Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 19

Leisure Sciences

ISSN: 0149-0400 (Print) 1521-0588 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ulsc20

A Structural Model of Leisure Constraints


Negotiation in Outdoor Recreation

Dave D. White

To cite this article: Dave D. White (2008) A Structural Model of Leisure Constraints Negotiation in
Outdoor Recreation, Leisure Sciences, 30:4, 342-359, DOI: 10.1080/01490400802165131

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/01490400802165131

Published online: 09 Jul 2008.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 9141

View related articles

Citing articles: 108 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=ulsc20
Leisure Sciences, 30: 342–359, 2008
Copyright C Taylor & Francis Group, LLC

ISSN: 0149-0400 print / 1521-0588 online


DOI: 10.1080/01490400802165131

A Structural Model of Leisure Constraints


Negotiation in Outdoor Recreation

DAVE D. WHITE
School of Community Resources & Development
Arizona State University, Phoenix, AZ, USA

A conceptual model tested the leisure constraints negotiation process of outdoor recre-
ation: motivation and the constraints to participate likely influenced by negotiation
efforts. Higher motivation to participate encourages using negotiation strategies and
resources to overcome constraints. Experiencing constraints was thought to trigger
negotiation efforts. Drawing from social cognitive theory, negotiation-efficacy was pro-
posed to encourage motivation, diminish the perception of constraints and promote
negotiation efforts, which indirectly influenced positive participation. The model tested
used data collected from a random sample of Arizona residents through hierarchical
confirmatory factor analysis and structural equation modeling. Results support the con-
ceptual model and suggest the constraints negotiation process is a dynamic interaction
of influences promoting outdoor recreation participation.

Keywords motivation, self-efficacy, social cognitive theory, structural equation


modeling

Constraints negotiation research has shed new light on social-psychological and behav-
ioral processes fundamental to leisure such as motivation, desired experiences, constraints,
negotiation and activity participation. The negotiation concept was introduced to explain
how leisure constraints may be overcome or navigated (Jackson, Crawford, & Godbey,
1993; Kay & Jackson, 1991; Scott, 1991). Initial empirical research was mostly descriptive
as researchers identified strategies and resources used by people to negotiate constraints
(Jackson & Rucks, 1995; Scott, 1991) and categorized respondents based on the use of
negotiation resources (Henderson et al., 1995).
Recent research has made advances in theory and method by placing negotiation in the
broader context of leisure behavior and by exploring relations between constraints and other
concepts using multivariate analysis. Notably, Hubbard and Mannell (2001) used confirma-
tory factor analysis and structural equation modeling to examine four competing models
of the negotiation process. They found support for a constraint-effects-mitigation model
in which encountering constraints activates the use of negotiation strategies that limit the
negative impact of constraints on activity participation. They also found that higher moti-
vation presages greater negotiation efforts. Loucks-Atkinson and Mannell (2007) extended
this research by drawing on social cognitive theory and incorporating a negotiation-efficacy
construct. In his overview of the epistemology of leisure constraints, Jackson (2005a) stated
that such empirical tests of theoretically derived models through confirmatory factor analysis

Received 26 September 2006; accepted 3 August 2007.


Address correspondence to Dave D. White, Arizona State University, School of Community Resources &
Development, 411 N. Central Ave. Ste. 550, Phoenix, AZ, 85044. E-mail: dave.white.@asu.edu

342
Constraints Negotiation in Outdoor Recreation 343

FIGURE 1 Structural model of leisure constraints negotiation with hypotheses and pa-
rameters to be estimated.

and structurally equation modeling is one of the most “innovative, and potentially fruitful
directions” (p. 8) for research.
Despite the significant contributions of the work by Mannell and colleagues (i.e.,
Hubbard & Mannell, 2001; Loucks-Atkinson & Mannell, 2007), certain admitted limi-
tations of these studies require additional research be conducted to clarify the conceptual
model and demonstrate generalizability of the findings in additional populations. Hubbard
and Mannell’s study was based on a limited and homogenous sample of employees from
companies that offered work site recreation programs and the authors admitted: “Caution
needs to be exercised in generalizing the findings of the present study. Further research with
other populations and activities will be needed to determine the universality of the pro-
cesses identified” (p. 158). Their study relied on a global measure of motivation comprised
of two items, which the authors recognized as a limitation as did others (e.g., Alexan-
dris, Tsorbatzoudis, & Grouios, 2002). The moderate sample size in Loucks-Atkinson
and Mannell’s study eliminated the possibility of using structural equation modeling, a
multivariate technique that accounts for measurement error in the model and establishes
relationships among constructs while holding constant the effects of the other constructs.
Thus, Loucks-Atkinson and Mannell’s analysis and interpretation relied on the assumption
of perfect measurement of the model constructs. Further, their conclusions were based on
correlational and not causal analysis. The sample in Loucks-Atkinson and Mannell’s study
was a purposively selected group of individuals with a specific physical disorder, and thus,
the generalizability of the findings was limited.
My study extends prior research by conducting an empirical test of a conceptual model
of constraints negotiation in the context of outdoor recreation (see Figure 1). The model
344 D. D. White

proposes that motivation is directly and positively related to outdoor recreation participation.
The positive influence of motivation is counteracted by the negative influence of constraints.
The model also proposes that these relationships are likely to be influenced by efforts to
negotiate: Higher motivation to participate in outdoor recreation is likely to encourage
the use of negotiation strategies and resources to overcome constraints. The experience of
constraints, however, also has an indirect positive impact on participation by triggering or
activating negotiation efforts. Negotiation-efficacy encourages motivation, diminishes the
perception of constraints, and encourages the use of negotiation efforts, thus having an
indirect positive influence on participation.
This extension is particularly important for several reasons. First, my study is based
on a statewide random sample of Arizona residents, which provided the opportunity to as-
sess the generalizability of previous findings in a diverse population. Second, the relatively
large sample size (n = 439) permitted the use of multivariate analyses to assess construct
validity, account for measurement error and test for mediation. Hierarchical confirmatory
factor analysis (CFA) was used to evaluate the factor structures of the constructs and test
the measurement model. Third, structural equation modeling (SEM) was used to specify,
estimate and assess the theoretical model to test hypothesized relationships. Further, rec-
ommendations from several scholars (Henderson et al., 1995; Hubbard & Mannell, 2001;
Loucks-Atkinson & Mannell, 2007) were followed to draw from social cognitive theory and
specifically self-efficacy to develop the theoretical specification of the negotiation process.
In addition to theory development, this study may provide recreation planners and man-
agers greater appreciation of the factors related to outdoor recreation participation. Such
improved understanding can give leisure service providers new insights into strategies to
limit constraints, encourage negotiation, and maximize the benefits of outdoor recreation
to individuals and society.

Review of the Literature


Constraints emerged as a central theme in leisure research over the past 25 years as demon-
strated by the proliferation of journal articles and the recent publication of a comprehensive
text on the subject (Jackson, 2005b). Early leisure constraints researchers framed the is-
sue in terms of barriers to recreation activity participation (e.g., Buchanan & Allen, 1988;
Searle & Jackson, 1985a, 1985b) with the implicit assumption that encountering barri-
ers necessarily resulted in nonparticipation. Constraints were defined as factors that may
inhibit activity participation or limit satisfaction (Jackson, 1988). Crawford and Godbey
(1987) argued that constraints affect not only participation but also acquisition of leisure
preferences. They organized constraints into three categories: intrapersonal constraints de-
fined as individual psychological qualities that affect the development of leisure preferences
(e.g., shyness), interpersonal constraints defined as social factors that affect development
of leisure preferences (e.g., lack of companions), and structural constraints comprised of
factors that intervene between development of leisure preferences and participation (e.g.,
financial resources). Crawford, Jackson, and Godbey (1991) extended this line of thinking
and presented their hierarchical model of leisure constraints, which posited that intraper-
sonal and interpersonal constraints affect leisure preferences whereas structural constraints
intervene between preferences and participation.
The origins of constraints research has been traced at least as far back as the U.S. Out-
door Recreation Resources Review Commission (ORRRC) reports of the 1960s (Jackson &
Scott, 1999). As Walker and Virden (2005) noted, however, research on constraints to out-
door recreation has been descriptive and not explanatory with little theoretical development.
Walker and Virden pointed out several constraints that scored high on scales across studies,
Constraints Negotiation in Outdoor Recreation 345

including lack of time, distance to recreation resources, crowding, lack of information,


costs, and family commitments. They also noted that constraints to outdoor recreation were
similar to other leisure activities but some, like lack of time, may have a stronger impact in
the outdoor recreation context because of the travel commitments sometimes necessary to
reach undeveloped natural areas. The conceptualization of leisure constraints and empirical
studies from outdoor recreation literature led to the first study hypothesis:

H1: Constraints have a direct negative effect on outdoor recreation participation.

It has become increasingly clear that constraints are not necessarily fixed barriers that
result in nonparticipation; rather, constraints once encountered might be overcome or ne-
gotiated (Crawford et al., 1991; Jackson et al., 1993; Kay & Jackson, 1991; Scott, 1991).
Researchers identified strategies and resources used by people to negotiate constraints. For
instance, Scott found that bridge players acquired information about opportunities, man-
aged time and schedules and developed new skills. Kay and Jackson discovered that when
faced with financial constraints, respondents saved money and identified less expensive
opportunities. When faced with time constraints, people reduced time spent on household
chores and reduced work time. Jackson et al. synthesized existing research and presented
a series of propositions that outlined the constraints negotiation process. Their first and
most central proposition stated that “participation is dependent not on the absence of con-
straints. . . but on negotiation through them” (p. 4). Jackson and Rucks (1995) were the
first to design a study explicitly to investigate negotiation. The descriptive study classified
negotiation strategies as cognitive or behavioral with the latter category subdivided into
time management, skill acquisition, changing interpersonal relations, improving finances,
physical therapy and changing leisure aspirations. Based on existing negotiation research,
the second hypothesis in this study was:

H2: Negotiation has a direct positive effect on outdoor recreation participation.

The role of motivation in the negotiation process was addressed explicitly for the first
time by Jackson et al. (1993) in their sixth or balance proposition, which stated that “both the
initiation and outcome of the negotiation process are dependent upon the relative strength
of, and interactions between, constraints on participating in activity and motivation for such
participation” (p. 9). In the outdoor recreation literature, motivation is often conceptual-
ized as desire for satisfying recreation experiences and operationalized through the use of
the Recreation Experience Preference (REP) scales (Driver, Tinsley, & Manfredo, 1991;
Manfredo, Driver, & Tarrant, 1996; Moore & Driver, 2005). The full REP scales include
21 factors each with 2–7 dimensions (Moore & Driver). The REP scales were not based
explicitly in motivation, human need or self-determination theory but were developed in-
ductively over 20 years through exploratory factor analyses. Although the REP scales may
be criticized for poor theoretical specification, Manfredo et al. (1996) concluded that the
scales “can be usefully applied when attempting to determine motivations for or the psy-
chological outcomes desired from leisure” (p. 204). Manfredo et al. determined that the
REP demonstrated overall consistency, construct validity and acceptable reliability in a
meta-analysis of 36 studies using them to measure leisure motivations. Studies of leisure
motivations in outdoor recreation led to the third hypothesis:

H3: Motivation, in the form of desire for satisfying recreation experiences, has a
direct positive effect on outdoor recreation participation.
346 D. D. White

The effect of motivation on negotiation efforts has received more attention in recent
years. In their studies of sport and physical activity participation in Greece, Alexandris and
colleagues (i.e., Alexandris, Barkoukis, Tsorbatzoudis, & Grouios, 2003; Carroll & Alexan-
dris, 1997) addressed the relations among motivation, constraint, and participation. Carroll
and Alexandris found a negative bivariate correlation between motivation and constraints,
suggesting that people who are less motivated to participate experience higher levels of
constraint. The significance of this finding was limited, however, because it was based
on correlational and not causal analysis. Additional research provided some contradictory
evidence.
By 2001 researchers had begun to develop conceptual models of constraint negotiation
and test these models with empirical data using multivariate analyses. Specifically, Hub-
bard and Mannell (2001) used CFA and SEM to test four competing models: the indepen-
dence model, the negotiation buffer model, the constraint-effects-mitigation model and the
perceived-constraint-reduction model. Each model included the constructs of motivation,
constraint, negotiation and participation (i.e., as the dependent variable). The constraint-
effects-mitigation model hypothesized that motivation and negotiation positively and di-
rectly influenced participation while constraint negatively and directly influenced partic-
ipation. The positive relationship between constraint and participation was hypothesized
to be mediated by negotiation. Of the four models tested, the constraint-effects-mitigation
model provided the best fit to the data. Hubbard and Mannell concluded this model was
superior. The model implied that encountering leisure constraints prompts a person to em-
ploy negotiation strategies, which mitigates the negative impact of constraint on leisure
participation.
The constraint-effects-mitigation model provided some additional insight into the role
of motivation in negotiation and participation. Although a weak bivariate correlation existed
between motivation and participation, once the effects of other variables were controlled,
the final structural model did not include a significant direct path from motivation to partic-
ipation. Hubbard and Mannell (2001) interpreted this finding to mean that the relationship
between motivation and participation was mediated by negotiation, although whether this
mediation was statistically significant was unclear from their analysis. Although Carroll
and Alexandris (1997) and other studies suggest that motivation plays a more pivotal role
in encouraging participation directly, Hubbard and Mannell indicated that motivation’s ef-
fect on participation is mediated by negotiation. Though the existing research is somewhat
equivocal on these points, hypotheses four and five for this study were:

H4: Constraint has a direct positive effect on negotiation.


H5: Motivation has a direct positive effect on negotiation.

As Hubbard and Mannell (2001) stated, however:

Whether motivation is an immediate antecedent and plays a stronger direct role in


countering the effects of constraints, when other types of leisure activities, motives,
and circumstances is involved, is unclear and will have to be determined by future
research. (p. 159)

Self-efficacy in Leisure Constraints Negotiation


Several authors discussed the potential utility of social cognitive theory and specifically the
self-efficacy construct to leisure constraint negotiation (Henderson et al., 1995; Hubbard
& Mannell, 2001). The idea that successful negotiation of leisure constraints may lead to
Constraints Negotiation in Outdoor Recreation 347

increased confidence in the ability to negotiate future constraints is implied by propositions


three and five from Jackson et al. (1993). Proposition three stated the “absence of the desire
to change current leisure behavior may be partly explained by prior successful negotiation
of structural constraints” (p. 6) and proposition six stated that “anticipation consists not
simply of the anticipation of the presence or intensity of a constraint but also of anticipation
of the ability to negotiate it” (p. 8).
Bandura (1994) defined perceived self-efficacy as “people’s beliefs about their capa-
bilities to produce designated levels of performance that exercise influence over events that
affect their lives” (p. 71). Self-efficacy involves individual judgment about competence to
perform a specific task or group of tasks in a given domain. Belief in self-efficacy in one task
or domain, however, may be generalized to enhance efficacious feelings in other tasks and
domains (Bandura, 1986). People with higher levels of perceived self-efficacy have greater
motivation to persevere in the face of adversity. Bandura (1997) argued that self-efficacy
is a powerful determinant of human behavior and changing self-efficacy perceptions is a
necessary precursor to changing behavior.
Bandura (1997) identified four sources of self-efficacy: enactive attainment through
mastery experience, vicarious experience, social persuasion and physiological and affective
states. Mastery experience is the most influential source of self-efficacy and is derived
from prior success at completing tasks and is undermined by failures. Self-efficacy is also
produced through vicarious experience by observing other people succeed or fail in their
efforts to accomplish tasks. The influence of vicarious experience on a subject’s self-efficacy
is greatest when the subject perceives similarity to the model. The third source of self-
efficacy is social persuasion. When people are encouraged to sustain their effort in the face
of challenges it promotes skill development, sense of achievement, and personal efficacy.
Finally, psychological and affective states play a role in the development of self-efficacy.
People interpret stress and tension as indicators of poor performance and positive mood as
indicators of success. Thus, self-efficacy can be generated by association of success with
positive mood.
Self-efficacy affects human functioning through four major psychological processes:
cognition, motivation, affect and selection (Bandura, 1994). First, people’s self-efficacy
beliefs influence cognitive processes in the way people think about and construct scenarios
about likely outcomes of future behaviors. Second, self-efficacy affects motivational pro-
cesses through causal attributions, outcome expectations and goals. Self-efficacy influence
people’s attributions of their success or failure individual effort or ability; their expectations
about the likelihood of valued outcomes of their behavior; and the goals they set, the amount
of effort expended to achieve their goals, and their perseverance and resilience in the face
of setbacks. Third, self-efficacy plays a role in regulating affective states such as stress,
anxiety arousal and depression through the exercise of control over disturbing thoughts.
Fourth, self-efficacy beliefs have an effect on the environments and activities people select,
thus influencing the course of their lives.
Self-efficacy has been employed in public health and exercise science studies through
constructs such as exercise self-efficacy and barriers self-efficacy (e.g., Bloomquist et al.,
2006; Marquez & McAuley, 2006; McAuley, 1992, 1993; Motl et al., 2005; Winters, Petosa,
& Charlton, 2003). Although the public health and exercise science literature has retained
the barriers framework as opposed to the leisure science conceptualization of constraints,
these studies provide further evidence to expect that higher levels of confidence in one’s
ability to overcome constraints to recreation activity participation (i.e., negotiation-efficacy)
could improve models of leisure constraint negotiation.
Loucks-Atkinson and Mannell (2007) examined the role of self-efficacy in the con-
straints negotiation process for individuals with fibromyalgia syndrome, a condition
348 D. D. White

characterized by chronic pain and related symptoms such as anxiety and depression. For
the study, the authors defined negotiation-efficacy as “people’s confidence in their ability to
successfully use negotiation strategies to overcome constraints they encounter” (p. 22). To
measure negotiation-efficacy, they asked respondents to report their level of confidence in
their ability to use 37 specific negotiation strategies on a confidence scale ranging from 0%
(Very Uncertain) to 100% (Very Certain). Their measure focused on assessing the level and
strength of specific efficacy expectations. They examined four nested models of the role of
self-efficacy in constraint negotiation using path analysis. Considering fit indices, model
comparisons and parsimony, they settled on a model that indicated:

(1) constraints negatively influenced participation and positively influenced nego-


tiation efforts, (2) negotiation efforts positively affected participation, (3) moti-
vation increased negotiation efforts, (4) negotiation-efficacy positively influenced
strength of motivation to participate and negotiation efforts, and (5) motivation pos-
itively influenced participation. Finally, these results suggested that negotiation-
efficacy did not have a significant negative effect on perceived constraints. (p. 32)

Loucks-Atkinson and Mannell’s findings provide support for the inclusion of the
negotiation-efficacy construct in models of the constraints negotiation process. Specifically,
a direct positive relationship was identified between negotiation-efficacy and negotiation
indicating that higher confidence in the ability to successfully negotiate constraints re-
sulted in greater efforts to negotiate. The final three hypotheses study stem from studies of
self-efficacy in leisure, recreation, and public health:

H6: Negotiation-efficacy has a direct positive effect on negotiation.


H7: Negotiation-efficacy has a direct positive effect on motivation.
H8: Negotiation-efficacy has a direct negative effect on constraints.

Methods
Data Collection and Sample
The data analyzed for this paper were collected as part of a larger study of leisure in Arizona
conducted to inform natural and cultural resource managers at Arizona State Parks. Data
were collected through a random digit-dialed (RDD) telephone survey of Arizona house-
holds followed by a self-administered mail questionnaire of respondents identified through
the telephone survey. The telephone questionnaire included questions about frequency of
visitation to parks, recreation areas and cultural sites in the past year, awareness of park
and recreation management agencies in Arizona, satisfaction with Arizona State Parks, and
respondent demographics. The telephone survey instrument was pretested, translated into
Spanish and administered by a professional survey research firm using computer-aided tele-
phone interviewing (CATI). The sample was drawn from a database of telephone area codes
and exchanges and an RDD procedure was used to select individual telephone numbers.
A total of 1,500 telephone interviews were completed in English (n = 1,419) and Spanish
(n = 81). Calculation of final outcome rates for the RDD household survey was based on
procedures outlined by the American Association for Public Opinion Research (AAOPR:
2006).
Response Rate 1 (RR1) or minimum response rate and Cooperation Rate 1 (COOP1)
or minimum cooperation rate were calculated. Minimum response rate is the total number
of complete interviews divided by number of interviews plus noninterviews plus all cases
Constraints Negotiation in Outdoor Recreation 349

of unknown eligibility. The RR1 for this study was 34%. Cooperation rate is proportion
of all complete interviews divided by number of complete interviews plus noninterviews
where an eligible respondent was contacted. The COOP1 for this study was 80%. Thus,
80% of all households where an eligible respondent was identified agreed to participate in
the study. The margin of sampling error for the telephone survey is +/− 2.5% at the 95%
confidence interval.
The demographic profile of telephone survey respondents was similar to U.S. Census
data for the state. Specifically, the percentage of respondents in the completed telephone
sample was within +/− 3% for gender and for each racial and ethnic category with the
exception of Hispanic. Fewer Hispanic respondents were in the telephone sample (15%)
than expected based on the Census estimates for the state (29%). At the conclusion of the
interview, each respondent was asked to participate in the mail survey and offered an incen-
tive in the form of an Arizona State Parks admission coupon. A mail survey questionnaire
was sent to 1,033 telephone survey respondents who agreed to participate, or 69% of those
contacted by phone.
The mail survey followed the tailored design method (Dillman, 2000) with five con-
tacts including the initial telephone request, survey mailing with incentive for all potential
respondents, postcard reminder, second full mailing and final phone or e-mail contact.
The mail survey included questions about visitation to parks, recreation areas, natural ar-
eas and historic sites in Arizona; motives for recreation participation; activity and set-
tings preferences; information seeking behavior; leisure constraints and constraints ne-
gotiation strategies; satisfaction with Arizona State Parks; and respondent demographics.
There were 439 completed and usable mail surveys returned and the RR1 for the mail
survey was 45%. Nonresponse bias analysis revealed no significant differences between
respondents and nonrespondents in terms of length of Arizona residency. Mail survey
respondents were, however, younger than nonrespondents (47 years vs. 52 years), and
fewer Hispanic respondents were in the completed mail survey sample (7%) than the tele-
phone sample (15%). Thus, with these limitations, the completed sample was deemed to
adequately reflect the state population with the notable exception of underrepresentation
of Hispanic residents. The data used for the study were taken from the 439 usable mail
questionnaires.

Measurement of the Constructs


The motivations or reasons for outdoor recreation participation were measured by 11 items
assessing the importance of desired experiences (see Table 1). Respondents rated each item
on a five-point Likert-type scale (1 = Not at all Important, 5 = Extremely Important). The
items were drawn from the recreation experience preference (REP) scales (Driver et al.,
1991; Manfredo et al., 1996; Moore & Driver, 2005). Manfredo et al. suggest that when
using REP scales to measure motivation choosing domains and items of relevance to the
specific study site and population is reasonable. A four factor solution was proposed for the
measurement model with the 11 observed items serving as indicators on one of four first-
order factors. The first-order factors in turn loaded on the higher order motivation factor.
The first-order factors are listed in Table 1.
To measure constraints, respondents rated the extent to which they agreed or disagreed
with 13 statements describing conditions that may limit their outdoor recreation participation
(see Table 1). A five-point scale (1 = Strongly Disagree, 5 = Strongly Agree) was used.
The list of items was adapted from previous research (Hubbard & Mannell, 2001; Raymore
et al., 1993). A three-factor solution was proposed for the measurement model with the
13 observed items serving as indicators on one of three first-order factors. The first-order
350 D. D. White

TABLE 1 Standardized Parameter Coefficients, Means, and Standard Deviations for


Motivation, Constraint, Negotiation Strategy, Negotiation-Efficacy, and Participation
Items

Second-order Factor
First-order Factor
Indicators Coefficient Mean SD

Motivation
Achievement
Gain a sense of accomplishment .873 2.65 1.168
Experience excitement/adventure .551 3.25 1.164
Gain a sense of self-confidence .786 2.61 1.213
Develop my skills and abilities .725 2.52 1.210
Enjoy Nature
Be close to nature .643 4.13 1.019
Observe the scenic beauty .833 4.50 .760
Enjoy the sounds and smells of nature .930 4.41 .867
Escape
Get away from the usual demands of life .741 4.07 1.063
Experience solitude .540 3.53 1.269
Socialize
Be with family or friends .592 3.99 1.079
Be with people who share my values .744 3.14 1.291
Constraints
Intrapersonal
Afraid of getting hurt by animals .599 1.7692 .982
Lack of interest .695 2.1389 1.087
Don’t feel welcome .760 1.9453 .957
Afraid of getting hurt by other people .612 2.1521 1.109
Lack of information .511 2.6559 1.113
Don’t have the skills or physical ability .563 2.4036 1.242
Interpersonal
Companions prefer other things .636 2.7369 1.017
Don’t have companions/people to go with .783 2.5820 1.213
The people I know live or work too far away .736 2.5291 1.057
Structural
Camping fees are too high .743 2.7150 1.074
Admission fees are too high .880 2.7338 1.184
It is too expensive .800 2.4231 1.133
Negotiation
Changing Interpersonal Relations
Try to find people with similar interests .660 3.3193 .985
Ask my family to share the chores .546 3.0628 1.020
Bring other people to make me feel safer .522 2.8324 1.120
Organize events with my own group .531 3.1494 1.009
Changing Leisure Aspirations
Go to areas that are less crowded .576 3.8437 .924
Go to areas where I feel comfortable .785 4.0365 .823
Improve Finances
Try to budget money .732 3.2840 1.011
Set aside money to use for recreation .894 3.2617 .963
(Continued on next page).
Constraints Negotiation in Outdoor Recreation 351

TABLE 1 Standardized Parameter Coefficients, Means, and Standard Deviations for


Motivation, Constraint, Negotiation Strategy, Negotiation-Efficacy, and Participation
Items (Continued)

Second-order Factor
First-order Factor
Indicators Coefficient Mean SD

Negotiation-efficacy
In the past, I have been successful getting around the .495 3.46 .929
barriers to my outdoor recreation
People I admire find ways around challenges they .737 3.55 .921
face when trying to recreate
My family and friends encourage me to participate .675 3.31 .990
in outdoor recreation, even when there are
obstacles
I enjoy overcoming obstacles to my outdoor .598 3.38 .959
recreation participation
Participation
Number of Arizona State Parks Visited in previous 727 15.888 7.709
12 months.
Outdoor recreation activity participation scale. .481 3.294 3.369

factors in turn loaded on the higher order constraints factor. The first-order factors are listed
in Table 1.
To measure negotiation, respondents were asked the level to which they agreed with
eight strategies to start, continue, or increase participation in outdoor recreation (see Table 1)
on a five-point scale (1 = Strongly Disagree, 5 = Strongly Agree). As with the constraints
items, an initial list of negotiation statements was developed from prior research (Hubbard
& Mannell, 2001; Jackson & Rucks, 1995). A three-factor solution was proposed for the
measurement model with the eight observed items serving as indicators on one of three
first-order factors. The first-order factors in turn loaded on the higher order negotiation
factor. The first-order factors are listed in Table 1.
Four items were developed specifically for this study to measure negotiation self-
efficacy (see Table 1). These items were designed to tap the four sources of self-efficacy
defined by Bandura (1997): mastery experience, vicarious experience, social persuasion
and psychological/emotional response. Respondents were asked their level of agreement
with each statement on a five-point scale (1 = Strongly Disagree, 5 = Strongly Agree). This
method of measuring negotiation self-efficacy differed from Loucks-Atkinson & Mannell
(2007) where respondents were asked to rate their confidence in their ability to employ
specific negotiation resources from 0% to 100%. Although that approach clearly has merits
for assessing perceived self-efficacy to perform specific tasks, the intent in this study was to
develop a unidimensional measure of perceived self-efficacy for the domain of negotiation.
The four items served as indicators of the negotiation self-efficacy construct in the model.
Two measures of participation were calculated. To assess frequency and diversity of
outdoor recreation participation, respondents reported frequency of engagement in each of
32 activities in the 12 months prior to the survey. Participation frequency was measured
on an ordinal scale ranging from 0 = Never to 1 = Occasionally to 2 = Frequently.) A
summative index was created for each respondent which could range from a minimum of 0
(i.e., never participated in any activities) to 64 (i.e., frequently participated in all activities).
352 D. D. White

For the second measure of participation, respondents reported whether or not they had
visited each of 29 Arizona State Parks in the year prior to the survey. The responses were
summed so that each respondent was assigned a score for state park visits ranging from 0
to 29. Due to the truncated ordinal scales used to measure participation, summative scales
were used instead of higher order factors.

Analysis
All variables in the model were screened for normality by examining skewness and kurtosis.
Although significance tests are available to assess normality, in studies such as this one with
samples of greater than 200 cases, variables with statistically significant skewness or kurtosis
typically do not deviate enough from normality to affect the analysis and thus:

If the sample is large it is a good idea to look at the shape of the distribution instead
of using formal inference tests. Because the standard errors for both skewness and
kurtosis decrease with larger N , the null hypothesis is likely to be rejected with
large samples when there are only minor deviations from normality” (Tabachnick
& Fidell, 2000, p. 74).

The variable distributions were assessed using SPSS 14.0 though frequency histograms
with superimposed normal distribution, and expected normal probability plots (P-P plots).
In P-P plots, the scores are ranked and sorted and compared to an expected normal value for
each case. The expected value is the zscore for a case with that rank, which would hold in a
normal distribution and the normal value is the zscore of the actual distribution. If the actual
variable distribution is normal, the cases cluster along a diagonal line in the P-P plots with
minor deviations shifting some points above and below the line. Based on these analyses,
all variables were deemed to be normally distributed and retained for examination using
CFA. The data were screened for missing values, which accounted for less than 4% of the
data for the variables used in the analysis. Regression data imputation was employed to
replace missing values by an estimate called an imputed value.
The measurement model was constructed using SPSS 14.0 and AMOS 6.0. Hierarchical
confirmatory factor analysis, a special case of CFA, was used to specify higher order factor
structures for the constraints, motivation and negotiation constructs. For these constructs,
first-order and second-order factors solutions were hypothesized to represent hierarchi-
cal relations. The hierarchical CFAs represent hypotheses that the first-order factors (e.g.,
structural, intrapersonal, interpersonal) measure the second-order factor (e.g., constraints).
Hierarchical CFA was not used for the negotiation-efficacy construct for several rea-
sons. First, negotiation-efficacy has not undergone adequate theoretical specification, con-
ceptualization or operationalization to allow for hypotheses about hierarchical relations.
Second, in this study the construct was measured using four observed variables and thus did
not meet the minimum requirements for hierarchical CFA. Thus negotiation-efficacy was
treated as a single factor with four observed indicators. Hierarchical CFA was not used for
the participation construct because the observed variables were measured on a truncated
ordinal scale. Participation was treated as a single construct with two indicators as described
earlier. The hierarchical CFA models for motivation, negotiation, and constraint were each
assessed for fit, deemed acceptable, and added to the measurement model along with the
single factor models for negotiation-efficacy and participation.
The full measurement model was then assessed for fit. The standardized parameter
coefficients were significant for all first-order factors on their respective indicators and
for all second-order factors on their respective first-order factors (all p-values < .001).
Multiple fit indices were used including minimum sample discrepancy functions (chi-square
Constraints Negotiation in Outdoor Recreation 353

TABLE 2 Summary of Fit Indices for Measurement and Structural Models

χ2 df p χ 2 /df GFI AGFI CFI RMSEA

Measurement model 1498.94 679 .001 2.20 .87 .84 .87 .05
Structural model 1517.76 681 .001 2.22 .86 .84 .86 .05

statistic, relative chi-square), measures based on population discrepancy (RMSEA), baseline


comparison measures (CFI), and goodness of fit (GFI) and parsimony adjusted goodness
of fit measures (AGFI) (see Table 2). The rules of thumb provided by Arbuckle (2005)
and Kline (2005) were used to assess model fit. A relative chi-square in the range of 3
to 1 indicates favorable fit between the hypothetical model and the sample data for large
samples (i.e., >200). An RMSEA value of 0.05 or less indicates a close fit to the data, a
value of 0.08 indicates a reasonable fit and a value of 0.10 or greater indicates a poor fit.
The CFI measure is based on discrepancy between the hypothesized model and a baseline
or comparison model (i.e., the independence model). This measures ranges from 0 to 1
with values close to 1 indicating a perfect fit and values below 0.90 indicating potential for
improvement. Values for GFI and AGFI of .9 indicate a good fit.

Results
A two-step procedure was used to test the study hypotheses. First, hierarchical confirmatory
factor analysis was employed to develop and assess the measurement model. Once an
acceptable fitting model was developed, the measurement model was modified to represent
the theoretical model and tested through structural equation modeling. Maximum likelihood
estimation was used to estimate both the measurement and structural models.

Measurement Model
The measurement model was accepted (see Table 3). The relative chi-square (2.20) indicated
a favorable fit to the data. The RMSEA value of .05 and the 90% confidence interval around
the RMSEA point estimate (.04 – .06) indicated a close fit. The values for the CFI (.87), GFI
(.87) and AGFI (.84) indicated potential for some improvement in model fit. Values for CFI
are lower when correlations among observed variables are low, as in the case in these data
when so many individual items and constructs are being assessed. With the measurement
accepted, the analysis proceeded to the structural equation model.

TABLE 3 Standardized Direct, Indirect, and Total Effects

F3 F1 F2 F4

D I T D I T D I T D I T

F5 0 .310 .310 −.305 .021 −.284 .465 .029 .494 .09 0 .09
F1 .433 0 .433 — — — — — — — — —
F2 −.249 0 −.249 — — — — — — — — —
F4 .354 .097 .451 .291 0 .291 .391 — .391 — — —
Keys: F1 = Constraints; F2 = Motivation; F3 = Negotiation-efficacy; F4 = Negotiation; F5
= Participation. D = Direct effects; I = Indirect effects; T = Total effects.
354 D. D. White

FIGURE 2 Structural model of leisure constraints negotiation with standardized parame-


ters estimates.

Structural Model
The structural model developed and tested in this study is presented in Figure 1. Following
Lee and Scott (2006), the figure represents the parameters to be estimated by the structural
model. In the figure the letter “L” represents standardized coefficients between observed
(manifest) variables and unobserved (latent) factors. The letter “B” indicates standardized
coefficients between higher order latent factors and first-order latent factors, and between
the exogenous latent factor and the endogenous latent factors. The letter “D” represents
errors of endogenous latent factors and the letter “e” represents error for each manifest
variable.
The fit indices (see Table 3) indicated an acceptable fit for the structural model. The
relative chi-square value (2.22) demonstrates a favorable fit to the sample data. The RMSEA
value of .05 and the 90% confidence interval around the RMSEA point estimate (.04–.06)
indicate a close fit. The values for the CFI (.86), GFI (.86), and AGFI (.84) indicated potential
for improvement in model fit. Again, values for CFI are lower when correlations among
some observed variables are low.
Constraints Negotiation in Outdoor Recreation 355

Hypothesis Tests
Figure 2 shows the standardized coefficients for the paths between higher order latent factors
and first-order latent factors and between the exogenous latent factor and the endogenous
latent factors. (The standardized coefficients between latent factors and observed indicators
are displayed in Table 1). The data supported all the Hypotheses except for Hypothesis 2:
the coefficient was in the predicted direction, but the path was not significant (p = 45). The
paths for Hypotheses 4 thru 7 were positive and statistically significant (p < .001).
In summary, the results of the hypothesis tests provide substantial evidence to conclude
that the data supported nearly all aspects of the structural model, with the exception of a path
from negotiation to participation that was in the predicted direction but was not significant.

Analysis of Direct, Indirect and Total Effects


Table 3 presents the direct, indirect and total effects for the exogenous factor on endogenous
factors and the direct, indirect, and total effects for all mediating endogenous factors on the
dependent endogenous factor. Overall, the table shows that most effects identified in this
study were direct effects, although there were some indirect effects.
Most notably negotiation-efficacy exhibited a weak to moderate positive influence on
participation with a total indirect effect of .310. This indirect effect of negation efficacy
on participation was mediated through motivation (indirect effect of .201 = .433 × .465),
constraints (indirect effect of .076 = −.249 × −.305), and negotiation (indirect effect of
.026 = .354 × .073). Thus, most of the indirect effect of negotiation-efficacy on participation
is attributable to the path from negotiation-efficacy through motivation to participation.
Whether part of the relationship between negotiation-efficacy and participation is accounted
for by the relationship each has with motivation cannot be determined by examining the path
coefficients alone. When paths on both sides of the hypothesized mediator are significant,
as is the case, Sobel (1986) recommended using the multivariate delta method to determine
whether mediation occurred. Tests for significance of the mediation effects provide partial
support for one mediated path. The indirect effect of negotiation-efficacy through motivation
on participation was not statistically significant using the delta method and t-test (p < .05,
t = 1.491). The relationship, however, was significant at p < .10 and thus it may be said
to be “trending” toward significance. The other indirect effects of negotiation-efficacy on
participation (through constraints and negotiation) were not significant.

Discussion
Research on constraints negotiation provides important insights into people’s leisure choices
and behavior. Researchers have identified constraints as factors that inhibit leisure activity
participation or diminish satisfaction (Jackson, 1988), organized constraints into conceptual
categories (Crawford & Godbey, 1987), and developed theoretical propositions to guide
empirical research (Jackson et al., 1993). Researchers also identified negotiation strategies
and resources people use to overcome constraints (Jackson & Rucks, 1995; Kay & Jackson,
1991; Scott, 1991). Recent studies have placed constraints negotiation in a broader context
by examining the relations between leisure constraints, negotiation, motivation, and activity
participation (Alexandris et al., 2002; Hubbard & Mannell, 2001). Several researchers
proposed that self efficacy may provide additional insight into the constraints negotiation
process (Henderson et al., 1995; Hubbard & Mannell, 2001), which led to the introduction
of the idea of negotiation-efficacy (Loucks-Atkinson & Mannell, 2007).
This study contributes to this research by examining the interaction of motivation,
constraints, negotiation, and negotiation-efficacy and their effects on outdoor recreation
356 D. D. White

participation. Results provided partial support for the proposed theoretical model and
showed that constraints negatively influenced outdoor recreation participation and posi-
tively affected negotiation. Negotiation exhibited a small positive impact on participation,
but contrary to the study’s hypothesis the relationship was not significant. Motivation, in
the form of desire for satisfying recreation experiences, had a relatively strong positive
impact on outdoor recreation participation and was the most important single predictor of
participation. Similarly, motivation exhibited a positive effect on negation, suggesting that
greater desire for satisfying recreation experiences also triggers the use of negotiation re-
sources and strategies. Negotiation-efficacy positively influenced motivation to participate
and negotiation efforts and negatively influenced perceived constraints. Finally the results
provide partial support for the notion that negotiation-efficacy exhibits an indirect effect on
participation mediated through motivation.
The results of this study provide support and clarification of a conceptual model of
leisure constraints negotiation reported elsewhere (Hubbard & Mannell, 2001; Loucks-
Atkinson & Mannell, 2007). The results support the notion proposed by the constraints-
effects-mitigation model that “encountering constraints appears to set in motion two
opposing forces – an inhibitory influence on participation stemming directly from the
constraints, and a facilitative influence resulting from the negotiation efforts triggered”
(Hubbard & Mannell, p. 158). In this study constraints exhibited a relatively strong di-
rect negative influence on participation and moderately strong direct positive influence on
negotiation.
The results suggest a central role for motivation than in the constraints negotiation
process. Motivation was both a moderately strong direct antecedent encouraging participa-
tion and a positive influence on efforts to negotiate. This finding is consistent with Jackson
et al.’s (1993) balance proposition, which stated that the initiation and outcome of the nego-
tiation process depends on the relative strength of and interaction between motivation and
constraints. The importance of motivation as a direct antecedent of participation contradicts
Hubbard and Mannell’s (2001) finding that motivation’s primary effect on participation
was indirect and mediated by negotiation. The results of this study, along with findings
from Loucks-Atkinson and Mannell (2007), provide mounting evidence that motivation
exerts influence as an immediate antecedent of participation as well as a potential trigger
for encouraging the constraint negotiation process.
The results provide support for the inclusion of a negotiation-efficacy construct in the
constraints negotiation process. As predicted, people with higher perceptions of negotiation-
efficacy were more motivated to participate in outdoor recreation. This finding is consistent
with predictions based on social cognitive theory. Self-efficacy is associated with causal
attribution, outcome expectations and goals (Bandura, 1994). Thus, people with higher
negotiation-efficacy would be more likely to attribute the causes of their leisure behavior to
their own individual efforts to negotiate constraints and ability to persevere in the face of
challenges. Thus, this study provides support for Loucks-Atkinson and Mannell’s (2007,
p. 34) suggestion to add a seventh proposition to Jackson et al.’s (1993) list. In the current
study, negotiation-efficacy also exhibited a direct negative influence on constraints and
a direct positive influence on negotiation, suggesting that people with higher levels of
negotiation-efficacy not only engage in greater efforts to negotiate, but also they perceive
fewer constraints. Proposition 7 could be revised to state: “The greater people’s confidence
in the successful use of negotiation resources to cope with constraints, the greater the
motivation, the greater the effort to negotiate, the lesser the perception of constraints, and
the higher the level of participation.”
Additional research is necessary to address measurement issues especially for the
negotiation-efficacy construct. Due to disciplinary differences and the domain-specific
Constraints Negotiation in Outdoor Recreation 357

nature of self-efficacy, a variety of related constructs in the literature including self-efficacy


to exercise, self-efficacy to recreate, self-efficacy to overcome barriers to physical exercise,
and now negotiation-efficacy exist. Future research might compare the items developed
here, for instance, with an eight-item scale measuring self-efficacy about physical activity
developed by Motl et al. (2000). That scale has been subjected to rigorous tests of factorial
validity and invariance, but in the context of more general leisure time physical activity.
Further, there remain opportunities for researchers to explore the theoretical linkages be-
tween perceived self-efficacy and perceived behavioral control (Motl et al., 2005) and to
explore the collective negotiation self-efficacy of groups by gender, race and ethnicity.
Continued theoretical development of the leisure constraint negotiation model pre-
sented here will also require additional studies of the factorial validity of negotiation strate-
gies. Despite early findings by Jackson and Rucks (1995) that negotiation strategies tend
to be consistent with the types of constraints encountered (e.g., a person whose leisure is
constrained by lack of skills would employ skill acquisition as a negotiation strategy), little
effort has been focused on developing categories of negotiation strategies that parallel in-
trapersonal, interpersonal and structural constraints. In a qualitative study Daniels, Drogin
Rodgers, and Wiggins (2005) suggested there is not necessarily a clear matchup. A struc-
tural constraint was found frequently to be negotiated through interpersonal or intrapersonal
means.
One limitation of my study is the use of participation as a dependent variable in the
analysis, which has been critiqued by both supporters and critics of constraints research.
This limitation is partially mitigated because multiple measures of participation were used
including a measure tapping not only frequency of participation but also diversity of partic-
ipation. Regardless, future researchers might consider alternative dependent variables that
tap affective and cognitive dimensions of leisure in addition to the behavioral. A second lim-
itation was the cross sectional survey design. Although this approach is useful for analyzing
relationships among variables in the completed sample, sample statistics could be produced
that are poor estimates of the true population parameters. Finally, the under-representation
of Hispanic residents was a limitation.
This study makes several significant contributions to the literature. First, by using hier-
archical confirmatory factor analysis and structural equation modeling this study presented
an empirical test of the constraints negotiation that examined the relations among motivation,
constraints, negotiation, negotiation-efficacy and leisure activity participation. Second, the
study assessed the constraints negotiation process in a statewide random sample of Arizona
residents, which gave additional weight to the generalizability of the processes identified
here. Finally, the study supports the utility of social cognitive theory and the negotiation-
efficacy construct for explaining negotiation. Future research will determine how universal
the processes identified in this study are in other contexts and with other populations.

Acknowledgements
This research was supported by grant ISA 03-220 from Arizona State Parks. The author
thanks Elizabeth Krug from Arizona State Parks and Randy J. Virden, Amy C. Racki and
Rebecca M. B. White from Arizona State University for their assistance.

References
Alexandris, K., Barkoukis, V., Tsorbatzoudis, H., & Grouios, G. (2003). A study of perceived con-
straints on a community-based physical activity program for the elderly in Greece. Journal of
Aging and Physical Activity, 11(3), 305–318.
358 D. D. White

Alexandris, K., Tsorbatzoudis, C., & Grouios, G. (2002). Perceived constraints on recreational sport
participation: Investigating their relationship with intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation and
amotivation. Journal of Leisure Research, 34(3), 233–252.
American Association for Public Opinion Research. (2006). Standard definitions: Final dispositions
of case codes and outcome rates for surveys. Lenexa, KS: American Association for Public
Opinion Research.
Arbuckle, J. L. (2005). AMOS 6.0 user’s guide. Chicago: Amos Development Corporation.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Bandura, A. (1994). Self-efficacy. In V. S. Ramachaudran (Ed.), Encyclopedia of human behavior
(Vol. 4, pp. 71–81). New York: Academic Press.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W. H. Freeman.
Bloomquist, C. D., Gyurcsik, N. C., Baillargeon, T., & McElroy, M. (2006). Strategies used to cope
with barriers to physical activity and related coping self-efficacy among first-year university
women. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 28, S35-S36.
Buchanan, D. R., & Allen, L. (1988). Barriers to recreation participation in later life cycle stages.
Therapeutic Recreation Journal, 19, 39–50.
Carroll, B., & Alexandris, K. (1997). Perception of constraints and strength of motivation: Their
relationship to recreational sport participation in Greece. Journal of Leisure Research, 29(3),
279–299.
Crawford, D. W., & Godbey, G. (1987). Reconceptualizing barriers to family leisure. Leisure Sciences,
9, 119–127.
Crawford, D. W., Jackson, E. L., & Godbey, G. (1991). A hierarchical model of leisure constraints.
Leisure Sciences, 13(4), 309–320.
Daniels, M. J., Drogin Rodgers, E. B., & Wiggins, B. P. (2005). “Travel Tales”: An interpretive
analysis of constraints and negotiations to pleasure travel as experienced by persons with physical
disabilities. Tourism Management, 26(6), 919.
Dillman, D. A. (2000). Mail and Internet surveys: The tailored design method (2nd ed.). New York:
J. Wiley.
Driver, B. L., Tinsley, H. E. A., & Manfredo, M. (1991). The paragraphs about leisure and recreation
experience preference scales: Results from two inventories designed to assess the breadth of the
perceived psychological benefits of leisure. In B. L. Driver, P. J. Brown & G. L. Peterson (Eds.),
Benefits of leisure (pp. 163–186). State College, PA: Venture Publishing, Inc.
Henderson, K. A., Bedini, L. A., Hecht, L., & Schuler, R. (1995). Women with physical disabilities
and the negotiation of leisure constraints. Leisure Studies, 14, 17–31.
Hubbard, J., & Mannell, R. C. (2001). Testing competing models of the leisure constraint negotiation
process in a corporate employee recreation setting. Leisure Sciences, 23(3), 145–163.
Jackson, E. L. (1988). Leisure constraints: A survey of past research. Leisure Sciences, 10, 203–215.
Jackson, E. L. (2005a). Leisure constraints research: Overview of a developing theme in leisure
studies. In E. L. Jackson (Ed.), Constraints to Leisure (pp. 3–19). State College, PA: Venture
Pub., Inc.
Jackson, E. L. (Ed.). (2005b). Constraints to leisure. State College, PA: Venture Pub., Inc.
Jackson, E. L., Crawford, D. W., & Godbey, G. (1993). Negotiation of leisure constraints. Leisure
Sciences, 15(1), 1–11.
Jackson, E. L., & Rucks, V. C. (1995). Negotiation of leisure constraints by junior-high and high-school
students: An exploratory study. Journal of Leisure Research, 27(1), 85–105.
Jackson, E. L., & Scott, D. (1999). Constraints to leisure. In E. L. Jackson & T. L. Burton (Eds.), Leisure
studies: Prospects for the 21st century (pp. 299–321). State College, PA: Venture Publishing.
Kay, T., & Jackson, G. (1991). Leisure despite constraint: The impact of leisure constraints on leisure
participation. Journal of Leisure Research, 23(4), 301–313.
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York:
Guilford Press.
Lee, J. H., & Scott, D. (2006). For better or worse? A structural model of the benefits and costs
associated with recreational specialization. Leisure Sciences, 28, 17–38.
Constraints Negotiation in Outdoor Recreation 359

Loucks-Atkinson, A., & Mannell, R. C. (2007). Role of self-efficacy in the constraints negotiation
process: The case of individuals with fibromyalgia syndrome. Leisure Sciences, 29, 19–36.
Manfredo, M. J., Driver, B. L., & Tarrant, M. A. (1996). Measuring leisure motivation: A meta-analysis
of the recreation experience preference scales. Journal of Leisure Research, 28(3), 188–213.
Marquez, D. X., & McAuley, E. (2006). Social cognitive correlates of leisure time physical activity
among Latinos. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 29(3), 281–289.
McAuley, E. (1992). The role of efficacy cognitions in the prediction of exercise behavior in middle-
aged adults. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 15(1), 65–88.
McAuley, E. (1993). Self-efficacy and the maintenance of exercise participation in older adults.
Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 16, 103–113.
Moore, R. L., & Driver, B. L. (2005). Introduction to outdoor recreation: Providing and managing
natural resource based opportunities. State College, PA: Venture Publishing.
Motl, R. W., Dishman, R. K., Trost, S. G., Saunders, R. P., Dowda, M., & Felton, G. (2000). Factorial
validity and invariance of questionnaires measuring social-cognitive determinants of physical
activity in adolescent girls. Preventive Medicine, 31, 257–265.
Motl, R. W., Dishman, R. K., Ward, D. S., Saunders, R. P., Dowda, M., & Felton, G. (2005). Com-
parison of barriers self-efficacy and perceived behavioral control for explaining physical activity
across 1 year among adolescent girls. Health Psychology, 24(1), 106–111.
Raymore, L. A., Godbey, G., Crawford, D., & von Eye, A. (1993). Nature and process of leisure
constraints – An empirical test. Leisure Sciences, 15(2), 99–113.
Scott, D. (1991). The problematic nature of participation in contract bridge – A qualitative study of
group-related constraints. Leisure Sciences, 13(4), 321–336.
Searle, M. S., & Jackson, E. L. (1985a). Recreation non-participation and barriers to participation:
Considerations for management of recreation delivery systems. Journal of Park and Recreation
Administration, 3, 23–36.
Searle, M. S., & Jackson, E. L. (1985b). Socioeconomic variations in perceived barriers to recreation
participation among would-be participants. Leisure Sciences, 7, 227–249.
Sobel, M. E. (1986). Some new results on indirect effects and their standard errors in structure equation
models. In N. Tuma (Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp. 156–186). Washington, DC: American
Sociological Association.
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2000). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn
and Bacon.
Walker, G. J., & Virden, R. J. (2005). Constraints on outdoor recreation. In E. L. Jackson (Ed.),
Constraints to leisure (pp. 201–219). State College, PA: Venture Publishing, Inc.
Winters, E. R., Petosa, R. L., & Charlton, T. E. (2003). Using social cognitive theory to explain
discretionary, “leisure-time” physical exercise among high school students. Journal of Adolescent
Health, 32(6), 436–442.

You might also like