Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/267237521

The Impact of Cognitive Conflict on Team Performance

Article · January 2008

CITATIONS READS

6 834

3 authors:

Rebecca Mitchell Stephen Nicholas


University of Newcastle 177 PUBLICATIONS   1,838 CITATIONS   
101 PUBLICATIONS   1,801 CITATIONS   
SEE PROFILE
SEE PROFILE

Brendan Boyle
University of Newcastle
53 PUBLICATIONS   795 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Seasonal and Monthly Patterns, Weekly Variations, and the Holiday Effect of Outpatient Visits for Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus Patients in China View project

SLPs' perceptions of working with assistants View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Rebecca Mitchell on 05 November 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Asia Pacific Management Review 13(3) (2008) 625-634

www.apmr.management.ncku.edu.tw

The Impact of Cognitive Conflict on Team Performance


Rebecca Mitchell*, Stephen Nicholas, Brendan Boyle
School of Business and Management, University of Newcastle, Australia
Accepted 27 February 2008

Abstract

The results of research on diversity in teams suggest that it offers both a great opportunity
for organisations as well as an enormous challenge. However, current research is plagued by a
lack of overall consistency, indicating that the relationship between diversity and team
performance is not well understood. This study examines the components of cognitive conflict
in order to assess whether construct operationalisation may explain this inconsistency.
Analysis of the existing operationalisations of cognitive conflict reveals that it incorporates
both disagreement about information and reasoning, and debate of rival hypotheses or
recommendations. We propose that functional diversity leads to cognitive disagreement but
not debate, and that debate enhances knowledge creation, with which cognitive disagreement
shows no relationship. Our results support these hypotheses, which provide a powerful
explanation for the contrary results found by researchers investigating cognitive conflict.
Given that extant measures of cognitive conflict include scale items which measure both
debate and cognitive disagreement, cognitive conflict may be viewed as an aggregate measure
of these two distinct constructs. This study contributes to research on diversity and conflict by
providing an explanation for contrary results, and by providing and a detailed
operationalisation of cognitive conflict and its component constructs. It also contributes to
research into creativity and innovation by providing insight into the dynamics underpinning
knowledge sharing and creation.

Keywords: Knowledge creation, teams conflict

1. Introduction1

The results of research on diversity in teams suggest that it offers both a great opportunity
for organisations as well as an enormous challenge. Numerous studies have suggested that
more diverse teams have the potential to consider a greater range of perspectives and to
generate more innovative and higher-quality solutions than less diverse teams (Austin, 1997).
For example, in a study of the top management teams of banks, Bantel and Jackson (1989)
found that the more heterogeneous the team was in terms of functional background, the
greater the number of innovations the bank had made. However, the mechanism through
which diversity affects performance is not straightforward. Ancona and Caldwell’s (1992)
study of 45 product teams indicated that diversity had a negative direct effect on innovation
and team-related performance, but it had a countervailing positive indirect effect on
innovation through its association with an increased frequency of communication with those
outside the product team. In contrast, meta-analyses have revealed a negative relationship,
inconsistent or no relationship between team heterogeneity and innovation (van Knippenberg
et al., 2004; Webber and Donahue, 2001).
What stands out in this literature is a lack of overall consistency, indicating that the

*
Corresponding author. E-mail: Rebecca.mitchell@newcastle.edu.au

625
Mitchell et al. / Asia Pacific Management Review 13(3) (2008) 625-634

relationship between diversity and team performance is not well understood. There have been
a series of suggestions regarding these conflicting results, many of which have cited
moderator or mediator variables. Researchers have proposed, for example, that the
relationship between functional diversity and performance is mediated by member
commitment, decision comprehensiveness and conflict (Jehn et al., 1999; Riordan and Shore,
1997; Simons et al., 1999). These studies have sought to understand how, that is through what
mediating factors, diversity impacts performance.
In this study, we investigate a single mediating variable, cognitive conflict, and its impact
on the relationship between functional diversity and knowledge creation. Previous research
investigating the mediating role of conflict has yielded inconsistent results (De Dreu and
Weingart, 2003; Jehn, 1995). This study examines the components of cognitive conflict in
order to assess whether construct operationalisation may explain this inconsistency. We
propose that cognitive conflict is composed of two distinct constructs that impact
differentially on dependent variables (such as team performance) and are affected differently
by independent variables (such as team composition). This proposition is significant because
it provides a powerful explanation for ambiguous findings in research investigating the
antecedents and consequences of cognitive conflict and, in particular, lends clarity to the
pathway through which diversity impacts decision-making and knowledge creation.

2. Conceptual background and hypothesis

Functional diversity is described as the extent to which a team is composed of members


with different functional backgrounds (Pelled et al., 1999; Shaw and Barrett-Power, 1998) and
is a defining characteristic of cross-functional teams (Pelled et al., 1999). Cross-functional
teams, those comprised of members from different functional areas (Pelled et al., 1999), are
increasingly used in organisations in central business tasks requiring the solution of complex
problems, particularly those requiring the generation of ideas (Van der Vegt and Bunderson,
2005; West and Anderson, 1996).
Research into functional diversity’s impact generally assumes that it can have either a
positive or negative influence on team outcome. A positive outcome is associated with access
to a diverse range of knowledge and perspectives which provides not only an increased pool
of resources, but also an opportunity for novel connections, cognitive stimulation and the
generation of creative ideas (Ancona and Caldwell, 1992; DeDreu and West, 2001; Paulus
and Yang, 2000). A negative outcome is associated with factors including increased
emotional conflict consequent to misinterpretation of disagreements or in-team/out-team
social categorisation (Amason, 1996; Pelled et al., 1999). This study investigates one variable,
cognitive conflict, defined as task-centred conflict behaviour including the questioning,
challenging and the direct and open presentation of rival hypotheses or recommendations
(Pelled et al., 1999; Simons et al., 1999) in an attempt to understand how functional diversity
contributes to positive teams outcomes. Cognitive conflict has been investigated previously;
however, the present study differentiates itself from previous research by explicitly
investigating the operationalisation of cognitive conflict.
Analysis of previous efforts to operationalise cognitive conflict reveals that it incorporates
both disagreement about information and reasoning, and debate of rival hypotheses or
recommendations (Jehn, 1995). For example, Jehn (1997) used the following four scale items
to measure task-related conflict: “How much conflict of ideas was there in your team?," "How
different were your views on the content of your project?," "How much did you talk through
disagreements about your team projects?," and "How much disagreement was there about task
procedure in your team?". It appears that the first two items reflect the level of cognitive
disagreement in the team, defined as the extent to which members disagree or have
differences of opinion relevant to the teams task, while the third item reflects a behavioural

626
Mitchell et al. / Asia Pacific Management Review 13(3) (2008) 625-634

response to conflict, which arguably reflects the construct of debate, defined as the extent to
which the group talked through their disagreements. It is proposed that the construct,
cognitive conflict, incorporates two components that are sufficiently different to warrant
conceptual distinction.
Hypothesis 1: Cognitive conflict is composed of two component constructs: cognitive
disagreement and debate.
Previous research investigating the antecedents of cognitive conflict, found that because
functional background is so related to work, people are particularly likely to draw on belief
structures based on functional background when addressing workplace issues (Pelled et al.,
1999). Group members with different functional backgrounds have divergent preferences,
interpretations and even values. Combined, these may constitute cognitive diversity sufficient
to trigger intra-group cognitive conflict. However, the argument that, as diversity increases,
task-related conflict is also likely to increase due to incongruent task perceptions, points only
to an increase in disagreement and not debate. Debate is a behaviour and can therefore be
differentiated from cognitive disagreement associated with a group’s task, which may emerge
or be perceived without consequent interaction or advocacy. Differences in knowledge do not
necessarily lead to debate (Simons et al., 1999). Debate requires the expression of alternative
views and knowledge, but is focused on the rigorously analysis, questioning and challenging
of issues and opinions (Chen and Tjosvold, 2002; Tjosvold and Poon, 1998).
Hypothesis 2: Functional diversity will be significantly and positively related to cognitive
disagreement, but show no significant relationship with debate.
Previous research has evidenced numerous connections between knowledge creation and
diversity. The extent to which group members actively utilise opposing or conflicting ideas
positively affects the groups’ ability to resist conformity pressures, increases the tendency for
conceptual differentiation and divergent thinking, and facilitates the application of different
perspectives to codified knowledge, all of which are linked to the emergence of new ideas
(Bhatt, 2000; Brown and Duguid, 2001; DeDreu and West, 2001; Gruenfeld et al., 1998;
Nemeth, 1986; Nemeth et al., 2001; Nemeth et al., 2003; Tushman, 1977; Van Dyne and
Saavedra, 1996). This indicates that diversity’s link to knowledge creation is underpinned by
the rigorous analysis of alternative perspectives, which has been identified as a consequence
of cognitive conflict. Cognitive conflict has been linked to improved performance because it
results in the consideration of more alternatives, the more careful consideration of alternatives,
and fosters a deeper understanding of task issues. However, while debate allows the
consideration and deeper understanding of more alternatives, and therefore promotes
integration across diverse knowledge sources (Simons et al., 1999), cognitive disagreement
does not necessarily facilitate deeper understanding. To enable effective debate, members
must express opposing and diverse views and knowledge, and rigorously analyse issues and
opinions relevant to the task (Chen and Tjosvold, 2002; Tjosvold and Poon, 1998), whereas
disagreement does not require open consideration of alternative perspectives.
Hypothesis 3: Debate will be significantly and positively related to knowledge creation,
however cognitive disagreement will demonstrate no significant relationship with knowledge
creation.

3. Method

3.1 Procedure and sample


In order to test the hypotheses, a survey-based study was conducted. The questionnaire
was distributed with a covering letter instructing participants to choose a team of which they
were a member and that had at least two other members, and to complete the questionnaire
referring to the same most recent situation in which the team made a non-routine, complex

627
Mitchell et al. / Asia Pacific Management Review 13(3) (2008) 625-634

decision. Instead of collecting data from all members of each participating team, we used an
informant sampling approach which has been utilised in similar studies (Simons et al., 1999;
Van der Vegt and Bunderson, 2005). Our requirement was to receive data from at least two
members – participants were required to complete one questionnaire themselves and request
another member of the team to fill out a separate questionnaire. The questionnaire instructed
participants that to be included in the study responses were required from two team members.
Five hundred and fifty questionnaires were distributed to postgraduate business students from
a university in Ireland and a university in Australia, who were asked to recruit a team from
their workplace. Responses from seventy teams were returned completed, representing a
response rate of thirteen percent.
3.2 Measures
Cognitive conflict: Four items measured cognitive conflict based on Jehn et al. (1997), for
example, “How many disagreements over ideas were there in your team?". The alpha
coefficient for cognitive conflict was 0.67. These four items included the debate and cognitive
disagreement items described below.
Debate: Two items measured debate based on Jehn et al. (1997), and Simons et al. (1999).
The two items were "How much did you talk through disagreements about your team
project?" and “Did the group discuss disagreements in an attempt to work them through to a
solution?”. The alpha coefficient for debate was 0.85.
Cognitive Disagreement: Two items measured cognitive disagreement based on Jehn et al.
(1997). The two items were “How many disagreements over ideas were there in your team?"
and "Were there differences of opinion about any important issues?”. The alpha coefficient
for cognitive disagreement was 0.73.
Knowledge Creation: Knowledge creation was operationalised in terms of its output, as
the generation of new ideas (Beech et al., 2002; Bryant, 2005; Fong et al., 2007; Parent and
Gallupe, 2000). Three items measured knowledge creation, for example, “Did the team
develop any new ideas that were incorporated into the decision?”. The alpha coefficient for
this measure was .78. This study employed a graphic line segment scale to measure dependent
responses, which represents nearly continuous dependent response measure (Russell and
Bobko, 1992). The independent variables were measured using a 7-point Likert scale which
are best classified as ordinal scales, however, according to Johnson and Creech (1983) there is
relatively little harm in treating Likert scales as continuous variables when there are five or
more categories.
In order to assess interrater reliability we calculated interrater agreement (James et al.,
1984) and intraclass correlation coefficients with ICC (1) indicating the ration of between-
team to total variance and ICC (2) representing the reliability of average team perceptions
(Bliese and Halverson, 1996). The results justified averaging responses and testing our
hypotheses on a team level. It is standard practice to consider aggregation of individual-level
measures to the team level as appropriate if the mean rwg value for the teams in the sample
equals or exceeds .70 (Klein and Kozlowski, 2000). The ICC (1) result indicated that team
membership accounted for a reasonable proportion of the variance in individual responses
(James, 1982). The ICC (2) result indicates that team means were reasonably stable (Bliese
and Halverson, 1996).
3.3 Control variables
In line with previous research indicating a relationship between available knowledge and
group outcome, task complexity and routine, and team size and tenure were used as control
variables (Jehn, 1995; Tushman and Nadler, 1978). Task complexity and routine was
controlled for by specifying that members had to complete their survey with reference to a
complex, non-routine task (Jehn, 1995). Team tenure was measured by respondents stating the

628
Mitchell et al. / Asia Pacific Management Review 13(3) (2008) 625-634

length of time that members had been working together as a group. Team size was measured
as the number of group members.

4. Analysis and results

The data were assessed for multicollinearity or high correlation between independent
variables. Table 1 shows the correlations among the independent variables. The largest
correlation among predictor variables was .28. These magnitudes suggest that
multicollinearity was not a serious problem as the general rule is that no correlation
coefficient should exceed .75 (Tsui et al., 1995).
In order to test hypothesis one, and to ensure that debate and cognitive disagreement were
distinct constructs, factor analysis of cognitive conflict was undertaken. The results generate
support for two distinct factors captured within the cognitive conflict scale items (Table 1).

Table 1. Variable correlation coefficients


1 2 3 4 5
1 Size
2 Tenure .02
3 Functional diversity .28* -.08
4 Cognitive disagreement -.08 -.16 .18
5 Debate -.08 .22 .03 .22
**p<.01
*p<.05

Multiple regression analysis was employed to test all further relationships. To test the
second hypothesis, the dependent variables, cognitive disagreement and debate, were
regressed on the independent variable, functional diversity. As expected, cognitive
disagreement was significantly and positively related to functional diversity (β = .22, p < .1),
however no significant relationship was evidenced between functional diversity and debate (β
= .07, ns).
To test the second hypothesis, the dependent variable, knowledge creation, was regressed
on the independent variables, cognitive disagreement and debate. As expected, debate was
significantly and positively related to cognitive diversity (β = .71, p < .01), however no
significant relationship was evidenced between cognitive disagreement and knowledge
creation (β = .01, ns).

Table 2. Rotated component matrix


Component one Component two

Cognitive disagreement Item 1 .05 .82


Cognitive disagreement Item 2 .14 .87
Debate item 1 .92 .12
Debate item 2 .93 .08
Extraction method: Principal component analysis
Rotation method: Varimax with Kaiser normalisation

5. Discussion

The purpose of this research was to investigate the impact of cognitive conflict on
knowledge creation. The results provide support for the existence of two distinct constructs
captured by extant cognitive conflict measures. These constructs, debate and cognitive

629
Mitchell et al. / Asia Pacific Management Review 13(3) (2008) 625-634

disagreement, have distinct relationships with the independent variable, functional diversity,
and impact differentially on knowledge creation.
This study has a number of limitations, including a reliance on single source data and
small sample size. It is possible that our findings reflect only common method variance. There
are strong reasons for believing that perceptual measures of knowledge creation are more
useful than objective, external measures. An objective measure would not take into account
the knowledge created that was developed within the team’s discussion, but not incorporated
into the final decision because of factors such as timing and political variables (Amason,
1996). Perceptual measures of relative status judged by those who are aware of the context
within which the decision is made, is an important attribute (Amason, 1996). Procedural
attempts were made to reduce the risk of bias caused by common method variance by
incorporating the remedies advocated by Podsakoff et al. (2003) into the research design. We
also investigated the possibility of single-source bias by employing a Harman’s single factor
test (Podsakoff and Organ, 1986). The underlying assumption of this procedure is that if
common method variance is an issue in this study, either a single factor will emerge or one
factor will account for the majority of covariance among variables (Podsakoff and Organ,
1986). In this analysis, the unrotated factor solution did not identify a single or common
factor as explaining the majority of the variance in the set of items. These results serve to
reduce the possibility that common method variance influenced the test of the hypothesis.
Nonetheless, future work could reduce the potential confounds associated with common
method variance by using longitudinal designs and objective measures.
The sample size may have lowered the power to verify construct reliability and to detect
significant relationships. However, the results supported most hypotheses and the response
size was larger than the average size reported by Cohen and Bailey (1997) for work teams
research. The response rate raises the possibility of selection bias. However, this poses less of
a threat when relationships among variables are studied, because the sample selection would
have to be linked to a relationship moderator in order to yield misleading results (Pelled et al.,
1999). Comparison to known population characteristics indicates that the pattern of
employment was similar for respondents and for enrolled students, which lessens the risk of
response bias (Armstrong and Overton, 1977; University College Dublin, 2004). Future
research should focus on replicating this research with larger samples to overcome the
limitations and restrictions to generalisability associated with the current sample size.
Despite these limitations, this study contributes to existing knowledge in a number of
areas. Our findings have potential to explain the contrary results found by researchers
investigating cognitive conflict. Given that extant measures of cognitive conflict include scale
items that measure both debate and cognitive disagreement, cognitive conflict may be viewed
as an aggregate measure of these two distinct constructs. When team behaviour in response to
cognitive disagreement incorporates debate, measures of cognitive conflict will be associated
with positive team outcomes. In contrast, when measures of cognitive conflict reflect higher
levels of cognitive disagreement of members, and lower levels of debate, such measures will
be associated with negative team outcomes.
The creation of knowledge has previously been linked to enhanced product development
and software development capabilities, innovation, quality of problem solution and overall
effectiveness at the team level (Fong, 2003; Nonaka and Takeuchi, 1996; Ravichandran and
Rai, 2003; Senge, 1990), and the results of this study indicate that this relies on the process of
debate which provides an enhanced capability to generate and analyse alternatives, and to
identify and resolve threats (Fong, 2003; Simons et al., 1999).
Previous research has advocated the use of functionally diverse teams to facilitate the
assimilation of disparate knowledge by teams members as they operate in a boundary-
spanning role by exchanging knowledge with members from distinct functional areas (Swan

630
Mitchell et al. / Asia Pacific Management Review 13(3) (2008) 625-634

et al., 2002). Bringing individuals from diverse functional areas together, provides the
potential to bridge structural holes, or gaps in the knowledge flow, between disconnected
functional areas (Burt, 2004). However, our results indicate that co-locating representatives
from different nodes in a knowledge network temporally and spatially is insufficient. Our
findings show that the process of debate is necessary to facilitate the brokerage of knowledge
across these previously disconnected nodes (Burt, 1997), thereby connecting previously
segregated accumulated knowledge (DeDreu and West, 2001; Huang and Newell, 2003; Jehn
et al., 1997; Randel and Jaussi, 2003; Vissers and Dankbaar, 2002). This study also yielded no
evidence of a relationship between cognitive disagreement and knowledge creation, which
may indicate that the manifestation of different ideas is less important than the processes used
to explore and challenge members’ positions. Future research should be directed to
investigating this proposition.
To conclude, this study contributes to research on diversity and conflict by providing an
explanation for contrary results, and by providing and a detailed operationalisation of
cognitive conflict and its component constructs. It also contributes to research into creativity
and innovation by providing insight into the dynamics underpinning knowledge sharing and
creation.

References

Amason, A. C. (1996) Distinguishing the effects of functional and dysfunctional conflict on


strategic decision making: Resolving a paradox for top management teams. Academy of
Management Journal, 39 (1), 123-148.
Ancona, D. G., Caldwell, D. F. (1992) Demography and Design: Predictors of new Product
Team Performance. Organization Science, 3 (2), 321-341.
Armstrong, J. S., Overton, T. S. (1977) Estimating nonresponse bias in mail surveys. Journal
of Marketing Research, 14, 396-402.
Austin, J. R. (1997) A cognitive framework for understanding demographic influences in
groups. International Journal of Organizational Analysis, 5 (4), 342-359.
Bantel, K. A., Jackson, S. E. (1989) Top management and innovations in banking - does the
composition of the top team make a difference. Strategic Management Journal,
10(Special Issue), 107-124.
Beech, N., MacIntosh, R., Maclean, D., Shepard, J., Stokes, J. (2002) Exploring constraints
on developing knowledge: On the need for conflict. Management Learning, 33 (4), 459.
Bhatt, G. (2000) Information dynamics, learning and knowledge creation in organizations.
The Learning Organization, 7 (2), 89-99.
Bliese, P. D., Halverson, R. R. (1996) Group size, group process effects and ICC values.
Academy of Management Proceedings, 333-337.
Brown, J. S., Duguid, P. (2001) Knowledge and organization: A social-practice perspective.
Organization Science, 12 (2), 198-216.
Bryant, S. E. (2005) The impact of peer mentoring on organizational knowledge creation and
sharing: An empirical study in a software firm. Group & Organization Management,
30(3), 319-338.
Burt, R. (1997) A note on social capital and network content. Social Networks, 19 (4), 355-
373.
Burt, R. (2004) Structural holes and good ideas. American Journal of Sociology, 110 (2),
349-399.
Chen, G., Tjosvold, D. (2002) Cooperative goals and constructive controversy for promoting
innovation in student groups in China. Journal of Education for Business, 78 (1), 46-50.
Cohen, S. G., Bailey, D. E. (1997) What makes teams work: Group effectiveness research
from the shop floor to the executive suite. Journal of Management, 23 (3), 239-290.

631
Mitchell et al. / Asia Pacific Management Review 13(3) (2008) 625-634

De Dreu, C. K. W., Weingart, L. R. (2003) Task versus relationship conflict and team
effectiveness: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 741-749.
DeDreu, C., West, M. (2001) Minority dissent and team innovation: The importance of
participation in decision-making. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86 (6), 1191-1201.
Fong, P. (2003) Knowledge creation in multidisciplinary project teams: an empirical study of
the processes and their dynamic interrelationships. International Journal of Project
Management, 21 (7), 479-486.
Fong, P. S. W., Hills, M. J., Hayles, C. S. (2007) Dynamic knowledge creation through value
management teams. Journal of Management in Engineering, 23 (1), 40-49.
Gruenfeld, D. H., Thomas-Hunt, M., Kim, P. H. (1998) Divergent thinking, accountability,
and integrative complexity: Public versus private reactions to majority and minority
status. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 202-226
Huang, J., Newell, S. (2003) Knowledge integration processes and dynamics within the
context of cross-functional projects. International Journal of Project Management, 21 (3),
167-176.
James, L. R. (1982) Aggregation bias in estimates of perceptual agreement. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 67 (2), 219-229.
James, L. R., Demaree, R. G., Wolf, G. (1984) Estimating within-group interrater reliability
with and without response bias. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69 (1), 85-98.
Jehn, K.A., Northcraft, G. B., Neale, M. (1999) Why differences make a difference: A field
study of diversity, conflict and performance in work groups. Administrative Science
Quarterly, 44 (4), 741-763.
Jehn, K. A. (1995) A multimethod examination of the benefits and detriments of intragroup
conflict. Administrative Science Quarterly, 40(June), 256–282.
Jehn, K. A. (1997) Qualitative analysis of conflict types and dimensions in organizational
groups. Administrative Science Quarterly, 42 (3), 530-557.
Jehn, K. A., Chadwick, C., Thatcher, S. M. B. (1997) To agree or not to agree: The effects of
value congruence, individual demographic dissimilarity, and conflict on workgroup
outcomes. International Journal of Conflict Management, 8 (4), 287-305.
Johnson, D. R., Creech, J. C. (1983) Ordinal measures in multiple indicator models: A
simulation study of categorization error. American Sociological Review, 48, 398-407.
Klein, K. J., Kozlowski, S. W. J. (2000) From micro to meso: Critical steps in
conceptualizing and conducting multilevel research. Organizational Research Methods, 3
(3), 211.
Nemeth, C. J. (1986) The differential contributions of majority and minority influence.
Psychological Review, 93 (23-32).
Nemeth, C. J., Connell, J., Rogers, J., Brown, K. (2001) Improving decision making by
means of dissent. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 31, 48-58.
Nemeth, C. J., Nemeth-Brown, B. (2003) Better than individuals? The potential benefits of
dissent and diversity for group creativity. In P. B. Paulus (Ed.), Group Creativity:
Innovation through Collaboration. Oxford University Press, London, pp. 63-84
Nonaka, I., Takeuchi, H. (1996) A theory of organizational knowledge creation. International
Journal of Technology Management, 11(7/8), 833-846.
Parent, M., Gallupe, R. B. (2000) Knowledge creation in focus groups: Can group
technologies help? Information & Management, 38 (1), 47.
Paulus, P. B., Yang, H. (2000) Idea generation in groups: A basis for creativity in
organizations, organizational behaviour and human decision processes
Pelled, L. H., Eisenhardt, K. M., Xin, K. R. (1999) Exploring the black box: An analysis of
work group diversity, conflict and performance. Administrative Science Quarterly, 44 (1),
1-27.

632
Mitchell et al. / Asia Pacific Management Review 13(3) (2008) 625-634

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J.-Y., Podsakoff, N. P. (2003) Common method
biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended
remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88 (5), 879-903.
Podsakoff, P. M., Organ, D. W. (1986) Self-reports in organizational research: Problems and
prospects. Journal of Management, 12 (4), 531-544.
Randel, A., Jaussi, K. (2003) Functional background identity, diversity and individual
performance in cross-functional teams. Academy of Management Journal, 46 (6), 763-
774.
Ravichandran, T., Rai, A. (2003) Structural analysis of the impact of knowledge creation and
knowledge embedding on software process capability. IEEE Transactions on Engineering
Management, 50 (3), 270-284.
Riordan, C., Shore, L. (1997) Demographic diversity and employee attitudes: Examination of
relational demography within work units. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 342-358.
Russell, C. J., Bobko, P. (1992) Moderated regression analysis and likert scales. Too coarse
for comfort. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 336-342.
Senge, P. (1990) The leader's new work: Building learning organizations. Sloan Management
Review, 32 (1), 7-23.
Shaw, J. B., Barrett-Power, E. (1998) The effects of diversity on small work group processes
and performance. Human Relations, 51 (10), 1307.
Simons, T., Pelled, L. H., Smith, K. A. (1999) Making use of difference: Diversity, debate,
and decision comprehensiveness in top management teams. Academy of Management
Journal, 42 (6), 662-673.
Swan, J., Scarbrough, H., Robertson, M. (2002) The construction of 'communities of practice'
in the management of innovation. Management Learning, 33 (4), 477-496.
Tjosvold, D., Poon, M. (1998) Dealing with scarce resources: Openminded interaction for
resolving budget conflicts. Group and Organization Management, 23 (3), 237-258.
Tsui, A. S., Ashford, S. J., St. Clair, L., Xin, K. R. (1995) Dealing with discrepant
expectations: Response strategies and managerial effectiveness. Academy of
Management Journal, 38 (6), 1515.
Tushman, M. L., Nadler, D. A. (1978) Information processing as an integrating concept in
organizational design. Academy of Management Review, 3, 613-624.
Tushman, M. T. (1977) Special boundary roles in the innovation process. Administrative
Science Quarterly, 22, 587-605.
University College Dublin. (2004) Class Profile. Retrieved 4 June, 2005, from
http://ucdbusiness.ucd.ie/index.cfm?area=content&action=contentselect&menuid=207&a
ncestorlist1=0,155,182&MainNavigationMenuID=15
Van der Vegt, G., Bunderson, J. S. (2005) Learning and performanmce in multidisciplinary
teams: The importance of collective team identification. Academy of Management
Journal, 48 (3), 532-547.
Van Dyne, L., Saavedra, R. (1996). A naturalistic minority influence experiment: Effects on
divergent thinking, conflict, and originality in work groups. British Journal of Social
Psychology, 35. (Special Issue on Minority Influence), 151-167.
van Knippenberg, D., De Dreu, C. K. W., Homan, A. C. (2004) Work group diversity and
group performance: An integrative model and research agenda. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 89 (6), 1008-1022.
Vissers, G., Dankbaar, B. (2002). Creativity in multidisciplinary new product development
teams. Creativity & Innovation Management, 11 (1), 31-42.
Webber, S. S., Donahue, L. M. (2001) Impact of highly and less job-related diversity on
work group cohesion and performance: a meta-analysis. Journal of Management, 27 (2),
141.

633
Mitchell et al. / Asia Pacific Management Review 13(3) (2008) 625-634

West, M. A., Anderson, N. R. (1996). Innovation in top management teams. Journal of


Applied Psychology, 81, 680-693.

634

View publication stats

You might also like