An International Comparison Between Different Theoretical Orientations of Psychotherapy: A Survey of Expert Opinions (Solem & Vogel, 2010)

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 24

ISSN 0278-8403

ASSOCIATION FOR BEHAVIORAL


AND COGNITIVE THERAPIES VOLUME 33, NO. 1 • JANUARY 2010

the Behavior Therapist


Contents
International Scene
International Scene
Stian Solem, Patrick A. Vogel, and Stefan Hofmann
An International Comparison Between Different
An International
Theoretical Orientations of Psychotherapy: Comparison Between
A Survey of Expert Opinions • 1
Different Theoretical
Clinical Forum Orientations of
Warren W. Tryon
Professional Identity Based on Learning • 9
Psychotherapy: A
Warren W. Tryon Survey of Expert
Learning as Core of Psychological Science
and Clinical Practice • 10
Opinions
Stian Solem and Patrick A. Vogel, Norwegian
Letters to the Editor
University of Science and Technology, and
Jedidiah Siev, Jonathan Huppert, and Dianne Chambless
Stefan G. Hofmann, Boston University
Treatment Specificity for Panic Disorder:
A Reply to Wampold, Imel, and Miller (2009) • 12
esearch in psychotherapy is an international
Stefan Hofmann and Jeffrey Lohr
To Kill a Dodo Bird • 14

David H. Barlow
R endeavour. There is, however, a paucity of
data available as to which forms of psy-
chotherapy are most commonly practiced through-
out the world. Most articles on this topic have been
The Dodo Bird—Again—and Again • 15 published with focus on certain treatments for spe-
Steven Taylor, Dean McKay, and Jonathan Abramowitz cific disorders in selected countries or regions. Little
More on the Brain Disease Model of Mental Disorders • 16 is known about a comparison between different
therapeutic orientations on an international level.
Several surveys of psychotherapy trends have
Book Reviews been conducted in the U.S. In a national survey
Reviewed by Rachel A. Annunziato with 100 American therapists (a response rate of
25%), Wildman and Wildman (1967) found eclec-
Cooper, Todd, & Wells (2009). Treating Bulimia Nervosa
tic therapy to be the most common, followed by
and Binge Eating: An Integrated Metacognitive and Cognitive
psychoanalysis and client-centered therapy. The
Therapy Manual • 18 frequent use of eclectic therapy has also been indi-
Reviewed by Carlo C. DiClemente cated by other surveys (Garfield & Kurtz, 1976,
Klingemann & Sobell (Eds.). (2007). Promoting Self-Change 1977; Norcross, 1986; Smith, 1982). Similarly,
Corrigan, Hess, and Garman (1998) found, in a
From Addictive Behaviors: Practical Implications for Policy,
sample of 55 psychologists (again a 25% response
Prevention, and Treatment • 19
rate), that 50% were actually trained under a cog-
nitive behavioral orientation (the majority), but
Classified • 8 more than 60% practiced according to eclectic
models. A study by Stevens and Dinoff (1996),
p CALL . . . for Papers: 2010 Annual Convention • 21 which did not include the eclectic orientation,
l found in a sample of 69 instructors (a response rate
CALL . . . for Award Nominations • 22
u of 47%) that cognitive behavioral therapy was
s CALL . . . for Officer Nominations, 2010 • back page

January • 2010 1
the Behavior Therapist

M
Published by the Association for
Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies

te
305 Seventh Avenue - 16th Floor
New York, NY 10001-6008
entorship

gradua
(212) 647-1890 / Fax: (212) 647-1865
www.abct.org

EDITOR · · · · · · · · · · · · Drew Anderson


Editorial Assistant . . . . . . . . Melissa Them
Behavior Assessment . . . Timothy R. Stickle directory
Book Reviews · · · · · · · · · · · C. Alix Timko
Clinical Forum · · · · · · · · · · · John P. Forsyth
Clinical Dialogues . . . . . . . Brian P. Marx Are you a faculty member in a graduate program?
International Scene . . . . . . . . Rod Holland Are you a student applying to graduate school?
Institutional
Settings. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . David Penn If so, check out the new ABCT Graduate Mentorship Directory. The
Tamara Penix Sbraga
Graduate Mentorship Directory is intended to provide students with
Lighter Side · · · · · · · · · · · · Elizabeth Moore
an opportunity to learn which individual ABCT members regularly
List Serve Editor . . . . . . . . Laura E. Dreer
mentor students in their respective graduate programs. The history
News and Notes. . . . . . . . . David DiLillo
of psychology, and especially the history of the cognitive and behav-
Laura E. Dreer
James W. Sturges ioral therapies, is one of lineage and relationships, where profession-
Public Health Issues. . . . Jennifer Lundgren als trace their lineage back three or four generations. This directory
Research-Practice is not intended as an exhaustive list of graduate programs; rather, it
Links · · · · · · · · · · · · · · · · David J. Hansen is a list of ABCT members affiliated with programs in which they are
Research-Training potentially available to serve as a mentor.
Links · · · · · · · · · · · · · · · · Gayle Y. Iwamasa
Science Forum · · · · · · · · · · · Jeffrey M. Lohr http://www.abct.org/Mentorship/?m=mMentorship&fa=meMain
Special Interest
Groups · · · · · · · · · · Andrea Seidner Burling
Technology Update. . . . . . James A. Carter INSTRUCTIONS Ñçê AUTHORS
ABCT President . . . . . . . Frank Andrasik The Association for Behavioral and Submissions must be accompanied by
Executive Director · · · · · · Mary Jane Eimer Cognitive Therapies publishes the Behavior a Copyright Transfer Form (a form is
Director of Education & Therapist as a service to its membership. printed on p. 24 of the January 2008 issue
Meeting Services . . . . . . Mary Ellen Brown Eight issues are published annually. The of tBT, or contact the ABCT central of-
Director of Communications David Teisler purpose is to provide a vehicle for the rapid fice): submissions will not be reviewed without
dissemination of news, recent advances, a copyright transfer form. Prior to publication
Managing Editor . . . . . Stephanie Schwartz
and innovative applications in behavior authors will be asked to submit a final
therapy. electronic version of their manuscript.
Copyright © 2010 by the Association for Behavioral Authors submitting materials to tBT do so
and Cognitive Therapies. All rights reserved. No Feature articles that are approximately
part of this publication may be reproduced or trans- 16 double-spaced manuscript pages may with the understanding that the copyright
mitted in any form, or by any means, electronic or of the published materials shall be as-
mechanical, including photocopy, recording, or any be submitted.
information storage and retrieval system, without signed exclusively to ABCT. Submissions
Brief articles, approximately 6 to 12 via e-mail are preferred and should be sent
permission in writing from the copyright owner.
Subscription information: the Behavior Therapist is double-spaced manuscript pages, are to the editor at drewa@albany.edu.
published in 8 issues per year. It is provided free to preferred.
ABCT members. Nonmember subscriptions are Please include the phrase tBT submission
available at $40.00 per year (+$32.00 airmail Feature articles and brief articles in the subject line of your e-mail. Include
postage outside North America). should be accompanied by a 75- to the first author’s e-mail address on the
Change of address: 6 to 8 weeks are required for
address changes. Send both old and new addresses to 100 -word abstract. cover page of the manuscript attachment.
the ABCT office. Letters to the Editor may be used to By conventional mail, please send manu-
ABCT is committed to a policy of equal opportu-
nity in all of its activities, including employment. respond to articles published in the scripts to:
ABCT does not discriminate on the basis of race, Behavior Therapist or to voice a profes- Drew A. Anderson, Ph.D.
color, creed, religion, national or ethnic origin, sex, sional opinion. Letters should be lim-
sexual orientation, gender identity or expression, SUNY–Albany
age, disability, or veteran status. ited to approximately 3 double-spaced Dept. of Psychology/SS369
All items published in the Behavior Therapist, manuscript pages. 1400 Washington Ave.
including advertisements, are for the information of
our readers, and publication does not imply endorse- Albany, NY 12222
ment by the Association.

2
among the most common forms of therapy other countries and regions also exists world and, more specifically, investigate
being used (30%), followed by perspectives (Ardila, 1982 [Latin America]; Danquah, whether there are variances for certain psy-
such as the psychodynamic (28%), interper- 1982 [Ghana]; De Silva & Samarasinghe, chological disorders. In order to achieve this
sonal (15%), humanistic (15%), and behav- 1985 [Sri Lanka]; Dowdall, 1982 [South goal, we sampled the opinions of leading re-
ioral (13%). Similar surveys have been Africa]; Mikulas, 1983 [Thailand]; Tsoi & searchers/clinicians from boards of influen-
conducted for training institutes (Sayette & Lam, 1991 [Hong Kong]; Yamagami, tial international therapy organizations.
Mayne, 1990; Weissman et al., 2006) and Okuma, Moringaga, & Nakao, 1982, Although this did not result in a representa-
in medical settings in the U.S. (Scott, [Japan]). tive sample of practicing clinicians (a desir-
Pollack, Otto, Simon, & Worthington, Although several studies have looked able but highly unrealistic goal), our
1999). into the status of certain psychotherapeutic method provided an important first step to-
Along with research employing survey orientations in certain regions of the world, ward a worldwide comparison of different
methodology to assess which psychothera- only one large international study has been theoretical orientations of psychotherapies
peutic orientations are being used, polls conducted, which included nearly 5,000 and it is the first study utilizing a standard-
have been used to predict which orienta- psychotherapists from 285 countries ized Internet survey format. Recent articles
tions are likely to increase in popularity. A (Orlinsky & Rønnestad, 2005). Analytic- (Sanderson & Bruce, 2007; Stallard,
panel of 62 psychotherapy experts (the psychodynamic (58%) was the most com- Udwin, Goddard, & Hibbert, 2007) have
Delphi poll) with diverse theoretical orien- mon orientation, followed by humanistic similarly utilized survey methods to assess
tations have predicted CBT, culture-sensi- (31%), cognitive (24%), systemic (21%), expert opinions on particular issues.
tive, cognitive, and eclectic/integrative and behavioral (14%). However, no com- Furthermore, expert consensus is com-
theories to increase the most in the follow- parisons regarding possible trends in differ- monly utilized in developing good practice
ing years, whereas classical psychoanalysis, ent countries or regions were analyzed. It guidelines.
solution-focused theories, and transactional seems intuitively important for researchers,
analysis has been expected to decline educators, clinical administrators, and po- Method
(Norcross, Hedges, & Prochaska, 2002). litical policymakers to be able to assess the Participants and Procedure
While eclectic therapy has been quite current status of different types of therapy
dominant in the U.S., there have been simi- for different types of problems. Possible participants were recruited via
lar surveys in Europe and many of them Surveying expert opinions regarding e-mail. Following the recommendations by
have documented an increased use of the psychotherapy practice may be a potentially Dillmann (2000), reminder e-mails were
cognitive-behavioral approach (Agathon, fruitful approach. It could give indications sent or personal contact was made after 2
1982 [France]; del Barrio & Carpintero, of trends and differences regarding the de- weeks of the initial contact. All participants
2003 [Spain]; Meazzini & Rovetto, 1983 velopment of psychotherapy progress in in- were recruited based on their affiliation
[Italy]; Valderhaug, Götestam, & Larsson, dividual countries. The goal of this study with one of three theoretical orientations of
2004 [Norway]). Some research document- was to compare the status of different treat- psychotherapy: cognitive behavioral, psy-
ing that behavior therapy has taken root in ment orientations in different parts of the choanalytically oriented, and eclectic/inte-

Table 1. Characteristics of the Participating Experts

Psychoanalysis/
CBT group psychodynamic
Eclectic group
group

Return rate 35 of 69 7 of 39 24 of 75
Female gender 45.7% 28.6% 12.5%
Average age 47.1 (9.0) 65.0 (9.1) 48.3 (15.7)
Countries represented 16 3 8
Type of work:
Research and teaching 89% 43% 67%
Adult psychotherapy 43% 71% 63%
Adolescent psychotherapy 14% 0% 17%
Child psychotherapy 11% 0% 4%
Psychotherapy with all
age groups 0% 14% 0%
More than five years of training
in own orientation 80% 100% 71%
Cognitive behavioural orientation 94% 0% 17%
Psychoanalytical orientation 0% 100% 4%
Eclectic (integrative) orientation 3% 0% 58%
Other orientation 3% 0% 21%

January • 2010 3
grative psychotherapies. A total of 66 ex- spondents were somewhat older than the Measures
perts participated in the survey, resulting in respondents from the other orientations.
A questionnaire was designed specifi-
a return rate of 36.1% (66/183) for the total Furthermore, the CBT group represented
cally for the present study in order to de-
sample, which is considered a low, but still 16 countries (10 from UK, 4 from
scribe the experts’ perception of the status
acceptable, response rate (Babbie, 2004) Australia, 4 from the USA, 3 from the
of different orientations for their country. A
and comparable to similar studies. Netherlands, 3 from Canada, and 1 each
total of 23 items were included. The first
The CBT group was chosen based on from New Zealand, Turkey, Slovenia,
their names being listed in the scientific items asked for name, gender, age, and
Poland, Serbia, Germany, Belgium, Austria,
committee and the international scientific Estonia, Iceland, and Japan). Even though country, followed by items asking for main
advisory committee for the 2007 World they were recruited for the CBT group, 2 of area of work and theoretical orientation.
Congress of Behavioural and Cognitive the participants listed other orientations as The participants could choose from four
Therapies in Barcelona. Out of 69 experts their own (eclectic and empirical valida- possible orientations: (a) CBT, (b) psycho-
contacted, 35 responded. The return rate tion). The eclectic/integrative group repre- analysis/psychodynamic, (c) eclectic, and (d)
was 50.7%, which is considered adequate. sented 8 countries (3 from U.K., 3 from other treatments. The final items asked for
Seventy-five integrative therapists were Canada, 4 from U.S., 3 from Switzerland, 7 the most common treatments for patients
contacted based on the fact that they were from Portugal, 2 from Italy, 2 from with depression, anxiety disorders, and per-
listed as authors at the congress for The Germany, and 2 from Japan). The sonality disorders. The last item was open-
Society for the Exploration of Psycho- eclectic/integrative group consisted of sev- ended, asking for general comments
therapy Integration in Lisbon 2007. eral different specific theoretical orienta- regarding the status of their orientation in
Twenty-four participants from this group tions, including experiential/emotion- their country. The survey was posted on our
(32.0%) responded. Psychoanalytically ori- focused therapy, humanistic/interpersonal/ university’s website and took 10 to 15 min-
ented psychotherapists were chosen based object relational, clinical biopsychology, and utes to complete.
on their names being listed on the board of systemic. The psychoanalysts were from the
representatives or the research advisory U.S. (n = 4), Canada (n = 2), and Brazil (n = Results
board for the International Psychoanalytic 1). Results were analyzed for the three ques-
Association. A total of 39 experts were con- The participants were then grouped into tions regarding what therapy is delivered in
tacted, and of these, 7 responded, resulting different clusters depending on their geo- the participants’ country for (a) anxiety dis-
in a return rate of 17.9 %, which is consid- graphical belonging. A total of 7 regions orders, (b) depression, and (c) personality
ered low. were identified: North America, United disorders. Participants responded in per-
Table 1 shows the demographic charac- Kingdom, Northern Europe (including centages (i.e., What percentage of people
teristics of the participants. Besides the dif- Central Europe), Southern Europe, Eastern with anxiety disorders receive CBT in the
ferences in response rates among the Europe, Oceania (Australia/New Zealand), U.K.?). Participants with percentages to-
theoretical orientations surveyed, the CBT and Japan. The experts from Turkey and talling more or less than 5% off the total
respondents were more likely to be of fe- Serbia were clustered as belonging to 100% were accepted. Nine participants had
male gender and the psychodynamic re- Eastern Europe. to be excluded because they reported per-
centages that did not meet this criterion.
Only one participant from South America
responded to the survey and was therefore
left out of the following analyses. This left a
total of 56 participants for the following
analyses.
Figure 1 shows error bars with means
and 95% confidence intervals for treatment
of anxiety disorders, depression, and per-
sonality disorders across all countries. CBT
was described as the most frequently used
therapy for anxiety disorders and depres-
sion, but in treatment of personality disor-
ders it was a different pattern with
psychoanalysis/psychodynamic therapy and
eclectic therapies being just as common.

Comparison Between Regions


Regions with more than one participant
were analyzed to examine specific regional
effects for the three different disorder
groups. Table 2 shows the pattern for the
Figure 1. Comparison between cognitive behavior therapy (CBT), psychoanalytic/psychodynamic treatment of anxiety, Table 3 shows the pat-
approaches (PA/PD) and eclectic/integrative orientations (ECL) and other approaches (Other) for tern for the treatment of depression, and
the treatment of depression, anxiety disorders, and personality disorders across all countries. The Table 4 shows the pattern for the treatment
Figure shows means and 95% confidence intervals. of personality disorders.

4 the Behavior Therapist


Houston OCD Program

~ Dedicated to providing quality care in a compassionate atmosphere ~

The Houston OCD Program is located in the heart of the Montrose neighborhood of Houston.
The program resides in a beautiful two-story home and offers a warm and inviting
treatment environment.

A full continuum of care is offered for OCD, OC Spectrum and other anxiety disorders:

Outpatient Services ~ Intensive Outpatient Program ~ Residential Support Program

Our team of clinicians came from the nationally recognized OCD Treatment Program
at The Menninger Clinic and is directed by Thröstur Björgvinsson, Ph.D.

www.HoustonOCDprogram.org ~ info@HoustonOCDprogram.org ~ (832) 298-7075

Anxiety disorders. For treating anxiety Oceania; psychodynamic treatment was vey suggest obstacles regarding the dissem-
disorders, CBT seemed to be the most com- somewhat more prevalent in Southern ination of CBT. How to overcome these ob-
mon therapy for most regions. Two excep- Europe and Japan; and eclectic therapy stacles is uncertain due to the lack of
tions were Japan, where psychoanalysis seemed more common in Eastern Europe. research conducted in this area, but a recent
seemed more common, and Eastern paper addressed some critical issues
Europe, where eclectic therapies were at Discussion (Shafran et al., in press). Patients are not re-
least as common for treating anxiety disor- There is, to a certain extent, general ceiving evidence-based and well-delivered
ders as CBT. In Southern Europe psychoan- agreement among the experts from several CBT in routine clinical care. Some of the ob-
alytic treatment was also seen as just as stacles to this dissemination could involve
countries and differing theoretical orienta-
common as CBT. However, the significance the structure of the health service delivery,
tions about the status of psychotherapy
of these differences is in doubt given the financial barriers, and knowledge and be-
practice throughout the world. A CBT ori-
large standard deviations and small samples liefs among practitioners.
entation is prevalent in many countries for
from each country, so these findings should Increasing the availability of training in
be considered suggestive of trends. the treatment of anxiety and depression as CBT, which has been identified as a priority
Depression. Similar to the results for anxi- predicted by previous research (Norcross, in NIMH’s strategic plans (Insel, 2009),
ety, the results from Northern Europe, the Hedges, & Prochaska, 2002). The three and including therapists/students in clinical
U.K., and Oceania indicated that CBT was major treatment orientations focused on in research could be an important first step in
the most common treatment for depression. this survey seem to be employed equally, socializing clinicians to the value of CBT
Southern Europe and Japan both provided often in the treatment of personality disor- approaches. Another important first step
more psychoanalytical treatment for de- ders. Some regional variations in this pat- would be making treatment manuals from
pression. In Eastern Europe an eclectic ap- tern occur, but only in a few regions. RCTs easily available, and in languages
proach was more common. Finally, in Southern Europe and Japan had more psy- other than English. The data from our
North America there appeared to be a dis- choanalytical treatment both for depression study showed poorest CBT dissemination
agreement as to whether it is CBT or eclec- and for anxiety disorders, while eclectic and response rates to our English survey
tic therapy that was the most common therapy is frequently used in North from non-English-speaking countries.
treatment for depression. America and in Eastern Europe. Treatment manuals based on effectiveness
Personality disorders. For treating person- Although there is mounting evidence studies are also needed to address clinician
ality disorders the results indicate that there supporting the effectiveness of CBT (e.g., concerns about patient complexity and co-
was no clear treatment trend. In general, Hofmann & Smits, 2008), the regional dif- morbidity. A therapeutic culture that en-
CBT was more commonly administered in ferences apparently uncovered by this sur- courages regular evaluation of treatment

January • 2010 5
outcomes needs to be developed and com-
petency requirements for therapist training
in CBT are also needed. The role of govern-
Table 2. Treatment of Anxiety Disorders ment incentives and the support of con-
sumer groups may be essential in this part
Region N CBT Psychoanalysis/ Eclectic Other of the process.
Psychodynamic A relatively new development that
could aid dissemination of CBT involves
North America 16 44 (18) 18 (11) 31 (19) 7 (9) more use of modern technology in both
UK 12 50 (24) 11 (8) 24 (15) 15 (13) treatment and supervision. Electronic com-
Northern Europe 10 43 (26) 21 (17) 14 (7) 23(24) munication systems like videoconferencing
Southern Europe 6 39 (14) 36 (10) 13 (4) 13 (6) and the Internet can make access to expert
Eastern Europe 5 31 (26) 23 (21) 39 (31) 7 (6)
supervision and, in some cases, expert treat-
Oceania 5 61 (33) 5 (5) 19 (13) 15 (21)
ment available in rural settings and around
Japan 2 20 (0) 40 (14) 30 (14) 10 (0)
the world (Himle et al., 2006). This could
be essential to avoiding theoretical “drift.”
Note. Figures represent mean percentage and standard deviation. N = number of Administration of poor-quality CBT could
responding experts from the selected country.
yield poorer outcomes, which would be
devastating to the dissemination of CBT.
Similar questions could arise when dis-
cussing minimal treatment dose and thera-
pist background/training required.
It seems quite clear that there are obvi-
Table 3. Treatment of Depression ous gaps in our current knowledge about
Region N CBT Psychoanalysis/ Eclectic Other training, measuring competence, how
Psychodynamic treatment works, especially with more
complex cases, and the minimum dose re-
North America 16 37 (15) 20 (11) 34 (20) 9 (12) quired for treatment. All these issues may
UK 12 45 (22) 11 (6) 26 (14) 17 (14) limit the adoption of CBT protocols to clin-
Northern Europe 10 37 (26) 22 (16) 15 (8) 26 (22) ical settings around the world.
Southern Europe 6 28 (9) 41 (11) 18 (6) 13 (8) An important limitation of this survey
Eastern Europe 5 29 (27) 13 (10) 47 (33) 11 (7) is the small number of experts sampled
Oceania 5 51 (28) 8 (8) 17 (11) 24 (38) with few or no respondents from several
Japan 2 20 (0) 40 (14) 25 (7) 15 (7) countries. The Internet-based survey oper-
ating with a strict time limit can have pro-
Note. Figures represent mean percentage and standard deviation. N = number of duced a lower response rate. The sample
responding experts from the selected country. size obviously limits the interpretability of
the results obtained. The low overall rate of
response, except from CBT-oriented ex-
perts, may have biased the results in favor
of suggesting more CBT prevalence than is
warranted. However, the responses from
Table 4. Treatment of Personality Disorders non-CBT-oriented respondents were not
significantly in disagreement from the
Region N CBT Psychoanalysis/ Eclectic Other CBT-oriented respondents on most issues.
Psychodynamic The number of respondents and the re-
sponse rate was also within the range re-
North America 16 23 (10) 28 (11) 35 (21) 15 (15)
ported in most previous studies. The study
UK 12 31 (20) 34 (15) 23 (12) 11 (14)
by Orlinsky and Rønnestad (2005) have a
Northern Europe 10 29 (19) 31 (18) 14 (7) 25 (24)
much larger sample, but cognitive behav-
Southern Europe 6 28 (11) 44 (10) 19 (9) 9 (7)
Eastern Europe 5 18 (20) 26 (23) 45 (33) 13 (12)
ioral therapists are not as well represented
Oceania 5 48 (38) 13 (9) 19 (19) 19 (38)
in that study, they did not assess the thera-
Japan 2 10 (0) 50 (28) 30 (28) 10 (0)
pists’ perception of the most commonly
used methods in their country, and no re-
gional comparisons were made. The prob-
Note. Figures represent mean percentage and standard deviation. N = number of
responding experts from the selected country.
lem of possible arbitrariness of the labels
employed for the three theoretical orienta-
tions is an additional limitation, and reduc-
ing practiced orientations to only four
theoretical frameworks may be insufficient
to describe in detail the practice through-
out different countries and regions. The low

6 the Behavior Therapist


number of non-CBT expert respondents re- Garfield, S.L. & Kurtz, R. (1977). A study of der: A survey of psychiatrists. The Journal of
mains an important limitation and the re- eclectic views. Journal of Consulting and Nervous and Mental Disease, 187, 755-757.
sults cannot be said to be representative of Clinical Psychology, 45, 78-83. Shafran, R., Clark, D. M., Fairburn, C. G.,
the opinions of psychodynamic or eclectic Himle, J. A., Fischer, D. J., Muroff, J. R., van Arntz, A., Barlow, D. H., Ehlers, A., et al. (in
expert opinions. A future study should try Etten, M. L., Lokers, L. M., Abelson, J. L., et press). Mind the gap: Improving the dissem-
to obtain more equal representation from al. (2006). Videoconferencing-based cogni- ination of CBT. Behaviour Research and
tive-behavioral therapy for obsessive-com- Therapy.
different theoretical perspectives on ther-
pulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Smith, D. (1982). Trends in counseling and psy-
apy. Therapy, 44, 1821-1829. chotherapy. American Psychologist, 37, 802-
The responses to this survey should be 809.
Hofmann, S. G., & Smits, J. A. J. (2008).
considered a “snapshot” of the current opin- Cognitive-behavioral therapy for adult anxi- Stallard, P., Udwin, O., Goddard, M., &
ions of many respected experts from around ety disorders: A meta-analysis of randomized Hibbert, S. (2007). The availability of cogni-
the world. Hopefully, the results presented placebo-controlled trials. Journal of Clinical tive behaviour therapy within specialist child
here will stimulate discussions within na- Psychiatry, 69, 621-632. and adolescent mental health services
tional and international organizations Insel, T. R. (2009). Translating scientific oppor- (CAMHS): A national survey. Behavioural
about these questions. Other forms of sur- tunity into public health impact: a strategic and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 35, 501-505.
veys that increase the representativeness plan for research on mental illness. Archives of Stevens, H.B., & Dinoff, B.L. (1996).
and sample size to these and other questions General Psychiatry, 66, 128–133. Orientations and characteristics of psy-
would be useful for further study. Meazzini, P., & Rovetto, F. (1983). Behaviour chotherapy instructors in clinical psychology
therapy: The Italian way. Journal of Behavior programs: A national survey. The Clinical
References Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 14, 5-9. Psychologist, 49, 11–15.
Mikulas, W. L. (1983). Thailand and behaviour Tsoi, M. M. & Lam, D. J. (1991). The practice of
Agathon, M. (1982). Behavior therapy in France behavior therapy among clinical psycholo-
1976-1981. Journal of Behavior Therapy and modification. Journal of Behavior Therapy and
Experimental Psychiatry, 14, 93-97. gists in Hong Kong. Journal of Behavior
Experimental Psychiatry, 13, 271-277. Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 22, 75-
Norcross, J. C. (1986). Handbook of eclectic psy-
Ardila, R. (1982). International developments in 82.
chotherapy. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
behaviour therapy in Latin America. Journal Valderhaug, R., Götestam, K. G., & Larsson, B.
of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Norcross, J. C., Hedges, M., & Prochaska, J. O.
(2004). Clinicians’ views on management of
Psychiatry, 13, 15-20. (2002). The face of 2010: A Delphi Poll on
obsessive-compulsive disorders in children
the future of psychotherapy. Professional
Babbie, E. (2004). The practice of social research. and adolescents. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry,
Psychology: Research and Practice, 33, 316–322
Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. 58, 125- 132.
Norcross, J. C., Karpiak, C. P., & Lister, K. M.
Corrigan, Hess, & Garman (1998). Results of a Weissman, M. M., Verdeli, H., Gameroff, M.J.,
(2005). What’s an integrationist? A study of
job analysis of psychologsts working in state Bledsoe, S. E., Betts, K., Mufson, L.,
self-identified integrative and (occasionally)
hospitals. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 54, 11- Fitterling, H., & Wickramaratne, P. (2006).
eclectic psychologists. Journal of Clinical
18 National Survey of Psychotherapy Training
Psychology, 61, 1587-1594.
Danquah, S. A. (1982). The practice of behav- in Psychiatry, Psychology, and Social Work.
Orlinsky, D. E., & Rønnestad, M. H. (2005). Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 925-934
iour therapy in West Africa: The case of How psychotherapists develop: A study of therapeu-
Ghana. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Wildman, R. W., & Wildman II, R. W. (1967).
tic work and professional growth. Washington,
Experimental Psychiatry, 13, 5-13. The practice of clinical psychology in the
DC: American Psychological Association.
del Barrio, V., & Carpintero, H. (2003). Clinical United States. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
Pachana, N. A., O’Donovan, A., & Helmes, E. 23, 291 – 295.
psychology in Spain. Journal of Clinical (2006). Australian clinical psychology train-
Psychology, 59, 687-699. Yamagami, T., Okuma, H., Morinaga, Y. and
ing program directors survey. Australian
De Silva, P., & Samarasinghe, D. (1985). Nakao, H. (1982). The practice of behaviour
Psychologist, 41, 168-178. therapy in Japan. Journal of Behavior Therapy
Behavior therapy in Sri Lanka. Journal of Sanderson, W. C., & Bruce, T. J. (2007). Causes and Experimental Psychiatry, 13, 21-26.
Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, and management of treatment-resistant
16, 95-100. panic disorder and agoraphobia: A survey of . . .
Dillman, D. A. (2000). Mail and internet surveys: expert therapists. Cognitive and Behavioral
The tailored design method. New York: John Practice, 14, 26-35.
Wiley & Sons. Dr. Hofmann is supported by NIMH grant
Sayette, M. A., & Mayne, T. J. (1990). Survey of R01MH078308 and Organon for issues and
Dowdall, T. (1982). Behavior therapy in South current clinical and research trends in clinical
projects unrelated to this study.
Africa: a review. Journal of Behavior Therapy psychology. American Psychologist, 45, 1263-
and Experimental Psychiatry, 13, 279-286. 1266. Correspondence to Stian Solem, Dept. of
Garfield, S. L., & Kurtz, R. M. (1976). Clinical Scott, E. L., Pollack, M. H., Otto, M. W., Simon, Psychology, Norwegian University of Science
psychologists in the 1970’s. American N. M., & Worthington, J. J. (1999). Clinician and Technology, 7491 Trondheim, Norway
Psychologist, 31, 1-9. response to treatment refractory panic disor- e-mail: stian.solem@svt.ntnu.no

January • 2010 7
Substance Abuse Research at Harvard Medical School
Two junior faculty to serve as project directors for two 5-year NIH-funded Harvard Medical School
randomized trials in Boston area and Worcester. Studies compare behavioral couples therapy (BCT) vs.
individual treatment for substance abuse patients. One project studies BCT with female drug patients;
the other examines group BCT with alcoholic patients.
Requires doctorate in psychology or related field, and excellent skills in managing project, staff, & data;
& in data analysis & writing. Prefer scientist practitioner clinical psychologist (licensed or licensed-eligible)
or outstanding non-clinician researcher with relevant experience. Prefer demonstrated success in clinical
sample recruitment, experience in large complex project, publications, grant writing experience.
Seeking individuals with career interest in families and addiction research (e.g., couple and family therapy,
IPV, child adjustment) to be a major collaborator on this and related research. Career development
opportunities include publishing from extensive archival data-sets and taking part in future work on dis-
semination of BCT. Application reviews will begin Nov 15, 2009 and will continue until positions are
filled.
To apply, submit a single PDF file (with your last name as file name) containing a cover letter, vita, names
of 3 references to Timothy O’Farrell, Ph.D., Families and Addiction Program, Harvard Psychiatry Dept by
email to: timothy_ofarrell@hms.harvard.edu with cc to leslie_reid@hms.harvard.edu. Informal inquiries
are welcome.
Harvard University is an Affirmative Action/Equal Opportunity Employer.

ADVERTISING in the Behavior Therapist


Classified Classified ads are only $4.00 per line. For ISSUE DEADLINE
a free price estimate, attach the text of January December 4
your ad in the form of a Word document
PROFESSIONAL OFFICE SPACE February January 5
and email Stephanie Schwartz at
AVAILABLE 2 to 3 days per week in sschwartz@abct. For information on dis- March February 3
well established pediatric speech ther- play ads, deadlines, and rates, contact S. April March 3
apy practice in medical building on East Schwartz at the email above or visit our June May 4
86th Street (between 2nd and 3rd) in website at www.abct.org and click on
September August 3
NYC. Great opportunities for referrals. ADVERTISE . Look to the right: you will find
October Sept. 3
Please contact linda.bejoian@gmail. the 2010 deadlines.
com or 212-861-7800. Winter November 3

half-page horizontal
display ad in tBT
ABCT
r
membe 7 1/8” x 4 3/4”
special
Don’t forget ABCT’s on-line . . .

$250 $185.00

JOB SEEKERS | EMPLOYERS Seize the Moment!


Contact sschwartz@abct.org
http://jobbank.abct.org

8 the Behavior Therapist


Clinical Forum plications. Such a move would bring us
back to the vision that the founding fathers
had for our organization (cf. Tryon, 2010).
Professional Identity Based on Learning References
Warren W. Tryon, Fordham University Bower, G. H., & Hilgard, E. R. (1997). Theories of
learning. New York: Prentice Hall.
Carlson, N. R. (2010). Physiology of behavior (10th
pen-minded professional identity ried a section discussing the fact that behav-

O
ed.). Boston: Pearson.
can be a good thing. It can give de- ior therapists are at another theoretical
Leahy, R. L. (2009). The confessions of a cogni-
finition and direction to research crossroad. This could set the occasion for tive therapist. the Behavior Therapist, 32, 1, 3.
and clinical work leading to consistent further turf wars that require public per-
Moran, D. J. (Ed.). (2008). The three waves of
methods with clear theoretical bases. Such sonas that may contrast with private profes- behavior therapy: Course corrections or navi-
clarity is preferable to a vague eclectic iden- sional practices. Staats (1983) long ago gation errors? [Special issue]. the Behavior
tity. A clear professional identity can also documented the corrosive effects that pro- Therapist, 31(8). Retrieved December 8,
help locate professionals with similar and/or fessional motives to be new and different 2009, from http://www.abct.org/docs
compatible research and/or clinical inter- can have. Perhaps it is time to focus on what /PastIssue/31n8.pdf
ests. However, Leahy’s (2009) “Confessions we share in common rather than what Staats, A. W. (1983). Psychology’s crisis of disunity:
of a Cognitive Therapist” indicates that we makes us different. Tryon (2000) reminded Philosophy and method for the revolution to a uni-
unfortunately have a degree of closed- us that “All successful psychotherapy, and fied science. New York: Praeger.
minded professional identity with us today especially behavior therapy, entails some de- Tryon, W. W. (2000). Behavior therapy as ap-
gree of new learning. Our theoretical differ- plied learning theory. the Behavior Therapist,
and apparently have had so for a long time.
ences concerning what is learned and how it 23, 131-134.
Leahy confessed that in addition to being a
is learned neither negate nor diminish the Tryon, W. W. (2005). Possible mechanisms for
cognitive therapist, he also uses behavioral
central relevance of learning per se to psy- why desensitization and exposure therapy
and existential methods, depending upon work. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 67-95.
chology and behavior therapy” (p. 131).
the presenting clinical situation. Leahy
Carlson (2010) wrote, “Learning refers to Tryon, W. W., & McKay, D. (2009). Memory
specifically raised the issue of professional
the process by which experiences change modification as an outcome variable in anxiety
identity when he wrote, “Many people who our nervous system and hence our behavior. disorder treatment. Journal of Anxiety
don’t really know me will easily identify me We refer to these changes as memories” (italics Disorders, 23, 546-556.
as a ‘cognitive therapist’ . . . ” (p. 1). The in the original) (p. 440). The main point Tryon, W. W., & Misurell, J. R. (2008).
word “confession” implies wrong-doing here is that had Leahy’s professional identity Dissonance induction and reduction: A pos-
that was hidden but is now revealed. been about learning rather than a specific sible principle and connectionist mechanism
Wrong-doing is typically hidden in order to mode of intervention he would not have for why therapies are effective. Clinical
avoid punishment, which, in his case, might Psychology Review, 28, 1297-1309.
had to hide a portion of his professional
involve public criticism, possible problems practice. The various clinical practices noted Tryon, W. W. (2010). Learning as core of psycho-
publishing future articles, and maybe by Leahy (2009) can be considered methods logical science and clinical practice. the
greater difficulty getting grants. Behavior Therapist, 33, 10-12.
for identifying what needs to be learned and
At least the following five features of how it should best be taught to specific
professional identity characterize both clients. On the other hand, one might ob- . . .
open- and closed-minded professional iden- serve that the history of psychology con-
tity to varying degrees: (a) psychologists are tains multiple theories of learning (e.g., Correspondence to Warren W. Tryon, Ph.D.,
classified by their theoretical orientation by Bower & Hilgard, 1997) and that fierce ri- Fordham University, 441 E. Fordham Rd.,
themselves and by other people; (b) psy- valries once existed among various schools Dept. of Psychology, Bronx, NY 10458;
chologists derive their professional identity and camps based on strong professional wtryon@fordham.edu
from this classification; (c) this identifica- identification with particular theories of
tion influences the professional organiza- learning. How is the recommended refocus-
tions they join, the journals they read, the ing on learning to avoid a return to such
manuscripts and grants they review, the conflicting schools and camps? The answer
meetings they attend, and what they teach is that modern neuroscience has replaced
through workshops and/or classes; (d) alle- theories of learning with empirically sup-
giance to the in-group, defined by similarly ported mechanisms of learning and mem-
classified individuals, is expected; and (e) ory formation (cf. Tryon, 2010). These
defense of the in-group is valued and mer- developments provide us with a unified em-
its/motivates opposition to alternative ap- pirically supported understanding of learn-
proaches. The resulting dominance ing and memory upon which we can build
competitions can obscure our focus on pa- our field (e.g.. Tryon, 2005; Tryon &
tient care and need to move our field for- McKay, 2009; Tryon & Misurell, 2008).
ward. Given our present position at yet another
The importance of Leahy’s “confession” theoretical crossroad, I recommend that our
is augmented by a recent special issue of the way forward is to emphasize our common
Behavior Therapist (Moran, 2008) that car- interest in learning and its therapeutic ap-

January • 2010 9
Clinical Forum (in press) and Tryon and McKay (2009) pro-
vided an overview of some of this mecha-
nism information. Squire et al. (2008), Bear,
Learning as Core of Psychological Science Connors, and Paradiso (2007), and Carlson
(2010) provided many more details. The
and Clinical Practice Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Adolescent Psychiatry has recently informed
Warren W. Tryon, Fordham University child psychiatrists about the clinical impli-
cations of biological learning mechanisms
(cf. Lombroso & Ogren, 2008, 2009).
recent special issue of the Behavior and memory mechanisms enable virtually Operant and respondent conditioning may

A Therapist (Moran, 2008) revealed


that behavior therapists seem to be
at another theoretical crossroad. As one of
all psychological development and inter-
ventions. We can therefore confidently con-
clude that all clinically effective empirically
supported psychological interventions en-
have been the first two controlled methods
that were used to systematically study the
functional relationships between experience
those ABCT members “… with ages that and behavior but people learn in other ways
start with 6 or more …” (Hayes, 2008, p. tail learning. A corollary point is that all ev- as well (e.g., observational learning).
150) who began their formal study of psy- idence of altered cognition, affect, and Psychologists deferred to psychiatrists re-
chology as an undergraduate in 1962, I behavior is also evidence that learning has garding diagnosis, who then developed the
write with a personal historical perspective occurred. Therapists, and the therapeutic ap- DSM series that now dominates psycholog-
on these transitions. I was a psychology proaches that currently divide us, differ only with ical research and clinical practice. Will psy-
major in college when Eysenck (1964) regard to what is to be learned and how it is to be chologists now also defer to psychiatrists
wrote, “Behaviour therapy may be defined acquired. It is therefore ironic that neglect of regarding learning and lose a second oppor-
as the attempt to alter human behaviour learning by both psychological science and tunity to lead our field?
and emotion in a beneficial manner accord- clinical practitioners has jointly exacerbated Psychologists require more than biologi-
ing to the laws of modern learning theory” the science-practice gap. Psychologists once cal mechanisms upon which to base their
(p. 1), when Wolpe and Lazarus (1966) studied learning but the cognitive revolu- explanations and interventions. Brains con-
wrote that behavior therapy entailed “... the tion abandoned such inquiry and focused sist of neural networks. Formal connection-
application of experimentally established intellectual and financial resources on how ist network models that extend informal
principles of learning” (p. 1). I was a gradu- people process information. Learning was psychological network models (e.g.,
ate student when Wolpe (1969) defined be- assumed rather than investigated. This shift Chemtob, Roitblat, Hamada, Carlson, &
havior therapy as “…the use of away from learning aggravated the science- Twenty-man, 1988; Creamer, Burgess, &
experimentally established principles of practice gap because it neglected to study Pattison, 1992; Foa, & Kozak, 1986; Lang,
learning for the purpose of changing un- the most basic process upon which thera- 1977; 1985; 1994) have been developed
adaptive behavior” (p. vii). It turned out pists depend: how to get people to learn to and are capable of simulating a broad range
that these definitions of behavior therapy change the way they think, feel, and act. of psychological and behavioral phenomena
were mainly aspirational because learning Clinicians also contributed to the science- (e.g., Arbib, 2002; Bechtel & Abrahamsen,
theory was in disarray at the time and only practice gap by moving away from learning 2002; McLeod, Plunkett, & Rolls, 1998;
partially able to support clinical practice (cf. principles and theory. For example, Hayes O’Reilly, & Munakata, 2000; Rumelhart &
Tryon, 2000, 2002). The cognitive revolu- (2008) noted that “Some previously foun- McClelland, 1986a, 1986b). These net-
tion that swept psychology in general, and dational ideas (e.g., behavior therapists work models are generalizations of the clas-
behavior therapy in particular, during the needed extensive training in the psychology sic S-O-R cognitive-behavioral model
1970s ushered in the second wave of behav- of learning) began heading toward extinc- (Tryon, in press). Siegle (2001) discussed the
ior therapies, and now a third wave seems tion” (p. 150). With regard to specific ther- relevance of connectionist models of psy-
under way. In the recent tBT discussion of apies, Hayes noted that “…the underlying chopathology and Siegle (1991) used con-
these issues cited above, DiGiuseppe (2008) principles became looser and less linked to nectionist models to explore attention
suggested “… that we explore what unites behavioral science, resulting in theories that biases in depression. Tryon (1999) applied
behavior therapy” (p. 155). were harder to disprove. The original goal the part-whole pattern completion connec-
I proposed that our interest in learning of empirically validated procedures was re- tionist principle and property of the
and memory provides us with a solid and tained, but the original vision of a transla- Bidirectional Associative Memory model to
useful professional identity (Tryon, 2010; tional applied science linked to explain posttraumatic stress disorder ac-
this issue). Carlson (2010) wrote, “Learning well-established basic principles weakened” cording to criteria specified by Jones and
refers to the process by which experiences (p. 151). These trends continue unabated Barlow (1990) and Brewin, Dalgleish, and
change our nervous system and hence our today. Joseph (1996). Tryon (2005) reviewed exist-
behavior. We refer to these changes as memo- During the several decades in which ing explanations of why systematic desensi-
ries” (italics in the original; p. 440). most psychologists neglected the study of tization and exposure therapy work, found
Learning requires memory and memories learning, neuroscience replaced our theories them all to be flawed, and proposed an al-
are learned. Learning and memory are two of learning (e.g., Bower & Hilgard, 1997) ternative connectionist network model con-
facets of one major developmental mecha- with detailed mechanism information re- taining a specific change mechanism. Tryon
nism. If infants were unable to learn and/or garding how experience-dependent plasticity and Misurell (2008) extended this connec-
form memories, they would never develop (EDP) enables learning to occur and memo- tionist model/change mechanism to depres-
into the children, adolescents, and adults ries to form through the modification of sion and formulated a dissonance
that we are familiar with. In short, learning synaptic architecture and function. Tryon induction/reduction (DIR) principle that ex-

10 the Behavior Therapist


plains why empirically supported treat- are often more effective than either behavior cognitive action theory of post-traumatic
ments for anxiety and depression work. therapy or pharmacotherapy. Fourth, con- stress disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 2,
Tryon (in press) described how network nectionist models of experience-dependent 253-275.
models form cognitions, explained how plasticity inform us regarding the course of Creamer, M., Burgess, P., & Pattison, P. (1992).
placebos, nocebos, and psychoactive med- clinical change. They predict that cogni- Reaction to trauma: A cognitive processing
ications work, and extended network learn- tion, affect, and behavior change simultane- model. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101,
ing principles to the cognitive specificity ously, in parallel, not sequentially (Tryon, 452-459.
hypothesis. Renewed interest in the mecha- 2005). Connectionist models predict that DiGiuseppe, R. (2008). Surfing the waves of be-
nisms by which empirically supported cognition does not change before affect havior therapy. the Behavior Therapist, 31,
154-155.
treatments work (e.g., Kazdin, 2007, 2008; and/or behavior, that affect does not change
Tryon, 2009b) has set the occasion to recon- before cognition and/or behavior, and that Eysenck, H. J. (1964). Experiments in behaviour
sider how we learn. The 12 network learn- behavior does not change before cognition therapy: Readings in modern methods of treatment
of mental disorders derived from learning theory.
ing principles identified by Tryon (2009a) and/or affect. Cognition, affect, and behavior New York: Macmillan.
constitute a phylogenetically general mod- change simultaneously and incrementally
Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional
ern learning theory based on empirically on every processing cycle. Research gener-
processing of fear: Exposure to corrective in-
supported principles (cf. Rosen & Davison, ally confirms this result. Fifth, a focus on formation. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 20-35.
2003) that might provide a unified way for- learning and memory expands our perspec-
Hayes, S. C. (2008). Avoiding the mistakes of
ward. Recognition of the key role played by tive on what constitutes a good outcome. the past. the Behavior Therapist, 31, 150-153.
learning and memory and recent advances Tryon and McKay (2009) discussed how
Jones, J. C., & Barlow, D. H. (1990). The etiol-
in neuroscience regarding the experience- learning therapies necessarily modify mem-
ogy of post-traumatic stress disorder. Clinical
dependent plasticity mechanisms that en- ory and suggested that such memory modi- Psychology Review, 10, 299–328.
able learning to occur and memories to fications may be used to assess outcome.
Kazdin, A. E. (2007). Mediators and mechanism
form enables our field to return to the theo- In conclusion, the modern learning the- of change in psychotherapy research. Annual
retical basis that Wolpe and Eysenck envi- ory provided by neuroscience and connec- Review of Clinical Psychology, 3, 1-27.
sioned for it. tionist models enables us to confidently
Kazdin, A. E. (2008). Evidence-based treatment
The learning perspective I recommend follow DiGiuseppe’s (2008) recommenda- and practice: New opportunities to bridge
informs and supports clinical practice but tion “… that we explore what unites behav- clinical research and practice, enhance the
space limitations require me to be brief. ior therapy” (p. 155)—our conviction that knowledge base, and improve patient care.
First, a focus on learning as our common learning and memory are fundamental to American Psychologist, 63, 146-159.
science base enables us to focus on what psychological development and change. Lang, P. J. (1977). Imagery in therapy: An infor-
needs to be learned and how best to teach it. Perhaps this reorientation will begin to close mation processing analysis of fear. Behavior
These themes should provide common the science-practice gap and thereby ad- Therapy, 8, 862-886.
ground for both clinicians and researchers vance our field. Lang, P. J. (1985). The cognitive psychophysiol-
and may suggest a way for investigators to ogy of emotion: Fear and anxiety. In A. H.
repackage their findings so that they appear References Turner & J. Maser (Eds.), Anxiety and the anx-
more relevant and useful to practicing clini- Allen, L. B., McHugh, R. K., & Barlow, D. H.
iety disorders (pp. 131-170). Hillsdale, NJ:
cians. Second, the empirically supported Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
(2008). Emotional disorders: A unified pro-
DIR principle can be used to optimize inter- tocol. In D. Barlow (Ed.), Clinical handbook of Lang, P. J. (1994). The motivational organization
ventions by finding ways to induce and sus- psychological disorders: A step-by-step treatment of emotions. In S. van Goozen, N. E., van de
tain specific forms of dissonance and control manual (4th ed., pp 216-249). New York: Poll, & J. A. Sergent (Eds.), Emotions: Essays on
its reduction. Motivational interviewing al- Guilford. emotion theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
ready explicitly uses dissonance induction Erlbaum.
Arbib, M. A. (Ed) (2002). The handbook of brain
and its controlled reduction as a planned in- theory and neural networks (2nd ed.). Lombroso, P., & Ogren, M. (2008). Learning and
tervention (Tashiro & Mortesen, 2006). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. memory, Part I: Brain regions involved in
two types of learning and memory. Journal of
Although derived from clinical experience Bear, M. F., Connors, B. W., & Paradiso, M. A.
the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
rather than connectionist network learning (2007). Neuroscience: Exploring the brain (3rd
Psychiatry, 47, 1219-1223.
theory, the unified protocol described by ed.). Baltimore: Lippincott, Williams &
Wilkins. Lombroso, P., & Ogren, M. (2009). Learning and
Allen, McHugh, and Barlow (2008) pre- memory, Part II: Molecular mechanisms of
pares clients for and then explicitly imple- Bechtel, W., & Abrahamsen, A. (2002).
synaptic plasticity. Journal of the American
ments the DIR principle. Creative clinical Connectionism and the mind: An introduction to
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 5-
variations prompted by client characteris- parallel processing in networks (2nd ed).
9.
tics and other constraints designed to maxi- Cambridge: Blackwell.
McLeod, P., Plunkett, K., & Rolls, E. T. (1998).
mize DIR illustrates the application of Bower, G. H., & Hilgard, E. R. (1997). Theories of
Introduction to connectionist modelling of cognitive
empirically supported principles that Rosen learning. New York: Prentice Hall.
processes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
and Davison (2003) recommended. Third, Brewin, C. R., Dalgleish, T., & Joseph, S. (1996).
Moran, D. J. (Ed.). (2008). The three waves of
recognition of the neuroscience mechanisms A dual representation theory of posttrau-
behavior therapy: Course corrections or navi-
that enable learning and memory by modi- matic stress disorder. Psychological Review,
gation errors? [Special issue]. the Behavior
103, 670–686.
fying synapses and neurotransmitters places Therapist, 31(8). Retrieved December 8,
learning-based therapists on the same page Carlson, N. R. (2010). Physiology of behavior (10th 2009, from http://www.abct.org/docs
as pharmacologists who also seek to alter ed.). Boston: Pearson. /PastIssue/31n8.pdf
synapses and neurotransmitters. This syn- Chemtob, C., Roitblat, H. L., Hamada, R. S., O’Reilly, R. C., & Munakata, Y. (2000).
ergy may explain why combined treatments Carlson, J. G., & Twentyman, C. T. (1988). A Computational explorations in cognitive neuro-

January • 2010 11
science: Understanding the mind by simulating the Squire, L. R., Berg, D., Bloom, F. E., du Lac, S., Tryon, W. W. (in press). Cognitive processes in
brain. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Ghosh, A., & Spitzer, N. C. (2008). cognitive and pharmacological therapies.
Rosen, G. M., & Davison, G. C. (2003). Fundamental neuroscience (3rd ed.). New York: Cognitive Therapy & Research.
Psychology should list empirically supported Elsevier. Tryon, W. W. (2010). Professional identity based
principles of change (ESP's) and not creden- Tashiro, T., & Mortensen, L. (2006). on learning. the Behavior Therapist, 33, 9.
tial trademarked therapies or other treat- Translational research: How social psychol- Tryon, W. W., & McKay, D. (2009). Memory
ment packages. Behavior Modification, 27, ogy can improve psychotherapy. American modification as outcome variable in anxiety
300−312. Psychologist, 61, 959-966. disorder treatment. Journal of Anxiety
Tryon, W. W. (1999). A bidirectional associative Disorders, 23, 546-556.
Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986a).
memory explanation of posttraumatic stress Tryon, W. W., & Misurell, J. R. (2008).
Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the Dissonance induction and reduction: A pos-
disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 19, 789-
microstructure of cognition, Vol. 1: Foundations. 818. sible principle and connectionist mechanism
Cambridge: MIT Press. for why therapies are effective. Clinical
Tryon, W. W. (2000). Behavior therapy as ap-
Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986b). plied learning theory. the Behavior Therapist, Psychology Review, 28, 1297-1309.
Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the 23, 131-134. Wolpe, J. (1969). The practice of behavior therapy.
microstructure of cognition psychological and bio- Tryon, W. W. (2002). Neural network learning Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press.
logical models, Vol. 2. Cambridge: MIT Press. theory: Unifying radical behaviorism and Wolpe, J., & Lazarus, A. A. (1966). Behavior ther-
Siegle, G. J. (2001). Connectionist models of cognitive neuroscience. the Behavior Therapist, apy techniques: A guide to the treatment of neu-
psychopathology: Crossroads of the cogni- 25, 53-57. roses. New York: Pergamon.
tive and affective neuroscience of disorder. Tryon, W. W. (2005). Possible mechanisms for . . .
Cognitive Processing, 2, 455-486. why desensitization and exposure therapy
Siegle, G. J. (1999). A neural network model of work. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 67-95. Correspondence to Warren W. Tryon, Ph.D.,
attention biases in depression. In J. A. Tryon, W. W. (2009a). A modern learning theory. Fordham University, 441 E. Fordham Rd.,
Reggia, E. Ruppin, & D. L. Glanzman (Eds.), Manuscript submitted for publication. Dept. of Psychology, Bronx, NY 10458;
Disorders of brain behavior and cognition: The Tryon, W. W. (2009b). Missing mechanisms in- wtryon@fordham.edu
neurocomputational perspective (pp. 415-441). formation. American Psychologist, 64, 273-
New York: Elsevier. 274.

First, five effect sizes were calculated for


Letter to the Editor primary panic-related domains: percent
panic-free, clinically significant change,
panic symptom measures, fear of anxiety,
Treatment Specificity for Panic Disorder: and panic-related cognitions. Wampold et
al. (2009) refer only to the one effect size
A Reply to Wampold, Imel, and Miller (2009) (panic symptom measures) for which the
Clark et al. (2009) study appeared to have
Jedidiah Siev, Massachusetts General Hospital/Harvard Medical School, Jonathan the largest effect, and ignore all four other
D. Huppert, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, and Dianne L. Chambless, indicators. They do not articulate a ratio-
University of Pennsylvania nale for doing so, and none is easily inferred.
Indeed, Öst and Westling (1995) found ef-
fects in favor of CBT similar to or larger
ampold, Imel, and Miller outcome measures. Wampold et al. dis- than the weighted means on at least three of

W (2009) raise a number of inter-


esting points, and overall, we
are pleased that they agree that techniques
count the differences between CBT and re-
laxation for panic disorder with two
assertions: (a) those differences were driven
by a single, flawed study (viz., Clark et al.,
those effect sizes (larger even than did Clark
et al. on one), and Öst developed applied re-
laxation, to which one might imagine he
are important. We are in agreement, as has an allegiance. By Wampold et al.’s own
well, that more fine-grained analyses are 1994), and (b) the fact that CBT outper- logic, Öst’s allegiance should have mini-
necessary to better identify and determine formed relaxation on panic-related, but not
mized the between groups differences. In
the active ingredients. We will leave it to secondary, measures implies “removing
any case, on what basis do Wampold et al.
other venues to address in more detail symptoms but not benefitting patients” (p.
148). select the single effect size out of five most
Wampold et al.’s critiques regarding the influenced by Clark et al. and assert that
specificity of treatment effects (e.g., in “evidence for specificity rests solely on this
PTSD), the circularity or consistency of the Between Groups Differences Are
Driven by a Single Study one study” (p. 147)? If we were seeking
term “bona fide treatments,” and the
treatment for panic disorder, we would care
“clear” evidence of the predictive power of Wampold et al. (2009) state that, “The deeply about the probability of being rid of
the alliance within CBT, as well as other advantage to CBT in the Siev and
points raised. We wish to reply herein to disabling panic attacks following treatment
Chambless (2007) meta-analysis was en-
two specific issues regarding the Siev and (i.e., percent panic free), a domain in which
tirely accounted for by the one study that
Chambless (2007) meta-analysis and the CBT unambiguously outperforms relax-
found a large effect for CBT” (p. 147). This
importance of specific effects on specific claim is unfounded. ation (even with the largest difference in
Clark et al.’s study and the smallest found

12 the Behavior Therapist


by Öst and Westling), but a domain ig- measures of the quality of patients’ lives” (p. disorder. The fact that CBT does not out-
nored by Wampold et al. 148). They also state, “removing symptoms perform relaxation on measures of depres-
Second, there was scant evidence of het- but not benefitting patients generally is not sion in a study of individuals with panic
erogeneity of effect sizes, even using a con- a desirable outcome to many—most impor- disorder may imply mostly that people do
servative alpha of .10. Wampold et al. tantly to patients” (p. 148). not get differentially better on symptoms of
(2009) correctly note that homogeneity There are two responses to this con- disorders that they do not have (even if they
tests are underpowered in such a small sam- tention, one clinical and one statistical. experience subclinical elevations on them).
ple; however, there was only mild to moder- Clinically, it is not plausible that a patient One might speculate about a possible floor
ate heterogeneity as evidenced by the I2 suffering from primary panic disorder de- effect or the possibility that common factors
index (Higgins & Thompson, 2002; rives as much benefit from reductions on are indeed sufficient for this secondary out-
Huedo-Medina, Sanchez-Meca, Marin- symptoms of depression as from improve- come.
Martinez, & Botella, 2006; in which a find- ments in panic-related measures. A patient
ing of 50 would be interpreted as moderate with a primary diagnosis of panic disorder is Summary
heterogeneity). Hence, the claim that Clark by definition suffering the most distress
In summary, Wampold et al. (2009) dis-
et al. represents an outlier is empirically un- from panic attacks and their sequelae (and
count meta-analytic data demonstrating
founded. not primarily from depression, for exam-
that CBT outperformed relaxation on pri-
Essentially Wampold et al. (2009) ple). Furthermore, in considering whether
CBT outperforms relaxation in treating mary measures of panic-related symptoms
would like to remove or discount the differ- on the grounds that the findings were dri-
ence in efficacy between CBT and relax- panic disorder, there is no tradeoff between
benefit in panic-related symptoms and im- ven entirely by a single study, and that dif-
ation for panic disorder on the basis of a ferential treatment response on primary but
large effect size in the Clark et al. (2009) provement in depression. That is, relaxation
does not outperform CBT on secondary not secondary outcomes does not indicate
study on one of five primary outcome mea- patient benefit. Regarding the former asser-
sures without evidence of heterogeneity. measures. Rather, the choice is between
treatments that are differentially effective tion, there is a consistent pattern of differ-
This is tantamount to saying that if we re- ential improvement in favor of CBT on
move all evidence to the contrary without for the specific presenting complaint, and
similarly effective in other areas. At the very measures in five panic-related domains, four
empirical justification, the data are consis- of which Wampold et al. ignore. Further-
tent with a different conclusion. least, a clinician who offers relaxation in-
stead of CBT to a patient seeking treatment more, there is no statistical justification to
Furthermore, Wampold et al. (2009)
for panic should inform the patient that re- remove that single study. Regarding the lat-
identify two ways in which Clark et al.
laxation may not be as successful as CBT in ter assertion, an individual with primary
(1994) “severely, if not fatally, modified”
treating the panic attacks and panic disor- panic disorder is by definition suffering
their relaxation treatment, namely by alter-
der for which the patient has sought treat- most from panic-related symptoms, and we
ing the treatment rationale and “more con-
ment, but the degree of change in find it unconvincing that greater improve-
sequential[ly]” by introducing exposure too
symptoms of depression and generalized ment on those symptoms does not indicate
early, such that “it is quite possible that
anxiety may be similar. greater benefit. Ethical considerations re-
Clark et al. may have actually conditioned
We agree, however, that reductions in quire that a clinician offering relaxation in-
panic symptoms . . . whereas Öst’s protocol
panic-related symptoms do not necessarily form a patient with panic disorder that the
correctly desensitized the patients” (p. 148).
imply maximal improvements in quality of patient is less likely to achieve improve-
It seems that this suggestion is essentially
life (e.g., Rapaport, Pollack, Wolkow, ments in panic-related domains than if the
that Clark et al. modified the active ingredi-
Mardekian, & Clary, 2000), although it may patient were to engage in CBT. Never-
ents necessary for maximal improvement. If
nonspecific factors account entirely for ther- depend on which symptoms (e.g., Telch, theless, we agree with Wampold et al. more
apeutic change, of what consequence is the Schmidt, Jaimez, Jacquin, & Harrington, broadly that specific mechanisms of psy-
particular timing of exposure? Similarly, 1995). It is therefore important to assess chotherapeutic gain, whether treatment
improvements in quality of life directly (i.e., techniques or therapist effects, require elu-
benefits would not depend on the mecha-
not relying only on primary or secondary cidation and that doing so has potential to
nism of conditioning. Therefore, paradoxi-
symptom measures). For a review of issues improve patient outcomes.
cally, this very critique of Clark et al.’s study
and challenges in assessing quality of life
apparently relies on the premise that spe- References
and the relationship to symptom improve-
cific techniques are responsible for improve-
ment, see Gladis, Gosch, Dishuk, and Crits-
ments via specific mechanisms of change. Arntz, A., & van den Hout, M. (1996).
Christoph (1999). Incidentally, these Psychological treatments of panic disorder
Symptom Reduction on Primary measures are routinely included in funded without agoraphobia: Cognitive therapy ver-
Measures Does Not Demonstrate clinical trials today but were not at the time sus applied relaxation. Behaviour Research and
Patient Benefit the studies in our meta-analysis were con- Therapy, 34, 113-121.
ducted. Clark, D.M., Salkovskis, P.M., Hackmann, A.,
Wampold et al. (2009) argue that “the Statistically, participants in a study of Middleton, H., Anastasiades, P., & Gelder,
issue of primary and secondary variables at- panic disorder are selected because they M. (1994). A comparison of cognitive ther-
tenuates the importance of psychological meet criteria for panic disorder. Some will apy, applied relaxation, and imipramine in
functioning of patients” and that the analy- have comorbid depression but many will the treatment of panic disorder. British
sis by Siev, Huppert, and Chambless (2009) not. For example, in Arntz and van den Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 759-769.
is “disappointing” because it demonstrates Hout (1996; one of the studies included in Gladis, M. M., Gosch, E. A., Dishuk, N. M., &
“relative advantage on targeted symptom the meta-analysis), only 6/36 participants Crits-Christoph, P. (1999). Quality of life:
measures but…little impact on important received a secondary diagnosis of a mood Expanding the scope of clinical significance.

January • 2010 13
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Letter to the Editor
67, 320-331.
Higgins, J. P. T., & Thompson, S. G. (2002).
Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analy-
sis. Statistics in Medicine, 21, 1539-1558.
To Kill a Dodo Bird
Huedo-Medina, T. B., Sanchez-Meca, J., Marin-
Martinez, F., & Botella, J. (2006). Assessing Stefan G. Hofmann, Boston University, and Jeffrey M. Lohr, University of Arkansas
heterogeneity in meta-analysis: Q statistic or
I2 index? Psychological Methods, 11, 193-206.
e have read with great interest of limited theoretical importance because it
Öst, L.-G., & Westling, B. E. (1995). Applied
relaxation vs cognitive behavior therapy in
the treatment of panic disorder. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 33, 145-158.
W the article “Barriers to the
Dissemination of Empirically
Supported Treatments: Matching Message
tells us very little about the active ingredi-
ents or the mechanism of treatment
change. Heuristically more useful is to ex-
amine the disorder-specificity of a treat-
Rapaport, M. H., Pollack, M., Wolkow, R., to the Evidence,” by Wampold, Imel, and
Mardekian, J., & Clary, C. (2000). Is placebo Miller (2009) published in the Behavior ment. In this case, a treatment T1 may be
response the same as drug response in panic Therapist. The authors provide a well-writ- more efficacious than T2 for treating symp-
disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, ten and scholarly discussion on the issue of toms S1 but not for treating symptoms S2.
1014-1016. treatment specificity and the dodo-bird ver- Even more informative is the specificity of
Siev, J., & Chambless, D. L. (2007). Specificity of dict. Much of the authors’ argument rests the treatment mechanism because this pro-
treatment effects: Cognitive therapy and re- on the meta-analyses by Siev and vides information about the mediation vari-
laxation for generalized anxiety and panic Chambless (2007) and Wampold et al. ables that are involved in a specific
disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical (1997). treatment.
Psychology, 75, 513-522. Efficacy studies alone are not overly in-
Not unusual for meta-analytic argu-
Siev, J., Huppert, J. D., & Chambless, D. L. ments, the authors might have missed some formative for this particular discussion be-
(2009). The dodo bird, treatment technique, cause they neither prove nor disprove the
important evidence that contradicts their
and disseminating empirically supported dodo-bird conjecture. However, this conjec-
treatments. the Behavior Therapist, 32, 69-76. own presumptions. We observed in our own
meta-analysis clear evidence that is incom- ture is incompatible with data supporting
Telch, M. J., Schmidt, N. B., Jaimez, L., Jacquin, disorder-specificity of treatment and also speci-
K. M., & Harrington, P. J. (1995). Impact of patible with the dodo-bird conjecture
cognitive-behavioral treatment on quality of (Hofmann & Smits, 2008). In our own ficity of treatment mechanism. Our meta-
life in panic disorder patients. Journal of meta-analysis, we examined high-quality analysis of CBT trials for anxiety disorders
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 823- randomized controlled trials examining the yielded a pooled effect size (Hedges’ g) of
830. efficacy of CBT for various anxiety disor- 0.73 (95% confidence interval, 0.88-1.65)
Wampold, B. E., Imel, Z. E., & Miller, S. D. ders. We found that (a) CBT is more effica- for continuous anxiety severity measures
(2009). Barriers to the dissemination of em- cious than credible control treatments; (b) and 0.45 (90% confidence interval, 0.25-
pirically supported treatments: Matching the various CBT protocols differ in their ef- 0.65) for depressive symptom severity mea-
messages to the evidence. the Behavior ficacy depending on the disorder they tar- sures. Because the confidence intervals are
Therapist, 32, 144-155. get; and, most important for this nonoverlapping, these data suggest that
. . . discussion, (c) CBT is disorder-specific be- CBT for anxiety disorders is treatment spe-
cause CBT for anxiety disorders primarily cific. In contrast, the dodo-bird conjecture
Correspondence to Jedidiah Siev, Ph.D., changes anxiety symptoms but to a much predicts that symptoms of anxiety and de-
Massachusetts General Hospital, 185 lesser degree depression symptoms. pression improve similarly well (or poorly).
Cambridge St., Simches Research Bldg., The last finding should pose a particular These data are in line with the meta-
Boston, MA 02114; jsiev@partners.org challenge for proponents of the dodo-bird analysis by Siev and Chambless (2007)
verdict because treatment specificity di- demonstrating disorder-specificity of CBT,
rectly falsifies the dodo-bird conjecture. In because CBT and relaxation therapy were
this context, it is important to define the efficacious for generalized anxiety disorder,
term treatment specificity. It can refer to (a) but CBT was more efficacious than relax-
specificity of treatment content, (b) specificity of ation therapy for treating symptoms of
treatment efficacy, (c) disorder-specificity of treat- panic disorder. Wampold et al. (2009) tried
ment, or (d) specificity of treatment mechanism. to make the case that the results of Siev and
In the case of specificity of treatment con- Chambless’ (2007) meta-analysis were bi-
tent, specific procedures are included to tar- ased because of one particular study with
get the disorder or dysfunctions for which very strong effects. However, using a differ-
treatment is sought. This involves the iden- ent approach and evidence base, our meta-
tification of “active ingredients” and re- analysis supports the conclusion by Siev and
quires consideration of Chambless.
component-controlled experimental analy- Wampold et al. (2009) might argue next
ses (Lohr, DeMaio, & McGlynn, 2003). In that the control conditions in our meta-
the case of specificity of treatment efficacy, analysis do not consist of bona fide interven-
one has to show that treatment T1 is more tions. However, most—if not all—of the
efficacious than treatment T2 for treating a comparison treatments included in the
specific disorder. This form of specificity is Hofmann and Smits (2008) trials include

14 the Behavior Therapist


bona fide interventions. These treatments of treatment change include a more bal- Siev, J., & Chambless, D. L. (2007). Specificity of
include supportive counseling, relaxation anced discussion that considers all available treatment effects: Cognitive therapy and re-
treatments, and anxiety management. In a data, even those that seem to contradict laxation for generalized anxiety and panic dis-
separate meta-analysis, we (Smits & orders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
one’s own assumptions.
Psychology, 75, 513-522.
Hofmann, 2009) examined the uncon-
trolled effect size of these control treat- References Smits, A. J., & Hofmann (2009). A meta-analytic
ments and found an average effect size of review of the effects of psychotherapy control
Hofmann, S. G. (2004). Cognitive mediation of conditions for anxiety disorders. Psychological
Hedges’ g = 0.45 (95% confidence interval, Medicine, 39, 229-239
treatment change in social phobia. Journal of
0.35-0.46). There was no evidence for pub- Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 392-399. Smits, J. A. J., Rosenfield, D., Telch, M. J., &
lication bias, nor was there a significant re-
Hofmann, S. G., & Smits, J. A. J. (2008). McDonald, R. (2006). Cognitive mechanisms
lationship between the effect size and of social anxiety reduction: An examination of
Cognitive-behavioral therapy for adult anxi-
diagnostic group, study year or number of specificity and temporality. Journal of
ety disorders: A meta-analysis of randomized
treatment sessions. In addition, these treat- placebo-controlled trials. Journal of Clinical Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 1203-
ments were associated with a relatively low Psychiatry, 69, 621-632. 1212.
attrition rate. Again, these data contradict Tang, T. Z., DeRubeis, R. J., Beberman, R., &
Hofmann, S. G., Meuret, A. E., Rosenfield, D.,
the argument that the control treatments in Suvak, M. K., Barlow, D. H., Gorman, J. M., Pham, T. (2005). Cognitive changes, critical
the Hofmann and Smits (2008) meta- Shear, M. K., & Woods, S. W. (2007). sessions, and sudden gains in CBT for depres-
analyses are biased in favor of CBT. Preliminary evidence for cognitive mediation sion. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
In addition to these arguments, we during cognitive behavioral therapy for panic Psychology, 73, 168-172.
would like to point out that a number of re- disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Wampold, B. E., Imel, Z. E., & Miller, S. D.
cent studies provide evidence for cognitive Psychology, 75, 374-379. (2009). Barriers to the dissemination of empir-
mediation of CBT effects for a variety of dis- ically supported treatments: Matching mes-
Kaysen, D., Scher, C. D., Mastnak, J., & Resick, P.
orders, including panic disorder (Hofmann sages to the evidence. the Behavior Therapist,
(2005). Cognitive mediation of childhood 32, 114-155.
et al., 2007), social anxiety disorder maltreatment and adult depression in recent
(Hofmann, 2004; Smits, Rosenfield, Telch, crime victims. Behavior Therapy, 36, 235-244. Wampold, B. E., Mondin, G. W., Moody, M.,
& McDonald, 2006), obsessive-compulsive Stich, F., Benson, K., & Ahn, H. (1997). A
Lohr, J.M., DeMaio, C., & McGlynn, F.D. (2003). meta-analysis o outcome studies comparing
disorder (Moore & Abramowitz, 2007), de- Specific and nonspecific treatment factors in bona fide psychotherapies: Empirically, “All
pression (Kaysen, Scher, Mastnak, & the experimental analysis of behavioral treat- must have prizes.” Psychological Bulletin, 122,
Resick, 2005; Tang, DeRubeis, Beberman, ment efficacy. Behavior Modification, 27, 322- 203-215.
& Pham, 2005), and pain (Price, 2000), to 368.
name only a few. These data provide addi- . . .
Moore, E. L., Abramowitz, J. S. (2007). The cog-
tional evidence against the dodo-bird con- nitive mediation of thought-control strate- Correspondence to Stefan G. Hofmann,
jecture. gies. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45,
Ph.D., Department of Psychology, Boston
We hope that future debates concerning 1949-1955.
University, 648 Beacon Street, 6th floor,
the dodo-bird verdict (we would like to Price, D. D. (2000). Psychological and neural Boston, MA 02215; shofmann@bu.edu.
eliminate this term, as indicated in our mechanisms of the affective dimension of pain.
title), treatment specificity, and mechanism Science, 288, 1769-1772.

different findings from Wampold’s latest ef-


Letter to the Editor forts. And, of course, numerous meta-
analyses exist showing efficacy of EBTs
compared to credible active controls. The
The Dodo Bird—Again—and Again main consequence of reading all of these
analyses is that most people will fall asleep.
David H. Barlow, Center for Anxiety and Related Disorders, Boston University For this reason I don’t trust any meta-analy-
sis conducted by anyone with an agenda,
and this includes treatment developers as
and the appearance of thousands of positive well as Bruce Wampold, and neither should

T he “dodo bird” argument, as many


people know, has been around since
1975, and there have been countless
rebuttals throughout the late 80s and 90s,
sometimes presented as special sections of
clinical trials demonstrating superiority of
psychological treatments carefully tailored
to presenting psychopathology compared
to some good alternatives? One reason is
anyone else. For example, Wampold (with
more kudos for full disclosure) is very clear
about his personal experience with and
strong allegiance to individual long-term
journals. Now it is coming up again (see that the primary methods used are retro- psychotherapy (see, for example, Wampold,
Siev, Huppert, & Chambless, 2009, and spective re-analyses of other work using 2001). This is reminiscent of a similar alle-
Wampold, Imel, & Miller, 2009) because meta-analytic procedures. But these proce- giance-related agenda reported by Gene
Bruce Wampold is presenting it in a schol- dures are notoriously subject to distortion Glass of Smith and Glass (1977) fame when
arly manner (much to his credit), with yet with just the slightest tweaks (see he noted:
another set of meta-analyses. But the ques- Dieckmann, Malle, & Bodner, 2009) and
tion to ask is why the dodo bird has never manuscripts meta-analyzing the exact same I left the University of Wisconsin in
gained any traction even in the early years data sets examined by Wampold are mak- 1965 with a brand new Ph.D. in . . .
before the rise of evidence-based practice ing their way to publication from evidence- statistics and a major league neurosis.
based treatment (EBT) types showing very Luckily, I found my way into psy-

January • 2010 15
chotherapy that year . . . and never left (HHS), and other similarly composed im- References
it until eight years later. . . . I was so im- partial policymaking bodies are capable of
pressed with the power of psychother- producing credible analyses, and produce Dieckmann, N. F., Malle, B. F., & Bodner, T. E.
(2009). An empirical assessment of meta-an-
apy as a means of changing my life and them they have. But even these need repli- alytic practice. Review of General Psychology,
making it better that by 1970 I was cation. More importantly, look at the impli- 13(2), 101-115.
studying clinical psychology . . . the cations of the dodo bird. Is there any
Glass, G. V. (2000). Meta-analysis at 25.
weight of academic opinion at that clinician out there who really believes that Retrieved November 25, 2009, from
time derived from Hans Eysenck’s fre- you can use exactly the same procedure http://glass.ed.asu.edu/gene/papers/meta25.
quent and tendentious reviews . . . that with, say, someone with chronic schizophre- html
proclaimed psychotherapy as worthless nia, specific phobia, bipolar disorder, or Siev, J., Huppert, J., & Chambless, D. L. (2009).
. . . I found this conclusion personally OCD as long as it’s a “bona fide” treatment The Dodo bird treatment technique, and dis-
threatening—it called into question that both patient and therapist believe in? seminating empirically supported treat-
not only the preoccupation of about a So client-centered therapy would work as ments. the Behavior Therapist, 32, 69-76.
decade of my life but my scholarly well for cognitive deficits in schizophrenia Smith, M. L., & Glass, G. V. (1977). Meta-analy-
judgment (and the wisdom of having as would cognitive remediation therapy, sis of psychotherapy outcome studies.
dropped a fair chunk of change) as well. and as well with OCD as ERP? The funda- American Psychologist, 32(9), 752-760.
I read Eysenck’s literature reviews and mental reason this argument has never Wampold, B.E. (2001). The great psychotherapy de-
was impressed primarily with their ar- gained traction is because it just plain does- bate: Models, methods, and findings. Hillsdale,
bitrariness. . . . I wanted to take on n’t make sense no matter how the clinical NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Eysenck and show that he was wrong: trials are reinterpreted. And it’s also easy to Wampold, B. E., Imel, Z. E., & Miller, S. D.
psychotherapy does change lives and sit back and “pick off ” any new study and (2009). Barriers to the dissemination of em-
make them better. (Glass, 2000) conclude that it’s not “perfect.” But to re- pirically supported treatments: Matching
ally prove the dodo-bird thesis, the propo- messages to the evidence. the Behavior
Only independent and impartial groups nents would need to do the hard work of Therapist, 32, 144-155.
such as the National Institute for Health conducting their own trials constructing . . .
and Clinical Excellence (NICE) in the U.K. “bona fide” treatments and comparing
utilizing their sophisticated methods, and them to well-established active treatments Correspondence to David H. Barlow, Ph.D.,
the Agency for Healthcare Research and using equivalence analyses, not just claim- Boston University, 648 Beacon St., Center for
Quality (AHRQ) within the U.S. ing that “no findings” (the null hypothesis) Anxiety and Related Disorders, Boston, MA
Department of Health and Human Services prove anything. 02215; dhbarlow@bu.edu.

Letter to the Editor plained by gene Y. Kendler concluded that


this approach is misguided because, among
other things, it ignores the importance of
More on the Brain Disease Model of Mental the environment and gene-environment in-
teractions (Kendler, 2005). Insel’s call for
Disorders research on “cure therapeutics” is unrealisti-
cally simplistic because it overemphasizes
Steven Taylor, University of British Columbia, Dean McKay, Fordham University, biological research to the neglect of envi-
ronmental (e.g., psychosocial) factors.
and Jonathan S. Abramowitz, University of North Carolina For behavioral researchers this is impor-
tant because the denigration of our subject
eacon and Lickel (2009) offer com- threats to the physical integrity of oneself or matter by funding bodies seriously impairs

D pelling arguments as to why an


eliminative materialism approach
to mental disorders, such as those promul-
loved ones (Searle, 1992).
NIMH Director Thomas Insel called for
the development of “cure therapeutics” of
mental disorders (Insel & Scolnick, 2006),
our investigation of the variables that we re-
gard, with good reason, as being important
in understanding psychopathology. As
Deacon and Lickel (2009) point out, there
gated by major funding bodies like NIMH, which is apparently a funding priority of are important and sometimes harmful prac-
is misguided, empirically inadequate, and NIMH. His proposed research program is a tical consequences. There are other impor-
potentially harmful to consumers of mental largely biological endeavor in the spirit of tant consequences that were not mentioned
health services. Eliminative materialism eliminative materialism. But are there any by Deacon and Lickel that we would like to
states that psychological conditions, such as leading researchers in the field of psy- highlight. This concerns a booklet recently
depression, can be simply reduced to brain chopathology who seriously endorse elimi- published by NIMH intended to educate
conditions, such as dysregulated neuro- native materialism as a viable explanation of the public about the nature and treatment
psychopathology? Likely not, except per- of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD;
transmission. Eliminative materialism has
haps for extreme biophiles. Kenneth NIMH, 2009). Given the debilitating na-
been roundly discredited by philosophers of Kendler, a leader in the field of biological ture of OCD, and the difficulty many suf-
mind because, among other things, it ne- and genetic factors in psychopathology, co- ferers have in identifying appropriate
glects psychosocial factors—for example, gently criticized a variant of eliminative ma- treatment resources, the dissemination of
the effects of aversive environments such as terialism that he called “GeneTalk”; that is, accurate educational materials is vital for
those involving poverty, discrimination, or the claim that disorder X can be simply ex- helping consumers make informed choices

16 the Behavior Therapist


about health care. We were concerned to see College of Psychiatrists, make it abundantly problems. Clearly, sshould take precedence
that the NIMH booklet offered distorted clear that behavioral or cognitive-behav- over the ideologies about the causes of men-
advice about treatment options. The booklet ioral interventions are first-line treatments, tal disorders.
offers the following advice to consumers: even as stand-alone interventions (e.g.,
American Psychiatric Association, 2007; References
There is help for people with OCD. The National Institute for Health and Clinical Abramowitz, J. S., Taylor, S., & McKay, D.
first step is to go to a doctor or health Excellence [NICE], 2006). For children and (2009). Obsessive-compulsive disorder. The
clinic to talk about symptoms. People adolescents with OCD, it has been recom- Lancet, 374, 489-497.
who think they have OCD may want to mended that CBT is the initial treatment of American Psychiatric Association (2007).
bring this booklet to the doctor, to help choice, and that medication should be only Practice guideline for the treatment of patients with
them talk about the symptoms in it. considered if there has been an insufficient obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arlington, VA:
The doctor will do an exam to make response to CBT (NICE, 2006). The follow- Author.
sure that another physical problem ing quotations illustrate these practice Deacon, B. J., & Lickel, J. L. (2009). On the brain
isn’t causing the symptoms. The doctor guidelines. disease model of mental disorders. the
may make a referral to a mental health Behavior Therapist, 32, 113-116.
specialist. Doctors may prescribe med- In choosing a treatment approach, the Insel, T. R., & Scolnick, E. M. (2006). Cure ther-
ication to help relieve OCD. It’s impor- clinician should consider the patient’s apeutics and strategic prevention: Raising
tant to know that some of these motivation and ability to comply with the bar for mental health research. Molecular
medicines may take a few weeks to pharmacotherapy and psychotherapy. Psychiatry, 11, 11-17.
start working. Medications can be pre- CBT and serotonin reuptake inhibitors Kendler, K. S. (2005). “A gene for…”: The na-
scribed by M.D.s (usually a psychia- (SRIs) are recommended as safe and ef- ture of gene action in psychiatric disorders.
trist) and in some states also by clinical fective first-line treatments for OCD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 1243-
psychologists, psychiatric nurse practi- (American Psychiatric Association, 1252.
tioners, and advanced psychiatric nurse 2007, p. 11) National Institute for Health and Clinical
specialists. Check with your state’s li- Excellence. (2006). Obsessive-compulsive disor-
censing agency for specifics. The kinds Adults with OCD with moderate func- der. London: British Psychological Society
of medicines used to treat OCD are tional impairment should be offered and the Royal College of Psychiatrists.
listed below. Some of these medicines the choice of either a course of an SSRI or National Institute of Mental Health. (2009).
are used to treat other problems, such more intensive CBT … because these When unwanted thoughts take over: Obsessive-
as depression, but also are helpful for treatments appear to be comparably ef- compulsive disorder. [Brochure]. Washington,
DC: Author. Retrieved December 9, 2009,
OCD. ficacious. … Adults with OCD with se- from http://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/pub-
• antidepressants,
vere functional impairment should be lications/when-unwanted-thoughts-take-
• antianxiety medicines, and
offered combined treatment with an over-obsessive-compulsive-disorder/complete
• beta-blockers.
SSRI and CBT. (NICE, 2006, p. 232) .pdf
Searle, J.R. (1992). The rediscovery of the mind.
Doctors also may ask people with Children and young people with OCD Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
OCD to go to therapy with a licensed with moderate to severe functional im-
social worker, psychologist, or psychia- Simpson, H.B., Foa, E.B., Liebowitz, M.R.,
pairment… should be offered CBT … Ledley, D.R., Hupert, J.D., Cahill, S., et al.
trist. This treatment can help people If psychological treatment is declined (2008). A randomized controlled trial of cog-
with OCD feel less anxious and fearful. by children or young people with OCD nitive-behavioral therapy for augmenting
(p. 4, emphasis added) … or they are unable to engage in pharmacotherapy in obsessive-compulsive
treatment, an SSRI may be considered disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165,
The booklet overstates the role of med- with specific arrangements for careful 621-630.
ication. It gives the misleading impression monitoring for adverse events. (NICE, . . .
that medications are the only first-line 2006, pp. 232-233)
treatments and that “therapy”—presum- Correspondence to Steven Taylor, Ph.D.,
ably a reference to behavioral or cognitive- The NIMH booklet does not reflect con- Department of Psychiatry, University of
behavioral therapy (CBT)—is simply an temporary practice guidelines. Instead, it British Columbia, 2255 Wesbrook Mall,
adjunct to medication. There is ample evi- gives the distorted impression that people Vancouver, BC, V6T 2A1, Canada; e-mail:
dence that behavior therapy and CBT, as suffering from OCD must take medication, taylor@unixg.ubc.ca.
stand-alone treatments, are just as effica- and that “talk therapy” is simply an adjunct
cious as medications (Abramowitz, Taylor, to medication. As such, the booklet does
& McKay, 2009). Such findings have not serve the best interests of the public. We
emerged from studies funded by NIMH. notified Dr. Insel of our concerns and hope
Indeed, a recent NIMH-funded study that our empirically informed suggestions
showed that CBT was efficacious even for can counterbalance the biocentric emphasis
OCD patients who failed to respond to an in the booklet. We gather that our sugges-
adequate course of pharmacotherapy tions are currently under consideration by
(Simpson et al., 2008). the unnamed authors of the OCD booklet.
Contemporary practice guidelines, such As Deacon and Lickel amply demonstrate, a
as those published by the American biocentric emphasis may hinder rather than
Psychiatric Association and by the Royal help our clients or patients overcome their

January • 2010 17
Book Review and rationale were well-organized, concise,
and quite feasible for one to imagine imple-
menting. The vignettes provided greatly
Cooper, M., Todd, G., & Wells, A. (2009). Treating Bulimia help in this regard. The authors are appro-
Nervosa and Binge Eating: An Integrated Metacognitive priately and thoroughly inspired by multi-
ple influences and they did an excellent job
and Cognitive Therapy Manual. New York: Routledge concisely presenting such areas. For exam-
ple, in their discussion of engagement and
Reviewed by Rachel A. Annunziato, Mount Sinai School of Medicine motivation for treatment, presentation of
Prochaska and DiClemente (1982) and
Miller and Rollnick’s (2002) work is pro-
reating Bulimia Nervosa and Binge ferences. The treatment does depart from vided. These descriptions are tailored to

T Eating, by Myra Cooper, Gillian


Todd, and Adrian Wells, compre-
hensively describes a cognitive approach for
Fairburn et al. (1993) in other ways as well.
Food records are not employed, and ratio-
nale is offered for why this could be counter-
productive. However, the frequency of
clinicians with a range of prior familiarity.
Considering the perspective of trainees, I
believe the overviews of such areas are likely
very welcoming and helpful (but not so ex-
addressing symptoms of binge eating
and/or purging. The method presented is purging and bingeing episodes is tracked. tensive as to alienate those with more expe-
applicable for patients diagnosed with bu- To perhaps assuage those who will be un- rience, in fact, to the contrary, they could be
limia nervosa (BN), subthreshold BN, and a comfortable with the absence of this core nice refreshers). Finally, the plethora of ma-
range of binge-eating presentations. technique, exceptions to the omission of terials presented in the book is outstanding.
Overall, the book offers a thorough ratio- food diaries are discussed. Behavioral strate- I found the forms and other references in the
nale for adopting a cognitive conceptualiza- gies are employed (e.g., stimulus control, Appendix to be of great interest and utility.
tion of the target symptoms; and the exposure exercises, etc.) but often they are The lingering questions I had while con-
presented as offshoots of the cognitive com- templating my use of the treatment are in
subsequent treatment techniques are pre-
ponents. For example, exposure exercises regards to its effectiveness and the relative
sented in a clear, user-friendly fashion that is
are routinely used, and very nicely de- importance and sequence of the presented
appropriate for clinicians with a wide range
scribed, to test the validity of specific beliefs. techniques. The authors review data sup-
of experience.
The treatment portion of the book is porting the development of their model and
The authors begin by providing exten-
structured like a manual. Throughout, the the chosen treatment targets. However,
sive rationale for treating BN with cognitive
reader is referred to exemplary forms, mate- data are not yet presented regarding the ef-
behavioral therapy (CBT), including a re- fectiveness of this approach and, of great
rials in the Appendix, and case examples.
view of the data supporting Fairburn, Prior to this, an extensive discussion of dis- importance, how this package compares to
Marcus, and Wilson’s (1993) well-known ordered eating assessment is provided, traditional CBT for BN. Secondly, the rela-
intervention package. However, it is noted which will likely be less relevant to more ex- tive weight of treatment targets was not
that traditional CBT for BN results in a perienced readers but very helpful to clear to me. For example, if a client strug-
large percentage of patients continuing to trainees. In addition, an overview of treat- gles with detached mindfulness, is that crit-
manifest clinically significant symptoms ment motivation and engagement is of- ical to treatment success? It seemed as if the
after long-term follow-up; and that CBT for fered. This discussion may help to authors advocate a specific sequence for de-
BN appears to be less effective than other determine who will or will not be appropri- livering the treatment and the order was
extensively studied CBT protocols such as ate for this treatment. The authors explain consistent with other cognitively oriented
those targeting anxiety disorders. how to integrate the assessment data into a packages (e.g., targeting core beliefs to-
Therefore, the authors hypothesize that bol- formulation using their cognitive model. wards the end of treatment) but I was curi-
stering the cognitive aspect of standard How to present and “sell” the model to ous if they recommended flexibility here.
CBT packages for BN will result in im- prospective patients is also very well articu- For example, would clinical judgment pre-
proved outcomes. lated. At this point, the authors address spe- vail if a therapist felt it helpful to address
A new cognitive model, describing both cific treatment targets, beginning with core beliefs earlier or is adhering to the pre-
the development and maintenance of BN, is metacognitive factors and proceeding to scribed order of value?
offered, drawing on several influences and negative self-beliefs. In some instances the For those with cognitive proclivities, this
integrating more recently described authors included “Troubleshooting” sec- text provides welcome additions to standard
metacognitive theory (Wells, 2000). The tions which I really liked. These cautions CBT for BN. The supplementary materials
specific treatment targets addressed in this captured well difficulties I envisioned hav- included by the authors to illustrate their
formulation are negative beliefs about the ing when implementing the treatment. concepts and techniques will likely serve as
controllability of bingeing and the conse- Finally, readiness for and the termination valuable resources for those treating disor-
quences of eating; positive beliefs about eat- process itself is briefly reviewed. ders characterized by symptoms of bingeing
ing-related behaviors and cognitions; and Treating Bulimia Nervosa and Binge Eating and/or purging as well as some that general-
negative self-beliefs. For those who have ex- has important strengths. Overall, the mate- ize to other presentations as well. I cannot
perience with other cognitively oriented rial is presented very clearly and interest- think of a book I have used recently that
treatments, targeting these foci will likely ingly. The interventions described, contains as many resources as this one.
feel quite familiar and comfortable. At this depending on the reader, may range from Theoretical rationale for both the core ten-
point, the reader will perhaps wonder ideas that one has widely used to quite novel ants and supplementary techniques is pro-
whether these components are added to tra- concepts. I felt that no matter where a strat- vided. The tone and scope of Treating
ditional CBT for BN or if there are other dif- egy fell on this spectrum, the description Bulimia Nervosa and Binge Eating very nicely

18 the Behavior Therapist


balances the needs of readers who will have a Fairburn, C.G., Marcus, M.D., & Wilson, G.T. Wells, A. (2000). Emotional disorders and metacog-
wide range of experience, which certainly is (1993). Cognitive behaviour therapy for nition. Chichester: Wiley.
not always an easy feat. Those who elect to binge eating and bulimia nervosa: A compre-
. . .
employ this method will be well-prepared. hensive treatment manual. In C.G. Fairburn
& G.T. Wilson (Eds.), Binge eating: Nature, as-
It will be quite exciting to learn more as Correspondence to Rachel A. Annunziato,
sessment, and treatment. New York: Guilford.
data are published on its use. Ph.D., Dept. of Psychiatry, Mount Sinai School
Miller, W.R., & Rollnick, S. (2002). Motivational of Medicine, One Gustave L. Levy Place, Box
References interviewing: Preparing people for change. New 1230, New York, NY 10029; email:
York: Guilford.
rachel.annunziato@mssm.edu
Cooper, M., Todd, G., & Wells, A. (2009). Prochaska, J.O. & DiClemente, C.C. (1982).
Treating bulimia nervosa and binge eating: An in- Transtheoretical therapy: Towards an inte-
tegrated metacognitive and cognitive therapy man- grative model of change. Psychotherapy Theory
ual. New York: Routledge. and Research Practice, 19, 276-288.

Book Review and societal influences. A final chapter de-


tails the growing wealth of resources and in-
formation for assessing and promoting
Klingemann, H., & Sobell, L. (Eds.). (2007). Promoting self-change.
Self-Change From Addictive Behaviors: Practical The current volume represents an up-
date of a prior work, Promoting Self-Change
Implications for Policy, Prevention and Treatment. From Problem Substance Abuse (Klingemann
New York: Springer et al., 2001), that originally brought to
light the research on the existence of self-
change and offered ideas and strategies to
Reviewed by Carlo C. DiClemente, University of Maryland, Baltimore harness the power of this process and pro-
mote self-change. Initial chapters provide a
wonderfully rich compendium of the state
n 1979 the National Cancer Institute is- have challenged some of the basic beliefs of of the science of self-change. After a

I sued a Request for Proposals that in-


cluded examining the phenomenon of
self-change of smoking. Researchers had
the addiction treatment community by
bringing to light the struggle of individuals
who, with minimal or no assistance from
treatment interventions, have been able to
thoughtful overview by Sobell of the impor-
tance of the existence of self-change among
addicted individuals and the many chal-
lenges to understanding and studying this
been developing many different programs
for smoking cessation over the years. change successfully one or more addictive experience, the next four chapters review
However, data indicated that the vast ma- behaviors and sustain that change over the research, primarily in alcohol and drug
jority of individuals who quit smoking did time. These pioneers also have contributed abuse, from classic to current (2005) stud-
so “on their own” without the use of any for- to the methodological sophistication with ies. The multiple reviews, however, become
mal treatments. This began a journey for which we now study self-change. Early redundant and a bit confusing as different
many of smoking cessation behavioral sci- studies were generally retrospective evalua- chapters discuss the same study in different
entists to understand the nature, mecha- tions of self-reported successful change that ways. The authors of these review chapters
nisms, and meaning of individual-initiated did not use formal treatment. The field has do not seem to have communicated about
and sustained change of addictive behavior. progressed greatly over the years, taking what studies and in what way they would
Although there were some indicators that into account severity, prior treatment, mu- cover the research. Thus, there is some sense
this phenomenon existed for other drugs of tual help experiences, legal status, mental of déjà vu as one goes from chapter to chap-
abuse and alcohol abuse and dependence, illness, and other complicating factors. ter, though it is interesting to get some dif-
most clinicians and researchers of “real” ad- However, it is interesting to note that in ferent perspectives. This may be a personal
dictive behaviors” (heroin, cocaine, mari- many more current studies methodological bias but I would also have like to see more
juana, and alcoholism) believed self-change rigor falls short of the standards promoted included about the experience of self-
was a phenomenon limited to nicotine ad- by these researchers. change of smoking beyond the 5-page
diction and even then believed that it really For this compendium on self-change, treatment that comes in a later chapter.
could not happen with individuals who Harald Klingemann and Linda Carter Smoking cessation offers a perspective on
were truly addicted. Sobell have brought together an interna- how societal, social group, and personal self-
This volume offers a comprehensive tional group of addiction scientists and ex- change interact and there is a growing un-
view of the halting and somewhat circuitous perts to explore the topic of self-change derstanding of these interactions emerging
path that led to our current understanding using survey data and research studies to in the literature. Despite these criticisms of
of self-change from all types of addictive be- focus on more traditional (alcohol, drug use, the early chapters, I believe that anyone
haviors. The editors are pioneers in the in- smoking, gambling, eating disorders) ad- who wants to know what has happened in
vestigation of this phenomenon of dictive behaviors and then to extend the the arena of self-change in alcohol and drug
self-change among individuals with serious view of self-change to new areas like stutter- abuse research will be delighted with the
drug and alcohol problems in multiple ing and crime. Later chapters offer views of breadth and depth of these reviews.
countries and continents. For the past 15 how to integrate self-change into our con- To keep the research grounded in the ex-
years both Drs. Klingemann and Sobell cepts of treatment, culture, policymaking, perience of the self-changers, the editors

January • 2010 19
have included quotes and reports reflecting The importance of context in the phe- At present, however, many different au-
experiences of those who have been able to nomenon of self-change is highlighted in diences can benefit from reading this book.
change addictive behaviors on their own. the chapter by the Klingemanns that de- Researchers will benefit from reading about
The welcome addition of these brief vi- scribes hostile and favorable social climates the scope of the research and will be chal-
gnettes helps the reader understand the ex- for self-change and the one by Barker and lenged about directions for future research.
perience and not just the research. Hunt that offers some thoughts about the Clinicians will be able to see the nature and
Unfortunately, these vignettes appear only cross-cultural challenges for the study and
scope of the addicted individual’s capacity
in a couple of chapters and could have been understanding of self-change among sub-
to change. Policymakers will be intrigued
more strategically placed throughout the groups of individuals embedded in various
cultural traditions and experiences. These by the possibilities of harnessing self-change
other chapters as well. for the betterment of society. The public,
The phenomenon of self-change chal- chapters highlight the fact that we have a
long way to go to understand and to control that includes all of us, will be challenged to
lenges many of our views of addiction, loss
or direct self-change of addictive behaviors. question our assumptions about addiction
of control, self-regulation, and the process
of change. Many of the chapters highlight However, they do offer ideas and concepts and change, to reflect on our attitudes
some of these issues. However, the overall that can help us move forward to study the about individuals engaged in addictive be-
perspective seems to contrast self-change contextual influences on self-change and haviors, and learn a little about how to pro-
and treatment change as separate entities or the types of policies and societal attitudes mote self-change among our colleagues,
that can foster change. friends, and families.
routes of change. This is understandable to
As with any good book, this one leaves
some extent since pioneers need to establish
this reader wishing for more. I would have References
the existence and reality of a previously un-
liked to have seen more theoretical and con-
derstudied experience that many in the field ceptual discussion of assumed mechanisms Klingemann, H., & Sobell, L. (Eds.). (2007).
did not believe existed—that is, the ability that influence and enable self-change. I Promoting self-change from addictive behaviors:
of addicted individuals to change on their would like to have had a more in-depth Practical implications for policy, prevention and
own without treatment or assistance. treatment of how self-change influences our treatment. New York: Springer.
However, as the field matures, it seems best understanding of addiction and our diag- Klingemann, H., Sobell, L., Barker, J.,
to consider all change in addictive behaviors nostic categorization of dependence. I Blomqvist, J., Cloud, W., Ellinstad, T., et al.
as self-change. Treatment simply enhances wanted a more integrated overview of the (2001). Promoting self-change from problem sub-
or supports the personal process of change. literature and a more detailed description of stance abuse. Dordrecht, The Netherlands:
Brief interventions, motivational interview- what self-change means for treatment and Kluwer.
ing, and policy changes that produce signif- healthcare policy. Finally, I would like to
icant individual change events seem to . . .
have seen more on the interaction of social
indicate the process of self-change can be in- forces and personal processes. However, the
fluenced by events and interventions that value of this book is that there are chapters in Correspondence to Carlo C. DiClemente,
one would consider contextual or minimal. which the discussion of each of these topics Ph.D., Psychology Dept MP340, Director of
Hopefully, as the field develops, there will is highlighted and the topic explored. MDQUIT Resource Center, UMBC, 1000
be less and less of a need to dichotomize self- Clearly, there will need to be another update Hilltop Circle, Baltimore, MD 21250;
change and treatment change. of this book in the future. e-mail: diclemen@umbc.edu

CALL FOR Workshop Submissions


44th Annual Convention | November 18–21, 2010 | San Francisco

Please send a 250-word abstract and a CV for each presenter to:


Jillian C. Shipherd, Ph.D.
Women’s Health Sciences Division (116B-3)
VA Boston Healthcare System
150 South Huntington Ave.
Boston, MA 02130
or email: Jillian.Shipherd@va.gov

20 the Behavior Therapist


CALL for PAPERS 44th Annual Convention | San Francisco
November 18–21, 2010
Unifying
COGNITIVE Diverse
Disciplines
BEHAVIORAL
With
THERAPY a Common
Thread

One of the many strengths of cognitive behavioral therapy is


the fact that it is flexible and can be adapted to treat a variety of
problems that individuals face. This has resulted in the tremendous
growth of diverse specialties that utilize cognitive behavioral prin-
ciples to help people return to a healthier state of both physical and
psychological functioning. While there are many specialties within
the fields of physical and mental health, our shared understanding
of the importance of applying evidence-based cognitive behavioral
practices is a common thread that joins us together.
Opportunities to share knowledge across disciplines could be
achieved through broadening the scope of our ABCT conference. As
multidisciplinary treatment teams are becoming more prevalent, it
is important to find avenues for increasing our communication
about ways that evidence-based practices can be applied more
broadly, adding to the richness of our knowledge about cognitive
behavioral theory and its potential applications.
The theme of the 44th annual meeting is intended to emphasize
the relevance of cognitive-behavioral theories across varied topics
and disorders and across diverse health - and mental-health related
professions and disciplines. We welcome submissions for research
Submissions may be in the
form of symposia, round
symposia, clinical sessions, and workshops focused on elucidating
tables, panel discussions, ways that cognitive behavioral treatments are relevant to diverse
and posters. Information groups of professionals that work with patients.
for submitting abstracts can
Submissions that highlight innovative applications of cognitive
be found on ABCT’s web
site or in the February behavioral treatments or submissions that help highlight ways that
issue of the Behavior we can broaden our focus about the populations, settings, and disci-
Therapist. plines in which cognitive behavioral treatments can be used are
encouraged and will receive special consideration. We welcome
Submission representation in areas or from disciplines that may have been
Deadline: underrepresented in recent years.
March 2, 2010 PROGRAM CHAIR: John D. Otis, Ph.D.

January • 2010 21
Call
1 6 t h An n ual Award s & Re c o g n itio n

Aw a r d
for
Nominations
The ABCT Awards and Recognition Committee, chaired by Shelley Robbins of Holy Family
University, is pleased to announce the 2010 awards program. Nominations are requested in
all categories listed below. Please see the specific nomination instructions in each category.
Please note that award nominations may not be submitted by current members of
the ABCT Board of Directors.

Outstanding Contribution by an Individual of your submission to ABCT, Student Dissertation Awards, 305
for for Research Activities Seventh Ave., New York, NY 10001.
Eligible candidates for this award should be members of ABCT in
good standing who have provided significant contributions to the Distinguished Friend to Behavior Therapy
literature advancing our knowledge of behavior therapy. Past Eligible candidates for this award should NOT be members of
recipients of this award include Alan E. Kazdin in 1998, David H. ABCT, but are individuals who have promoted the mission of cog-
Barlow in 2001, Terence M. Keane in 2004, and Thomas nitive and/or behavioral work outside of our organization.
Borkovec in 2007. Please complete an on-line nomination form Applications should include a letter of nomination, three letters of
at www.abct.org. Then, e-mail the completed forms to srob- support, and a curriculum vitae of the nominee. Past recipients of
bins@holyfamily.edu. Also, mail a hard copy of your submission this award include Jon Kabat-Zinn, Nora Volkow, John Allen,
to ABCT, Outstanding Researcher, 305 Seventh Ave., New York, Anne Fletcher, Jack Gorman, Art Dykstra, and Michael Davis.
NY 10001. Please complete an on-line nomination form at www.abct.org.
Then, e-mail the completed forms to srobbins@holyfamily.edu.
Also, mail a hard copy of your submission to ABCT, Distinguished
Outstanding Mentor Friend to BT Award, 305 Seventh Ave., New York, NY 10001.
This year we are seeking eligible candidates for the Outstanding
Mentor award who are members of ABCT in good standing who Career/Lifetime Achievement
have encouraged the clinical and/or academic and professional Eligible candidates for this award should be members of ABCT in
excellence of psychology graduate students, interns, postdocs, good standing who have made significant contributions over a
and/or residents. Outstanding mentors are considered those who number of years to cognitive and/or behavior therapy.
have provided exceptional guidance to students through leader- Applications should include a letter of nomination, three letters of
ship, advisement, and activities aimed at providing opportunities support, and a curriculum vitae of the nominee. Past recipients of
for professional development, networking, and future growth. this award include Albert Ellis, Leonard Ullman, Leonard Krasner,
Appropriate nominators are current or past students of the men- Steve Hayes, and David H. Barlow. Please complete an on-line
tor. The first recipient of this award was Richard Heimberg in nomination form at www.abct.org. Then, e-mail the completed
2006, followed by G. Terence Wilson in 2008. Please complete forms to srobbins@holyfamily.edu. Also, mail a hard copy of your
an on-line nomination form at www.abct.org. Then, e-mail the submission to ABCT, Career/Lifetime Achievement Award, 305
completed forms to srobbins@holyfamily.edu. Also, mail a hard Seventh Ave., New York, NY 10001.
copy of your submission to ABCT, Outstanding Mentor, 305
Seventh Avenue, NY, NY 10001. NOMINATIONS FOR THE FOLLOWING AWARD ARE SOLICITED
FROM MEMBERS OF THE ABCT GOVERNANCE :

Student Dissertation Awards: Outstanding Service to ABCT


• The Virginia A. Roswell Student Dissertation Award Members of the governance, please complete an on-line nomina-
• The Leonard Krasner Student Dissertation Award tion by visiting www.abct.org. Then, e-mail the completed forms to
Each award will be given to one student based on his/her doc- srobbins@holyfamily.edu. Also, mail a hard copy of your submis-
toral dissertation proposal. The research should be relevant to sion to ABCT, Outstanding Service to ABCT Award, 305 Seventh
behavior therapy. Accompanying this honor will be a $1,000 Ave., New York, NY 10001.
award to be used in support of research (e.g., to pay participants,
to purchase testing equipment) and/or to facilitate travel to the
Questions? Contact: Shelley Robbins, Ph.D., Chair, ABCT Awards
ABCT convention. Eligible candidates for this award should be
& Recognition Committee; e-mail: srobbins@holyfamily.edu
student members who have already had their dissertation pro-
posal approved and are investigating an area of direct relevance
to behavior therapy, broadly defined. A student's dissertation Nominate on line: www.abct.org
mentor should complete the nomination. Please complete an on-
line nomination form at www.abct.org. Then, e-mail the com- Deadline for all nominations:
pleted forms to srobbins@holyfamily.edu. Also, mail a hard copy Monday, March 2, 2010
22 the Behavior Therapist
Elsie Ramos Poster Award Winners (l to r) Landon Fuhrman, Thomas
Armstrong, and Nisha Sethi (with Lily McNair, Chair, Elsie Ramos
Poster Award)
John P. Forsyth (left) receiving the Outstanding Training Program
Award from David A. F. Haaga, Awards & Recognition Chair

43rd Annual
Convention

Awards
Ceremony

Philip C. Kendall receiving the Outstanding Lata McGinn, Program Chair; Edna Foa receiving the Lifetime Achievement Award
Contribution by an Individual for Education/ and Robert Leahy, President
Training (2008-09)

Top row, l to r: Jonathan Abramowitz, Chair, Self-Help Book of Merit Committee; Martin Antony, Self-Help Book of Merit; Richard Swinson, Self-Help Book
of Merit; Dennis Greenberger, Self-Help Book of Merit; Robert Leahy, ABCT President; Arthur Freeman, Outstanding Service to ABCT; Diane Logan,
Virginia Roswell Dissertation; B. Timothy Walsh, Distinguished Friend to ABCT; John P. Forsyth, Outstanding Training Program (SUNY-Albany Doctoral in
Clinical Psychology); Bunmi Olatunji, President’s New Researcher; Sally Moore, Neil S. Jacobson Research Award • Seated, l to r: Landon Fuhrman, Nisha
Sethi, and Thomas Armstrong, Elsie Ramos Poster Award; Michael Anestis, Leonard Krasner Student Dissertation; Eddie Selby, Neil S. Jacobson Research
Award; Rex Forehand, Self-Help Book of Merit; Dave Haaga, Chair, Awards and Recognition

January • 2010 23
the Behavior Therapist PRSRT STD
Association for Behavioral
and Cognitive Therapies U.S. POSTAGE
305 Seventh Avenue, 16th floor PAID
New York, NY 10001-6008 Hanover, PA
212-647-1890 | www.abct.org Permit No. 4
ADDRESS SERVICE REQUESTED

NOMINATE the Next Candidates for ABCT Office 2010 Call for Nominations
I nominate the following individuals Every nomination counts! Encourage colleagues to run
for office or consider running yourself. Nominate as many
for the positions indicated: full members as you like for each office. The results will be
tallied and the names of those individuals who receive the
P R E S I D E N T- E L E C T ( 2 0 1 0 – 2 0 1 1 )
most nominations will appear on the election ballot next
April. Only those nomination forms bearing a signature
and postmark on or before February 1, 2010, will be
counted.
R E P R E S E N TAT I V E - AT- L A R G E ( 2 0 1 0 – 2 0 1 3 )
Nomination acknowledges an individual's leadership
abilities and dedication to behavior therapy and/or cogni-
 tive therapy, empirically supported science, and to ABCT.
When completing the nomination form, please take into
consideration that these individuals will be entrusted to
represent the interests of ABCT members in important pol-
NAME (printed) icy decisions in the coming years. Contact the Leadership
and Elections Chair for more information about serving
ABCT or to get more information on the positions.
Please complete, sign, and send this nomination form
S I G N AT U R E ( r e q u i r e d ) to Ray DiGiuseppe, Ph.D., Leadership & Elections Chair,
ABCT, 305 Seventh Ave., New York, NY 10001.

You might also like