Society For Research in Child Development, Wiley Child Development

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

The Plasticity of Intellectual Development: Insights from Preventive Intervention

Author(s): Craig T. Ramey, Keith Owen Yeates and Elizabeth J. Short


Source: Child Development, Vol. 55, No. 5 (Oct., 1984), pp. 1913-1925
Published by: Wiley on behalf of the Society for Research in Child Development
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1129938
Accessed: 24-06-2016 23:54 UTC

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
http://about.jstor.org/terms

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted
digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about
JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Society for Research in Child Development, Wiley are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize,
preserve and extend access to Child Development

This content downloaded from 159.178.22.27 on Fri, 24 Jun 2016 23:54:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
The Plasticity of Intellectual Development:
Insights from Preventive Intervention

Craig T. Ramey, Keith Owen Yeates,


and Elizabeth J. Short
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

RAMEY, CRAIG T.; YEATEs, KEITH OWEN; and SHORT, ELIZABETH J. The Plasticity of Intellectual
Development: Insights from Preventive Intervention. CHILD DEVELOPMENT, 1984, 55, 1913-1925.
Debates regarding the plasticity of intelligence are often fired by a confusion between 2 distinct
realms of development, that is, between developmental functions (e.g., a group's average IQ over
time) and individual differences (e.g., the relative rank ordering of individual IQs within a group).
Questions concerning the stability of these 2 realms are statistically independent. Thus there are 2
kinds of intellectual plasticity, and there may be no developmental convergences between them. In
the present study, data from an early intervention program were used to investigate the 2 kinds of
plasticity separately and to examine certain possible convergences between them. The program
involved children at risk for developmental retardation who were randomly assigned at birth to 2
rearing conditions (i.e., educational daycare vs. no educational intervention) and whose intellectual
development was then studied longitudinally to 4 years of age. Our findings indicate that develop-
mental functions are moderately alterable through systemic early education, particularly after in-
fancy, whereas individual differences are moderately stable, again particularly after infancy. They
also indicate that the 2 kinds of plasticity are independent; the alteration of developmental functions
through daycare affects neither the stability nor the determinants of individual differences. We
discuss the implications that these findings have for current models of mental development, for the
nature-nurture debate, and for arguments concerning the efficacy of early intervention programs.

Debates regarding the plasticity of intel- The experimentalists frequently stress intel-
lectual development have often generated lectual change, because findings from both ex-
sparks among their participants. Those sparks, perimental and natural interventions suggest
moreover, have occasionally erupted into that average levels of intellectual functioning
flames, as when Jensen (1969) published his can be altered significantly (see Clarke &
monograph concerning the boosting of IQ. Clarke, 1976; Lazar, Darlington, Murray,
Flames, unfortunately, sometimes generate Royce, & Snipper, 1982; Ramey, Bryant, &
smoke. Thus, as McCall (1981) noted, our per- Suarez, in press; Ramey, Sparling, Bryant, &
spective on the malleability of intellectual de- Wasik, 1982). The correlationalists frequently
velopment has been obscured unnecessarily. stress intellectual consistency, because a con-
Specifically, we have fallen victim to the divi- siderable literature indicates that individual
sion between Cronbach's (1957) two disci- differences become remarkably consistent
plines of scientific psychology--"experi- during development (see Bayley, 1970;
mentalists," on the one hand, with their Bloom, 1964; McCall, 1979). The problem is
manipulations of variables and comparisons of that the positions of the two disciplines are
group means, and "correlationalists," on the apparently contradictory, yet the contradiction
other, with their observations of individual is just that-apparent, not real. As McCall
differences and descriptive correlations. (1981) and Ramey and Haskins (1981) have
pointed out, the two disciplines actually pro-
Both disciplines generate convincing po- vide independent and complementary per-
sitions regarding intellectual malleability. spectives on intellectual development.

We would like to note that the authors made equal contributions to this paper; the order of
authorship reflects the overall contributions of the authors to the ongoing research project that
supplied the data reported here. The research has been supported by grants from the National
Institute of Child Health and Human Development, the Spencer Foundation, the Carnegie Founda-
tion, the Office of Special Education, U.S. Department of Education, the Department of Human
Resources of the State of North Carolina, and the Department of Social Services of Orange County,
North Carolina. We would like to thank four anonymous reviewers for their comments on an earlier
version of the paper and Marie Butts and Pam Mann for their help in preparing the manuscript.
Requests for reprints should be sent to Craig T. Ramey, Frank Porter Graham Child Development
Center, University of North Carolina, Highway 54 Bypass West, Chapel Hill, North Carolina 27514.
[Child Development, 1984, 55, 1913-1925. @ 1984 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.
All rights reserved. 0009-3920/84/5505-0001$01.00]

This content downloaded from 159.178.22.27 on Fri, 24 Jun 2016 23:54:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1914 Child Development

To illustrate the independence of the two parent disagreements, though, they will con-
disciplines, consider first a figure with a line tinue to argue the presence rather than the
that plots, for a sample of children, the aver- timing, limits, or determinants of intellectual
age values of mental age over chronological plasticity.
age. This line would serve to describe the de-
The second shortcoming common to both
velopmental function of mental age for those
disciplines is that they continue the study of
children (see Wohlwill, 1973). Consider also a
intellectual plasticity separately. Although
second figure with lines that plot, for the same
the two kinds of plasticity are potentially inde-
sample, each child's separate values for men-
pendent and therefore can be studied sepa-
tal age over chronological age. These lines
rately, we should remain aware of potential
would serve to illustrate the stability of the
empirical convergences between them. For
children's relative rank ordering on mental
instance, we might ask whether alterations
age over time-that is, the consistency of their
in developmental functions of intelligence
individual differences. also produce changes either in the stability
Now suppose that the children had been of individual differences or in the relative im-
randomly assigned at birth to two rearing portance of the determinants of individual dif-
conditions, which presumably varied in their ferences. Several research designs are poten-
potential for enhancing intellectual develop- tially able to answer such questions (see, e.g.,
ment. We might ask whether the develop- McCall, Eichorn, & Hogarty, 1977; Scarr &
mental functions of the two groups differ Weinberg, 1983); early intervention programs,
across time. We also might ask whether the for example, can be particularly appropriate.
individual differences within the groups are Such programs are designed to produce aver-
stable across time. The first question is experi- age group gains in IQ, or in other words to
mental; the second is correlational. Experi- alter the developmental function; however,
mental research, then, concerns the malleabil- they also may simultaneously alter the stabil-
ity of the developmental function, whereas ity and determinants of individual differences
correlational research concerns the malleabil- in IQ.
ity of individual differences. Developmental One such program, for instance, provided
functions and individual differences are statis- educational daycare for a sample of disadvan-
tically independent, though, because the taged children. As was hoped, their average
means of two sets of scores are unrelated to IQ was higher than expected given their en-
the correlation between the two sets. Thus, vironmental background. In addition, though,
experimentalists and correlationalists study the children's individual differences in IQ
separate aspects of mental development, and were more stable than expected given the pre-
their positions regarding intellectual plasticity vious intelligence test literature (Ramey,
are distinct because they actually concern dif- Campbell, & Nicholson, 1973). A subsequent
ferent kinds of plasticity. program experimentally compared educa-
tional daycare with no intervention for a ran-
The failure to fully recognize this distinc-
domly assigned sample of disadvantaged chil-
tion is largely responsible for the obscurity in
dren. As was intended, the average IQ of the
debates about the plasticity of intellectual de-
experimental group was significantly higher
velopment, because as its consequence both than that of the control group. Additionally,
disciplines suffer from two critical shortcom-
though, the correlation between child IQ and
ings. The first is that both disciplines tend to
maternal IQ was not significant (r = -.04) in
stress either the presence or absence of plas-
the experimental group but was significant
ticity, without attending to developmental
(r = .43) in the control group (Ramey & Has-
timing and the constraints it places on intel-
kins, 1981). Thus, results from early interven-
lectual plasticity. Development, though, im-
tion programs illustrate at least some of the
plies change (McCall, 1977; Wohlwill, 1973).
convergences that might occur between the
Within the context of debates regarding the
two kinds of plasticity. Of course, these as
plasticity of intellectual development, there- well as other kinds of convergences should
fore, the issues should not be whether change
continue to be investigated; given the divi-
occurs, but instead when it occurs, what its sion between the experimental and correla-
limits are, and how it is determined-again,
tional disciplines, though, they rarely are.
with reference to which kind of plasticity is
under discussion. In other words, a more pro- In summary, we risk considerable misun-
ductive debate might concern the balance derstanding by studying changes in different
struck by and the determinants of change and aspects of intellectual development and their
stability during ontogenesis. As long as the hypothesized determinants separately and by
two disciplines remain distracted by their ap- ignoring the developmental timing of those

This content downloaded from 159.178.22.27 on Fri, 24 Jun 2016 23:54:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Ramey, Yeates, and Short 1915

changes. The present study attempted to in- vention program designed to prevent the oc-
vestigate the plasticity of intellectual develop- currence of developmental retardation and
ment, but to avoid these sources of misunder- known as the Carolina Abecedarian Project
standing. To achieve this goal, we relied on (see Ramey, MacPhee, & Yeates, 1982, for a
data collected during an ongoing early inter- detailed description). Families were referred
vention program. The program involved chil- to the program through local hospitals and
dren at risk for developmental retardation, clinics, the county Department of Social Ser-
who were randomly assigned at birth to two vices, and other community agencies. Once
rearing conditions (i.e., systematic educa- referred, preliminary interviews were con-
tional daycare vs. no educational intervention) ducted to determine whether the families ap-
and then studied longitudinally to 4 years of peared to meet the selection criteria de-
age. The data drawn from the program in- scribed below. If so, mothers were invited to
cluded measures of maternal IQ, which was participate in further assessments.
assessed before the children's births, and both
During these assessments, which typi-
child IQ and the quality of the home environ-
cally occurred during the last trimester of
ment, which were assessed at regular inter-
pregnancy, mothers provided demographic
vals during the children's first 4 years of life. information about themselves and their
Using these data, we asked a series of four
families. In addition, their intelligence was as-
specific questions regarding malleability
sessed using the Wechsler Adult Intelligence
within the two realms of intellectual develop-
Scale (WAIS; Wechsler, 1955). Final determi-
ment.
nation of eligibility followed these proce-
Our first two questions concerned the dures. Criteria for selection included 13 fac-
plasticity of developmental functions and in- tors, such as maternal IQ, maternal education,
dividual differences considered separately. and earned family income, that were
That is, we asked, first, if and when the mean weighted and combined to yield a score on a
IQ performances of the experimental and con- High-Risk Index (see Ramey & Smith, 1977,
trol group children differ, and, second, if and for details). Only families at or above a prede-
when the children's individual differences in termined cutoff score were considered eligi-
IQ are stable. Our latter two questions con- ble.
cerned possible convergences between the Four cohorts of families were admitted
two kinds of plasticity. That is, given mean IQ between 1972 and 1977. Of 122 families
performance differences between groups, we
judged to be eligible and invited to join the
asked if systematic early education might af-
program, 121 accepted the condition of ran-
fect either, third, the patterns of stability of
dom assignment to the experimental or con-
individual differences within groups or,
trol groups. One of the pregnant mothers mis-
fourth, the configurations of maternal IQ and
home environment as determinants of such
carried, thereby reducing the sample to 120
families. Of these 120 families, 112 (93%) ac-
differences within groups. Taken together,
cepted their actual group assignment. Since
these questions allow the experimental and
assignment, four children have died, two have
correlational disciplines to make separate as
been diagnosed as retarded due to organic
well as combined contributions, which we
etiology, and two have been reassigned from
hope will thereby yield a clearer perspective
the control group to the experimental group
on the plasticity of intellectual development
because of life-threatening medical conditions
during infancy and early childhood.'
judged by physicians to be more optimally
treated at the daycare center than at home;
Method
none of these families is included in this re-
Subjects port. The base sample, then, consists of 104
The families participating in this study families that produced 106 children (one pair
were drawn from an experimental early inter- of siblings and one pair of twins), of which 54

1 We should note that Ramey and Haskins (1981) used data from the same program reported
here to address questions somewhat similar to our own. Our research, however, includes all four
cohorts of children who have been involved in the intervention program, whereas Ramey and
Haskins (1981) included only the first two cohorts. Our research also involves between-group com-
parisons of within-group stabilities and determinants of individual differences; Ramey and Haskins
(1981) reported only within-group comparison of those correlations. Perhaps most important, the
distinction drawn here between developmental functions and individual differences was not drawn
explicitly by Ramey and Haskins (1981). Thus our report reflects a considerable methodological and
theoretical extension.

This content downloaded from 159.178.22.27 on Fri, 24 Jun 2016 23:54:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1916 Child Development

are in the experimental group and 52 are in Children began attending the program as
the control group. Of these 106, eight (N = 4 young as 6 weeks of age, and attendance be-
in each group) have dropped out of the pro- gan by age 3 months for 98% of the children.
gram, resulting in an attrition rate of approxi- The daycare center operated from 7:45 A.M. to
mately 7%. 5:30 P.M. each weekday for 50 weeks per year.
Transportation to and from the center was pro-
For the purposes of this study, we also vided for the children. The settings for the
eliminated all families where the child did not
children were structured so as to be age and
live with the mother through the first 4 years developmentally appropriate. Generally, chil-
of life (N = 4 for the experimental group; N = dren were grouped according to age, with sec-
3 for the control group) or where complete tions for infants, toddlers, and so on. Twelve
data were unavailable during that period (N = teachers and assistants, aided by three admin-
4 for the experimental group; N = 1 for the istrative staff members, were responsible for
control group). Thus, the final sample consists providing the educational program for the
of 41 experimental group families, with 23 children. The typical teacher/child ratio var-
male and 19 female children, and 44 control ied from 1:3 for infants to 1:6 for 4-year-olds.
group families, with 19 male and 25 female
children. Table 1 presents a summary of fam- The educational program itself was de-
ily characteristics for each group during the signed to foster intellectual development,
year of the children's births. In composite, this with particular emphasis on language devel-
sample represents a portion of the general opment. The approach to language develop-
population whose socioeconomic and intel- ment relied heavily on the frameworks devel-
lectual disadvantages have been demon- oped by Blank (1973) and Tough (1976). The
strated to place children at risk for later intel- aim was to promote a particular kind and
lectual retardation and academic failure (see, amount of verbal interaction between teacher
e.g., Heber, Dever, & Conry, 1968; Ramey & and daycare pupil. The kind of verbal interac-
tion was modeled on what a middle-class
Finkelstein, 1981; Ramey, Stedman, Borders-
Patterson, & Mengel, 1978). mother establishes with her child; the amount
was higher, perhaps more like what a tutorial
Intervention Program hour might provide. Because our program was
The program to which these families competing with extensive experience in an-
were admitted consisted of two components. other kind of linguistic environment, namely,
First, all families were provided with nutri- the children's homes, we assumed that it
tional supplements and health care. These could not be as casual and diluted as the nor-
services were included in order to reduce po- mal family interaction. To foster certain types
tential Hawthorne effects in the experimental of linguistic functioning in the children's rep-
group and to insure the delivery of a set of ertoires, then, we were trying to provide a
services already guaranteed, in principle, to large number of practice opportunities. (For a
all members of our society. Second, the ex- more detailed treatment of our curricular ap-
perimental group children received direct proach to language development, see Ramey,
educational programming through the provi- McGinness, Cross, Collier, & Barrie-Blackley,
sion of systematic, developmental daycare. 1982.)

TABLE 1

FAMILY CHARACTERISTICS IN THE YEAR OF THE CHILDREN'S BIRTHS

GROUP

Experimental Control
CHARACTERISTIC (N = 41) (N = 44)

Maternal IQ (M/SD) ................ 85.88/11.96 84.11/11.52


Maternal education (M/SD)a.......... 10.52/1.73 10.14/1.82
Maternal age (M/SD) .............. 19.57/4.11 20.57/6.05
Unmarried mother (%) ............. 76 71
Father absent (%) ................... 76 75
Firstborn child (%) .............. 67 64
No earned income (%) .............. 48 59
Black (%) .............. ........ 95 100
a Total years completed.

This content downloaded from 159.178.22.27 on Fri, 24 Jun 2016 23:54:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Ramey, Yeates, and Short 1917

The provision of these practice opportu- surement of the Environment (HOME) inven-
nities, finally, took place primarily within the tory (Caldwell, Heider, & Kaplan, Note 1) at 6,
context of social interactions. The nature of 18, 30, and 42 months of age. The HOME in-
these social interactions was dictated by the ventory consists of 45 items representing six
standard curricula used in the program, which categories of environmental stimulation at 6,
can be divided into two segments. The first 18, and 30 months of age, and of 80 items rep-
was formally applied up to the children's third resenting seven categories of environmental
birthdays. It consisted of the Carolina Infant stimulation at 42 months of age. Scoring of the
Curriculum and the Task Orientation Cur- items requires a combination of direct obser-
riculum, both developed by Sparling and vation and interviewing, with approximately
Lewis (1979). These curricula consist of over two-thirds of the items scored from direct ob-
300 items in language, motor, social, and cog- servation of the mother-child dyad. Both cate-
nitive areas (see Ramey, McGinness, et al., gory and total scores are obtained; for the pur-
1982, for a more complete description). The poses of this report, only the total scores were
goal of these curricula was to enrich early de- used. All HOME data for this study were col-
velopment in these four realms and to prepare lected by three female interviewers, again
the children for the more structured educa- more or less at random, in the families' own
tional curricula that composed the second seg- homes when the primary caregiver and child
ment of the program. were both present. Interobserver agreement
across items in both this sample (see Ramey,
The second segment, which began after
Mills, Campbell, & O'Brien, 1975) and other
the children's third birthdays and lasted until
samples (e.g., Elardo, Bradley, & Caldwell,
school entry, was designed to provide chil- 1975) has consistently exceeded 90%.
dren with a systematic exposure to areas such
as science, math, and music. The formal cur- Table 2 presents the means, standard de-
ricula, which teachers could draw on as viations, and correlations for the raw total
needed for particular children, included the HOME scores for both groups. The means for
GOAL math program (Karnes, 1978), the Pea- both groups, as might be expected, are lower
body Early Experience Kit (Dunn, Chun, than those typically found in middle-class
Crowell, Dunn, Avery, & Yachel, 1976), the homes (see Ramey et al., 1975), although the
Bridges to Reading program (Greenberg & two groups do not differ significantly at any
Epstein, 1973), and the Wallach and Wallach occasion. The stability correlations are compa-
(1976) reading program. In addition, adaptive rable to those found in other studies (e.g.,
social behavior was encouraged through the Bradley, Caldwell, & Elardo, 1979), with me-
use of My Friends and Me program (Davis, dian 1-year stabilities of .48 in the experimen-
1977). In summary, the educational program tal group and .62 in the control group. The
was designed to test the hypothesis that de- HOME inventory, then, seems to provide a
clines in tested intelligence in a high-risk reliable and stable measure that differentiates
sample of children could be prevented by pro- our high-risk families from more advantaged
viding an early education program during the families.
preschool years.
We wanted, however, to have summary
Procedure indexes of home environments for each child
Intellectual assessments.-As mentioned
IQ assessment. Because of scaling differences
previously, mothers were given a WAIS at the in the HOME inventory across the assessment
time they were interviewed for the program. occasions, the scores at each assessment were
Children were tested with the Bayley Scales
first standardized (M = 0, SD = 1). A total
of Infant Development (Bayley, 1969) at 6, 12, HOME score was then computed for each
and 18 months of age (the Mental Develop-
family at each outcome age, consisting of the
ment Index, or MDI, is reported here) and sum of all standardized HOME total scores
with the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale
temporally prior to the respective 12-, 24-, 36-,
(Terman & Merrill, 1973) at 24, 36, and 48 and 48-month child IQ assessments. This pro-
months of age. The tests were administered cedure is justified, we feel, because of the
by 11 experienced female examiners whose high stability of HOME total scores from year
assignment to children was performed more to year (see Table 3) and because the effects of
or less at random, depending on who was home environments appear to be cumulative
available at a given time. The child's primary (see Yeates, MacPhee, Campbell, & Ramey,
caregiver was present during all assessments. 1983). These standardized and summed
Home environment assessments.-The scores, then, represent indexes of the environ-
home environments of the families were as- mental stimulation available in children's
sessed using the Home Observation for Mea- homes prior to particular intellectual assess-

This content downloaded from 159.178.22.27 on Fri, 24 Jun 2016 23:54:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1918 Child Development
TABLE 2

EXPERIMENTAL AND CONTROL GROUP MEANS, STANDARD DEVIATIONS, AND


STABILITY CORRELATIONS OF RAW TOTAL SCORES ON THE HOME INVENTORY

Variable (Months) 1 2 3 4 M SD

1. HOME 6 ...... ... .41 .08 .27 28.14 4.78


2. HOME 18 ..... .43 ... .48 .57 30.10 5.33
3. HOME 30 ..... .39 .62 ... .53 30.29 5.93
4. HOME 42 ..... .24 .56 .68 ... 55.50 8.55
M ................ 27.36 28.66 30.45 55.14
SD .............. 5.04 5.99 5.62 9.60

NOTE.--Experimental group is above diagonal, with N = 42; control group is below


diagonal, with N = 44. All boldface correlations are significant, p < .05. HOME = Home
Observation for Measurement of the Environment inventory.

TABLE 3

EXPERIMENTAL AND CONTROL GROUP MEAN IQ PERFORMANCES

GROUP

Experimental Control
(N = 42) (N = 44)
OCCASION
(Months) M SD M SD F (1,84)
6 ........... 106.26 16.68 101.36 15.76 1.96
12 .......... 112.14 15.92 105.73 15.33 3.62
18 .......... 108.64 14.55 89.68 12.90 40.99*
24 .......... 96.60 11.40 84.98 9.26 27.02*
36 .......... 101.67 13.88 84.41 13.77 33.49*
48 .......... 102.71 11.93 89.00 13.48 24.86*

NOTE.-At 6, 12, and 18 months, Bayley MDIs are presented; at 24, 36, and 48
months, Stanford-Binet IQs are presented.
*p < .001.

ments; hereafter, they are referred to as total 538.21, p < .001, because of an overall de-
home indexes. crease in IQ at 24 months followed by a slow
increase. The analysis also reveals a main ef-
Results
fect for group, F(1,84) = 28.87, p < .001,
Developmental Function Plasticity which favors the experimental group children.
As mentioned earlier, our first concern Both of these main effects, though, are
was the plasticity of the developmental func- qualified by a significant age x group interac-
tion for intelligence, which would be demon- tion, F(5,80) = 4.92, p < .001. The interaction
strated if significant IQ differences were arises because the average IQ difference be-
found between experimental and control tween groups becomes larger during develop-
group children. Such differences would imply ment; tests of the simple effect of group at
that the rate of cognitive development is re- each age show that the group differences are
sponsive to environmental manipulation- not significant at 6 and 12 months of age but
that is, that the shape of developmental func- are significant and favor the experimental
tions is alterable through early education. To group children at 18, 24, 36, and 48 months
investigate this possibility, the IQ scores of (see Table 3). These findings offer support for
the children were subjected to a 2 (group) x 6 the notion that developmental functions for
(age) repeated-measures analysis of variance, intellectual development are at least moder-
using a multivariate approach to repeated mea- ately alterable through systematic early edu-
sures (McCall & Appelbaum, 1973). cation, particularly after the first year of life.

The analysis first reveals a main effect for Individual Difference Plasticity
age of assessment, F (5,80) = 200.28, p < .001. Our second concern was the plasticity
This effect is primarily quadratic, F(1,84) = of individual differences in IQ; we asked

This content downloaded from 159.178.22.27 on Fri, 24 Jun 2016 23:54:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Ramey, Yeates, and Short 1919
whether individual differences would demon- so in one group than in the other; this was our
strate stability during development for either third concern. We therefore conducted a com-
or both groups. Both groups do evidence sig- parison of the two groups' correlation matri-
nificant stability from 6 to 48 months (see ces, using Jennrich's (1970) asymptotic X2 test.
Table 4). Of the 15 stability correlations com- This test was not significant, X2(15) = 15.72, p
puted for each group, only one would be ex- > .05. The stability of individual differences
pected to be significant by chance alone. in IQ and the developmental trend of that sta-
Nonetheless, 14 are significant in the experi- bility, then, do not appear responsive to ma-
mental group, and 11 are significant in the nipulations of the developmental function.
control group.
Determinant Configuration Plasticity
We should note, though, that both groups
Our final concern was whether those vari-
also evidence a significant increase in the sta-
ables that presumably help to determine indi-
bility of individual differences from 6 to 48
vidual differences in children's intelligence
months.2 Using 12-month intervals to illus- would show differential contributions as the
trate this developmental trend, we find that in
result of alterations in the developmental
the experimental group the correlations be-
function via educational daycare. This con-
tween 6- and 18-month MDIs (r = .52) and
12-month MDIs and 24-month Stanford-Binet cern was addressed by conducting separate
hierarchical regression analyses to predict the
IQs (r = .45) are both significantly lower than
the correlation between 36- and 48-month children's 12-, 24-, 36-, and 48-month intelli-
gence test scores. Two sets of predictors were
Stanford-Binet IQs (r = .75; both z's > 1.82,
entered into the regression equations. One set
both p's < .05). Similarly, for the control
consisted of treatment group membership,
group, the correlations between 6- and 18-
maternal IQ, and the appropriate total home
month MDIs (r = .27) and 12-month MDIs
index (with treatment-group membership
and 24-month Stanford-Binet IQs (r = .44) are coded as a dichotomous classification vari-
both significantly lower than the correlations
able). This set was entered into the equations
between 24- and 36-month Stanford-Binet IQs first. The other set consisted of two cross-
(r = .70, both z's > 1.94, both p's < .05) and
product terms, which were created by multi-
36- and 48-month Stanford-Binet IQs (r = .78;
plying group membership by both maternal
both z's > 2.57, both p's < .01). Thus, both
IQ and the appropriate total home indexes.
groups show not only stable individual differ-
These cross-product terms were entered into
ences in IQ but also similar tendencies for
those individual differences to become in- the equations second. If they were found to be
significant predictors of children's intelli-
creasingly predictable during development.
gence after the variance explained by the first
Given their different developmental set of predictors had been removed, they
functions, though, the two groups might also would indicate differential contributions of
differ in their patterns of stability of individual maternal IQ and the home environment to
differences. That is, although stable in both children's intelligence in the experimental
groups, individual differences might be more and control groups (Cohen & Cohen, 1975).

TABLE 4

EXPERIMENTAL AND CONTROL GROUP IQ STABILITY CORRELATIONS

Variable (Months) 1 2 3 4 5 6

1. MDI 6 .......... ... .70 .52 .49 .35 .25


2. MDI 12 ......... .53 ... .61 .45 .45 .31
3. MDI 18 ......... .27 .51 ... .79 .60 .46
4. SB 24 .......... .49 .44 .55 ... .67 .65
5. SB 36 .......... .28 .28 .59 .70 ... .75
6. SB 48 ...........33 .21 .52 .66 .78

NOTE.-Experimental group correlations are above diagonal, with N = 42; control


group correlations are below diagonal, with N = 44. All boldface correlations are signifi-
cant, p < .05. MDI = Bayley Mental Development Index; SB = Stanford-Binet IQ.

2 Because the tests reported here involve comparisons of dependent correlations, the Fisher z
test for comparing independent correlations is inappropriate. The appropriate tests, which were used
for these analyses, include the Hotelling-Williams z test and the asymptotic-variance z test (Darling-
ton, 1975).

This content downloaded from 159.178.22.27 on Fri, 24 Jun 2016 23:54:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1920 Child Development

Table 5 presents the standardized regres- tervention project, therefore, we asked a


sion weights for the first and second set of series of questions regarding the plasticity of
predictors when they are entered in the order the two realms of intellectual development.
described above and tested for significance in Our first two questions concerned the plastic-
that order. As Table 5 illustrates, none of the ity of developmental functions and individual
cross-product terms add significantly to the differences considered separately. Our results
prediction of child IQ at any of the four assess- indicate that developmental functions are
ment occasions. Therefore, the results indi- moderately plastic, particularly after infancy,
cate that the contributions of maternal IQ and whereas individual differences are relatively
the home environment to individual differ- stable, again particularly after infancy. Our lat-
ences in IQ are similar for the experimental ter two questions concerned possible conver-
and control group children. Collapsing across gences between the two kinds of plasticity.
groups, the contributions can be summarized Here our results indicate that the two kinds of
as follows: at 12 months, the two predictors plasticity are independent. The plasticity of
account for 6% of the variance in children's developmental functions affects neither the
intelligence, with the home environment stability nor the determinants of individual
playing a uniquely significant role; at 24 differences. Taken together, then, our find-
months, the two predictors account for 8% of ings illustrate the independence, both con-
the variance, but with maternal IQ playing a ceptually and empirically, of plasticity in the
uniquely significant role; at 36 months, the two realms of intellectual development.
two predictors account for 14% of the vari- The validity of these findings and their
ance, and both maternal IQ and the home en-
interpretation, of course, might be challenged.
vironment are significant predictors; finally, at
Consider the plasticity of developmental
48 months, the two predictors account for 26%
functions. Our argument that such functions
of the variance, and again both maternal IQ
are moderately and increasingly plastic dur-
and the home environment are significant pre-
ing early childhood is based on mean IQ per-
dictors. The overall pattern reflects a mono- formance differences between experimental
tonic increase in the predictability of child IQ,
and control group children. Our interpretation
which occurs within the context of shifts in
of such differences might be questioned on
the relative contributions of maternal IQ and
the grounds that the intervention program
the home environment (cf. Yeates et al., 1983).
"taught" the tests used for evaluation. The
Individual differences in IQ become less vari-
teaching staff or the educational curricula
able during development, then, and the con-
might have guided the program such that the
tributions of factors that help to determine the
mean IQ differences between groups repre-
magnitude of such differences do not appear sent the acquisition by experimental group
particularly alterable by educational daycare. children of specific bits of knowledge about
It should be especially noted from these re- test items rather than of more general cogni-
gression analyses that group assignment be- tive abilities.
comes a highly salient predictor of children's
IQ beginning in the second year of life, with Within our program, however, the teach-
standardized regression weights in Set 1 of ing staff never witnessed the administration of
.46, .50, and .43 at 24, 36, and 48 months of cognitive tests, and the curriculum-develop-
age, respectively. Thus, from 24 to 48 months ment team tried never to create curriculum
the most powerful predictor of the IQs of the items that were identical or very similar to test
children in this sample was whether or not items. More generally, coaching on IQ tests
they received educational intervention. When has been found to produce negligible gains in
the variables of group assignment, maternal performance on such tests (Jensen, 1980). We
IQ, and home environments are considered in would argue, therefore, that the mean IQ dif-
aggregate, we observe an orderly progression ferences between groups represent true gains
in percentage of IQ variance accounted from in mental development by children who re-
10% at 12 months to 50% at 48 months. ceived educational daycare rather than simply
an artifact of teaching to the tests.
Discussion
Such IQ differences between groups may
At the outset, we argued that the failure still be atypical, though, because they might
of experimentalists and correlationalists to be unlike those produced by other interven-
distinguish between developmental functions tion programs. Nonetheless, the mean IQ dif-
and individual differences has resulted in ferences reported here are consistent with
heated debates regarding the plasticity of in- those found in other programs with similarly
telligence. Using data from a longitudinal in- disadvantaged children (see Lazar et al., 1982;

This content downloaded from 159.178.22.27 on Fri, 24 Jun 2016 23:54:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
C11f
C.,

CC o,1 ooo,
cl c-i co
"- X
W CI)

Z00

C u a1 . b

QQ1"4

-. 3:D
HO m X"
Oc~

""n
0

CB
uo ,QJ.:C1d -c

-- " .o

4-00
CCO)

p OCO=
fa cd
0
z 0. b
W
(.1
C0.

"0rd

W-z
z

o u

N . . . . . * . .

This content downloaded from 159.178.22.27 on Fri, 24 Jun 2016 23:54:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1922 Child Development

Ramey, Bryant, et al., in press; Ramey, Spar- Moreover, the stability of the determinants of
ling, et al., 1982). More specifically, the mag- individual differences has also been repli-
nitudes of the IQ differences produced here cated; adoption studies find that the quality of
are somewhat larger than those produced by adoptive homes affects average IQ (i.e., devel-
less intense programs, but somewhat smaller opmental functions) but not the relative rank
than those produced by the most intense pro- ordering of IQ (i.e., individual differences;
gram, which is known as the Milwaukee Proj- Scarr & Weinberg, 1983). The increasing sta-
ect (Garber & Heber, 1981). We should ac- bility of individual differences demonstrated
knowledge that mean IQ differences have not here, therefore, is also probably neither an ar-
been produced with more advantaged chil- tifact of methodology nor an atypical finding.
dren, but perhaps only because experimental
intervention programs to date have not in- In summary, our findings appear valid.
cluded large numbers of such children-in What, then, are their broader implications?
other words, educational effects on such chil- First, they support a more general model of
dren remain an empirical issue. The plasticity mental development proposed by McCall
of developmental functions demonstrated (1981), which rests on the concept of canaliza-
here, therefore, is probably neither an artifact tion. According to Waddington (1957), canali-
of our methods nor an unusual instance of zation refers to a species-typical develop-
such plasticity. mental path followed because of strong
biological self-righting tendencies. McCall
Consider, then, the plasticity of individ- (1981) argued that "mental development is
ual differences. Our argument that individual highly canalized during the first 18-24
differences in intelligence are moderately and months of life but thereafter becomes less
increasingly stable is based on an examination canalized" (p. 5). As a consequence, McCall
of the IQ correlation matrices for both groups (1981) contended, the two aspects of mental
of children. Our interpretation of those matri- development would have certain characteris-
ces might be questioned on the grounds that tics. Because of the early canalization, devel-
our assessment schedule called for changes in opmental functions are less malleable during
tests, and therefore might have produced early infancy than somewhat later. Because of
spurious correlations. More specifically, the the early self-righting tendencies, individual
increasing stability of IQ within groups might differences during early infancy will be
reflect the shift from the Bayley MDI to the neither as stable nor as correlated with ge-
Stanford-Binet IQ rather than an actual netic variation or environmental variation as
change in the stability of individual differ- they will later. Our findings support McCall's
ences in intelligence. (1981) arguments. We found that develop-
Our correlation matrices, however, are mental functions were alterable only after 12
not affected in any obvious manner by the months, that individual differences became
shift from the Bayley MDI to the Stanford- increasingly stable, and that the contributions
Binet IQ; the correlations involving the Bay- of maternal IQ and home environment to
ley and the Stanford-Binet are often higher child IQ increased in both groups (which may
than those involving either the Bayley or the illustrate the increasing roles of genetic and
Stanford-Binet alone (see Table 4). Further- environmental variation, respectively, as de-
more, the reliabilities of both these measures terminants of individual differences; see
are quite high, exceeding .85 for all of the ages Yeates et al., 1983). The investigation of intel-
reported here (Bayley, 1969; Terman & Mer- lectual plasticity, then, can have important im-
rill, 1973), and do not differ in magnitude plications for understanding the process of in-
sufficiently to explain our results. Thus we tellectual development.
would argue that the increasing stability of IQ Investigations of plasticity also have im-
within groups represents increasingly stable plications for the never-ending nature-nurture
individual differences in intelligence rather debate, because that debate has been clouded
than the results of changing from a less to a
by the same confusion found in debates re-
more reliable test.
garding intellectual plasticity. Jensen (1969)
Again, though, such increasingly stable claimed that we should not expect to eradicate
individual differences might still be atypical. the average IQ performance differences typi-
The correlation matrices reported here, how- cally found between social-class and ethnic
ever, are patterned much like those in other groups; he based this claim partly on evidence
studies-both showing greater stability given showing that the heritability of IQ within
shorter intervals between tests and greater sta- groups is quite high. Average IQ differences
bility given later ages of initial testing (see between groups are unrelated to the heritabil-
Bayley, 1970; Bloom, 1964; McCall, 1979). ity of IQ within groups, however, because

This content downloaded from 159.178.22.27 on Fri, 24 Jun 2016 23:54:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Ramey, Yeates, and Short 1923

they are two different aspects of mental devel- larly malleable during infancy (Hunt, 1961),
opment. A heritability estimate describes the as illustrated by the low stability of individual
importance of genetic variation as a determi- differences in IQ at that time as compared to
nant of individual differences in IQ in a par- later periods of development. Intervention
ticular population, given its particular envi- programs, though, are evaluated primarily by
ronment; it does not concern the population's comparing group differences (i.e., develop-
developmental function. Two distinct popula- mental functions). The results of the present
tions can have the same large heritability, but, study show that the developmental function of
as a result of environmental differences, also the experimental group was altered only after
have different average IQs (i.e., different de- the first year of life by preventing a decline in
velopmental functions). In the present study, IQ relative to the control group. One way to
average IQ differences between groups coex- quantify this issue is to express the magnitude
isted with similar contributions of maternal of the intervention impact as an effect size.
IQ and home environment to individual dif- Effect sizes are usually expressed as the dif-
ferences in IQ within groups. Thus it is en- ference of an experimental group's mean
tirely possible, and probably likely, that both score minus a control group's mean score di-
groups have the same heritability of IQ, yet vided by the standard deviation of the control
significantly different average IQs. Investiga- group at a particular measurement occasion
tions of plasticity that distinguish between the (see Glass, 1976). Performing such computa-
two aspects of intellectual development, then, tions on the MDI and IQ data in Table 3 re-
can help show that nature-nurture arguments veals an average effect size of a nonsignificant
are often specious. .36 during the first year of life. From 18
months through 48 months, however, the av-
Such investigations can also help inform erage effect size is a highly significant 1.25
arguments concerning the efficacy of inter- standard deviation units favoring the group
vention programs designed to enhance intel- that received educational daycare. This
lectual development. The present study ad- finding, when considered in light of McCall's
dresses two important issues concerning such (1981) argument that early mental develop-
programs. The first is program generalizabil- ment is highly canalized, suggests that inter-
ity. The differential effectiveness of early in- vention programs for initially healthy children
tervention programs for different kinds of dis- might be more beneficial during early child-
advantaged children has been a continuing hood than during early infancy. Of course,
concern for social policy analysts (see Bron- early intervention might prepare children for
fenbrenner, 1974; Datta, 1979; Ramey, Yeates, the transition to less canalized intellectual de-
& MacPhee, 1984). Our regression analyses velopment in a way that later intervention
were conducted to test the effect of interven- might not, and this hypothesis is worthy of
tion on the contributions of maternal IQ and empirical test. Nonetheless, the apparent sta-
home environment as frequently hypoth- bility of the developmental function during
esized determinants of individual differences early infancy requires careful thought as fu-
in IQ. Nonetheless, they also can be concep- ture intervention programs are considered for
tualized as tests of the effect of variations in implementation.
such determinants on the efficacy of interven-
tion. In fact, the results of these analyses sug- Studies of the plasticity of intellectual de-
gest that our intervention program was velopment, therefore, can have important
equally effective across the range of maternal methodological, empirical, theoretical, and
IQs and home environments represented in political implications. The distinction be-
this disadvantaged sample. This finding is in tween developmental functions and individ-
accord with similar findings from the Consor-
ual differences must be clear in such studies,
tium for Longitudinal Studies (Lazar et al., though, if we are to avoid misunderstanding.
1982), and supports their suggestion that high- The present study provides an example of
such a distinction, but its results are only one
quality intervention programs "can be effec-
tive for different kinds of low-income chil- step in a continuing process. Further investi-
dren" (p. 65). gation of intellectual plasticity is clearly war-
ranted in different populations, across differ-
The second issue concerning interven- ent age ranges, and using different methods
tion programs that is addressed by the study of (see Appelbaum & McCall, 1983, for a de-
intellectual plasticity is developmental tim- scription of alternative methodologies). Such
ing. One of the most common arguments has investigation, of course, may not douse the
been that intervention should begin early in fires in debates regarding the plasticity of in-
infancy. The argument rests on the assump- tellectual development, but it may help to
tion that intellectual development is particu- clear the smoke.

This content downloaded from 159.178.22.27 on Fri, 24 Jun 2016 23:54:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1924 Child Development
Reference Note Garber, H., & Heber, R. The efficacy of early inter-
vention with family rehabilitation. In M. Be-
1. Caldwell, B., Heider, J., & Kaplan, B. The in-
gab, H. C. Haywood, & H. L. Garber (Eds.),
ventory of home stimulation. Unpublished
Psychosocial influences in retarded perfor-
manuscript, Syracuse University, 1968.
mance. Baltimore: University Park Press, 1981.
Glass, G. V. Primary, secondary, and meta-analysis
References
of research. Educational Research, 1976, 5, 3-
Appelbaum, M. I., & McCall, R. B. Design and 8.

analysis in developmental psychology. In W. Greenberg, P., & Epstein, B. Bridges to reading.


Kessen (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology. Morristown, N.J.: General Learning Corp.,
(Vol. 1): History, theory, and methods. New 1973.
York: Wiley, 1983. Heber, F. R., Dever, R. B., & Conry, J. The in-
Bayley, N. Manual for the Bayley Scales of Infant fluence of environmental and genetic variables
Development. New York: Psychological Corp., on intellectual development. In H. Prehm,
1969. L. A. Hamerlynck, & J. E. Crosson (Eds.), Be-
Bayley, N. Development of mental abilities. In havioral research in mental retardation.
P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael's manual of Eugene: University of Oregon, 1968.
child psychology. New York: Wiley, 1970. Hunt, J. McV. Intelligence and experience. New
Blank, M. Teaching learning in the preschool: A York: Ronald, 1961.
dialogue approach. Columbus, Ohio: Merrill, Jennrich, R. I. An asymptotic x2 test for the equality
1973. of two correlation matrices. Journal of the
Bloom, B. S. Stability and change in human charac- American Statistical Association, 1970, 65,
teristics. New York: Wiley, 1964. 904-912.

Bradley, R. H., Caldwell, B. M., & Elardo, R. Home Jensen, A. R. How much can we boost IQ and scho-
environment and cognitive development in the lastic achievement? Harvard Educational Re-
first two years: A cross-lagged panel analysis. view, 1969, 39, 1-123.
Developmental Psychology, 1979, 15, 246-250. Jensen, A. R. Bias in mental testing. New York:
Bronfenbrenner, U. A report on longitudinal evalu- Free Press, 1980.
ations of preschool programs. (Vol. 2): Is early Karnes, M. B. GOAL Program: Mathematical con-
intervention effective? (DHEW Publication cepts. Springfield, Mass.: Melton-Bradley,
No. [OHD] 72-24). Washington, D.C.: Office of 1978.
Child Development, 1974. Lazar, I., Darlington, R., Murray, H., Royce, J., &
Clarke, A. M., & Clarke, A. D. B. Early experience: Snipper, A. Lasting effects of early education:
Myth and evidence. London: Open Books, A report from the Consortium for Longitudinal
1976. Studies. Monographs of the Society for Re-
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. Applied multiple regres- search in Child Development, 1982, 47 (2-3,
sion: Correlation analysis for the behavioral Serial No. 195).
sciences. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1975. McCall, R. B. Challenges to a science of develop-
Cronbach, L. J. The two disciplines of scientific mental psychology. Child Development, 1977,
psychology. American Psychologist, 1957, 12, 48, 333-344.
671-684. McCall, R. B. The development of intellectual func-
Darlington, R. B. Radicals and squares: Statistical tioning in infancy and the prediction of later
methods for the behavioral sciences. Ithaca, IQ. In J. D. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant
N.Y.: Logan Hill, 1975. development. New York: Wiley, 1979.
Datta, L. Another spring and other hopes: Some McCall, R. B. Nature-nurture and the two realms of
findings from national evaluations of project development: A proposed integration with re-
Head Start. In E. Zigler & J. Valentine (Eds.), spect to mental development. Child Develop-
Project Head Start: A legacy of the war on pov- ment, 1981, 55, 1-12.
erty. New York: Free Press, 1979. McCall, R. B., & Appelbaum, M. I. Bias in the anal-
Davis, D. E. My friends and me. Circle Pines, ysis of repeated-measures designs: Some alter-
Minn.: American Guidance Service, 1977. native approaches. Child Development, 1973,
Dunn, L. M., Chun, L. T., Crowell, D. C., Dunn, 44, 401-415.
L. G., Avery, L. G., & Yachel, E. R. Peabody McCall, R. B., Eichorn, D. H., & Hogarty, P. S.
early education kit. Circle Pines, Minn.: Amer- Transitions in early mental development.
ican Guidance Service, 1976. Monographs of the Society for Research in
Elardo, R., Bradley, R. H., & Caldwell, B. M. The Child Development, 1977, 42(3, Serial No. 171).
relation of infants' home environments to men- Ramey, C. T., Bryant, D. M., & Suarez, T. M. Pre-
tal test performance from six to thirty-six school compensatory education and the mod-
months: A longitudinal analysis. Child Devel- ifiability of intelligence: A critical review. In
opment, 1975, 46, 71-76. D. K. Detterman (Ed.), Current topics in hu-

This content downloaded from 159.178.22.27 on Fri, 24 Jun 2016 23:54:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Ramey, Yeates, and Short 1925

man intelligence. Norwood, N.J.: Ablex, in tardation during infancy. Prevention in Human
press. Services, 1982, 1(4), 61-83.
Ramey, C. T., Campbell, F. A., & Nicholson, J. The Ramey, C. T., Stedman, D. S., Borders-Patterson,
predictive power of the Bayley Scales of Infant A., & Mengel, W. Predicting school failure
Development and the Stanford-Binet Intelli- from information available at birth. American
gence Test in a relatively constant environ- Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1978, 82, 524-
ment. Child Development, 1973, 44, 790-795. 534.
Ramey, C. T., & Finkelstein, N. W. Psychosocial Ramey, C. T., Yeates, K. O., & MacPhee, D. Risk for
mental retardation: A biological and social retarded development among disadvantaged
coalescence. In M. Begab (Ed.), Psychosocial families: A systems theory approach to preven-
influences and retarded performance: Strate- tive intervention. In B. K. Keogh (Ed.), Ad-
gies for improving competence (Vol. 1). Balti- vances in special education: Documenting pro-
more: University Park Press, 1981. gram impact (Vol. 4). Greenwich, Conn.: JAI
Ramey, C. T., & Haskins, R. The modification of Press, 1984.
intelligence through early experience. Intelli- Scarr, S., & Weinberg, R. A. The Minnesota adop-
gence, 1981, 5, 5-19. tion studies: Genetic differences and malleabil-
Ramey, C. T., MacPhee, D., & Yeates, K. O. Pre- ity. Child Development, 1983, 54, 260-267.
venting developmental retardation: A general Sparling, J. J., & Lewis, I. S. Learningames for the
systems model. In L. Bond & J. Joffe (Eds.), first three years: A guide to parent-child play.
Facilitating infant and early childhood devel- New York: Walker, 1979.
opment. Hanover, N.H.: University Press of Terman, L. M., & Merrill, M. A. Stanford-Binet In-
New England, 1982. telligence Scale: Manual for the third revision,
Ramey, C. T., McGinness, G. D., Cross, L., Collier, Form L-M. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin, 1973.
A. M., & Barrie-Blackley, S. The Abecedarian Tough, J. Listening to children talking. London:
approach to social competence: Cognitive and Ward Lock, 1976.
linguistic intervention for disadvantaged pre- Waddington, C. H. The strategy of the genes. Lon-
schoolers. In K. Borman (Ed.), The social life of don: Allen & Son, 1957.
children in a changing society. Hillsdale, N.J.: Wallach, M. A., & Wallach, L. Teaching all children
Erlbaum, 1982. to read. Chicago: University of Chicago Press,
Ramey, C. T., Mills, P., Campbell, F. A., & O'Brien, 1976.
C. Infants' home environments: A comparison Wechsler, D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale.
of high-risk families and families from the gen- New York: Psychological Corp., 1955.
eral population. American Journal of Mental Wohlwill, J. F. The study of behavioral develop-
Deficiency, 1975, 80, 40-42. ment. New York: Academic Press, 1973.
Ramey, C. T., & Smith, B. J. Assessing the intellec- Yeates, K. O., MacPhee, D., Campbell, F. A., &
tual consequences of early intervention with Ramey, C. T. Maternal IQ and home environ-
high-risk infants. American Journal of Mental ment as determinants of early childhood intel-
Deficiency, 1977, 81, 318-324. lectual competence: A developmental analysis.
Ramey, C. T., Sparling, J. J., Bryant, D. M., & Wasik, Developmental Psychology, 1983, 19, 731-
B. H. Primary prevention of developmental re- 739.

This content downloaded from 159.178.22.27 on Fri, 24 Jun 2016 23:54:12 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

You might also like