Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/326408617

Sexual Assault and Intimate Partner Violence

Chapter · July 2018


DOI: 10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_1722-1

CITATION READS

1 686

2 authors:

Paul Robert Gladden Anthony Cleator


Middle Georgia State University Middle Georgia State College
34 PUBLICATIONS   650 CITATIONS    2 PUBLICATIONS   6 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Life History Strategies View project

Selectionist Approaches to Proximate Mechanisms View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Paul Robert Gladden on 15 July 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


S

Sexual Assault and Intimate abuse and violence, psychological abuse, and
Partner Violence sexual abuse and/or violence directed toward
a romantic or sexual partner (Centers for Disease
Paul R. Gladden and Anthony M. Cleator Control 2008).
Department of Psychology, Sociology, and
Criminal Justice, Middle Georgia State
University, Macon, GA, USA Introduction

Sociocultural theorists of both sexual assault


Synonyms and intimate partner violence often ignore
and reject (or sometimes vilify) discussion of
Domestic violence; Interpersonal aggression; evolutionary analyses and genetic influences on
Mate retention; Rape; Sexual abuse; Sexual these behaviors as irrelevant at best (or pernicious
coercion and dangerous at worst). However, evolutionary
perspectives often emphasize environmentally
contingent adaptations which point to situational
Definitions and socio-ecological influences interacting with
evolved psychological mechanisms. Because all
Rape can be defined as “copulation resisted” by behavior depends on evolved behavioral mecha-
a victim “unless such resistance would probably nisms and because evolved mechanisms are adap-
result in death or serious injury to the victim or in tively responsive to socio-ecological conditions
death or injury to individuals the victim com- (e.g., Buss 1991), understanding sexual coercion
monly protects.” Sexual assault can also include and intimate partner violence with evolution-
“oral or anal penetration of a man or woman” ary perspectives shouldn’t, in principle, present
(Thornhill and Palmer 2000). We use the term a threat to the shared goals of (1) an accurate
sexual coercion (SC) broadly to mean the use of scientific understanding of the phenomena or
force, intimidation, lying, or psychoactive sub- (2) the moral obligation of trying to gain control
stances to receive or perform sexual acts involv- over those influences to stamp them out to the
ing another individual without that individual’s degree possible. A complete and accurate scien-
consent and sometimes without their knowledge tific understanding of the causes (rather than ideo-
or explicit awareness of that act (Gladden et al. logical beliefs about the causes) should be the best
2008). Intimate partner violence (IPV) is a broad potential way to try to control these behaviors.
term including acts of stalking as well as physical Here we review some of the theory and evidence
# Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018
T. K. Shackelford, V. A. Weekes-Shackelford (eds.), Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_1722-1
2 Sexual Assault and Intimate Partner Violence

that account for sexual coercion (SC) and inti- Shared genetic variation within families
mate partner violence (IPV) as related phenomena might at least partially account for the
understood from evolutionary perspectives. “intergenerational transmission” of IPV. Using a
nationally representative sample of American
adolescents, Barnes et al. (2013) found that heri-
Theoretical and Empirical Review table variation accounted for “24% of the variance
in hitting one’s partner, 54% of the variance in
Behavioral Genetics of IPV and Sexual Assault: injuring one’s partner, and 51% of the variance in
(Epi)Genetic Differences Matter forcing sexual activity on one’s partner.” The
There are individual differences in most unshared environment accounted for the rest of
behavioral traits including sexual coercion the variance. As is common in behavioral genetics
and intimate partner violence (IPV). Turkheimer results, the shared environment explained none of
(2000) suggested that the first law of behavioral the variance in IPV. These findings seem to con-
genetics is that nearly every trait that varies is tradict a within-family social learning account of
partially heritable and discussed how best to inter- why IPV consistently runs in families. However,
pret such empirical findings (see also Polderman it’s possible that genetic differences between indi-
et al. 2015). Although evolutionary psychologists viduals lead some individuals to be more (and
have tended to focus on explaining human uni- others less) likely to develop violent behaviors
versals and sex differences, a comprehensive evo- via social learning. If this were the case, heritable
lutionary understanding of human psychology variation would be the correctly identified
needs to include explanations for the main- source of variance in IPV, despite exposure to
tenance of heritable genetic differences correlated violence (within and/or outside of families) mech-
with phenotypic psychological differences. Selec- anistically interacting with genes in the develop-
tion acts on reproductive outcomes, influenced ment of mechanisms underlying IPV. As Barnes
by traits that develop within one individual, et al. (2013) point out, epigenetic research indi-
often through complex, epigenetic gene-by-gene cates that “environmental” stimuli affect gene-
and geneXenvironment interactions (Turkheimer tic “switches,” which turn genes “on” and “off.”
2000). In other words, selection acts on the repro- “To the extent that exposure to IPV in the home
ductive results of differences in intraindividual environment flips these relevant switches,” trying
gene-experience interactions, not on the effects to identify “genetic” versus “environmental”
of genes alone in a vacuum, somehow miracu- causes of behavioral variation in IPV is futile
lously separated from the effects of the environ- because genes would be causally involved in the
ment (Tooby and Cosmides 1992). (Note: Within “effects of social learning.” From an evolutionary
each individual and intraindividual are in italics perspective, genes and gene switches are designed
above to distinguish the mechanisms of individual to respond to the environment in ways that have
development from the sources of variation caus- been regularly adaptive. This view points to
ing individual differences.) Thus, a scientifically a need for sociocultural influences to be integrated
coherent explanation of any complex human with evolutionary thinking and genetic/epigenetic
behaviors, including IPV and sexual coercion, research in order to identify new predictions about
must consider (1) how genes adaptively respond how such socialization interacts with evolved
(e.g., epigenetically) to particular situations and psychological mechanisms.
experiences, (2) that “selection regulates how Some heritable variation is also implicated
environments shape organisms” (Tooby and in whether someone rapes an adult victim
Cosmides 1992), and (3) heritable individual (Långström et al. 2015). It is beyond the scope
differences in behavioral traits rather than just of this entry to do a thorough review of heritability
sex-differentiated human universals. research and its limitations, but the evidence
Sexual Assault and Intimate Partner Violence 3

reviewed here implies that genetics contributes relationship between quality of a couple’s sexual
something important to the etiology of IPV and relationship and male-perpetrated IPV. Both
SC (however tortuous, indirect, and synergistic female infidelity and male jealousy were part
those causal routes may be). The relevant point of the sexual relationship quality variable that
for us concerning the evolutionary hypotheses strongly predicted IPV.
regarding IPV and SC discussed below is that Some evolutionary hypotheses suggest mate
genetic variation in something or other matters guarding and intimate partner violence will be
for understanding the etiology of IPV and SC. directed more often toward jealously guarding
someone more desirable (Buss and Shackelford
1997) because they have more options to leave the
Intimate Partner Violence as a Form of
relationship partner in favor of a rival. Surpris-
Sexually Coercive Mating Effort
ingly, Figueredo et al. (2001, 2009) found that
As implied in the definition of IPV above, IPV
perceptions of a partner’s high mate value pre-
often involves sexual coercion of a partner.
dicted less spousal abuse perpetrated by both
Evolutionary psychologists have interpreted
sexes. In other words, when a partner perceived
IPV, more generally, as a form of sexual coercion
the other partner as having more desirable and
wherein perpetrators use violence to guard
attractive traits, they were less likely to abuse
and retain a mate, restrict their potential choice
them. Figueredo et al. (2001) argued the reason
of another mate, and prevent rivals from trying to
is that there is a correlation between one’s own
poach a mate. For example, Figueredo and
mate value and their partner’s mate value (0.50 in
McCloskey (1993) interpreted IPV as a sexually
their own sample). Therefore, “competitively dis-
coercive mate retention strategy used by compet-
advantaged females” (CDFs) tend to partner with
itively disadvantaged males (i.e., relatively low
competitively disadvantaged males (CDMs) in
mate value) to secure mates. Rowe et al. (1997)
long-term relationships. They suggested CDM’s
define mating effort as the psychological effort
perpetrate escalated mate guarding (including
used to “obtain and guard short-term mates.”
IPV) toward a low-quality partner because
If IPV is a set of mate retention behaviors, it is,
females, more so than males, can obtain a better
thus, a form of mating effort. Consistent with
quality mate as a short-term sexual partner
this idea, mating effort is associated with being
(possibly to capture a rival’s genes for her off-
male, intrasexual competition, sexual coercion
spring), even if unable to attract the affair partner
(Lalumiere and Quinsey 1996), and delinquency
as a long-term mate. This increases her CDM
in general (Rowe et al. 1997). If male-perpetrated
partner’s paternity uncertainty. Partly consistent
IPV functions to sexually coerce a female partner,
with this interpretation, the mate retention efforts
we should expect IPV to effectively prevent rivals
of relatively “unsexy” men (at risk of infidelity)
from mate poaching, prevent sexual infidelity
evidently increase when their partners are most
or cuckoldry, and prevent partners from defecting
fertile (Gangestad et al. 2005). Whereas male
from the relationship. We should also expect
partners of highly attractive women guard them
sexual jealousy to be linked with IPV because
more throughout their ovulatory cycle, partners
men’s jealousy is thought to function to prevent
of less attractive women intensify their mate
infidelity and increase paternity certainty. Jones
guarding only when it matters for preventing
et al. (2007) found that higher self-reported
impregnation by another male.
mating effort (on which men score higher)
predicted (1) higher levels of upset in response
to imagined sexual infidelity and (2) self-reported
“punitive intentions” in response to imagined
sexual and emotional infidelity. Figueredo
and McCloskey (1993) found a large negative
4 Sexual Assault and Intimate Partner Violence

Extra-Pair Copulation and Sperm general sexual frustration or mating deprivation.


Competition: Sexually Coercive IPV as an Anti- Similarly, risk of infidelity correlates with mate
Cuckoldry Tactic in Men retention behaviors, including intrasexual compe-
Goetz and Shackelford (2009) tested the hypoth- tition tactics such as threatening rivals (Starratt
esis that partner rape is an adaptation for sperm et al. 2007).
competition. According to this view, partner rape If IPV is ultimately about mating competition,
by men becomes more likely when a female part- as evolutionary theorists hypothesize, we should
ner is sexually unfaithful or suspected of being expect mate retention and intrasexual competition
unfaithful – and functions to prevent a male rival tactics to be used by the same individuals when
from successfully fertilizing one’s partner. They faced with threats or perceived threats of infidel-
found, across two studies, that men’s partner- ity. Figueredo et al. (2018) reported that in samples
directed sexual coercion was somewhat positively of both sexes, across five countries, interpersonal
correlated with women’s infidelity and suspected aggression toward both opposite-sex partners and
infidelity. Men’s (nonphysical) controlling behav- toward same-sex rivals was positively correlated
iors also predicted their partner-directed sexual with each other and with other measures of mating
coercion. aggression. In other words, measures of IPV, mea-
Time spent apart from one’s partner increases sures of aggression toward one’s same sex, and
risk/opportunities for extra-pair copulations. If measures specifically created by evolutionary
sexual coercion toward a partner is an anti- psychologists to measure “mating aggression”
cuckoldry tactic toward increasing paternity cer- (i.e., intrasexual competition, competitor deroga-
tainty, we should expect increased interest in cop- tion tactics, mate retention, and mate guarding)
ulation with a partner and sexual coercion directed were loaded upon by a single interpersonal aggres-
toward a partner when the couple has spent an sion factor. Interpersonal aggression toward both
increased proportion of time apart (a cue to the sexes was accounted for by the same influences in
risk of female infidelity). Shackelford et al. (2007) a model including self-reported “psychopathic
found that risk of infidelity, as measured by and aggressive attitudes” (including high mating
increased proportion of time away from a partner effort), short-term mating orientation (socio-
since the couple’s last copulation, predicted men’s sexuality), antagonistic social schemata, poor
greater “sexual interest in his partner,” “distress in executive functioning, and fast life history strate-
response to his partner’s sexual rejection,” and gies. In other words, the same basic model of
“sexual persistence in response to his partner’s influences that accounted for IPV accounted for
sexual rejection. . .independent of total time interpersonal aggression directed toward one’s
since the couple’s last copulation and the man’s own sex as well as toward the opposite sex,
relationship satisfaction.” McKibbin et al. (2011) suggesting that many individuals committing
found that proportion of time spent apart since last IPV are generally antisocial and aggressive
in-pair copulation also correlates with male use (rather than “specialists” in IPV). Also, since fast
of sexual coercion toward a partner but only life history strategies (described below) are char-
among men who perceive higher risk of partner acterized by short-term mating and promiscuity,
infidelity. McKibbin et al. (2011) note that partner- such individuals are particularly at risk of infidel-
directed sexual coercion is related to the total ity (both their partner’s and their own) and, thus,
proportion of time spent apart since last in-pair might be more likely to exhibit violent jealousy.
copulation, not total time since last in-pair copu- Taken together, these results support the evolu-
lation, supporting the sperm competition hypoth- tionary interpretation that much interpersonal vio-
esis while contradicting an alternative view that lence (directed toward both sexes) is ultimately
partner-directed sexual coercion is the result of about mating competition.
Sexual Assault and Intimate Partner Violence 5

Men’s Pornography Preferences, Sperm have increased reproductive success regularly for
Competition, and Sexual Coercion of a Partner some men under certain socio-ecological condi-
Since a partner’s infidelity is bad for men’s tions. Consistent with this possibility, it appears
paternity certainty and many men desire sexual that forced copulation is more likely to lead to
variety, it is, perhaps, surprising that some studies pregnancy than consensual copulation after con-
(Pound 2002; McKibbin et al. 2013) find evidence trolling for use of oral contraception (Gottschall
that pornographic depictions of sexual situations and Gottschall 2003). Thornhill and Palmer
involving one female with multiple males (i.e., (2000) considered several possibilities for rape-
high sperm competition situations) are more specific adaptations including potential psycho-
common than depictions involving one male logical mechanisms for (1) evaluating the vulner-
with multiple females (i.e., low sperm competi- ability of victims (e.g., during wartime when
tion situations) (Pound 2002) and that many men unprotected), (2) motivating some men to rape
may prefer to view those high sperm competition when under conditions where they are deprived
situations (McKibbin et al. 2013; but see Prokop of mating opportunities or when lacking sufficient
2015). Kilgallon and Simmons (2005) reported resources to attract desirable mates, (3) evaluating
that when men masturbated while viewing images the attractiveness of potential victims differently
depicting high sperm competition situations, than consensual partners (e.g., different age pref-
they produced a higher percentage of more motile erences for victims at peak fertility), (4) resulting
sperm compared to when masturbating to images in differences in male sexual arousal in response
depicting low sperm competition situations. to depictions of forced sex compared to consen-
These results seem to suggest that some men sual mating, (5) resulting in differences in sperm
may become more aroused while viewing high counts in rape contexts compared to consensual
sperm competition situations, including cuck- contexts, and (6) motivating males to rape as
oldry risk depictions. Wright (1994) suggested a sperm competition/anti-cuckoldry tactic.
that mechanisms such as these might make adap- In contrast, the maladaptive by-product
tive sense to outcompete rival males’ sperm in hypothesis of rape proposes that sexual coercion
situations where a partner has copulated with is a maladaptive side effect of selection for
another man. In other words, these findings fit (sexually dimorphic) traits important for men’s
well with findings that men’s sexual interest in consensual reproductive opportunities: (1) desire
and sexual coercion of a partner increase when for sexual variety without commitment and
there has been a risk or perceived risk of infidelity (2) inclinations toward instrumental aggression
(McKibbin et al. 2011; Shackelford et al. 2007; (Palmer 1991). The by-product view predicts
Starratt et al. 2007). that sexual coercers will have particularly high
levels of consensual sexual activity (as well as
Rape: Adaptation or By-Product? unconsensual sexual activity), so the by-product
Thornhill and Palmer (2000) considered whether, hypothesis is supported by findings that sexual
over evolutionary time, human rape was re- coercers report higher levels of mating effort
productively advantageous to some men under (Lalumiere and Quinsey 1996) and sexual experi-
some socio-ecological conditions, favoring spe- ence (Lalumière et al. 1996). The by-product the-
cific mechanisms for forced copulation, or if ory shares similar features with the confluence
human rape is better explained as a maladaptive model (Malamuth et al. 1995), which suggests
by-product of other psychological adapta- that sexual aggression results from two “paths”
tions that evolved for other functions. In other that may converge: hostile masculinity and pro-
words, they debated whether or not rape is miscuous and impersonal sex.
a specifically evolved reproductive tactic. If rape
were an evolved reproductive tactic, it must
6 Sexual Assault and Intimate Partner Violence

Are Sexually Coercive Men Competitively sexual partners, but Figueredo (personal commu-
Disadvantaged, Mate Deprived, Both, or nication) argues that they are divergent theories
Neither? and that a CDM view is consistent with the find-
Thornhill and Thornhill (1983) hypoth- ings of a more extensive sexual history among
esized that sexual assault may be a condi- sexual coercers. For example, Figueredo et al.
tional (environmentally contingent) adaptation (2000) reported that psychosocial deficits directly
expressed among some men when they are predicted higher “noncriminal sexual deviance”
outcompeted for resources and status, which on the path to predicting nonsexual general crim-
are important for attracting desirable romantic inality and finally sexual criminality. Their inter-
and sexual partners. Consistent with this compet- pretation was that competitive disadvantages
itively disadvantaged male hypothesis, Figueredo “might lead to frustration in the mainstream sex-
et al. (2000) found that male adolescent sex- ual marketplace and therefore to the selection of
ual offenders were “competitively disadvantaged” deviant avenues of sexual expression.” According
(i.e., low mate value), particularly by having to this view, these “deviant expressions,” driven
psychosocial deficiencies: poor mental abili- by an “escalation of means of obtaining sexual
ties, poor social skills, and more psychopatho- gratification,” are consistent with a developmen-
logy. According to their interpretation of their tally contingent increase in mating effort (and,
evolutionary-developmental model, these psy- perhaps, relaxation of mate standards) and short-
chosocial deficits indirectly led to sexual term mating tactics generally, which predict more
offending over time as a result of “repeated frus- sexual activity (including, evidently, non-
tration and failure” in sexual relationships, “lead- consensual sexual activity). In other words, a
ing to an escalation of more and more extreme CDM mating strategy may be to mate with as
means of obtaining sexual gratification” (p. 322). many females as possible, regardless of her desir-
Alternatively, the mate deprivation hypothesis ability (favoring quantity over quality). So, the
suggested deprivation of sexual access itself CDM hypothesis should not necessarily be con-
increases likelihood of sexual coercion in some flated with the refuted mate deprivation hypothe-
men. The competitively disadvantaged male sis. But, the available evidence doesn’t currently
(CDM) hypothesis has been conflated with support the CDM hypothesis over the maladaptive
the mate deprivation hypothesis. For example, by-product hypothesis, which predicts generally
Lalumière et al. (1996) suggested “if sexually higher levels of sexual activity among sexual
coercive males are disadvantaged in mate com- coercers.
petition, they should report a less extensive his- In our view, the currently available evidence
tory of sexual experience (e.g., smaller number that male-perpetrated sexual coercion of partners
of different sexual partners)” (p. 301). may function as a sperm competition tactic is
Lalumière et al. (1996) found sexually coer- stronger than evidence suggestive of specific
cive males report a higher number of sexual adaptions for sexual coercion of nonpartners.
partners and a more, not less, extensive sexual This raises the possibility that men coercive of
history. Similarly, psychopathy, which correlates partners are CDMs since coercion of partners is a
with sexual coercion of both partners and non- form of IPV. Starratt et al. (2008) found that if men
partners (Camilleri and Quinsey 2009b), is asso- perceived their partner as more desirable than
ciated with higher sociosexuality and mating themselves as a long-term partner, perception of
effort (Figueredo et al. 2018). These findings fal- his partner’s infidelity predicted increased use of
sify the mate deprivation hypothesis. Both the sexually coercive “commitment manipulation.” If
mate deprivation and CDM hypotheses suggest men perceived their partners as roughly equally
sexually coercive behaviors are conditional adap- desirable as long-term partner, his partner’s infi-
tations, and it seems like “competitively disadvan- delity also predicted higher levels of other sexu-
taged” in mating competition would imply a ally coercive tactics. But there was no relationship
likelihood of being deprived of mates and fewer between perceptions of women’s infidelity and
Sexual Assault and Intimate Partner Violence 7

sexually coercive tactics among men who per- risk-taking, and decreased rule governance
ceived their partner as relatively more desirable (Rushton 1985). In contrast, individuals living
than themselves as a long-term partner. This in safe, stable, and controllable socio-ecological
seems to contradict the CDM hypothesis with conditions will exhibit clusters of the opposite
respect to partner-directed coercion because we pattern of traits (“slow” LH strategy traits).
might expect relatively less desirable males to Figueredo and Jacobs (2010) argued that fast
coerce more desirable female partners. However, LH strategists develop antagonistic social sche-
in light of Figueredo et al.’s (2001) interpretation mata and interpret the world as a zero-sum game
of their findings on IPV generally (see above), in that they perceive others’ interests as in conflict
because CDFs tend to pair with CDMs as long- with theirs and focus on immediate gratification of
term partners, a less desirable female partner is their own interests (e.g., obtaining and securing
often mated with a low desirability male (relative a mate). In contrast, slow LH strategists perceive
to rivals), and she may have many short-term others’ interests as mutually consistent with their
opportunities for infidelity, regardless of her own and tend to delay gratification to serve long-
long-term desirability relative to her partner. So, term goals, including mutually beneficial altruism
her relative desirability as a long-term partner and supportive interpersonal and romantic rela-
compared to her partner may not be terribly tionships. Thus, a fast LH strategy is more strate-
important for testing the CDM hypothesis. gically consistent with low self-control (Dunkel
et al. 2013), risk-taking, breaking rules, and inter-
Use of Sexual Coercion in Both Sexes personal aggression and conflict than a slow LH
Up to this point, we have mostly considered male- strategy. These fast LH traits seem consistent with
perpetrated sexual coercion and IPV as forms of the use of both IPV and sexual coercion.
mating aggression and mate retention. But Using a college student sample including both
females report using some forms of sexual coer- sexes, Gladden et al. (2008) found that various
cion too, though not as frequently as males. apparent intercorrelated indicators of slow LH
Camilleri and Quinsey (2009a) reported finding strategy predicted reduced frequencies of sexually
that, unlike in males, interest in partner-directed coercive behaviors. Slow LH indicators included
sexual coercion in females is unrelated to changes long-term mating orientation, low psychopathy,
in the risk of infidelity. Considering females have and high self-perceived mate value. According
maternity certainty, these findings support the to comparisons of path-analytic models, this “pro-
anti-cuckoldry risk hypothesis of men’s sexual tective” slow LH factor fully mediated the rela-
coercion of a partner. And while female sexual tionship between biological sex of participants
coercion of men may function to capture a partic- and frequencies of sexually coercive behaviors.
ular man’s genes or to force a particular man into As in the cross-cultural studies on interpersonal
long-term paternal investment, a relationship, or aggression discussed earlier (Figueredo et al.
as an attempt to extract immediate resources, it 2018), these results were interpreted as suggesting
cannot function to ensure maternity certainty. that slow LH strategy inhibits sexual aggression.
Consistent with a LH strategy explanation of sex-
Life History Strategy, Antagonistic Social ual coercion, Dunkel and Mathes (2011) found
Strategies, and Sexual Coercion that individuals’ (both men’s and women’s) self-
Life history (LH) theory suggests that individuals reported willingness to use sexually coercive tac-
developing in unpredictable and uncontrollable tics changed when they were asked to imagine
conditions (e.g., high extrinsic morbidity and different life expectancies (5 months left to live,
mortality risk, low parental support) will exhibit 5 years left to live, or at least 50 years left to live).
clusters of “fast” LH strategy traits including low Furthermore, fast LH strategy correlated with
somatic effort, low parental effort, high mating willingness to use sexual coercion at the shortest
effort, short-term mating orientation (e.g., high and longest imagined life expectancy conditions,
quantity of genetically diverse offspring), high and short imagined life expectancy increased
8 Sexual Assault and Intimate Partner Violence

willingness to use sexual coercion in individuals women. This was expected because fast LH strat-
that reported a short-term mating orientation. egy individuals should exhibit increased intra-
Since LH theory suggests that developing under sexual competition and antagonistic social
short life expectancy conditions will lead to a attitudes and interactions toward both sexes gen-
faster LH strategy and a more short-term mating erally (Figueredo and Jacobs 2010). Since women
orientation, these results support a LH perspective exhibited significant hostile attitudes toward other
on sexual coercion. Using the same approach, women, these results seem inconsistent with per-
Dunkel et al. (2013) found that both reported spectives suggesting a sociocultural problem found
self-control and general criminal intent also particularly among men. In sum, fast LH strategies
increased when asked to imagine different life are linked with traits theorized to influence sexual
expectancies as described above. Furthermore, coercion (and IPV) including psychopathy, aggres-
self-reported LH strategy moderated these imag- sion, negative androcentrism, short-mating prefer-
ined behavioral effects such that trait LH strategy ences, low perceived mate value, and low self-
was most associated with self-control and crimi- control. A LH strategy account of sexual coercion
nal intent when imagined life expectancy was is consistent with sexual coercion as either a spe-
short. This suggests that slow LH may be protec- cific design feature of an overall LH strategy or as a
tive factor against general criminality (including by-product of a combination of various LH traits
sexual coercion and interpersonal aggression). that are designed for consensual mating (Gladden
et al. 2008).
LH Strategy, Negative Androcentrism, and
Mating Aggression
Since fast LH strategies are associated with Conclusion
interest in short-term mating, mating effort,
aggression, and psychopathy (Gladden et al. There are a variety of evolutionary perspectives
2008), fast LH strategies appear to be linked on IPV and sexual coercion. Some are adaptation-
with both aspects of the maladaptive by-product ist hypotheses, and others suggest these behaviors
hypothesis of sexual coercion and, perhaps, both result as side effects of selection for other traits.
“paths” of the confluence model of sexual coer- But each suggests that IPV and sexual coercion
cion (Malamuth et al. 1995). The confluence are ultimately linked in some way to mating
model implicates hostile masculinity and other competition.
forms of negative androcentrism (bias against Many personality characteristics have been
women), along with interest in impersonal sex, repeatedly identified as correlates of IPV and sex-
in causing sexual coercion. Gladden et al. (2013) ual coercion overlap (e.g., psychopathy, hostile
found that fast LH directly predicted forms of masculinity, mating effort). LH theory is a broad
negative androcentrism among both sexes and theoretical framework that tries to explain many
that fast LH strategies fully mediated between somewhat interrelated psychological traits.
being male and negative androcentrism. So, Figueredo et al. (2018) suggest LH theory is an
according to the model, men only expressed inclusive framework for integrating many seem-
higher average levels of hostile attitudes toward ingly disparate findings about interpersonal
women because of average sex differences in var- aggression and sexual coercion. For example,
ious fast LH characteristics. This parallels the LH strategies are theoretically and empirically
findings of Gladden et al. (2008) that fast LH traits associated in some way with various proposed
fully mediated between being male and sexually influences on sexual coercion and IPV. Although
coercive behaviors. So a LH explanation of sexual LH theory may help as an integrative evolutionary
coercion is consistent with the possible influ- framework to better understand relations among
ence of negative androcentric attitudes on men’s various findings that were not all originally con-
sexual coercion. But the results also show more sidered from an evolutionary LH theoretical per-
hostile attitudes toward women among fast LH spective, much more work needs to be done to
Sexual Assault and Intimate Partner Violence 9

identify and understand how specific characteris- Figueredo, A. J., & Jacobs, W. J. (2010). Aggression, risk-
tics associated with an overall LH strategy taking, and alternative life history strategies:
The behavioral ecology of social deviance. In
uniquely lead to the behavioral expression of M. Frias-Armenta & V. Corral-Verdugo (Eds.), Bio-
interpersonal aggression and sexual coercion. psycho-social perspectives on interpersonal violence
Evolutionary perspectives on IPV and sexual (pp. 3–28). Hauppauge: NOVA Science Publishers.
coercion have much to gain by considering influ- Figueredo, A. J., & McCloskey, L. A. (1993). Sex,
money, and paternity: The evolutionary psychology of
ences identified by standard social science per- domestic violence. Ethology and Sociobiology, 14(6),
spectives and vice versa. 353–379.
Figueredo, A. J., Sales, B. D., Becker, J. V., Russell, K., &
Kaplan, M. (2000). A Brunswikian evolutionary-
developmental model of adolescent sex offending.
Cross-References Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 18, 309–329.
Figueredo, A. J., Corral-Verdugo, V., Frı́as-Armenta, M.,
▶ Competitively Disadvantaged Male Hypothesis Bachar, K. J., White, J., McNeill, P. L., &
del PilarCastell-Ruiz, I. (2001). Blood, solidarity,
▶ Cuckoldry
status, and honor: The sexual balance of power and
▶ Life History Strategy spousal abuse in Sonora, Mexico. Evolution and
▶ Mating Effort Human Behavior, 22(5), 295–328.
▶ Psychopathy Figueredo, A. J., Rojas, E. M., Armenta, M. F., &
Verdugo, V. C. (2009). Individual differences and social
▶ Socio-Sexuality
contexts: The absence of family deterrence of spousal
▶ Sperm-Competition abuse in San José, Costa Rica. Journal of Social, Evo-
lutionary, and Cultural Psychology, 3(1), 29.
Figueredo, A. J., Jacobs, W. J., Gladden, P. R.,
Bianchi, J. M. Patch, E. A., Kavanagh, P. S.,
References Beck, C. J. A., Sotomator-Peterson, M., Li, N., &
Jiang, F. (2018). Intimate partner violence, interper-
Barnes, J. C., TenEyck, M., Boutwell, B. B., & sonal aggression, and life history strategy. Evolutionary
Beaver, K. M. (2013). Indicators of domestic/intimate Behavioral Sciences, 12(1), 1.
partner violence are structured by genetic and non- Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Garver-Apgar, C. E.
shared environmental influences. Journal of (2005). Adaptations to ovulation. In The handbook of
Psychiatric Research, 47(3), 371–376. evolutionary psychology (pp. 344–371). New York:
Buss, D. M. (1991). Evolutionary personality psychology. Wiley.
Annual Review of Psychology, 42(1), 459–491. Gladden, P. R., Sisco, M., & Figueredo, A. J. (2008).
Buss, D. M., & Shackelford, T. K. (1997). From vigilance Sexual coercion and life history strategy. Evolution
to violence: Mate retention tactics in married couples. and Human Behavior, 48, 731–735.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, Gladden, P. R., Figueredo, A. J., Andrejzak, D. J.,
346–361. Jones, D. N., & Smith-Castro, V. (2013). Reproductive
Camilleri, J. A., & Quinsey, V. L. (2009a). Testing the strategy and sexual conflict: Slow life history strategy
cuckoldry risk hypothesis of partner sexual coercion inhibits negative androcentrism. Journal of Methods
in community and forensic samples. Evolutionary and Measurement in the Social Sciences, 4(1), 48–71.
Psychology, 7(2), 164–178. Goetz, A. T., & Shackelford, T. K. (2009). Sexual coercion
Camilleri, J. A., & Quinsey, V. L. (2009b). Individual in intimate relationships: A comparative analysis of the
differences in the propensity for partner sexual effects of women’s infidelity and men’s dominance and
coercion. Sexual Abuse, 21(1), 111–129. control. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38(2), 226–234.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2008). Inti- Gottschall, J. A., & Gottschall, T. A. (2003). Are per-incident
mate partner violence. Retrieved from https://www. rape-pregnancy rates higher than per-incident consensual
cdc.gov/violenceprevention/intimatepartnerviolence/ pregnancy rates? Human Nature, 14(1), 1–20.
index.html. Jones, D. N., Figueredo, A. J., Dickey, E. D., & Jacobs,
Dunkel, C. S., & Mathes, E. (2011). The effect of individ- W. J. (2007). Relations among individual differences
ual differences and manipulated life expectancies on in reproductive strategies, sexual attractiveness, affec-
the willingness to engage in sexual coercion. tive and punitive intentions, and imagined sexual or
Evolutionary Psychology, 9(4), 588–599. emotional infidelity. Evolutionary Psychology, 5(2),
Dunkel, C. S., Mathes, E., & Beaver, K. M. (2013). Life 147470490700500212.
history theory and the general theory of crime: Life Kilgallon, S. J., & Simmons, L. W. (2005). Image content
expectancy effects on low self-control and criminal influences men’s semen quality. Biology Letters, 1(3),
intent. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural 253–255.
Psychology, 7(1), 12.
10 Sexual Assault and Intimate Partner Violence

Lalumiere, M. L., & Quinsey, V. L. (1996). Sexual devi- Rowe, D. C., Vazsonyi, A. T., & Figueredo, A. J. (1997).
ance, antisociality, mating effort, and the use of sexu- Mating-effort in adolescence: A conditional or alterna-
ally coercive behaviors. Personality and Individual tive strategy. Personality and Individual Differences,
Differences, 21(1), 33–48. 23(1), 105–115.
Lalumière, M. L., Chalmers, L. J., Quinsey, V. L., & Rushton, J. P. (1985). Differential K theory: The sociobi-
Seto, M. C. (1996). A test of the mate deprivation ology of individual and group differences. Personality
hypothesis of sexual coercion. Ethology and and Individual Differences, 6(4), 441–452.
Sociobiology, 17(5), 299–318. Shackelford, T. K., Goetz, A. T., McKibbin, W. F., &
Långström, N., Babchishin, K. M., Fazel, S., Lichtenstein, P., Starratt, V. G. (2007). Absence makes the adaptations
& Frisell, T. (2015). Sexual offending runs in families: grow fonder: Proportion of time apart from partner,
A 37-year nationwide study. International Journal of male sexual psychology, and sperm competition in
Epidemiology, 44(2), 713–720. dyv029. humans (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative
Malamuth, N. M., Linz, D., Heavey, C. L., Barnes, G., & Psychology, 121(2), 214–220.
Acker, M. (1995). Using the confluence model of Starratt, V. G., Shackelford, T. K., Goetz, A. T., &
sexual aggression to predict men's conflict with McKibbin, W. F. (2007). Male mate retention behaviors
women: A 10-year follow-up study. Journal of vary with risk of partner infidelity and sperm competi-
Personality and Social Psychology, 69(2), 353. tion. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 39(3), 523–527.
McKibbin, W. F., Starratt, V. G., Shackelford, T. K., & Starratt, V. G., Popp, D., & Shackelford, T. K. (2008).
Goetz, A. T. (2011). Perceived risk of female infidelity Not all men are sexually coercive: A preliminary inves-
moderates the relationship between objective risk of tigation of the moderating effect of mate desirability on
female infidelity and sexual coercion in humans the relationship between female infidelity and male
(Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, sexual coercion. Personality and Individual
125(3), 370–373. Differences, 45, 10–14.
McKibbin, W. F., Pham, M. N., & Shackelford, T. K. Thornhill, R., & Palmer, C. T. (2000). A natural history of
(2013). Human sperm competition in postindustrial rape. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
ecologies: Sperm competition cues predict adult DVD Thornhill, R., & Thornhill, N. W. (1983). Human rape:
sales. Behavioral Ecology, 24(4), 819–823. art031. An evolutionary analysis. Ethology and Sociobiology,
Palmer, C. T. (1991). Human rape: Adaptation or 4(3), 137–173.
by-product? Journal of Sex Research, 28(3), 365–386. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological
Polderman, T. J., Benyamin, B., De Leeuw, C. A., foundations of culture. In The adapted mind:
Sullivan, P. F., Van Bochoven, A., Visscher, P. M., & Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture
Posthuma, D. (2015). Meta-analysis of the heritability (pp. 19–136). New York: Oxford University Press.
of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Turkheimer, E. (2000). Three laws of behavior genetics
Nature Genetics, 47(7), 702–709. and what they mean. Current Directions in
Pound, N. (2002). Male interest in visual cues of sperm Psychological Science, 9(5), 160–164.
competition risk. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23(6), Wright, R. D. (1994). The moral animal: Why we are, the
443–466. way we are: The new science of evolutionary
Prokop, P. (2015). Perception of intensity of sperm psychology. New York: Vintage.
competition on the part of males. Personality and
Individual Differences, 76, 99–103.

View publication stats

You might also like