Teacher Training: George Sugai

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

ABSTRACT

Twenty preservice special education teachers


participated in a study to examine the effects of
the combined technique of peer observation
with observation systems technology on the
teaching performance of teacher-trainees. The
subjects were divided into two groups: experi-
mental and control. Experimental group
trainees used well-defined observation systems
to collect teacher/pupil data for the provision of
feedback to peers, control trainees developed
their own systems of anecdotal report. Three
major types of data were collected and ana-
lyzed: () behavior/lesson management, (
a )
b
teacher-trainees’ instructional management
) teacher-trainees’ feedback strategies.
skills,
c
(
Results showed that the experimental group
maintained a higher level of pupil accuracy dur-
ing direct instruction, maintained a brisker pre-
sentation and correct rate, and tended to
prompt correct responses less often than the
control group. These findings and other obser-
vations are discussed
.

Traditionally, practicum and student teaching


supervision has involved the observation of
trainee performance and the provision of feed-
back to the teacher trainee relative to the dem-
onstration of targeted behavioral competen-
cies. According to this model, the supervisor’s
role is to define and communicate performance
expectations so trainees may achieve criterion
TEACHER
levels of teaching performance.
In practice, the effectiveness of field supervi-
sion has been restricted by several factors.
TRAINING
Often the frequency and regularity of field ob-
servations by program supervisors is dimin- IMPROVING TRAINEE
ished by large numbers of trainees &dquo;who are in
dispersed and/or remote locations&dquo; (Semmel &
PERFORMANCE THROUGH
Englert,1978, p. 28). Budgetary restrictions also PEER OBSERVATION AND
are adding to the number of trainees assigned
to supervisors, limiting the of visits OBSERVATION SYSTEM
and the amount of contact time the su-
pervisory personnel and preservice teacher-
TECHNOLOGY
trainees. Such limitations make it increasingly
unlikely that systematic training can be deliv- Carol SueEnglert
ered using traditional supervisory procedures
(Allilunas, 1972; Cumming, 1971; Merrill & George Sugai
Schuchman, 1973; Semmel & Englert, 1978).
One method for overcoming the negative ef-
fects of fewer and shorter observation opportuni- Carol Sue Englert and George Sugai are both Assis-
ties involves the use of peer observers equipped tant Processors at the University of Kantucky.

Downloaded from tes.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 5, 2016


with efficient and precise observation systems. METHOD
Observation systems have been shown to have
strong advantages over other feedback sys- Subjects an Setting
tems for teacher observation and training. This study involved 20 preservice special educa-
Whereas the function of teacher observation is tion teachers from two sections of a 3-hour prac-
the provision of feedback for the improvement ticum course offered for special education ma-
of teaching, observers’ comments are often per- jors in learning and behavior disorders. The
ceived by the trainee as being subjective and course was taken by special education stu-
biased (Semmel & Thiagarajan, 1973). However, dents the semester prior to student teaching, so
given feedback based upon objective observa- all students were in their 4th or 5th college year
tion instruments, teacher trainees can be pre- and had finished a majority of their required
sented factual information with which to evalu- coursework. One section of 8 students was arbi-
ate their performance and establish teaching trarily designated the control group; the other
objectives. By involving teacher trainees them- group of 12 students was designated the experi-
selves in the observation process, including the mental group.
measurement and charting of teacher-pupil be- The study was conducted in the practicum
haviors, trainees can become increasingly ac- sites in which the special education trainees
countable for monitoring their achievement of were placed. These sites included 5 elementary
stated teacher or pupil objectives (Walker, resource rooms for the educable mentally hand-
1978). When used in this manner, observation in- icapped, 13 elementary resource rooms for the
struments could affect teacher trainees’ atti- learning disabled, and 2 learning disabled re-
tudes toward the practicum and increase the source rooms at the secondary level.
probability that new teaching behaviors and
skills will be acquired. Independent Variables
Given increasing restrictions on university While both groups completed similar peer ob-
supervision, this investigation focused specifi- servation tasks, the experimental group was
cally on the applications of peer observation to provided with two observation instruments con-
teacher training. Skoog (1980) has suggested taining specific, well-defined observational cat-
that structured feedback from peer observation egories. The first of these observation instru-
is an effective means of improving teaching per- ments was based on an interval recording sys-
formance. Similarly, Ellis, Smith, and Abbott tem developed by Dollar (1980). This observation
(1979) have reported that peer observation posi- system was designed to provide feedback to
tively affects inservice teachers’ attitudes teacher trainees on their behavior management
toward observation and their receptivity to su- skills, particulary with respect to trainees’ abili-
pervision. However, there have been few studies ties to manage appropriate student behavior
examining applications of the combined tech- through teacher praise and approval and inap-
nique of peer observation with observation sys- propriate student behavior through teacher criti-
tems technology at the preservice level. Exam- cism, disapproval, or planned ignoring.
ination of this question might suggest the As shown in Figure 1, peer observers using
feasibility of different formats for structuring the Dollar system coded the occurrence of two
the peer observation experience. categories of teacher behavior and two categor-
The present study examined the effective- ies of pupil behavior. Each column of the obser-
ness of peer observation in a practicum setting vation system represented a single, 15-second
under two observation and feedback condi- interval. Peer observers observed continuously
tions : peer observation conducted with well-de- for 15 seconds, then recorded one teacher and
fined observation systems, and peer observa- pupil behavior by slashing the respective code
tion conducted with less-defined observation letters of the observed behaviors: &dquo;a&dquo; for appro-
systems based on anecdotal reports. Given the priate pupil behavior, &dquo;i&dquo; for inappropriate pupill
prior demonstrated effectiveness of observa- behavior, &dquo;A&dquo; for teacher approval, &dquo;D&dquo; for
tion systems, it was hypothesized that peer teacher disapproval. To make the observation
observation with well-defined observation sys- system specific to each individual trainee’s
tems would result in greater trainee demonstra- teaching situation and objectives, categories of
tion of behavior management and direct instruc- pupil appropriatelinappropriate and teacher ap-
tion behaviors. provalldisapproval were targeted and defined
8

Downloaded from tes.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 5, 2016


FIGURE 1
Behavior rnanagerrrent observation form

by the trainees themselves before the observa- the start of each learning trial, peer observers
tion. Peer observers then used these definitions were instructed to code information related to
to collect data on the target behaviors of con- (a) whether the teacher trainee gave an atten-
cern to the teacher trainee. tion signal, (b) whether the pupils attended to
The second observation instrument was mod- the instructional stimulus, and (c) whether the
ified from an existing observation system teacher’s question was followed by a correct/in-
(CASE) intended for inservice teacher evalua- correct student response. Subsequent teacher-
tion and supervision (Tawney, 1979). The CASE pupil interactions (e.g., prompts, reinforcement)
observation system focused on teachers’ direct involving the same stimulus were coded in a
instruction strategies, as measured by the pres- single learning trial. Peer observers coded
ence or absence of the following teacher/pupil teacher/pupil behavior by slashing, circling, or
behaviors (observational codes indicated in par- inserting the code letters for the respective ob-
entheses) : teacher’s attention signals (at), servational categories. A learning trial ended
pupil’s attending behavior (A), correct/incorrect when the trainee presented information or ques-
academic response from pupil (+ / -), teacher tions related to a new stimulus or when the
reinforcement of correct academic response teacher told pupils the correct answer following
(R), teacher correction or prompting of incorrect an incorrect resconse.
academic response (C), and teacher telling of In contrast tc the experimental group, peer
correct answers following an incorrect re- observers in the -ont:,-’ group not given
sponse (T). The recording sheet containing these well-defined observation systems for the
these direct instruction categories is presented collection of teacher/pupil data. Instead, after
in Figure 2. discussion of desirable teacher strategies and
As indicated on the Direct Instruction record- methods for behaviors management and direct
ing form, each column of the form represented a instruction, con±=o&dquo; peer observers developed
single learning trial involving the presentation their own systems of recording teacher/pupil
of a new stimulus, such as a new sight word, behavior during lessons focusing either on be-
math problem, or comprehension question. At havior management or direct instruction tech-
9

Downloaded from tes.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 5, 2016


FIGURE 2
Direct instruction observation form

10
Downloaded from tes.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 5, 2016
niques. Typically, these systems evolved into trial or teacher prompt.
narrative records and anecdotal descriptions of 2. incorrect academic response. Percentage of
the key instructional and behavior management incorrect pupil responses following a learning
behaviors displayed by the teacher trainee and trial or teacher prompt.
pupils. Thus, continuous and systematic obser- 3. Appropriatelinappropriate social behavior.
vation methods were not used by the control Rate at which pupils demonstrated social be-
group of trainees. haviors in compliance or noncompliance with
stated or understood classroom and/or lesson
Dependent Variables rules (e.g., on-task, off-task behavior).
To measure and record low inference teacher 4. Lesson noise. Rate of occurrence of off-task
and pupil behaviors which resuited from the use talk or transition activities which are not inap-
of peer observation systems, the Direct irstruc- propriate, but clearly not related to the lesson
tion Observation System (DIOS) (Englert & content (e.g., &dquo;When can we play the game?&dquo;).
Sugai, 1980) was developed. As shown in Figure
3, DIOS functioned as a narrative record that Observers
provided information on dependent variables re- Three observers trained to code DIOS us-
were
lated to teachertrainees’ behavior management ing audiotapes and videotapes of classrooms
and direct instruction behaviors, as well as similar to those used in the study. Two of the ob-
pupils’ academic and social behaviors. servers had no knowledge of the trainees’ group
The following types of data were collected for membership. Reliability measures were taken
teacher-trainees and pupils in the two study at the start of the study. Mean reliability of the
groups. observers was 93%, with a range from 89 to
95%. lnterobserver agreement scores greater
Teacher Trainee Behaviors. than 80% were judged to be acceptable.

1. Presentation of trial. Rate at which teacher-


Procedures
trainee presented new learning task or stimu-
As stated previously, teacher traineesin the
lus. In DIOS, each new task or stimulus signaled
study taken from two sections of a practi-
were
the next learning trial and was coded on a separ-
cum course in special education. These sec-
ate line.
tions were offered at different times of the day
2. Prompts following correct academic re-
but were taught by the same instructor. Both ex-
sponse. Percentage of correct or partially cor-
rect academic responses followed by a teacher
perimental and control group subjects received
similar course content to fulfill the objectives of
prompt (e.g., &dquo;Tell me more&dquo;; &dquo;Give me a sen- the course. Content covered during the semes-
tence using that word correctly&dquo;).
3. Reinforcement of correct academic re-
ter-long course included (a) behavior manage-
ment (i.e., use of rules, individual and group
sponses. Percentage of pupils’ correct aca-
demic responses followed by teacher reinforce-
contingency management, verbal/nonverbal
ment (e.g., &dquo;Great job&dquo;; &dquo;Good answer&dquo;).
management techniques, and reinforcement
4. Specific reinforcement. Rate at which
strategies), (b) planning instructional programs
teacher trainees praised specific academic or
(i.e., task analyses, lesson planning, and con-
social behaviors (e.g., &dquo;I really like the way you
cept analysis), (c) direct instruction (i.e., atten-
tion signals, model-lead-test formats, prompt-
corrected yourself&dquo;; &dquo;Thank you for raising your
ing and questioning techniques), and (d) evalua-
hand&dquo;). tion (i.e., daily measurement, record keeping,
5. Prompt following an incorrect response. Per-
and graphing student performance). In addition,
centage of incorrect student responses foliowed instructional emphasis in both classes was
by a teacher correction prompt (e.g., &dquo;That’s a placed on the importance of a brisk instruc-
short ’a’ sound&dquo;; &dquo;Remember our rules for bor-
tional pace, teacher reinforcement of correct
rowing ?&dquo;). academic and social responses, the mainten-
6. Tells answer. Percentage of incorrect aca-
ance of a high degree of pupil attention by mini-
demic responses followed by teacher telling the
correct answer (e.g., &dquo;That word is ’dog&dquo;’).
mizing disruptions to the learning task, and the
correction of incorrect student responses

Behaviors.
through teacher prompting.
Pupil Teacher trainees in both the experimental
1. Correct academic response. Percentage of and control groups were required to team up
correct pupil responses following a learning with one of their peers in the same practicum

11
Downloaded from tes.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 5, 2016
FIGURE 3
Direct instruction observation system (DIOS)

12
Downloaded from tes.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 5, 2016
section to complete five peer observations. sented to trainees as a measurement tool by
Each trainee served as observer for five lessons which they could evaluate their own teaching ef-
and subject for five lessons. Although the expe- fectiveness. It was explained that by targeting
rimental and control groups used different ob- their own acceleration and deceleration targets,
servation techniques, both groups focused on as well as their strategies for changing student
the same target skill areas, including (a) two ob- behavior, teacher trainees could get direct feed-
servations focusing on the peer’s behavior man- back from peer observers on their success in af-
agement techniques and (b) three observations fecting the target behaviors of concern. Teacher
focusing on the peer’s direct instruction tech- success was measured in terms of changes in
niques. Behavior management and direct in- levels of student behavior over time and in-
struction techniques were observed separately creases in the proportion of student responses
for reasons associated with greater efficiency correctly responded to by the trainee (e.g., a fol-
in teacher training and peer observation. In par- lowed by A, i followed by D). Although it was
ticular, since all subjects were novice teachers, originally intended that teacher trainees would
they did not have the automaticity or knowledge target social behaviors, a majority of the
necessary to selectively attend to and recognize trainees pinpointed academic behaviors for
all categories of behavior management and data collection, including pupil correct
direct instruction simultaneously. By narrowing responses (a), pupil incorrect responses (i),
the observation categories to a subset of teacher praise of correct responses (A), and
teaching skills, it was expected that trainee ob- teacher prompts or correction of errors (D).
servers could develop greater automaticity in For the observation of direction instruction
observing and generating the target behaviors behaviors, experimental trainees were in-
of concern. structed in the use of CASEthrough learning ac-
After the completion of each peer observa- tivities that gave them experience coding video-
tion, peer observers were directed to discuss tapes and audiotapes of teacher trainees from
their observations and comments with the ob- previous semesters. This provided experi-
served peer. Feedback typically focused on spe- mental trainees with direct experience in using
cific behavior management, instructional, or the observation system to code direct instruc-
feedback strategies for teaching a particular tion behaviors. Teacher trainees were told to
skill to the learner. In addition, at the completion use the observational data as a basis for deter-
of the required number of observations for the mining their ability to maintain student atten-
two targeted skill areas (i.e., two for behavior tion, high student accuracy, and correct student
management, three for direct instruction), peer responding through appropriate feedback pro-
observers gave the observed teacher trainee cedures involving reinforcement following cor-
their written comments and observational data. rect responses and correction prompts follow-
This information then was summarized in a brief ing incorrect student responses.
report written by the teacher trainee. The report Only one modification was made in the writ-
described their intructional methods and find- ten report requirement for experimental teacher
ings obtained by the peer observers. To mini- trainees. Instead of writing a detailed report
mize the likelihood of false reporting of trainee describing their strategies and results, greater
performance, grades were not assigned on the emphasis was placed on the summarization of
basis of these observations. Teacher trainees datathrough graphing of teacher-pupil data. Us-
were informed that the peer observations were ing the graph, trainees were encouraged to com-
strictly for their own benefit and would not influ- pare across the several days of observation to
ence their course grade. To ensure that observa- determine whether they effectively had
tion assignments were completed, a standard changed either trainee or pupil behaviors.
point value was assigned to each report.
Control Group. The control group received
Experimental Group. The experimental group similar instruction on the desired teacher and
completed two behavior management observa- pupil behaviors and on general instruction and
tions using the Dollar (1980) recording scheme. behavior management techniques. The same
Prior to its use, teacher trainees were instructed management techniques and categories of in-
in the use of behavior management strategies structional behaviors were discussed with both
to accelerate and decelerate target pupil behav- groups. time, however, was spent in dis-
iors. The observation scheme than was pre- cussion of instructional techniques with control

13

Downloaded from tes.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 5, 2016


group subjects and less time was spent on the levels of inappropriate student behavior and
direct observation and coding of observational lesson noise (i.e., transition time). A two-factor
categories. analysis of variance (ANOVA) for inappropriate
Unlike the experimental group, specific per- behavior and lesson noise, however, showed no
formance pinpoints and observation instru- significant main effects or interactions. As
ments were not provided to control trainees. shown in Table 1, inappropriate behavior and
Peer observers were required to develop their lesson noise generally occurred at low levels for
own data-collection and information-sharing both groups, although experimental teacher
procedures. Using the observational data and trainees maintained a slightly lower rate of
feedback from their peer observers, trainees lesson noise during lessons.
were directed to evaluate their success in af-
fecting pupil behavior through the use of instructional Manageroent Skills
behavior management or direct instruction The instructional skills of the two groups also
techniques. were examined in terms of trainees’ ability to
maintain a high degree of pupil accuracy during
direct instruction. The results of the ANOVA
RESULTS showed that experimental teacher trainees
maintained a higher degree of pupil accuracy
Three major types of data were collected and than control trainees, F(1,36) = 3.92, p < .05,
analyzed: (a) behavior/lesson management, (b) with proportionately fewer pupil errors, F(1,36)
instructional management skills, and (c) feed- =
6.01, p < .05. Where experimental teacher
back strategies. These data were collected by trainees maintained pupil accuracy at 81 °lo,
continuous event recording procedures using control teacher trainees maintained pupil ac-
DIOS (see Figure 1). Sincethe datayielded abso- curacy at only 73%. The occurrence of a statisti-
lute frequency counts which were quantified as cally significant main effect for time, F(1,36) =
rates and percentage of occurrence, these data 7.48, p < .01, showed that both groups of
were analyzed in a 2 (group) x 2 (trial) analysis teacher trainees demonstrated an increased
of variance for each of the respective teacher ability to maintain higher levels of pupil accur-
and pupil categories of DIOS. acy across time (T1 M = 76.9, T2M = 78.6).
An additional measure of trainees’ manage-
Behavior ~~~a~~ em~nt Skills ment of pupil accuracy was analyzed; that is,
Two measures of trainees’ behavior manage- their provision of frequent, successful response
ment skills were their ability to maintain low opportunities per minute (correct rate). The
TABLE 1
Means and standard deviations for DIOS

14
Downloaded from tes.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 5, 2016
ANOVA for correct rates revealed a significant As expected, experimental teacher trainees
main effect for group, F(1,34) = 4.37, p < .05. who were provided with specific teacher/pupil
Experimental teacher trainees maintained a data differed in several respects from control
higher correct rate than control teacher trainees teacher trainees. First, experimental teacher
(3.3 compared to 2.3 per minute). Test for the sig- trainees maintained a higher level of pupil ac-
nificance of time and the interaction of time and curacy during direct instruction. Although both
group were not significant. groups established their own instructional objec-
tives and were involved in the direct instruction
Feedback Strategies of those objectives, only the trainees given spe-
A third aspect of group performance was teach- cific data and feedback achieved higher aca-
ers’ feedback strategies (e.g., prompts, rein- demic accuracy. Thus, teacher trainees who
forcement, telling correct answer) following were provided with specific performance data
either a correct or incorrect pupil response. The were better able to monitor and manage pupils’
results of the ANOVA showed that the groups achievement of stated academic outcomes.
were differentiated by two feedback strategies. The lower pupil performance levels of the
For teacher feedback following correct re- control trainees also suggested that novice
sponses, group differences were statistically teachers may have had difficulty monitoring
significant, F(1,36) = 5.17, p < .05. As shown student accuracy during direct instruction with-
in Table 1, control teacher trainees tended to out specific feedback. For the novice teachers,
prompt correct responses more often than ex- the instructional act itself might have required a
perimental teacher trainees. Moreover, a main great deal of attention, so that less attention
effect for time, F(1,36) = 6.72, p < .05, indi- was available for monitoring student re-
cated that both groups tended to prompt correct sponses. However, given specific feedback and
responses less often over time. Since no inter- data from peer observers, novice teachers were
action was found, these findings suggested better prepared to bridge the gap between the
that group differences remained relatively instructional presentation and the monitoring
stable across time, with both groups decreas- of its outcomes. In this way, specific feedback
ing their prompting at Time 2. allowed novice teachers to become increas-
Analysis of teacher trainees’ feedback strat- ingly accountable for monitoring, managing,
egies following incorrect pupil responses also and evaluating their own instructional effective-
showed differential prompting patterns. While ness.
both groups tended to prompt errors ratherthan A second finding was that experimental
tell correct answers, a significant group by trial teacher trainees maintained a brisker presenta-
interaction, F(1,36) = 5.17, p < .05, was found tion and correct ratethan teachertrainees in the
for telling correct answers. Comparison of the control group. By doing so, experimental teach-
group means at Time 1 and 2 showed that, er trainees not only maintained higher pupil ac-
where experimental trainees maintained a low curacy, they also provided more practice and
level of telling correct answers at both Time 1 rehearsal opportunities for student mastery of
and 2, control trainees initially demonstrated a concepts. In sum, they taught more in less time,
high level of telling correct answers at Time 1, a competence considered essential to direct in-
with a decreasing reliance upon this strategy struction methodology (Becker & Carnine,
across time. 1981).
Two other analyses were conducted for Third,teacher trainees in the experimental
teacher trainees’ reinforcement of correct re- group differed in terms of their feedback strat-
sponses and prompts of errors. However, no sig- egies for correct and incorrect student re-
nificant main effects or interactions were found sponses. Experimental teacher trainees tended
for either variable. to prompt correct responses less often than
control teacher trainees at both trials and were
less likely to tell answers following incorrect re-
DISCUSSION sponses. Since previous research has indicated
that both of these feedback techniques are cor-
Peer observation is one technique to facilitate related positively to student learning in low-
supervision and teacher training. This study ex- achieving populations (Brophy & Evertson,
amined the effects of two peer observation 1977; Medley, 1977), this finding favored the ex-
techniques on teacher trainee performance. perimental teacher trainees.

15

Downloaded from tes.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 5, 2016


No significant differences were found be- to trainees who were not provided with such in-
tween the behavior management skills of the struments.
two groups. However, inappropriate student be- in summary, peer observation was found to
havior occurred at low levels for both groups. be a useful technique for extending the teacher-
This finding supports previous research which training and supervision experience. With the
indicates that students demonstrate low levels increasing restrictions on supervisory person-
of off-task behavior and misbehavior during nel, peer observation should be considered seri-
direct instruction (Berliner & Rosenshine, 1977; ously as a means of providing continuous feed-
Rosenshine, 1980). In the present study, teacher back to teacher-trainees for the purposes of
trainees who were involved in structured direct trainee self-monitoring, self-recording, and self-
instruction with a single student or a small evaluation. The results of this study suggest
group of students maintained relatively high that observation systems provide the most use-
rates of socially correct responding. Thus the ful format for structuring the peer observation
necessity to establish intervention programs experience. Moreover, interviews with the
for inappropriate behavior was reduced; conse- teacher-trainees confirmed the value of peer ob-
quently, differential patterns of student inap- servation. The trainees in the study found it an
propriate behavior and teacher trainees’ man- extremely valuable technique and recommended
agement strategies did not emerge. its continuation in subsequent semesters.
A question which must be addressed to ex- A determination that can not be made in this
plain these findings is &dquo;What factors accounted study is the relative contribution that each of
for the differential instructional behaviors of the the following variables had on the overall ef-
two groups of trainees?&dquo; In response to this fects observed: (a) training in the categories of
question, it is important to note that peer obser- the observation system, (b) participation in
vation alone cannot explain the differences be- coding peer behaviors using observational cate-
tween experimental and control trainees since gories, and (c)the provision of specific feedback
peer observers were used by both groups. Fur- relative to observation categories from peers.
thermore, both groups were given simiiar obser- Future investigations should focus on the
vation assignments and feedback based on the nature of the relationships and interactions
same categories of teacher/pupil behavior. This among these variables.
procedure assured that differential patterns of
instruction would not result because of the
trainees’ perceived importance of categories
associated with the observation instruments
themselves. REFERENCES
The major differences between the two
groups, however, were the nature of the obser- Allilunas, L.J. Changes ahead for cooperating teach-
ers. Teacher Educator, 1972, 8
, 13-17.
vation instruments and the type of data each
provided the teacher-trainee. During observa- Becker, W.C., & Carnine, D.W. Direct instruction: A be-
tion trials, the experimental teacher trainees havior theory model for comprehensive educa-
tional intervention with the disadvantaged. In
were given observation systems with categor-
S.W. Bijou & R. Ruiz (Eds.), Behavior modifica-
ies of pupil social behaviors (i.e., appropriate/in- tion: Contributions to education
. Hillsdale, N.J.:
appropriate) and of teacher trainee responses Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1981.
(i.e., feedback strategies). Consequently, exper- Berliner, D.C., & Rosenshine, B.V. The acquisition of
imental trainees specifically attended to, knowledge in the classroom. In R.C. Anderson,
modeled, and received performance data re- R.J. Spiro, & W.E. Montague (Eds.), Schooling
and the acquisition of knowledge
. Hillsdale, N.J.:
lated to the behaviors designated by the cate-
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1977.
gories of the observation instrument. The con-
trol group did not have these structured obser- Brophy, J.E., & Evertson, C.M. Teacher behavior and
student learning in second and third grades. In
vation instruments, and instead developed data G.D. Borich (Ed.), The appraisal of teaching: Con-
collection systems that were narrative records cepts and process
. Reading, Mass.: Addison-
and anecdotal descriptions. Thus, it seems spe- Wesley, 1977.
cific observation instruments used within a Cumming, J. Back to the sticks for student teaching.
peer observation framework increased the like- Journal of Teacher Education
, 1971, , 294-302.
22
lihood that certain desirable pupils behaviors Dollar, S. Behavior management observation form.
were observed and maintained, when compared Lexington: University of Kentucky, 1980.

16
Downloaded from tes.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 5, 2016
Ellis, E.C., Smith, J.T., & Abbott, W.H. Peer observa- Semmel, M.E., & Englert, C.S. A decision-making ori-
tion: A means for supervisory acceptance. Edu- entation applied to student teaching supervi-
cational Leadership
, 1979, , 423-426.
36 sion. Teacher Education and Special Education
,
Englert. C.S. & Sugai, G.M. The direct instruction ob- 1978, 1(2), 28-36.
servation system: DIOS
. Lexington: University of Semmel, M.I., & Thiagarajan, J. Observation systems
Kentucky, 1981. and special education teachers. Focus on Ex-
Medley, D.M. Teacher competence and teacher effec- ceptional Children
, 1973, , 1-12.
5
tiveness : A review of process-product research. Skoog, G. Improving college teaching through peer
Washington, D.C.: American Association of Col- observation. Journal of ,
Teacher Education 1980,
leges for Teacher Education, 1977. , 23-25.
31
Merrill, E.C., & Schuchman, B.J. Professional student Tawney, J.W. C.A.S.E.: Comprehensive analysis of
teaching programs Danville, III.: Interstate special education
. Lexington: University of Ken-
Printers and Publishers, 1973. tucky, May, 1979.
Rosenshine, B. How time is spent in elementary class- Walker, J.A. The practicum supervision inches toward
rooms. In C. Denham & A. Lieberman (Eds.), Time competence: Preliminary thoughts on a process.
to learn
. Washington, D.C.: National Institute of Teacher Education and Special Education 1978,
Education, 1980. (2), 14-27.
1

17

Downloaded from tes.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on March 5, 2016

You might also like