to both eagerness and willingness to
complex tasks require cooperation and coordination among team
share (De Vries et al., 2006). Therefore, shared leadership, or lead-
members (Akgun et al., 2005). The characteristics of task complex-
ership distributed among team members, may be positively associ-
ity may be separated into two main perspectives: (a) characteristics
ated with knowledge sharing. From these findings, the following
related to the a priori determinability of tasks, and (b) characteris-
hypothesis is proposed:
tics related to the extent of tasks. In this study, we selected ‘‘task
analyzability’’ to reflect the a priori determinability of tasks and
Hypothesis 2. Shared leadership is positively related to knowledge
‘‘task variety’’ to represent the extent of tasks. High-level task vari-
sharing.
ety and low-level analyzability represent the high degree of task
complexity. If tasks are related to the transmission of complex
knowledge within teams, strong ties and accurate cognitive net- 2.6.3. The moderating effect between shared leadership and knowledge
works among team members might be helpful to achieve the goals. sharing
Therefore, as task complexity increases, team members are ex- In terms of two underlying dimensions, a unit’s task complexity
pected to spend more time gathering or exchanging knowledge. may be categorized into variety and analyzability. Task variety rep-
On the other hand, if task complexity is not measured by an objec- resents the number and frequency of exceptional, unexpected, or
tive criterion, but ‘‘perceived’’ by team members, it might represent novel events that occur in the task process (Song, 2008). With a
the presence of weak interactions within teams. high level of task variety, employees are expected to spend a larger
amount of time gathering or exchanging knowledge in order to re-
2.6. Research hypotheses spond to unexpected events as well as to acquire knowledge from
others (Song, 2008). Task analyzability refers to the way individu-
2.6.1. Demographic diversity and knowledge sharing als are able to respond to problems that arise in the process of task
Race and gender are generally thought to be innate in nature, completion. When task processes are analyzable, employees typi-
and nobody can stop or lengthen a person’s age and tenure. There- cally follow objective and computational procedures to respond
fore, age, race, gender, and tenure are not easily permeated. In con- to problems. Analyzable tasks also have more rules and procedures
trast, functional background is more penetrable. In organizations, (Song, 2008). Non-routine tasks are more difficult to analyze in
employees frequently move from one functional area to another terms of alternative courses of action, costs, benefits, and out-
in voluntary or compulsory ways. Many companies make employ- comes. In our study, the same assignment was given to each team,
ees experience both technical and business positions in order to as we seek to distinguish perceived task complexity at the team le-
help them develop their skills (Ryan, 1991). Previous studies indi- vel from objective one. In this respect, if the level of perceived task
cate that the minority status or diversity of team members relates complexity is high at the team level, then team members mutually
to knowledge sharing. According to the similarity-attraction para- depend upon the leadership of others on the team. On the basis of
digm, team members are less likely to engage in sharing knowl- these studies, the following hypotheses are proposed:
edge with team members when they perceive themselves as
minorities based on gender, marital status, or education (Ojha, Hypothesis 3a. Perceived task variety enhances the positive
2005). In the R&D teams of large organizations, a higher proportion associations between shared leadership and knowledge sharing
of women in the workforce more likely enables team members to within teams.
engage in knowledge sharing (Sawng et al., 2006). Some studies
have been conducted on the role of social connections with other
Hypothesis 3b. Perceived task analyzability reduces the positive
group members in knowledge sharing (Phillips, Mannix, Neale, &
associations between shared leadership and knowledge sharing
Gruenfeld, 2004; Thomas-Hunt, Ogden, & Neale, 2003), with the
in teams.
results indicating that socially isolated members are more likely
to disagree with others and contribute their unique knowledge
within a heterogeneous team. On the other hand, within a func- 2.6.4. Demographic diversity and team creativity
tionally varied team, it is important for team members to acknowl- Some researchers have found that age diversity reduces conflict
edge other members’ expertise for knowledge sharing (Thomas- at the group level and increases members’ satisfaction with and
50 D.S. Lee et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 42 (2015) 47–56

commitment to the group (Jehn, Northcraft, & Neale, 1999; Pelled facilitator of creative collaboration (Mamykina, Candy, & Edmonds,
et al., 1999). As mentioned above, age homogeneity with a shared 2002). Therefore, from a resource-based perspective, knowledge
life history might produce a feeling of camaraderie and comfort in sharing will lead to knowledge accumulation, which fosters team
sharing ideas. Namely, diversity in age might be negatively related creativity. These findings suggest the following hypothesis:
to knowledge sharing. However, age is an attribute used for social
comparison in terms of achievement (Chattopadhyay, George, & Hypothesis 6. Knowledge sharing is positively related to team
Lawrence, 2004). Some scholars have shown that age, gender, creativity.
and race result in high or low status and the competence expecta-
tions of a particular individual (Chattopadhyay, 1999; Tsui, Porter,
& Egan, 2002). Team members generally compete with each other 3. Methods
for the same resources (e.g., promotion, reward) so as to outper-
form when they have the same life and career development. This 3.1. Sample
competitive performance pressure might enable team members
to yield new and useful ideas, processes, and procedures (Morrison Empirical data were gathered through the use of a question-
& Phelps, 1999). For this reason, age similarity might enhance naire survey. The participants were undergraduate students who
members’ voluntary efforts to implement their tasks with creativ- had taken a web-based e-learning course called ‘‘Digital Informa-
ity (LePine & Van Dyne, 2001). tion Technology and its Application’’ at a university in South Korea.
In contrast, functional diversity might bring about informa- To secure unbiased, self-motivated participation, we announced
tional or idea diversity, which in turn may influence team creativ- that bonus credit would be given to students who submitted ques-
ity (Jehn, 1997). Functional diversity generally increases group tionnaires without omitting answers or apparently skewing the
performance, particularly in decision-making tasks (Williams & distribution of answers. We organized 40 teams consisting of four
O’Reilly, 1998). Therefore, a team with greater functional diversity to eight members and gave each team an assignment that required
may stimulate more creative behavior among its members. Under creativity, notifying the participants that the assignment would
differentiated performance evaluations (high heterogeneity), there have a critical effect on their grade. To avoid selection bias, we pro-
may be a more performance-oriented climate for team members, vided questionnaires to all students in the course. We yielded 249
which will in turn encourage individuals into high performance useable cases (78%). Table 1 presents the characteristics of the
(Kluger & DeNisi, 1996). This might stimulate creativity. On the ba- sample.
sis of these studies, we proposed the following hypotheses:
3.2. Measures
Hypothesis 4a. Diversity in age is negatively related to team
creativity. 3.2.1. Demographic diversity
Participants were asked to provide categorical answers about
Hypothesis 4b. Diversity in gender is negatively related to team age, gender, major, and role. The use of categorical demographic
creativity. variables at the team level is designed to avoid problems of bias
stemming from individual-based data aggregated to the level of
Hypothesis 4c. Diversity in major is positively related to team cre- the team. We adopted an approach suggested by Pfeffer and O’Reil-
ativity. ly (1987), in which an entropy-based diversity index is calculated
Hypothesis 4d. Diversity in role is positively related to team by the following equation:
creativity. X
n
H¼ Piðln PiÞ; ð1Þ
i¼1
2.6.5. Shared leadership and team creativity
where n is the number of possible categories, and Pi is the fractional
Much contemporary research on creativity has been guided by P
share of ith category with Pi = 1.
intrinsic motivation theory (Amabile, 1996; Oldham & Cummings,
1996; Shalley, 1995; Zhou, 1998). Intrinsic motivation is a critical
3.2.2. Shared leadership
factor in creativity, and is often considered the mechanism by
This study emphasizes a social network approach (Carson et al.,
which situational factors such as leadership contribute to creativ-
2007; Mayo et al., 2003) that uses density, a measure of the total
ity (Oldham & Cummings, 1996). Transformational leadership pos-
amount of leadership displayed by team members as perceived
itively contributes to follower creativity (Shin & Zhou, 2003).
by others on the team. Eq. (2) shows how to calculate density for
However, previous researchers have generally focused on the anal-
shared leadership:
ysis of vertical leadership and individual creativity. We selected
shared leadership, which is an emergent team property, as another Density ¼ S=7NðN  1Þ: ð2Þ
kind of leadership in our analysis of team creativity. Shared
In this equation, S is the sum of all values that team members would
leadership is also a relational phenomenon that involves mutual
rate each other for leadership. N equals the number of team
influence among team members (Mehra et al., 2006). On the basis
members; N (N  1) is the total number of possible ties in a team.
of these studies, we proposed the following hypothesis:
The number 7 represents the maximum value rated by a peer in a
team.
Hypothesis 5. Shared leadership is positively related to team
creativity.
3.2.3. Self-reported constructs (knowledge sharing, team creativity,
task variety, and task analyzability)
2.6.6. Knowledge sharing and team creativity Each item in our constructs was measured on a seven-point Lik-
Employees share their ideas with others to further develop ert scale, with answers ranging from ‘‘strongly disagree’’ to
those ideas and to facilitate creativity (Oldham, 2003). An effective ‘‘strongly agree’’ (see Table 2). The items in the survey were devel-
working relationship exists when team members exchange knowl- oped by adapting existing measures validated by other researchers
edge resources to foster progress and to resolve difficulties of both or by converting the definitions of the construct into a question-
technical and artistic natures. Sharing knowledge is an important naire format.
D.S. Lee et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 42 (2015) 47–56 51

Table 1
Sample characteristics.

Characteristics Frequency Percent Characteristics Frequency Percent


Age Major
20 and below 142 57 Natural science 33 13.3
21–22 30 12 Engineering 31 12.4
23–24 43 17.3 Economics and management 72 28.9
25–26 25 10 Social science 50 20.1
27–28 8 3.2 Liberal arts 54 21.7
29 and over 1 0.4 Language and literature 9 3.6
Grade Gender
Freshman 147 59 Male 167 67.1
Sophomore 40 16.1 Female 82 32.9
Junior 22 8.8
Senior 40 16.1
Total 249 100 Total 249 100

3.2.4. Controls ment. Compilation implies that there are obvious differences
We controlled for task variety and analyzability (as moderators between aggregated and nonaggregated data (LeBreton & Senter,
described above) because previous researchers have found that the 2008). Therefore, we considered both IRA and IRR for team creativ-
nature of group tasks often influences group interactions and per- ity and used only IRA for knowledge sharing and task complexity
formance (e.g., Weingart, 1992). Also, when testing for moderating (TV and TA) so as to judge appropriateness. In this sense, the value
effects, it is necessary to control for the main effect of the moder- of ICC (1) for team creativity showed that group membership
ating variable. Group size was included as a control variable in our greatly influenced judges’ ratings. On the other hand, the value
study because exiting literature on groups indicates that size is an (>.7) of ICC (K) for team creativity as a compilation implied that
important variable affecting group dynamics and performance, and the assessments were reliable.
because larger teams have more potential for diversity (Jackson
et al., 1991). 3.2.6. Data analysis – confirmatory factor analysis
To validate our measurement model, we undertook validity
3.2.5. Data analysis – interrater agreement (IRA) and interrater assessments of content and discriminant and convergent validities
reliability (IRR) by employing partial least squares (PLS). The content validity of
We tested whether assessments were appropriate using the our survey was established from the existing literature, and our
within-rater agreement statistic (rwg) (James, Demaree, & Wolf, measures were constructed by adopting constructs validated by
1993) and used intraclass correlation coefficients, ICC (1) and ICC other researchers.
(K), to estimate raters’ similarity (McGraw & Wong, 1996). For The discriminant validity of our instrument was assessed by
the items of self-reported constructs, rwg, ICC (1) and ICC (K) were examining correlations among questions on the survey. The rule
calculated. As shown in Table 3, each rwg of the constructs indi- of thumb in discriminant validity is that a measure should corre-
cated moderate agreement (>.5) according to LeBreton and Senter’s late with all measures of the same construct more highly than it
guideline (2008). In other words, there was a high level of agree- does with any measures of other constructs (Chin, 1998). As shown
ment among judges in regards to rating the internal team environ- in Table 4, our composite reliability values (except demographic

Table 2
Construct and measurement.

Construct Items Measurement Related literature


a
Shared SL  To what degree does your team rely on this individual for leadership? Carson et al. (2007), and Mayo et al. (2003)
leadership
Knowledge KS1  People in this team are willing to share knowledge/ideas with others Staples and Webster (2008), and Connelly and
sharing KS2  People in this team share their ideas openly Kelloway (2003)
KS3  People in this team with expert knowledge are willing to help others in this
team
KS4  This team is good at using the knowledge/ideas of employees
Team creativity TC1  Our work unit comes up with many new ideas about how work should be done Song, Nerur, and Teng (2007), and Gibson and
TC2  If a new way of doing work is introduced, it often comes from within the work Vermeulen (2003)
unit
TC3  Our work unit is frequently the source of ideas that are copied by other work
units
Task variety TV1  When a problem arises, it takes a lot of experience and training to know what to Song (2008)
do
TV2  There is variety in the events that cause the work
TV3  Tasks in my work unit require an extensive and demanding search for a solution
Task TA1  We need to know a lot of procedures and standard practices to do our work well
analyzability TA2  Normal work activities in our jobs are guided by standard directives, rules and
procedures
TA3  Established materials (professional books, directives, manuals, statutes) cover
our work
a
SL: a measure of the total amount of leadership displayed by team members as perceived by others on a team.
52 D.S. Lee et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 42 (2015) 47–56

Table 3 From Model 3 (results presented in Table 6), we found a signif-


IRA and IRR. icant interactive effect between shared leadership and task variety
Self-reported constructs rwg ICC (1) ICC(K) (beta = 1.24, p < .05). We plotted this interaction in Fig. 1 from
Team creativity (TC) 0.59 0.18 0.84 the split-sample regression analysis. As shown, when task variety
Knowledge sharing (KS) 0.55 0.27 0.92 is high, there is a positive relationship between shared leadership
Task variety (TV) 0.62 0.13 0.79 and knowledge sharing (simple slope estimate = 3.76, p < .01). On
Task analyzability (TA) 0.60 0.03 0.42 the other hand, when task variety is low, this relationship
strengthens to a significant level (simple slope estimate = 6.84,
p < .01). Therefore, Hypothesis 3a (perceived task variety enhances
diversity and shared leadership, which have a single measure) ran- the positive associations between shared leadership and knowledge
ged from 0.886 to 0.949, above the acceptable levels. For variance sharing within teams) was not supported.
extracted by measures, a score of 0.5 indicates acceptability in the The results for Model 4 are presented in Table 6. We found a sig-
Table 5. nificant interactive effect between shared leadership and task ana-
lyzability (beta = 1.42, p < .1). We plotted this interaction in Fig. 2
from the split-sample regression analysis. As shown, when task
4. Results analyzability is high, there is a positive relationship between
shared leadership and knowledge sharing (simple slope
When evaluating the significance of predicted effects, we used estimate = 4.60, p < .01). When task analyzability is low, this
one-tailed tests, which are suitable for directional hypotheses. A relationship strengthens to a significant level (simple slope
structural equation model (SEM) was not an option for this study estimate = 5.51, p < .01). Therefore, Hypothesis 3b (perceived task
because it was inappropriate for our sample size (n = 40; Breckler, analyzability reduces the positive associations between shared leader-
1990). Therefore, we performed hierarchical regression analysis. ship and knowledge sharing in teams) was partially supported.
Table 6 presents the results of the regression analysis with The results of the regression analysis with team creativity as the
knowledge sharing as the dependent variable. Model 2 was used dependent variable are presented in Table 7. We did not fully find a
to test Hypotheses 1a and 1b, which state that diversity in age significant relationship between diversity and team creativity. In
and gender is negatively related to knowledge sharing, and other words, Hypotheses 4a, 4b, and 4d were not supported. Con-
Hypotheses 1c and 1d, which posit that diversity in major and role trary to Hypothesis 4a, age diversity directly and positively con-
is positively related to knowledge sharing. Unexpectedly, the rela- tributed to team creativity (beta = .22, p < .01). Consistent with
tionship between diversity in major and role and knowledge shar- Hypothesis 4c, role diversity directly influences team creativity in
ing was insignificant. Conversely, the relationship of diversity in Model 4 (beta = .15, p < .05). On the other hand, though gender
age and gender with knowledge sharing was positively significant diversity did not directly influence team creativity, as presented
(beta = .13, p < .1; beta = .17, p < .05 respectively). Therefore, in Table 7, it had a positive association with knowledge sharing,
Hypotheses 1a, 1b, 1c, and 1d were not supported. On the other as presented in Table 6. Therefore, knowledge sharing might have
hand, consistent with Hypothesis 2, shared leadership had a signif- a fully mediating effect between gender diversity and team
icant and positive association with knowledge sharing (beta = .57, creativity.
p < .01).

Table 4
Confirmatory factor analysis.

Constructs Items Factor loading t-Value Cronbach’s Alpha Composite reliability AVE
Age diversity (AD) AD NA NA NA NA NA
Gender diversity (GD) GD
Major diversity (MD) MD
Role diversity (RD) RD
Shared leadership (SL) SL
Knowledge sharing (KS) KS1 0.913 42.614 0.929 0.949 0.824
KS2 0.898 30.587
KS3 0.919 35.832
KS4 0.900 17.967
Team creativity (TC) TC1 0.929 31.348 0.919 0.949 0.861
TC2 0.928 28.070
TC3 0.926 37.414
Task variety (TV) TV1 0.826 12.026 0.844 0.906 0.763
TV2 0.873 7.889
TV3 0.920 16.950
Task analyzability (TA) TA1 0.836 12.352 0.806 0.886 0.721
TA2 0.831 10.097
TA3 0.879 14.525
SL  task variety SLTV1 0.931 4.016 0.957 0.972 0.921
SLTV2 0.981 9.463
SLTV3 0.966 7.962
SL  task analyzability SLTA1 0.898 3.132 0.917 0.947 0.857
SLTA2 0.942 4.530
SLTA3 0.936 4.888

Notes:
1. AVE = average variance extracted.
2. NA = not applicable.
D.S. Lee et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 42 (2015) 47–56 53

Table 5
Correlation of latent variables.

Constructs AD GD MD RD SH KS TC TV TA SL  TV SL  TA
Age diversity (AD) N/A
Gender diversity (GD) 0.03 N/A
Major diversity (MD) 0.13 0.22 N/A
Role diversity (RD) 0.06 0.01 0.34 N/A
Shared leadership (SL) 0.06 0.05 0.22 0.13 N/A
Knowledge sharing (KS) 0.12 0.30 0.22 0.14 0.78 0.91
Team creativity (TC) 0.20 0.18 0.25 0.24 0.78 0.83 0.93
Task variety (TV) 0.03 0.26 0.34 0.10 0.56 0.63 0.65 0.87
Task analyzability (TA) 0.01 0.25 0.25 0.16 0.54 0.66 0.68 0.70 0.85
SL  task variety 0.00 0.07 0.22 0.18 0.42 0.48 0.51 0.51 0.42 0.96
SL  task analyzability 0.04 0.13 0.32 0.26 0.52 0.52 0.53 0.48 0.40 0.91 0.93

Note:
1. The number on the diagonal denotes the square root of average variance extracted.
2. For adequate discriminant validity, the diagonal elements should be greater than the corresponding off-diagonal elements.
3. NA = not applicable.

Table 6
Hierarchical regression analysis predicting knowledge sharing (N = 40).

Independent variables Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4


Controls
Group size 0.33*** 0.20** 0.23*** 0.20**
Task variety (TV) 0.44*** 0.19* 0.67** 0.18*
Task analyzability (TA) (reverse) 0.32** 0.17* 0.15* 0.76*
Predictors
Demographic diversity
Age diversity (AD) 0.13* 0.12* 0.14*
Gender diversity (GD) 0.17** 0.17** 0.15*
Major diversity (MD) 0.09 0.10 0.11
Role diversity (RD) 0.03 0.01 0.00
Shared leadership (SL) 0.57*** 1.48*** 1.58**
Interactions
SL  task variety (TV) 1.24**
SL  task analyzability (TA) (reverse) 1.42*
R-squared 0.61 0.80 0.81 0.81
F 18.67*** 15.25*** 14.65*** 14.23***

Note:
1. Entries are standardized regression coefficients. One-tailed tests reported.
*
p < .10.
**
p < .05.
***
p < .01.

Consistent with Hypotheses 5 and 6, shared leadership and 1997). In this sense, if age and gender do not represent minority
knowledge sharing positively contributed to team creativity status, they might be easily permeated into the group or organiza-
(beta = .36, p < .01; beta = .37, p < .05, respectively). Thus, knowl- tion. Moreover, previous studies have shown that age diversity
edge sharing has a partially mediating role between shared leader- could actually reduce conflict at the group level and increase mem-
ship and team creativity. bers’ satisfaction with and commitment to the group (Jehn et al.,
1999; Pelled et al., 1999). Thus, the positive effects of age diversity
might be justified in that our sample was gathered from students
5. Discussion in a college against minority group discrimination. Apart from
our hypotheses, our results showed that knowledge sharing might
5.1. Theoretical and managerial implications have a fully mediating effect between gender diversity and team
creativity. On the other hand, role diversity was found to directly
In this study, we did not find support for the hypothesized neg- influence team creativity in our study. This shows that a team with
ative effects of age and gender diversity on knowledge sharing and greater functional diversity can stimulate more creative behavior
team creativity. Instead, positive relationships were found be- among its members.
tween age and gender diversity and knowledge sharing in our The interactionist model of creativity (Woodman et al., 1993)
study. In addition, age diversity had a positive association with posits that individual creativity requires antecedent conditions,
team creativity. If age and gender represent minority status, they including cognitive styles and abilities, personality, motivational
might have negative impacts on job-related issues. Ojha (2005) factors, and knowledge. These individual factors are influenced
showed that team members who perceive themselves as a minor- by, and influence, both the social and contextual factors in the
ity were less likely to engage in knowledge sharing with team model. When individual creativity occurs, social influences on indi-
members. However, the concern with demographic diversity vidual creativity are established in the group at once, with the two
drives from organizational strategies (Dean & Snell, 1991) as well influencing one another (Woodman et al., 1993). Group creativity
as the presence of women and minorities in the workforce (Buhler, is affected by the factors representing group features, such as
54 D.S. Lee et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 42 (2015) 47–56

Table 7
Hierarchical regression analysis predicting team creativity (N = 40).

Independent variables Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4


Controls
Group size 0.18* 0.11 0.12 0.07
Task variety (TV) 0.46*** 0.43*** 0.25** 0.11
Task analyzability (TA) (reverse) 0.33** 0.39*** 0.18* 0.16*
Predictors
Demographic diversity
Age diversity (AD) 0.21** 0.22***
Gender diversity (GD) 0.02 0.02
Major diversity (MD) 0.07 0.07
Role diversity (RD) 0.14 0.15**
Shared leadership (SL) 0.52*** 0.36***
Knowledge sharing (KS) 0.37**
R-squared 0.55 0.60 0.71 0.81
F 14.87*** 6.98*** 21.60*** 14.35***

Note:
1. Entries are standardized regression coefficients. One-tailed tests reported.
*
p < .10.
**
p < .05.
***
p < .01.

Fig. 1. The interaction effect of shared leadership and task variety. on individual and team creativity in the interactionist model. On
the other hand, our results support the effect of knowledge sharing
on creativity as an important facilitator of creative collaboration.
Based on West’s (1990) model of team climate for innovation, we
verified that knowledge sharing is regarded as participative safety,
which is a part of team climate and refers to the sense that team
members can freely share ideas.
This study has important implications for team leaders and
managers. First, the findings suggest that organizations should
help raise shared leadership and knowledge sharing in order for
employees to enhance team creativity. Team leaders should
encourage team members to bring up shared leadership. Second,
if leaders utilize diversity as an organizational strategy, they must
consider functional diversity in order to increase team creativity.
Moreover, they should exclude negative diversities that could fos-
ter minority discrimination. Finally, given the need for team crea-
tivity in solving the complex challenges faced by organizations,
managers should ensure that each team has a clear, shared sense
of direction and purpose. When managers promote participation
in team activities and identify the organizational contexts, shared
leadership and knowledge sharing are most likely to positively
influence team creativity.

5.2. Limitations and future research

In our study, there are limitations to be addressed in future re-


Fig. 2. The interaction effect of shared leadership and task analyzability.
search. First, our sample included students in an e-learning course,
not full-time employees. Because team members who are full-time
employees work in different organizational settings than students
group composition, characteristics, and processes (e.g., diversity, in a classroom, the results might differ in an actual workplace. Con-
cohesiveness, group size, problem-solving strategies, and so on) sequently, future researchers should pursue comparative studies in
as well as contextual influences (Woodman et al., 1993). Thus, various environments. Second, we measured constructs when data
the interactionist model of organizational creativity (Woodman were collected after the team assignments were complete, but
et al., 1993) supports our results and interpretation. when considering shared leadership as an emergent phenomenon,
Shared leadership is a social influence that affects individual longitudinal designs should be implemented in order to illustrate
creativity. According to Amabile (see Avolio et al., 1996), intrinsic how shared leadership develops over time by looking at changes
motivation is a mechanism by which contextual factors influence in a leadership network across stages of team development (Carson
creativity. As an intrinsic motivator, leadership has been used in et al., 2007). Our cross-sectional design supports causal relation-
previous creativity studies. For example, Sosik, Kahai, and Avolio ships among the selected constructs. An important point for future
(1998) found that transformational leadership is positively related research is the long-term effects (i.e., whether team members’
to group creativity. As shared leadership is an emergent team reactions to constructs were temporary or permanent) of demo-
property and a relational phenomenon that involves mutual influ- graphic diversity, shared leadership, and knowledge sharing. Fu-
ences among team members, it is a social or contextual influence ture studies need to gather longitudinal data so as to examine
D.S. Lee et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 42 (2015) 47–56 55

the causality and interrelationships between variables. Finally, we James, L. R., Demaree, R. G., & Wolf, G. (1993). RWG: An assessment of within-group
interrater agreement. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78(2), 306–309.
did not make allowance for many other important antecedents of
Jehn, K. A. (1997). A qualitative analysis of conflict types and dimensions in
team creativity. Future researchers should explore how personal organizational groups. Administrative Science Quarterly, 42(3), 530–557.
traits (e.g., level of education and working experience) and organi- Jehn, K. A., Northcraft, G. B., & Neale, M. A. (1999). Why differences make a
zational characteristics (e.g., firm size and industry type) may difference: A field study of diversity, conflict, and performance in workgroups.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 44(4), 741–763.
moderate the relationships among model constructs. Shared lead- Kankanhalli, A., Tan, B. C. Y., & Wei, K. K. (2005). Contributing knowledge to
ership is well suited to the social network approach, so future electronic knowledge repositories: An empirical investigation. MIS Quarterly,
researchers should consider introducing other measures of social 29(1), 113–143.
Kluger, A. N., & DeNisi, A. (1996). The effects of feedback interventions
network analysis into the team creativity model. on performance: A historical review, a meta-analysis, and a
preliminary feedback intervention theory. Psychological Bulletin, 119(2),
254–284.
Acknowledgment Lapierre, J., & Giroux, V. (2003). Creativity and work environment in a high-tech
context. Creativity and Innovation Management, 12(1), 11–23.
Lawrence, B. S. (1997). The black box of organizational demography. Organization
This work was supported by the National Research Foundation Science, 8(1), 1–22.
Grant funded by the Korean Government (NRF-2009-342-B00015). LeBreton, J. M., & Senter, J. L. (2008). Answers to twenty questions about interrater
reliability and interrater agreement. Organizational Research Methods, 11,
815–852.
References LePine, J. A., & Van Dyne, L. (2001). Voice and cooperative behavior as contrasting
forms of contextual performance: Evidence of differential relationships with big
five personality characteristics and cognitive ability. Journal of Applied
Akgun, A. E., Byrne, J., Keskin, H., Lynn, G. S., & Imamoglu, S. Z. (2005). Knowledge
Psychology, 86(2), 326–336.
networks in new product development projects: A transactive memory
Loden, M., & Rosener, J. B. (1991). Workforce America! Managing employee diversity
perspective. Information & Management, 42(8), 1105–1120.
as a vital resource. Homewood, IL: Business One Irwin.
Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Lubart, T. I. (1994). Creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Thinking and problem solving
Amabile, T. M., Conti, R., Coon, H., Lazenby, J., & Herron, M. (1996). Assessing the
(pp. 289–332). London: Academic Press.
work environment for creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 39(5),
Mamykina, L., Candy, L., & Edmonds, E. (2002). Collaborative creativity.
1154–1184.
Communications of the ACM, 45(10), <http://www.dl.acm.org/
Ancona, D. G., & David, F. C. (1992). Demography and design: Predictors of new
citation.cfm?doid=570907.570940>.
product team performance. Organization Science, 3(3), 321–341.
Mayo, M., Meindl, J. R., & Pastor, J. C. (2003). Shared leadership in work teams: A
Avolio, B. J., Jung, D. I., & Sivasubramaniam, N. (1996). Building highly developed
social network approach. In C. L. Pearce & J. A. Conger (Eds.), Shared leadership:
teams: Focusing on shared leadership processes, efficacy, trust, and
Reframing the hows and whys of leadership (pp. 193–214). Thousand Oaks, CA:
performance. In M. M. Beyerlein & D. A. Johnson (Eds.), Advances in
Sage.
interdisciplinary study of work teams: Team leadership, 3 (pp. 173–209).
McGraw, K. O., & Wong, S. P. (1996). Forming inferences about some intraclass
Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
correlation coefficients. Psychological Methods, 1(1), 30–46.
Bakker, M., Leenders, R. T. A. J., Gabbay, S. M., Kratzer, J., & Van Engelen, J. M. L.
Mehra, A., Smith, B., Dixon, A., & Robertson, B. (2006). Distributed leadership in
(2006). Is trust really social capital? Knowledge sharing in product
teams: The network of leadership perceptions and team performance.
development projects. The Learning Organization, 13(6), 594–605.
Leadership Quarterly, 17(3), 232–245.
Bock, G. W., & Kim, Y. G. (2002). Breaking the myths of rewards: An exploratory
Milliken, F. J., & Martins, L. L. (1996). Searching for common threads: Understanding
study of attitudes about knowledge sharing. Information Resources Management
the multiple effects of diversity in organizational groups. Academy of
Journal, 15(2), 14–21.
Management Review, 21(2), 402–433.
Bock, G. W., Zmud, R. W., Kim, Y. G., & Lee, J. N. (2005). Behavioral intention
Morrison, E. W., & Phelps, C. C. (1999). Taking charge at work: Extrarole efforts
formation in knowledge sharing: Examining the roles of extrinsic motivators,
to initiate workplace change. Academy of Management Journal, 42(4),
social-psychological forces, and organizational climate. MIS Quarterly, 29(1),
403–419.
87–111.
Nelson, A., Sabatier, R., & Nelson, W. (1996). Toward an understanding of global
Brand, A. (1998). Knowledge management and innovation at 3M. Journal of
entrepreneurial knowledge management (EKM) practices: A preliminary
Knowledge Management, 2(1), 17–22.
investigation of EKM in France and the US. Journal of Applied Management and
Breckler, S. J. (1990). Applications of covariance structure modeling in psychology:
Entrepreneurship, 11(2), 70–89.
Cause for concern? Psychological Bulletin, 107(2), 260–273.
Offerman, L. R., & Gowing, M. K. (1990). Organization of the future: Change and
Buhler, P. (1997). Scanning the environment: Environmental trends affecting the
challenges. American Psychologist, 45(2), 95–108.
workplace. Supervision, 58(7), 24–26.
Ojha, A. K. (2005). Impact of team demography on knowledge sharing in software
Campbell, D. J. (1988). Task complexity: A review and analysis. Academy of
project teams. South Asian Journal of Management, 12(3), 67–78.
Management Review, 13(1), 40–52.
Oldham, G. R., & Cummings, A. (1996). Employee creativity: Personal and contextual
Carson, J. B., Tesluk, P. E., & Marrone, J. A. (2007). Shared leadership in teams: An
factors at work. Academy of Management Journal, 39(3), 607–634.
investigation of antecedent conditions. Academy of Management Journal, 50(5),
Oldham, G. R. (2003). Stimulating and supporting creativity in organizations. In S. E.
1217–1234.
Jackson, M. A. Hitt, & A. S. DeNisi (Eds.), Managing knowledge for sustained
Chattopadhyay, P. (1999). Beyond direct and symmetrical effects: The influence of
competitive advantage: Designing strategies for effective human resource
demographic dissimilarity on organizational citizenship behavior. Academy of
management (pp. 243–273). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Management Journal, 42(3), 273–287.
Parker, G. M. (1994). Working with allies, enemies, and other strangers. San Francisco,
Chattopadhyay, P., George, E., & Lawrence, S. A. (2004). Why does dissimilarity
CA: Jossey-Bass.
matter? Exploring self-categorization, self-enhancement, and uncertainty
Pearce, C. L., & Conger, J. A. (2003). Shared leadership: Reframing the hows and whys of
reduction. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(5), 892–900.
leadership. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Chin, W. W. (1998). The partial least squares approach to structural equation
Pearce, C. L., & Sims, H. P. (2002). The relative influence of vertical vs. shared
modeling. In G. A. Marcoulides (Ed.), Modern methods for business research.
leadership on the longitudinal effectiveness of change management teams.
Mahway, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(2), 172–197.
Connelly, C. E., & Kelloway, K. (2003). Predictors of employees’ perceptions of
Pearce, C. L., Yoo, Y., & Alavi, M. (2004). Leadership, social work and virtual teams:
knowledge sharing cultures. Leadership and Organizational Development Journal,
The relative influence of vertical vs. shared leadership in the nonprofit sector. In
24(5), 294–301.
R. E. Riggio & S. Smith-Orr (Eds.), Improving leadership in nonprofit organizations
Day, D. V., Gronn, P., & Salas, E. (2004). Leadership capacity in teams. The Leadership
(pp. 180–203). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Quarterly, 15(6), 857–880.
Pelled, L. H. (1996). Demographic diversity, conflict, and work group outcomes: An
De Vries, R. E., Van den Hooff, B., & De Ridder, J. A. (2006). Explaining knowledge
intervening process theory. Organization Science, 7(6), 615–631.
sharing: The role of team communication styles, job satisfaction, and
Pelled, L. H., Eisenhardt, K. M., & Xin, K. R. (1999). Exploring the black box: An
performance beliefs. Communication Research, 33(2), 115–135.
analysis of work group diversity, conflict, and performance. Administrative
Dean, J. W., & Snell, S. A. (1991). Integrated manufacturing and job design. Academy
Science Quarterly, 44(1), 1–28.
of Management Journal, 34(4), 776–804.
Pfeffer, J., & O’Reilly, C. (1987). Hospital demography and turnover among nurses.
DeNisi, A. S., Hitt, M. A., & Jackson, S. E. (2003). Managing knowledge for sustained
Industrial Relations, 36, 158–173.
competitive advantage: Designing strategies for effective human resource
Phang, M., & Foong, S. Y. (2005). Knowledge sharing in professional accountancy
management. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
firms: The effects of task complexity and management control system. In
Gibson, C., & Vermeulen, F. (2003). A healthy divide: Subgroups as a stimulus for
Proceedings of the 4th Global Conference on Business and Economic 2005.
team learning behavior. Administrative Science Quarterly, 48(2), 202–239.
Oxford, England.
Jackson, S. E., Brett, J. F., Sessa, V. I., Cooper, D. M., Julin, J. A., & Peyronnin, K. (1991).
Phillips, K. W., Mannix, E. A., Neale, M. A., & Gruenfeld, D. H. (2004). Diverse groups
Some differences make a difference: Individual dissimilarity and group
and information sharing: The effects of congruent ties. Journal of Experimental
heterogeneity as correlates of recruitment, promotions and turnover. Journal
Social Psychology, 40(4), 497–510.
of Applied Psychology, 76(5), 675–689.
56 D.S. Lee et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 42 (2015) 47–56

Purvis, R. L., Sambamurthy, V., & Zmud, R. W. (2001). The assimilation of knowledge Staples, D. S., & Webster, J. (2008). Exploring the effects of trust, task
platforms in organizations: An empirical investigation. Organization Science, interdependence and virtualness on knowledge sharing in teams. Information
12(2), 117–135. Systems Journal, 18(6), 617–640.
Ryan, A. J. (1991). Job rotations grease long-term IS wheels. Computer-World, 25(24), Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1995). Defying the crowd: Cultivating creativity in a
76. culture of conformity. New York: Free Press.
Ryu, S., Ho, S. H., & Han, I. (2003). Knowledge sharing behavior of physicians in Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1999). The concept of creativity: Prospects and
hospitals. Expert Systems with Applications, 25(1), 113–122. paradigms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Sawng, Y. W., Kim, S. H., & Han, H. S. (2006). R&D group characteristics and Thomas-Hunt, M. C., Ogden, T. Y., & Neale, M. A. (2003). Who’s really sharing?
knowledge management activities: A comparison between ventures and Effects of social and expert status on knowledge exchange within groups.
large firms. International Journal of Technology Management, 35(1–4), Management Science, 49(4), 464–477.
241–261. Tsui, A. S., Porter, L. W., & Egan, T. D. (2002). When both similarities and
Sessa, V. I., & Jackson, S. E. (1995). Diversity in decision-making teams: All dissimilarities matter: Extending the concept of relational demography. Human
differences are not created equal. In M. M. Chemers, S. Oskamp, & M. A. Relations, 55(8), 899–929.
Costanzo (Eds.), Diversity in organizations: new perspectives for a changing Wasko, M. M., & Faraj, S. (2005). Why should I share? Examining social capital and
workplace (pp. 133–156). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. knowledge contribution in electronic networks of practice. MIS Quarterly, 29(1),
Shalley, C. E. (1995). Effects of coaction, expected evaluation, and goal setting on 35–57.
creativity and productivity. Academy of Management Journal, 38(2), 483–503. Weingart, L. (1992). Impact of group goals, task component complexity, effort, and
Shin, S. J., & Zhou, J. (2003). Transformational leadership, conservation, and planning on group performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77(5), 682–693.
creativity: Evidence from Korea. Academy of Management Journal, 46(6), West, M. A. (1990). The social psychology of innovation in groups. In M. A. West & J.
703–714. L. Farr (Eds.), Innovation and creativity at work: Psychological and organizational
Song, S. (2008). Work unit’s knowledge processing style: An empirical examination strategies (pp. 101–122). Chichester: Wiley.
of its determinants. International Journal of Knowledge Management, 4(2), 1–16. Williams, K. Y., & O’Reilly, C. A. III, (1998). Demography and diversity in
Song, S., Nerur, S., & Teng, J. T. C. (2007). An exploratory study on the roles of organizations: A review of 40 years of research. In B. M. Staw & L. L.
network structure and knowledge processing orientation in work unit Cummings (Eds.). Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 20, pp. 77–140).
knowledge management. ACM SIGMIS Database, 38(2). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Sosik, J. J., Kahai, S. S., & Avolio, B. J. (1998). Transformational leadership and Woodman, R. W., Sawyer, J. E., & Griffin, R. W. (1993). Toward a theory of
dimensions of creativity: Motivating idea generation in computer-mediated organizational creativity. Academy of Management Review, 18(2), 293–321.
groups. Creativity Research Journal, 11(2), 111–121. Yukl, G. (2002). Leadership in organizations. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Srivastava, A., Bartol, K. M., & Locke, E. A. (2006). Empowering leadership in Zhou, J. (1998). Feedback valence, feedback style, task autonomy, and achievement
management teams: Effects on knowledge sharing, efficacy, and performance. orientation: Interactive effects on creative performance. Journal of Applied
Academy of Management Journal, 49(6), 1239–1251. Psychology, 83(2), 261–276.

You might also like