Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 9

Current Psychology

https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-9914-7

Effect of cognitive - behavioral play therapy on working memory,


short-term memory and sustained attention among school-aged
children with specific learning disorder: a preliminary randomized
controlled clinical trial
Amir Azizi 1 & Fazlollah Mir Drikvand 1 & Mohamad Ali Sepahvandi 1

# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018

Abstract
It has been shown by several studies that cognitive behavioral play therapy (CBPT) can ameliorate the condition in children
diagnosed with specific learning disorder (SLD). However, there is still need for randomized controlled clinical trials, in order to
evaluate this line of action as a standard option. The essence of this preliminary study was to assess the effect of CBPT on
working memory (WM), short-term memory (STM) and the sustained attention (SA) of school-aged children with SLD using a
randomized controlled clinical trial (RCT). In the present study, 35 eligible children with a DSM-5 SLD diagnosis, aged 7–9,
were randomly allocated in CBPT (n = 18) and control groups (n = 17). Evaluation of all participants was conducted for WM,
STM and SA using the digit span subscale of WISC-III, Wechsler memory scale-4th edition (WMS-IV) test and visual contin-
uous attention test (DAUF), respectively, both at the time of inclusion to the project and one month later. The intervention group
participated in 8 group sessions of CBPT and the control group was evaluated without any intervention. The study was completed
by a total of 16 boys and 14 girls (n = 15 per group). The mean ± S.D. of participants’ age were 8.29 ± 1.12 years and 8.20 ±
1.28 years in the CBPT and control groups, respectively. The treatment was effective on variables of the DAUF test (p = 0.001).
In addition, there was no significant improvement in any variables of WMS-IV and the digit span subscale of WISC-III tests
between two groups (p < 0.05). In this work, it was found that CBPT is effective on SA improvement among children with SLD,
but this intervention lacked effect on WM and STM in children with SLD. This study recommends more RCTs with large sample
sizes, more individual therapeutic sessions, longer time of follow-up and different protocols.

Keywords Cognitive - behavioral play therapy . Working memory . Short-term memory . Sustained attention . Specific learning
disorder

Introduction and Cornoldi 2015). The diagnosis of SLD in children, is


regarded as important when the inability of the person is con-
Despite the fact that children of school-age suffering from sidered not the result of sensory disabilities such as vision,
SLD (specific learning disorders) show an average intelli- hearing and when it is not the result of illiteracy or lack of
gence in the early days of school, it is very tasking for them education (American Psychiatric Association 2013). Records
to acquire the basic social and academic skills including read- show that students with SLD are more likely to drop out of
ing, writing, and arithmetic skills (DuPaul et al. 2013; Giofrè school (Vogel and Reder 1989), remain unemployed (Shapiro
and Lentz 1991) or even have suicidal thoughts compared to
other students (Daniel et al. 2006).
* Fazlollah Mir Drikvand Working memory (WM) impairments have been greatly
Fazlolah2020@gmail.com
discussed and identified as being associated with SLD
Amir Azizi
(Maehler and Schuchardt 2016). Based on the model of
amir28144@gmail.com Baddeley (1986), WM is comprised of three components:
the modality-free central executive, and two slave systems,
1
Department of Psychology, Lorestan University, Khoram Abad, Iran the phonological loop as well as the visual-spatial sketchpad
Curr Psychol

(Pickering et al. 2001).Studies have revealed that children and nonverbal forms of communication (Knell 2009). It has
suffering from specific reading disabilities/dyslexia and spell- been reported that CBPT can be successfully used with diverse
ing disabilities are incapable of properly conducting phono- populations. For instance, it has been used to treat children
logical processing (Schuchardt et al. 2008; Brandenburg et al. having selective mutism disorder, (Knell 1993) encopresis
2015). Also, it was found that their storage and central exec- disorder, (Knell and Moore 1990) as well as people who have
utive functioning is inhibited (Landerl et al. 2004). It has been experienced unpleasant events in life, such as sexual abuse
exposed that people with dyscalculia, experience hindrance in (Springer et al. 2012). There is scarcity of data on the standard
the visual-spatial sketchpad and the central executive treatment protocol using CBPT. A randomized controlled clin-
(Passolunghi 2006). However, results in respect to the phono- ical trial should be conducted to nominate CBPT for the treat-
logical loop, have been inconsistent (see Geary et al. 2000 vs. ment of school-aged children with SLD in terms of the three
Landerl et al. 2004). functions of cognitive status namely working memory (WM),
SLD has been linked with short-term memory (STM) prob- short-term memory (STM) and sustained attention (SA).
lems (Narimoto et al. 2013). STM serves the purpose of tem- The effects of CBPT on WM, STM, and SA of school-aged
porary storage and retaining of information for a limited peri- children suffering from SLD, was investigated using a ran-
od of time. There is temporary availability of information domized controlled clinical trial.
when it is not yet stored for long-term retention (Cowan
2008; Baddeley 2012). Evidences show that children facing
the risk of non-verbal learning disabilities (NLD), find it dif- Methods
ficult to encode relational information between spatial items.
This difficulty causes their impaired spatial short-term mem- Study Design and Subjects
ory (Narimoto et al. 2013). Also, NLD involves difficulties
with graphomotor aspects of writing, language comprehen- In this RCT, thirty-five children aged 7–9, diagnosed with a
sion (Mammarella et al. 2009; Mammarella et al. 2015), social DSM-5 SLD and attending the Learning Disability Center
interaction, (Semrud-Clikeman et al. 2010), tactile and visual (Tabriz, IR Iran), were randomly allocated in two groups:
perception, visuospatial reasoning and memory, as well as CBPT (n = 18) and control group (n = 17).
psychomotor coordination (Rourke 1989). For eligibility assessment, a psychiatrist was employed to
The ability to hinder executive functions like sustained evaluate the children prior to their participation in the study.
attention (SA), is among the important characteristics of The inclusion criteria are as follows: (a) being between 7 and
SLD (Sterr 2004). The psychological construct ‘SA’ describes 9 years old, (b(having educational grade (grade 1 to 3), (c)
a basic aspect of attention which is characterized by the sub- having at least 1 year of education, (d) showing no evidence of
ject’s readiness to discover rarely and unpredictably occurring mental retardation or neurodevelopmental disorders such as
signals over prolonged periods of time without being distract- attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), communica-
ed (Sarter et al. 2001; Bergera and Cassutob 2014). Studies on tion disorder (CD), developmental coordination disorder
children experiencing specific reading disabilities have shown (DCD) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) or other mental
that cognitive functions such as SA is hindered (Duncan et al. disorders such as anxiety disorders (AD), depression disorders
1994). In visual continuous attention test, it was found that (DD) and bipolar disorders (BD) according to the participants’
children with dyslexia disorder required more resolution time parents and recorded in the psychiatric cases in school, (e)
(Facoetti et al. 2010). Generally, few researchers are of the having no history of brain injury. The exclusion criteria are
opinion that it is difficult for dyslexic children to focus their as follows: (a) receiving CBPT prior to entering the research
attention on a given text (Facoetti and Molteni 2000). and (b) failure to cooperate.
Moreover, it has been shown that the slow attentional capture
and increased reaction time makes it difficult to process a Intervention
rapid sequence of stimuli in all sensory modalities (Hari and
Renvall 2001; Facoetti et al. 2010). Cognitive - Behavioral Play Therapy (CBPT)
CBPT is a widely known therapy method with its own
special structure and goal. The use of CBPT among school- Eight (8) group sessions of CBPT occurred twice a week.
aged children has been fully justified. The use of CBPT for Each training session lasted between 60 to 90 min (min).
school-age children, offers a relatively unique adaptation of Behavior therapy (BT), cognitive therapy (CT), and cognitive
cognitive behavioral therapy as it was originally developed for behavioral therapy (CBT) formed the basis of the concepts
adults. This approach utilizes a play therapy paradigm, as well and theoretical grounds for CBPT (Drewes 2009). The basic
as a combination of the cognitive and behavioral interven- therapeutic tools of CBPT include the use of developmentally
tions. In the CBPT therapy method, problems are settled appropriate games, and a play therapy method (Knell 1998;
through play activities including the application of verbal Knell 2009).
Curr Psychol

A CBPT program was completed by the subjects (see under critical time conditions. It is possible for the top of each
Table 1). The CBPT training was supervised by a certified triangle to be either upwards or downwards. After watching a
CBPT training coach, who was completely independent from previously determined downward triangle, the reaction button
the study team. should be pressed by the subject. The basis of SA assessment is
determined by the number of correct and incorrect responses as
Measurements well as the mean reaction time. In terms of the research goals,
the acceptable validity of DAUF was supported by some stud-
The Digit Span Subscale of WISC-III ies (Rostami et al. 2017). The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for
DAUF 0.72 was used to estimate reliability.
For WM assessment, the digit span subscale of WISC-III was
utilized. A part of the WISC-III subscale was used to evaluate
WM for verbal material (Figueiredo 2002). Previous studies Outcome Assessment
used this measure as an index of WM (Gathercole et al. 2004).
The WISC-III had sufficient reliability and validity in Iranian The members of the different groups were evaluated for WM
children (Mosanezhad Jeddi and Nazari 2013). In this study, using WISC-III, STM using WMS-IV and SA using DAUF.
reliability was estimated through the use of Cronbach’s alpha WM, STM, and SA were applied twice: at the baseline and a
coefficient for WISC-III 0.90. month afterwards. Prior to and after 8 sessions of CBPT, par-
ticipants in the experimental group underwent evaluation.
Prior to receiving any treatments, participants in the control
Wechsler Memory Scale-4th Edition (WMS-IV) group were evaluated at the baseline and a month after enter-
ing the research in the respective order.
The Wechsler memory scale-4th edition (WMS-IV) was used
for STM evaluation. The following eight subtests were eval-
uated in this scale: general information, memory quotient, Statistical Analysis
orientation, mind control, learning associations, logical mem-
ory, visual memory and repeat numbers. There are studies Quantitative variables were shown as mean (M), standard de-
which support the acceptable validity of WMS-IV (Soble et viation (S.D) and frequency (percentage), respectively. Using a
al. 2018). Reliability was estimated through the use of multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA), the differ-
Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for WMS-IV 0.77. ences between groups in terms of WM, STM and SA variables
and also before and after the interventions were evaluated. The
Visual Continuous Attention Test (DAUF) following categorical variables (sex, age and grade) were also
analyzed by x2 test. The effect of sizes (estimated as Cohen’s d)
We used the Visual continuous attention test (DAUF), In a bid was also presented. The effect of sizes were rated as small,
to determine SA, the Visual continuous attention test (DAUF) medium and large for d of 0.02, 0.15, and 0.35, respectively
was applied. This is a computer-based test which uses the (Cohen 1992). All statistical analyses were performed in IBM
sustained attention procedure. The DAUF test uses rows of SPSS Statistics, version 20.0 (IBM Corp 2011). P values less
triangles on the computer screen by the subjects; this is done than 0.05 were regarded as statistically significant.

Table 1 CBPT program overview

Session number Intervention component Examples of games and techniques

1 Encourage and strengthen relationships Reading the story of little rabbit, playing and pencil drawing.
2 Training, identification and separation of feelings Play dough, musical chairs, pantomime, pictures and color your
(sadness, anger, happiness and fear) own world
3 Distinguishing thoughts and feelings (Magic circle) Water colors, finger paint and Listen to the story
4 Identifying the relationship of behavior, thoughts and feelings Detective game, missing ring and spider trap games and also,
positive emphasis
5 Training and identification automatic thoughts and Traffic signals and detective game
thinking errors (negative look, labels, or zooming)
6 Cognitive retraining techniques Painting, finger puppets and modeling
7 Relaxation training and breathing relaxation training Rock jelly game, bubbles, flowers and candles making games
8 Training in problem solving News reading game, role playing and Game animal power
Curr Psychol

Results Table 2 Participant demographics

CBPT group Control group P


Demographic Data (n = 15) (n = 15)
M ± S.D M ± S.D
In this study, 42 children with SLD were recruited and
Sex 0.45
assessed for eligibility (Fig. 1). Of these, 7 did not
Boys, n (%) 16 (60)
meet the eligibility criteria: 4 revealed the presence of
Girls, n (%) 14 (40)
ADHD and 1 showed the presence of CD using the
Age, years 8.29 ± 1.12 8.20 ± 1.28 0.72
parents and recorded in the psychiatric cases while 1
Grade 0.50
received CBPT before entering the study, 1 was over
11 years of age. The remaining 35 children were ran- First grade 7.13 ± 1.25 7.20 ± 0.92
domized to one of the two groups. During CBPT, three Second grade 8.42 ± 0.89 8.37 ± 1.34
children dropped out at the pre-test and two children Third grade 9.33 ± 1.23 9.04 ± 1.59
dropped out at the post-test in the control group. The M, mean; S.D., standard deviation; P values refer to the χ2 test
study was completed by 35 children: CBPT (n = 15)
and Control group (n = 15).
Table 2 shows that the final sample is comprised as Descriptive Findings
follows: 16 (60%) boys and 14 (40%) girls; first grade,
2 boys and 3 girls, mean ± S.D. of age 7.13 ± 1.25 years Table 3 presents each variable of the digit span subscale of
in CBPT groups, 2 boys and 2 girls, mean ± S.D. of age WISC-III, WMS-IV and DAUF evaluated at the baseline and
7.20 ± 0.92 years in the Control group; second grade,3 after one month of inclusion between the two groups.
boys and 2 girls, mean ± S.D. of age 8.42 ± 0.89 years There was an improvement in the number of correct an-
in the CBPT group, 4 boys and 3 girls, mean ± S.D. of swers, reaction time and number of incorrect answers from
age 8.37 ± 1.34 years in the Control group; and third pre to post for the intervention group but not for the control
grade, 4 boys and 1 girl, mean ± S.D. of age 9.33 ± group (Table 3).
1.23 years in the CBPT group, 1 boy and 3 girls, mean
± S.D. of age 9.04 ± 1.59 years in the Control group. Inferential Findings
The mean ± S.D. of participants’ age were 8.29 ±
1.12 years, 8.20 ± 1.28 years and 8.53 ± 1.63 years in The intergroup comparison of the WISC-III, WMS-IV, and
CBPT and the control groups, respectively. Considering DAUF variables at the baseline and one month after com-
the demographic variables (sex, age and grade) (p < mencing the research are presented in Table 4.
0.05), no statistically significant differences were found At the baseline and one month after entering into the re-
between both groups. search, the intergroup evaluation of each variable of DAUF

Fig. 1 Distribution of study Children 7-9 years old with SLD


children (screening) Did not meet criteria (n = 7):
(n = 42) Comorbid ADHD, CD (n = 5)
Age > 11 years (n = 1)
Receiving CBPT (n = 1)

Incorporated into the study


(n = 35)

Randomization (n = 35)

Experimental Control group


group (n=18) (n=17)

Pre- test n=15 n=15

Dropped out (n Dropped out (n


= 3) = 2)
Post- test Reason: not Reason: not
cooperating cooperating
n=15 n=15
Curr Psychol

Table 3 Mean and standard


deviation of the WISC-III, WMS- Variable Groups M ± S.D M ± S.D
IV and DAUF variables at the At the baseline At the one month
baseline and the one month of
inclusion between the two groups SA
Number of correct answers CBPT 46.54 ± 13.26 51.72 ± 14.56
Control 47.83 ± 13.05 48.00 ± 13.10
Reaction time CBPT 3.73 ± 0.22 1.44 ± 0.35
Control 3.65 ± 0.43 3.99 ± 1.17
Number of incorrect answers CBPT 16.92 ± 4.33 13.32 ± 4.14
Control 15.33 ± 4.25 15.02 ± 3.76
WM CBPT 13.26 ± 1.32 15. 28 ± 1.64
Control 13.05 ± 1.16 15.07 ± 1.03
STM
Memory quotient CBPT 80.32 ± 33.05 81.20 ± 32.10
Control 78.05 ± 24.11 80.60 ± 22.13
General information CBPT 3.18 ± 1.05 4.52 ± 1.45
Control 4.23 ± 0.89 4.72 ± 1.01
Orientation CBPT 3.51 ± 1.19 4.37 ± 2.03
Control 3.95 ± 1.32 4.26 ± 1.87
Learning association CBPT 11.66 ± 4.07 13.00 ± 3.75
Control 10.55 ± 3.95 12.91 ± 4.25
Mind control CBPT 3.74 ± 2.31 3.30 ± 2.68
Control 3.20 ± 2.84 4.09 ± 2.11
Logical memory CBPT 6.53 ± 2.77 5.54 ± 2.71
Control 6.43 ± 3.00 4.81 ± 1.62
Repeat numbers CBPT 7.84 ± 1.90 7.00 ± 2.15
Control 7.79 ± 0.33 7.02 ± 1.41
Visual memory CBPT 7.60 ± 3.46 6.86 ± 3.69
Control 8.25 ± 2.50 7.77 ± 2.10

M, mean; S.D., standard deviation; SA, sustained attention; WM, working memory; STM, short-term memory;
CBPT, cognitive behavioral play therapy
N = 30

was conducted twice. At p < 0.05, MANCOVA revealed a the number of incorrect answers and reaction time in DAUF
statistically significant difference for the groups in terms of test were reduced after CBPT training and there was an
the number of correct answers, F(1,18) = 47.91, p = 0.001, re-
action time, F(1,18) = 70.76, p = 0.001, and number of incorrect
Table 4 Comparison of the WISC-III, WMS-IVand DAUF variables at
answers, F(1,18) = 14.47, p = 0.001. A medium effect size, was the baseline and the one month of inclusion between the CBPT and
shown for the eta squared statistic for number of correct an- control groups
swers and reaction time scores of (0.12) and (0.19), respec-
F P d
tively. Also, a large effect size indicated the eta squared statis-
tic for the number of incorrect answers score (0.44). SA
In this study, intergroup evaluations of each parameter of Number of correct answers 47.91 0.001 0.12
WMS-IV and the digit span subscale of WISC-III were con- Reaction time 70.76 0.001 0.19
ducted twice: 1) at the baseline and 2) one month after enter- Number of incorrect answers 14.47 0.001 0.44
ing into the research. Within this period, calculation was done WM 3.52 0.077 0.16
for the inter and intragroup discrepancy of each variable
STM
(Table 4). It is shown in the table that the discrepancies were
Memory quotient 2.87 0.107 0.13
not statistically significant (p < 0.05).
General information 1.13 0.300 0.05
Orientation 0.77 0.390 0.04
Learning association 3.33 0.085 0.15
Discussion
Mind control 2.36 0.142 0.11
Logical memory 0.63 0.435 0.03
Through the use of the digit span subscale of WISC-III,
Visual memory 2.18 0.156 0.10
WMS-IV and DAUF tests in children with SLD, the objective
Repeat numbers 1.17 0.293 0.06
of this study was to determine the efficacy of CBPT on WM,
STM and SA. Regarding this objective, it can be finalized that P values refer to multivariate analysis of covariance, d = Cohen’s effect
CBPT were able to effect changes on DAUF test. Therefore, size
Curr Psychol

increase in the number of correct answers after this interven- follow-up of school-aged children), it becomes easier to in-
tion (p = 0.001,Table 2). Conversely, there was no improve- vestigate the effects of CBPT. Therefore, it is suggested that
ment in the WMS-IV variables, as well as the digit span sub- other researchers should study the children for a longer period
scale of WISC-III tests between CBPT and the control groups of time.
(p < 0.05, Table 4).
It has been reported in various studies that school-aged
Limitations
children with SLD have deficits in SA. Also, there could be
improvement in academic skills such as reading, writing, and
This study has some limitations, since it was conducted only
math among people with LD (Swanson and Jerman 2007), as
on school-aged children with SLD and it cannot be considered
a result of important correlates of cognitive functioning
as intervention on other samples (such as high school-aged
(Willcutt et al. 2011) and improvement of abilities.
children with SLD). Therefore, generalization of the results
Conversely, WM problem have been associated with specific
on other individuals is not without failure. Other limitations
reading disabilities/dyslexia and spelling disabilities
of this study include lack of follow-up of these RCTs as a
(Schuchardt et al. 2008; Brandenburg et al. 2015).
result of the limited access to children.
Furthermore, results have shown the resolution of the forward
and backward digit span in children suffering from dyslexia
and other specific learning difficulties is hindered (Jeffries and
Everatt 2004). Conclusion
As revealed by the literature review, this is the only study
that investigated the effects of CBPT among school-aged chil- It was concluded that CBPT is effective on ameliorating SA
dren diagnosed with SLD along with the control group, in among school-aged children with SLD, but this treatment was
terms of the three functions of cognitive status (WM, STM, not effective on WM and STM in school-aged children with
and SA). Notwithstanding, there are reports on the useful ef- SLD. However, it should be noted that more studies are re-
fects of CBPT training on the reduction of challenges faced by quired to confirm the effects of CBPT in children with SLD.
children diagnosed with selective mutism (Knell 1993), Future studies should be conducted with larger sample sizes
encopresis (Knell and Moore 1990), as well as people who among various participants regarding educational levels; they
have experienced unpleasant events in life, like sexual abuse also should consist of more sessions that are therapeutic indi-
(Springer et al. 2012). vidually, have longer follow-up interval, and follow different
It has been found that in CBPT, children have improved protocols.
focus on maintaining constant and purposeful attention as a
result of the pleasant atmosphere of the game, the students’ Compliance with Ethical Standards
activation and also collaboration with the therapist
(Schaefer 1993), because a wide range of toys, art supplies, Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of
interest.
dolls, game demos and other materials are used to stimu-
late their attention (Drewes 2009). Moreover, the use of Ethical Approval All procedures performed in studies involving human
diverse educational games along with a goal-oriented ap- participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institu-
proach, encourage children with SLD to use the opportu- tional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki
nities created in the game to maintain focus on the game declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
without distraction (through modeling and role playing as a
Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual
critical component of CBPT, Knell 2009). participants included in the study.
The advantages of CBPT on children with SLD have been
confirmed; however, the effect of CBPT to improve STM and
WM loss in SLD children is uncertain. The ineffectiveness of
the CBPT appears to be associated with the treatment protocol. References
This is because the standard CBPT- protocol of Drewes
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). DSM 5: American Psychiatric
(2009), does not employ assignments adequate for the correc- Association.
tion of memory defects despite sufficient empirical evidence. Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University
It is the author’s opinion that the treatment protocol should be Press.
re-evaluated and other researchers should utilize the modified Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and contro-
program of the memory-focused CBPT. In addition, it is very versies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1–29.
Bergera, T., & Cassutob, H. (2014). The effect of environmental
likely that future studies will revise the proposed treatment distractors incorporation into a CPT on sustained attention and
program for CBPT and its number of sessions (explained in ADHD diagnosis among adolescents. Journal of Neuroscience
the method section). Also, after a longer interval (more Methods, 222, 62–68.
Curr Psychol

Brandenburg, J., Klesczewski, J., Fischbach, A., Schuchardt, K., Büttner, Maehler, C., & Schuchardt, K. (2016). Working memory in children with
G., & Hasselhorn, M. (2015). Working memory in children with specific learning disorders and/or attention deficits. Learning and
learning disabilities in reading versus spelling: Searching for over- Individual Differences, 49, 341–347.
lapping and specific cognitive factors. Journal of Learning Mammarella, I. C., Meneghetti, C., Pazzaglia, F., Gitti, F., Gomez, C., &
Disabilities, 48(6), 622–634. Cornoldi, C. (2009). Representation of survey and route spatial de-
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155– scriptions in children with nonverbal (visuospatial) learning disabil-
159. ities. Brain and Cognition, 71(2), 173–179.
Cowan, N. (2008). What are the differences between long-term, short- Mammarella, I. C., Meneghetti, C., Pazzaglia, F., & Cornoldi, C. (2015).
term, and working memory? Progress in Brain Research, 169, 323– Memory and comprehension deficits in spatial descriptions in chil-
338. dren with nonverbal and reading disabilities. Frontiers in
Daniel, S. S., Walsh, A. K., Goldston, D. B., Arnold, E. M., Reboussin, B. Psychology, 5, 1534.
A., & Wood, F. B. (2006). Suicidality, school dropout, and reading Mosanezhad Jeddi, E., & Nazari, M. A. (2013). Effectiveness of EEG-
problems among adolescents. Journal of Learning Disabilities, biofeedback on attentiveness, working memory and quantitative
39(6), 507–514. electroencephalography on reading disorder. Iranian Journal of
Drewes, A. A. (2009). Blending play therapy with cognitive behavioral Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, 7(2), 35–43.
therapy: Evidence-based and other effective treatments and Narimoto, T., Matsuura, N., Takezawa, T., Mitsuhashi, Y., & Hiratani, M.
techniques. New Jersey: Wiley. (2013). Spatial short-term memory in children with nonverbal learn-
Duncan, C. C., Rumsey, J. M., Wilkniss, S. M., Denckla, M. B., ing disabilities: Impairment in encoding spatial configuration. The
Hamburger, S. D., & Odou-Potkin, M. (1994). Developmental dys- Journal of Genetic Psychology: Research and Theory on Human
lexia and attention dysfunction in adults: Brain potential indices of Development, 174(1), 73–87.
information processing. Psychophysiology, 31(4), 386–401. Passolunghi, M.C. (2006). Working memory and arithmetic learning dis-
DuPaul, G. J., Gormley, M. J., & Laracy, S. D. (2013). Comorbidity of ability. In T. P. Alloway, & S. E. Gathercole (Eds.),Working memory
LD and ADHD: Implications of DSM-5 for assessment and treat- and neurodevelopmental disorders (pp. 113–138). Hove:
ment. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46, 43–51. Psychology Press.
Facoetti, A., & Molteni, M. (2000). Is attentional focusing an inhibitory Pickering, S. J., Gathercole, S. E., Hall, M., & Lloyd, S. A. (2001).
process at distractor location? Cognitive Brain Research, 10, 185– Development ofmemory for pattern and path: Further evidence for
188. the fractionation of visuo-spatial memory. The Quarterly Journal of
Facoetti, A., Trussardi, A. N., Ruffino, M., Lorusso, M. L., Experimental Psychology, 54, 397–420.
Cattaneo, C., Molteni, M., & Zorzi, M. (2010). Rostami, R., Salamati, P., Yarandi, K. K., Khoshnevisan, A., Saadat, S.,
Multisensory spatial attention defi- cits are predictive of pho- Kamali, Z. S., Ghiasi, S., Zaryabi, A., Ghazi Mir Saeid, S. S. H.,
nological decoding skills in developmental dyslexia. Journal Arjipour, M., Saeid Rezaee-Zavareh, M., & Rahimi-Movaghar, V.
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 1011–1025. (2017). Effects of neurofeedback on the short-term memory and
Figueiredo, V. L. M. (2002). WISC-III: Escala de Inteligência Wech- sler continuous attention of patients with moderate traumatic brain inju-
para crianças: amostra brasileira. São Paulo: Casa do Psicó- logo. ry: A preliminary randomized controlled clinical trial. Chinese
Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). Journal of Traumatology, 20(5), 278–282.
The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Rourke, B. P. (1989). Nonverbal learning disabilities: The syndrome and
Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 177–190. the model. New York: Guilford Press.
Geary, D. C., Hamson, C. O., & Hoard, M. K. (2000). Numerical and Sarter, M., Givens, B., & Bruno, J. P. (2001). The cognitive neuroscience
arithmetical cognition:A longitudinal study of process and concept of sustained attention: Where top-down meets bottom-up. Brain
deficits in children with learning disability. Journal of Experimental Research. Brain Research Reviews, 35(2), 146–160.
Child Psychology, 77, 236–263. Schaefer, C. E. (1993). The therapeutic powers of play. Northvale: Jason
Giofrè, D., & Cornoldi, C. (2015). The structure of intelligence in chil- Aronson.
dren with specific learning disabilities is different as compared to Schuchardt, K., Maehler, C., & Hasselhorn, M. (2008). Working memory
typically development children. Intelligence, 52, 36–43. deficits in children with specific learning disorders. Journal of
Hari, R., & Renvall, H. (2001). Impaired processing of rapid stimulus Learning Disabilities, 41(6), 514–523.
sequences in dyslexia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(12), 525– Semrud-Clikeman, M., Walkowiak, J., Wilkinson, A., & Christopher, G.
532. (2010). Neuropsychological differences among children with
IBM Corp. (2011). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 20.0. IBM Asperger syndrome, nonverbal learning disabilities, attention deficit
Corp, Armonk, NY. disorder, and controls. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35(5),
Jeffries, S., & Everatt, J. (2004). Working memory: Its role in dyslexia 582–600.
and other specific learning difficulties. Dyslexia, 10, 196–214. Shapiro, E. S., & Lentz, F. E. (1991). Vocational-technical programs:
Knell, S.M. (1993). To show and not tell: Cognitive-behavioral play Follow-up of students with learning disabilities. Exceptional Child,
therapy in the treatment of selective mutism. In T. Kottman & C. 58(1), 47–59.
Schaefer (Ed &.) Play therapy in action: A casebook for Soble, J. R., Bain, K. M., Bailey, K. C., Kirton, J. W., Marceaux,
practitioners (pp. 169–208). Northvale, New Jersey: Manson. J. C., Critchfield, E. A., MacCoy, K. J. M., & O’Rourke, J.
Knell, S. M. (1998). Cognitive-behavioral play therapy. Journal of J. F. (2018). Evaluating the accuracy of the Wechsler mem-
Clinical Child Psychology, 27(1), 28–33. ory scale-fourth edition (WMS-IV) logical memory embedded
Knell, S. M. (2009). Cognitive-behavioral play therapy. Northvale: Jason validity index for detecting invalid test performance. Applied
Aronson Inc. Neuropsychology: Adult, 8, 1–8.
Knell, S. M., & Moore, D. J. (1990). Cognitive-behavioral play therapy in Springer, C., Misurell, J. R., & Hiller, A. (2012). Game-based cognitive-
the treatment of encopresis. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, behavioral therapy (GB-CBT) group program for children who have
19(1), 55–60. experienced sexual abuse: A three-month follow-up investigation.
Landerl, K., Bevan, A., & Butterworth, B. (2004). Developmental Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 21(6), 646–664.
dyscalculia and basic numerical capacities: A study of 8–9-year- Sterr, A. M. (2004). Attention performance in young adults with learning
old students. Cognition, 93, 99–125. disabilities. Learning and Individual Differences, 14, 125–133.
Curr Psychol

Swanson, H. L., & Jerman, O. (2007). The influence of working memory disabilities, Literacy, and Adult Education (pp.5–28). Baltimore,
on reading growth in subgroups of children with reading disabilities. MD: Paul H. Brookes.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96(4), 249–283. Willcutt, E. G., Boada, R., Riddle, M. W., Chhabildas, N., DeFries, J. C.,
Vogel, S.A, & Reder, S. (1989). Educational attainment of edults with & Pennington, B. F. (2011). Colorado learning difficulties question-
learning disabilities. In S.A. Vogel & S. Reder (Eds.), Learning naire: Validation of a parent-report screening measure.
Psychological Assessment, 23(3), 778–791.
Reproduced with permission of copyright owner. Further reproduction
prohibited without permission.

You might also like