Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

This article was downloaded by: [University of Massachusetts, Amherst]

On: 10 August 2012, At: 09:12


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41
Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

The Journal of Educational Research


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vjer20

Teaching Young Students Strategies for Planning and Drafting


Stories: The Impact of Self-Regulated Strategy Development
a a b
Brenda Tracy , Robert Reid & Steve Graham
a
University of Nebraska
b
Vanderbilt University

Version of record first published: 07 Aug 2010

To cite this article: Brenda Tracy, Robert Reid & Steve Graham (2009): Teaching Young Students Strategies for Planning and Drafting
Stories: The Impact of Self-Regulated Strategy Development, The Journal of Educational Research, 102:5, 323-332

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.3200/JOER.102.5.323-332

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction,
redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly
forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be
complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should be independently
verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand, or
costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the
use of this material.
Teaching Young Students Strategies
for Planning and Drafting Stories:
The Impact of Self-Regulated
Strategy Development
BRENDA TRACY STEVE GRAHAM
ROBERT REID Vanderbilt University
University of Nebraska
Downloaded by [University of Massachusetts, Amherst] at 09:12 10 August 2012

ABSTRACT. In the present study, participants were 127 well are at a considerable disadvantage. Their grades are
3rd-grade students, to 64 of whom (33 boys, 31 girls) the likely to suffer, particularly in classes where writing (e.g.,
authors taught a general strategy and a genre-specific strategy through tests, reports) is the primary means for assessing stu-
for planning and writing stories; procedures for regulating the dents’ knowledge (Graham, 2006a). They are less likely than
use of these strategies, the writing process, and their writing
behaviors; and knowledge about the basic purpose and char- their more skilled classmates to use writing to support and
acteristics of good stories. The other 63 3rd-grade students extend learning (see Graham & Perin, 2007a). They are also
(30 boys, 33 girls) formed the comparison group and received less likely to attend college, as writing is now commonly used
traditional-skills writing instruction (mostly on spelling, to evaluate applicants’ qualifications. Beyond school, writ-
grammar, and so forth). Strategy-instructed students wrote ing has increasingly become a gateway for employment and
stories that were longer, schematically stronger, and qualita-
tively better. Strategy-instructed students maintained over a promotion, particularly in salaried positions (see reports by
short period of time the gains that they had made from pretest the NCW, 2004, 2005), because it is expected that workers
to posttest. In addition, the impact of story-writing strategy can create clearly written documents, memoranda, technical
instruction transferred to writing a similar but untaught genre, reports, and electronic messages. Moreover, participation in
that of a narrative about a personal experience. Strategy- civic life and the community at large is influenced by writ-
instructed students wrote longer, schematically stronger, and
qualitatively better personal narratives than did children in ing, especially because the use of e-mail and text messaging
the control condition. has become so widespread.
Although it is important that the writing needs of
Keywords: composition, strategy instruction, writing students at all grade levels be addressed, it is especially
important to address them in the primary grades. There
is a growing consensus that waiting until higher grades to

C
address literacy difficulties that begin in early elementary
urrently, there is considerable concern about the school is not particularly successful (Slavin, Madden, &
writing capabilities of students in the United Karweit, 1989). Providing effective writing instruction
States. For example, the National Commission on to young children should reduce the number of students
Writing (NCW; 2003), an organization representing more who fail to develop the writing skills needed to fully meet
than 4,300 colleges, claimed that the writing of school- classroom demands in higher grades.
aged children “is not what it should be” (p. 7). Concerns One promising approach to enhancing the writing skills
such as these are fueled by actual assessments of children’s of students is to directly teach them strategies for carrying
writing as well as estimates of how well students write. For out basic writing processes such as planning, drafting, and
instance, findings from the most recent National Assess- revising. In Graham and Perin’s (2007a, 2007b) recent
ment of Educational Progress (NAEP; Persky, Daane, & meta-analysis of the writing intervention literature with
Jin, 2003) showed that two thirds or more of students in students in Grades 4–12, explicit and systematic strategy
Grades 4, 8, and 12 demonstrated only partial mastery of instruction had a strong impact on improving the overall
the writing skills needed at their respective grade levels. quality of students’ papers. Students’ writing quality was
The results from the previous NAEP evaluation yielded enhanced in each of the 20 writing-strategy studies included
similar findings (Greenwald, Persky, Ambell, & Mazzeo, in the meta-analysis, yielding an average weighted effect
1999). Likewise, college teachers estimated that 50% of
high school graduates are not prepared for college-level
Address correspondence to Steve Graham, Vanderbilt University,
writing demands (Achieve, Inc., 2005). P.O. Box 228, Nashville, TN 37023, USA. (E-mail: steve
It is unfortunate that so many children do not develop .graham@vanderbilt.edu)
stronger writing skills. Students who do not learn to write Copyright © 2009 Heldref Publications
323
324 The Journal of Educational Research

size of 0.82. Such instruction enhanced the writing of strug- plan, and use and upgrade a plan when writing. To help
gling writers (average weighted effect size = 1.02) as well them organize possible ideas into a writing plan, they were
as the writing of students in typical classrooms (average also taught story-specific planning strategies. This involved
weighted effect size = 0.70). In 8 of the 20 studies, writing asking themselves a series of questions, with each question
strategies were taught using a specific model, Self-Regulated focusing on a particular element commonly found in stories
Strategy Development (SRSD; Harris & Graham, 1996). (Stein & Glenn, 1979), such as “Who are the main charac-
These SRSD studies were especially effective, yielding an ters?” and “What do they want to do?” Students addition-
average weighted effect size of 1.14 (compared with 0.62 ally learned about the characteristics and basic parts of a
for the other 12 studies combined). This approach was also story, which provided them with knowledge essential to
highly effective in improving the writing performance of using the aforementioned strategies. Students were further
students in Grades 2–11 in a meta-analysis of 27 single- taught self-regulator procedures, including how to use self-
subject-design SRSD studies (Rogers & Graham, 2008). talk to facilitate performance, set goals to write complete
With SRSD, students are explicitly taught writing strate- papers (i.e., ones that included all of the basic elements of a
gies for accomplishing specific writing tasks, such as com- story), monitor and graph their personal success in achiev-
posing a story or persuasive essay (Harris & Graham, 1996, ing these goals, compare their preinstructional performance
Downloaded by [University of Massachusetts, Amherst] at 09:12 10 August 2012

1999). The strategies typically focus on planning, drafting, with their performance during instruction, and credit their
revising, editing, or some combination of those processes. success to effort and the use of the target strategies. Teach-
Students are further taught any skills or knowledge (e.g., the ers further emphasized the importance of mastering these
attributes and elements of a good story) needed to apply the strategies and applying them independently. Mastery goals,
inculcated strategies effectively. Students also learn to use evaluation of personal growth, self-reflection, development
one or more self-regulation procedures (e.g., self-instruction, of autonomy, and attributional practices such as these are
goal setting, self-monitoring, self-reinforcement) to enhance thought to enhance students’ effort and motivation (Ames,
motivation and regulate their use of the target strategies, the 1992; Schunk & Zimmerman, 1998; Urdan, 1997).
writing task, and their behavior during writing. The focus of On the basis of previous experimental research with
this instruction is on students’ independent, effective, and older typically developing students (see Graham & Perin,
flexible use of the target strategies. Consequently, procedures 2007a, 2007b) as well as single-subject-design studies (see
for promoting maintenance and generalization are embed- Rogers & Graham, 2008) and two previous experimental
ded throughout the instructional regime. This approach is studies (Graham et al., 2005; Harris et al., 2006) with strug-
consistent with theories on how competence and expertise gling second- and third-grade students, we anticipated that
develop in subject-matter domains (see Alexander, 1992; SRSD story-writing instruction would enhance the sche-
Graham, 2006a; Pintrich & Schunk, 1996). These con- matic structure, quality, and length of students’ composi-
ceptualizations emphasize that learning depends, in large tions. Such effects have been consistently found in previous
part, on changes that occur in strategic knowledge, domain- research. Further, we expected that writing gains would be
specific knowledge, and motivation. maintained over time for SRSD-instructed students, because
Although there are over 40 studies (experimental and the effects of such instruction have been found to be durable
single-subject-design investigations) examining the effec- over time (see Graham, 2006b). Last, we hypothesized that
tiveness of SRSD, its effectiveness with students in typical teaching students strategies for planning and drafting stories
primary-grade classrooms has not been assessed. However, would generalize to a similar genre, personal narratives (a
7 of the 27 single-subject-design SRSD studies investigated story about one’s friends or one’s self). Such transfer has
the effectiveness of teaching planning and drafting strategies occurred in prior SRSD studies with young struggling writers
for either story or persuasive writing to struggling writers in (Graham et al.; Harris et al.). However, it is important to
Grades 2 or 3 (Rogers & Graham, 2008). The writing of each note that we did not expect the effect sizes for the students
student participating in these studies improved. In addition, in this study to be as large as those obtained with struggling
two experimental studies have examined the effectiveness of primary-grade writers in studies by Graham et al. and Har-
SRSD with struggling writers in Grades 2 (Harris, Graham, ris et al. Although Graham and Perin (2007a, 2007b) have
& Mason, 2006) and 3 (Graham, Harris, & Mason, 2005). found that strategy instruction benefited struggling writers
SRSD students made considerably greater progress than did and classrooms composed of typical writers, it yielded larger
control students (who received process-writing instruction), effects for the former.
as effect sizes for schematic structure, quality, and length of
compositions were all above 0.80 following instruction. Method
In the present study, we examined the effectiveness of
SRSD instruction in improving the story writing of students Setting
in regular third-grade classrooms. Students were taught a
general strategy that emphasized planning. This strategy The present study was conducted during the spring semes-
reminded them to carry out three basic processes: select a ter of 2006 in a rural elementary school in the Midwestern
topic to write about, organize possible ideas into a writing United States. The demographics of the school follow: 96%
May/June 2009 [Vol. 102(No. 5)] 325

of students were Caucasian, 1% as African American, 2% (r = .89). In the present study, students’ writing perfor-
were Hispanic, and 1% as Asian. A total of 8% of the stu- mance on the test was in the normal range (see Table 1),
dents received free or reduced-price lunch. All instruction and there was no statistically significant difference between
took place in the general education classroom. SRSD and control students (p = .51).
We also obtained scores from students’ files for a reading
Participants test and cognitive test administered by the school to groups
of students. The reading measure was from the Terra Nova
In the present study, participants were 127 children California Achievement Test (2nd ed.; CTB/McGraw-
from six third-grade classrooms. All of these students had Hill, 2008). The cognitive measure was taken from InView
completed both the story writing pre- and posttests. The (CTB/McGraw-Hill, 2006), a test that measures verbal,
7 children who had not completed the posttest were not nonverbal, and quantitative reasoning. The Cognitive
included in the analyses (3 students from the treatment Skills Index, a score derived from this test, provided a
group and 4 students from the control group). They did measure of academic aptitude. As a group, students scored
not differ in any meaningful way at the start of the study in the normal range on each of these tests (see Table 1).
(writing, reading, or cognitive performance) from the other Furthermore, there were no statistically significant differ-
Downloaded by [University of Massachusetts, Amherst] at 09:12 10 August 2012

participants. Three classrooms were randomly assigned to ences between SRSD (treatment) and control students
the SRSD treatment condition (n = 64), and the other on these two tests, nor was there a statistically significant
three classrooms served as controls (n = 63). Table 1 pres- difference between groups in terms of chronological age
ents participant characteristics. As can be seen, there was (all ps > .58).
almost an equal number of boys and girls in each condition,
and few students received special education services or a Assessments and Measures
free or reduced-price lunch.
To establish participating students’ initial writing level, Writing prompts. Immediately before (pretest) and after
we administered all students the Story Construction Sub- (posttest) SRSD instruction, all students completed a story
test from the Test of Written Language (3rd ed.; TOWL-3; and personal narrative writing probe. The personal narrative
Hammill & Larsen, 1996). This subtest measures students’ writing assignments provided a near transfer measure, as
ability to write a complete and interesting story by examin- SRSD students were not taught to apply the targeted strate-
ing if specific thematic elements are included in the com- gies to this genre, but the assignment was similar to story
position. Students are provided with a picture and given writing and involved writing about a real event that the
15 min to write a story to go with it. Evidence summarized child experienced. At 2 weeks following the end of instruc-
in the test manual supports that this test reliably differ- tion, students in the SRSD (treatment) condition were
entiates between students with strong and weak writing administered another story-writing prompt (maintenance).
skills, is moderately correlated to other measures of writing We did not administer the maintenance writing assignment
performance, and is reliable with primary-grade students to students in the control condition because the school had

TABLE 1. Participant Characteristics

SRSD group (n = 64) Control group (n = 62)


Variable n M SD n M SD

Age 9.10 0.24 9.92 0.10


Gender
Female 31 33
Male 33 30
Free or reduced lunch
Yes 6 5
No 58 58
Students with disabilities 4 6
TOWL-3 Story Construction
standard score 9.67 2.53 10.00 2.35
Terra Nova Reading 58.64 14.12 58.58 11.80
In View Cognitive Skills Index 106.56 12.52 106.35 11.97

Note. SRSD = self-required strategy development; TOWL-3 = Test of Written Language–Third Edition
(S. Hammill & D. Larsen, 1996; M = 10, SD = 3); Terra Nova Reading (M = 50, SD = 21.06); InView
Cognitive Science Index (M = 100, SD = 15). For age, units of M and SD are years.
326 The Journal of Educational Research

asked that students in the control condition receive the awarded if the ending was unique). The total number of
SRSD intervention once posttest measures were collected. possible points for this scale was 20. Both raters received
The story-writing prompts were line-drawn pictures (i.e., extensive training in using the scale (interrater scoring
a boy swinging through the jungle on a vine, a girl looking reliability during training was .86). One rater scored
at a giant egg that is cracking open, and two people look- all compositions, and another scored a random sample
ing out the window of their house at a tiny flying saucer of 25% of all papers to establish reliability. Interrater
that has landed on their lawn). The two narrative prompts reliability between the two scorers was .80.
asked students to write a paper about a time when they We assessed overall quality using a holistic measure (the
were younger or a time when they had fun. Earlier investi- most common procedure for scoring writing quality; see
gations have shown that these story-writing and narrative Graham & Perin, 2007b). We scored stories and personal
prompts are equivalent in terms of how much and how well narratives separately. With the holistic scoring method,
primary-grade students write (Graham et al., 2005; Harris et raters read each paper attentively, but not laboriously, to
al., 2006). Furthermore, we judged each of the prompts used obtain a general impression of overall writing quality. They
in this study as suitable for use with third-grade students by were told that ideation, organization, sentence structure,
teachers and children at this grade level (Graham et al.). aptness of word choice, and grammar should all be taken
Downloaded by [University of Massachusetts, Amherst] at 09:12 10 August 2012

The three story prompts were randomly assigned to pre- into account when forming a judgment about the writing
test, posttest, and maintenance, whereas the two narrative quality, but no single factor should receive undue weight.
prompts were randomly assigned to pretest and posttest. Papers were scored using a 7-point scale, with higher scores
Identical procedures were used to administer the story- (e.g., 7) representing higher quality writing and lower scores
writing and narrative prompts at each assessment point (e.g., 1) representing poorer quality. For each genre (stories
(pretest, posttest, and maintenance). Students were given and personal narratives), we provided raters with a repre-
as much time as they needed to complete a story, encour- sentative paper (or anchor point) for each score of 2, 4, and
aged to plan in advance of writing, and told that they could 6. They used these anchor points to help them score papers.
only ask for help to spell a word. The prompts for the per- We developed the anchor points in prior investigations (see
sonal narrative writing assignments were read to students. Graham et al., 2005), and they capture the range of writing
Writing measures. We scored each composition for number performance in third-grade classes. Writing quality for each
of story parts (schematic structure), number of words, and genre was scored by two former primary school teachers who
overall writing quality. We scored personal narratives for were unfamiliar with the design and intent of the study. The
story parts, as the writing assignment asked students to tell raters received extensive training in how to accurately and
a story from their life. Prior to scoring, we typed each paper reliably use the holistic scoring system (interrater reliability
and saved it as a Word document. We removed identifying during training was .88 for stories and .87 for personal narra-
information and corrected spelling, capitalization, and punc- tives). Each rater independently scored all papers produced
tuation in each paper to minimize any bias that may occur by students, and interrater reliability was .73 for stories and
when examiners scored papers. The appearance of text- or .80 for personal narratives. The average score of the two
surface-level features, such as spelling miscues, can influence raters was used when analyzing data.
judgments about writing quality (see Graham, 1999). Treatment fidelity. To ensure that SRSD conditions were
We scored the number of words for each paper, using delivered as planned, we implemented the following steps.
the statistics properties in Word. In contrast, two human The teacher had a lesson plan that contained step-by-step
raters, who were previously elementary school teachers procedures for each lesson. Intensive instruction on how to
and unfamiliar with the design and purpose of the study, deliver the SRSD program was provided before the start of
scored papers using a scale devised by Graham and Har- the study. As each step was completed during instruction,
ris (1989). Each paper was examined to determine if it the teacher was asked to check off that step on the plan.
contained eight basic story elements (Stein & Glenn, Examinations of all lesson plans showed that 98% of steps in
1979): (a) main character(s), (b) time, (c) locale, (d) all lessons were completed. In addition, 20% of the instruc-
precipitating event that caused character(s) to act, (e) tional sessions were also observed by a graduate student who
what the main character(s) wants (want) to do (goals), had extensive experience in implementing the SRSD story-
(f) action to achieve goal(s), (g) consequence of actions, writing intervention. The observer used a checklist identical
and (h) characters’ reactions. We assigned a score of 0 if to the one the teacher had and checked off steps as they were
a part was not present. If a part was present, we assigned completed. This independent analysis showed that 99% of
a score of 1 or 2 depending on how well the writer devel- the steps in the lessons were implemented. Thus, the experi-
oped the specific element. The only exceptions involved mental condition was implemented as intended.
scores for three parts: goals (an extra point was added if
there were multiple goals that were clearly articulated), Instructional Procedures
action (an extra point was added if there was more than
one episode or if students were ingenious in describing the SRSD condition. With SRSD (Harris & Graham, 1996,
action of characters), and ending (an additional point was 1999), students are directly and systematically taught how
May/June 2009 [Vol. 102(No. 5)] 327

to use specific strategies to accomplish particular academic then asked the question, “What is a good story?” In discuss-
tasks (in this case, planning and drafting a story). This ing what makes a good story, the teacher emphasized that
includes teaching students the knowledge needed to apply a good story has many characteristics, and students should
the targeted strategies (in the present study, this involved remember that a good story (a) makes sense, (b) is fun to
knowledge about the characteristics and parts of a good write and read, (c) uses interesting vocabulary or “million
story) as well as self-regulatory procedures (in this case, dollar” words, and (d) includes all seven story parts. To help
goal setting, self-monitoring, and self-speech) that help students remember these seven parts, a mnemonic device
them regulate the use of the strategies, the writing process, (WWW; 2 What and 2 How questions) was introduced as
and their behaviors. Motivation is advanced through a a trick for remembering them. Students then listened as the
variety of procedures, including emphasizing the role of teacher read a story, with the children helping the teacher
effort in learning, making the positive effects of instruction identify each of the seven parts. As students identified and
concrete and visible (through self-monitoring and graph- described each part, the teacher wrote it in the appropriate
ing), and promoting an “I can do” attitude. section of a chart with the story part reminder. This contin-
SRSD instruction emphasizes that the primary goal of ued with additional stories until students could identify all
instruction is students’ independent use of the strategies and parts accurately. Last, the term transfer was introduced to
Downloaded by [University of Massachusetts, Amherst] at 09:12 10 August 2012

accompanying self-regulation procedures (Graham, Harris, explain how a strategy could be used in other places or situ-
& Reid, 1992). Students learn when, where, and how to ations. Students were asked to identify where they could
apply the strategies, and instruction is scaffolded so that use these types of strategies and set a goal to use what had
teachers initially take the lead in helping students apply been learned before the next session. A few minutes during
the strategies, but the responsibility is gradually shifted each succeeding lesson were spent rehearsing POW and the
to students as quickly as possible. Students are viewed as story part mnemonic device, as well as what they stood for,
active and contributing participants in the learning pro- until students memorized them.
cess, and instruction is criterion based rather than time In the second stage of instruction, discuss it, students
based. Students move through each instructional stage further considered the rationale for using the strategy.
at their own pace and do not move to the next stage of Self-monitoring procedures were introduced as well. Each
instruction until they have demonstrated adequate mastery. child analyzed their baseline story to determine how many
Students receive instruction until they can use what they of the seven story parts were included in the story (i.e.,
have been taught to independently write a story with all of self-monitoring). Students then graphed the number of
the basic parts. parts in their story by coloring the corresponding number
Students in the SRSD condition were first taught a of segments on a rocket ship with seven segments. Next,
general planning strategy represented by the mnemonic teacher and students discussed which parts were or were
device POW: Pick my idea (i.e., decide what to write about), not included. The teacher established that the students’
Organize my notes (i.e., organize possible writing ideas into goal was to include all seven parts when writing a story and
a writing plan), and Write and say more (i.e., make changes emphasized that even if a story part was included, it could
and increase the amount that is written). To help them be improved (e.g., adding additional detail). Most impor-
carry out the second step of POW (organize my notes), they tantly, the teacher and students discussed how using POW
were also taught a genre-specific strategy that prompted and the WWW mnemonic devices could improve story
them to generate ideas for each of the basic parts of a story. writing. At the end of the session, the teacher asked the
In using this strategy, students ask themselves the follow- students to identify how they had used some aspect of what
ing questions before writing (represented by the mnemonic they had learned since the previous session. The responses
device WWW; 2 What and 2 How questions): Who are the were discussed and written on a chart. The students then
main characters? When does the story take place? Where set a new goal to use what they had learned outside of the
does the story take place? What do the main characters instructional setting. This process of identifying instances
do or want to do? What happens next? How do the main of transfer, discussion, and goal setting continued in all
characters feel? How does that story end? For each ques- subsequent sessions.
tion, they generate notes on possible ideas that they may In the third stage of instruction, model it, the teacher
use in their story. We detail the procedures for teaching showed students how to apply the strategies and introduced
these strategies below, emphasizing each stage of the SRSD the concept of self-instruction. Before writing a story, the
model (Harris & Graham, 1996, 1999). teacher discussed with students that the goal of writing a
In the first stage of instruction, develop background knowl- story is to have it make sense, use “million dollar” words,
edge, students acquired the knowledge and skills needed to make it fun to write, and include all seven parts. The teach-
apply the planning and writing strategies. First, POW and er then modeled, speaking out loud, how to plan and write
its steps were introduced, and the teacher and students dis- a story using POW and the story parts reminder (WWW).
cussed what it stood for and why each step was important. The students helped the teacher by generating ideas for
Before moving to the next activity, students explained the the parts of the story as well as additional ideas while writ-
three steps and the importance of each step. Students were ing it. They recorded their notes for the story on a graphic
328 The Journal of Educational Research

organizer that included a prompt for each part of the WWW program (taken from a form developed by Agate & Graham
mnemonic device. While modeling, the teacher used a [2007] assessing commonly recommended writing practices
variety of self-statements to assist with problem definition at the primary-grade level). The most common activities
(e.g., What do I have to do here?), planning (e.g., What during writing time involved teaching basic writing skills;
comes next?), self-evaluation (e.g., Does that make sense?), control teachers taught spelling, grammar, punctuation,
self-reinforcement (e.g., I really like that part!), and coping and capitalization several times per week, with sentence-
(e.g., I’m almost finished!). Once the story was completed, constructions skills being taught at least weekly. Much less
the importance of what one says to oneself was discussed, emphasis was placed on teaching writing processes because
and the types of self-statements used by the teacher were teachers only taught planning, text organization, and revis-
identified. Students identified at least three self-statements ing strategies ranging from several times a month to several
that they would use while writing and recorded them on a times a year (none of the teachers taught the strategies or
small chart. The teacher and students also verified that all self-regulation procedures that we used with students in the
seven elements were included in the story, highlighting each SRSD condition). Having students plan and revise compo-
element and graphing the results. The teacher and students sitions was infrequent, with this occurring monthly or less
further discussed why it was important to write papers that with two of the teachers and several times a month with
Downloaded by [University of Massachusetts, Amherst] at 09:12 10 August 2012

contained enough detail, and they counted and graphed the the other teacher. Teachers rarely (several times a year) con-
number of words in the story. The teacher praised students ferred with students about their writing; had students share
for their hard work and effort; such praise was delivered their writing with others, read their own writing to students,
where appropriate in all preceding lessons. help each other with writing (except for one teacher, who
The next stage, support it, started with a collaborative did this weekly); monitored students’ progress; or established
writing experience. First, the teacher and students set a goal connections with the home concerning students’ writing.
to include all seven parts in the story. Second, they planned
the story together using POW, the story parts reminder, the Results
graphic organizer, and students’ self-statements. However,
this time students directed the process, and the teacher only We analyzed data for stories and personal narratives
provided support as needed. Third, using the collaboratively separately. For each genre, we computed a separate two-
generated notes, students wrote their own story. Fourth, way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) for each measure
after the story was completed, the student identified each (quality, story parts, and length). The independent variable
story part by highlighting it. Then each student determined was treatment condition (SRSD vs. control), the depen-
if he or she had met the goal and graphed the results. They dent variable was students’ posttest performance, and the
also graphed the number of words written. Last, the teacher covariate was students’ pretest performance. To determine
and students discussed how the strategies helped to write a if SRSD students’ story performance differed from their
better story. pre- and posttest performances, we computed a one-way
In subsequent sessions, students were gradually weaned repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) for each
from relying on the planning graphic organizer. The teacher measure. The repeated measure was SRSD students’ per-
explained that the graphic organizer was helpful but would formance at pretest, posttest, and maintenance. Table 2
not always be readily accessible when the child wanted to contains means and standard deviations by condition for
write a story. Students were taught to write the story part quality, parts, and length of stories and narratives.
reminder at the top of the page to assist them in planning
and writing a complete story. Students continued to set a Stories
goal of including all seven parts and graphed their success
in doing so as well as the number of words written. For all After controlling for initial pretest performance, SRSD-
of the papers written during this stage, the teacher provided instructed students wrote stories at posttest that were
support and encouragement as needed, but the level of qualitatively better, F(1, 124) = 20.60, MSE = 1.522, p =
assistance was less. .037; received higher scores on the story parts scale, F(1,
The final stage, independent performance, was reached 124) = 17.19, MSE = 3.95, p < .001; and were longer than
when students could successfully write a story with all those written by children in the control condition, F(1,
seven story elements independently without assistance 124) = 10.15, MSE = 49,368.02, p = .002. The obtained
from the teacher. They continued to set a goal to include differences for story parts and number of words written
all seven parts of the story and increase the amount of were moderate in magnitude (Cohen’s d effect sizes were
words that were written in their stories, and they graphed 0.71 and 0.55, respectively), whereas the impact of SRSD
their progress. on the quality of students’ story writing was smaller (effect
Control condition. The three teachers in the control con- size = 0.35).
dition primarily used a traditional basic-skills approach To determine if SRSD-instructed students maintained
to instruction. This was determined through observation the gains they made following treatment, we first ran an
and information that they completed about their writing omnibus test to determine if these students’ pretest, posttest,
May/June 2009 [Vol. 102(No. 5)] 329

and maintenance scores were statistically different. This


was the case for quality, F(1, 60) = 4.89, MSE = 2.23, p

M adj.

165.40 124.60 53.80 125.90

99.70
= .031; parts, F(1, 60) = 31.31, MSE = 5.16, p < .001;
and length, F(1, 60) = 11.89, MSE = 10,623.23, p = .001.
Control

117.60 55.17

84.60 43.70
95.50 67.00
Post hoc analyses revealed that there was no statistically
SD significant difference between posttest and maintenance
performances for any of the three measures and that
posttest and maintenance performances exceeded pretest
M
Length

performance for quality (both ps < .03), parts (both ps <

138.60
.001), and length (both ps < .002). Thus, 2 weeks follow-
M adj.

ing the end of instruction, SRSD students maintained the


TABLE 2. Means, Standard Deviations, and Adjusted Means for Story and Personal Narrative Quality, Parts, and Length, by Treatment Condition

improvements they had made in their story writing.


SRSD

124.90 57.70
166.70 86.50
174.60 99.30

101.70 54.20
142.80 79.70
SD

Personal Narratives
Downloaded by [University of Massachusetts, Amherst] at 09:12 10 August 2012

To determine if the impact of SRSD story-writing


M

instruction generalized to a similar but uninstructed writ-


M adj.

ing genre, we had students write a personal narrative before


8.34

7.90

and after instruction. After controlling for initial pretest


performance, SRSD-instructed students wrote personal
Control

narratives that were qualitatively better, F(1, 114) = 9.00,


2.34
2.40

2.66
2.34
SD

MSE = 0.74, p = .003; received higher scores in terms of


story parts, F(1, 114) = 15.53, MSE = 4.42, p < .001; and
7.92
8.27

7.66
7.91
M

were longer than those written by children in the control


Parts

condition, F(1, 114) = 8.58, MSE = 4,880.06, p = .004.


M adj.

The obtained differences for quality, story parts, and num-


9.81

9.45
Personal narrative

ber of words written were moderate in magnitude (effect


sizes were 0.53, 0.73, and 0.53, respectively). Thus, the
Stories
SRSD

2.26
1.71
1.87

2.47
1.91

impact of SRSD story-writing instruction generalized to


SD

the writing of personal narratives.


8.37
9.88
10.26

7.45
9.43
M

Discussion
M adj.

There is considerable evidence that students’ writing can


3.82

3.06

be improved by directly teaching the students strategies


Note. SRSD = self-regulated strategy development; M adj. = adjusted mean.

for planning, drafting, revising, and editing (Graham &


Control

1.46
1.42

1.13
1.13
SD

Perin, 2007a, 2007b; Rogers & Graham, 2007). However,


an important limitation of this evidence is that it mostly
applies to students in Grades 4 and higher. Strategy instruc-
3.62
3.79

3.04
3.04
M

tional studies conducted with students in the primary


Quality

grades have strictly involved students who were experi-


M adj.

encing difficulty in learning to write. We addressed this


4.29

3.55

limitation in the present study by examining the effec-


tiveness of such instruction with primary-grade students
SRSD

1.18
1.23
1.32

0.87
0.68
SD

in general. Specifically, third-grade students were taught


a previously validated strategy for planning and writing
stories (Graham et al., 2005; Harris et al., 2006; Sad-
3.79
4.31
4.26

3.17
3.57
M

dler, Moran, Graham, & Harris, 2004) using the SRSD


approach to strategy instruction (Harris & Graham, 1996,
1999). There is considerable evidence that this model of
strategy instruction is effective in teaching academic strat-
Maintenance

egies to a wide range of school-age children (see Graham,


Condition

2006b; Graham & Harris, 2003).


Posttest

Posttest
Pretest

Pretest

Third-grade students receiving SRSD instruction in this


study were taught a general strategy and a genre-specific
strategy for planning and writing stories; procedures for
330 The Journal of Educational Research

regulating the use of these strategies, the writing process, = 0.70). Thus, the smaller effect sizes obtained in the pres-
and their writing behaviors (self-monitoring, goal setting, ent study (compared with those done with young struggling
and self-instruction); and knowledge about the basic pur- writers) may in part reflect the differential effectiveness of
pose and characteristics of good stories. In comparison with this approach with different types of students.
students who received a traditional-skills-oriented writing Although effect sizes for story quality, parts, and length
program, SRSD-instructed students’ stories were longer, may have been smaller in this study versus SRSD writing
schematically stronger (in terms of inclusion and quality investigations with young struggling writers, this was not
of story parts), and qualitatively better. Furthermore, the the case for the near-generalization writing measure. In the
story-writing gains that SRSD students made from pretest present study and in prior studies with struggling writers in
to posttest were maintained over a short period of time the primary grades (Graham et al., 2005; Harris et al., 2006),
(i.e., 2 weeks). Just as importantly, SRSD students gener- the effect sizes were of similar magnitude for these measures,
alized what they learned to a similar but untaught genre. with one exception, which involved the schematic structure
When asked to write a personal narrative about a real-life measure for story parts. In the studies with struggling writ-
experience, SRSD-instructed students wrote longer, sche- ers, the effect sizes for this measure exceeded 1.0 (the effect
matically stronger, and qualitatively better compositions size in this study was 0.73). One possible explanation for
Downloaded by [University of Massachusetts, Amherst] at 09:12 10 August 2012

than did children in the control condition. this difference is that the scales used to measure schematic
These findings provide the first empirical support structure in this and the prior studies differed. In the current
for directly teaching writing strategies to primary-grade investigation, we examined both the inclusion and quality
students. This is consistent with evidence from over 40 of elements, whereas in the studies with young struggling
studies (experimental and single subject design) showing writers, just the inclusion of elements as examined. Another
that such instruction can enhance the writing of older possible explanation involves the capabilities of the writers
(Graham & Perin, 2007a, 2007b) and primary-grade chil- in the present and prior studies. Struggling writers in the
dren who experience difficulty with writing (Graham, previous investigations included only two or three story parts
2006b; Rogers & Graham, 2007). Of course, the findings in their pretest stories, whereas the students in the present
from the present study need replication, and the impact of study wrote pretest stories that were more complete (i.e.,
such instruction over a longer period of time needs to be contained more parts). Thus, struggling writers in previous
examined. In addition, it may be fruitful to study if writ- investigations had more room for improvement than did the
ing strategy instruction works with even younger, typically students in the present study.
developing writers, as it has been effective with second- In summary, the present study showed that the writing
grade children who find writing challenging (Graham et performance of young writers can be improved by teaching
al., 2005; Harris et al., 2006; Saddler et al., 2004). them strategies for planning and writing in conjunction
It is important to note that the effect sizes for SRSD with the knowledge and self-regulatory procedures needed
instruction in the present study were not as large as those to use these strategies effectively. Additional research
typically obtained in past research. On the story-writing is needed to verify this finding, using a larger sample of
posttest, the effect size for schematic structure and length classrooms, and comparing the effects of this instruction to
of stories was moderate (effect sizes were 0.71 and 0.55, those of other types of writing programs that place greater
respectively), whereas the effect size for writing quality emphasis on writing and the writing process (such as the
was small (0.35). In studies with struggling writers in the process writing approach).
primary grades, effect sizes for these measures immediately
after instruction exceeded 0.80 (Graham et al., 2005; Har- REFERENCES
ris et al., 2006). The two most likely explanations for this
difference involve how instruction was administered and Achieve, Inc. (2005). Rising to the challenge: Are high school graduates pre-
pared for college and work? Washington, DC: Author.
who was taught. First, one important difference between Agate, L., & Graham, S. (2007). Primary grade writing instruction: A
this investigation and the prior SRSD group studies, in national survey. Manuscript submitted for publication.
which the same strategies were taught using identical Alexander, P. (1992). Domain knowledge: Evolving issues and emerging
concerns. Educational Psychologist, 27, 33–51.
lesson plans (Graham et al.; Harris et al.), is that the former Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms, goals, structures, and student motivation.
involved whole-class instruction and the latter involved Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 261–271.
teaching two students at a time. Teaching two students at CTB/McGraw-Hill. (2006). InView. Monterey, CA: Author.
CTB/McGraw-Hill. (2008). Terranova (2nd ed.). Monterey, CA: Author.
a time likely provided additional opportunities for each Graham, S. (1999). Handwriting and spelling instruction for students with
student to receive more individual assistance and atten- learning disabilities: A review. Learning Disability Quarterly, 22, 78–98.
tion. As Hogan and Pressley (1997) noted, this can have a Graham, S. (2006a). Writing. In P. Alexander & P. Winne (Eds.), Hand-
book of educational psychology (pp. 457–477). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
positive impact on learning. Second, a recent meta-analysis Graham, S. (2006b). Strategy instruction and the teaching of writing. In
of writing-strategy instructional studies with older students C. MacArthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing
(Grades 4 and greater) indicated that larger gains are made research (pp. 187–207). New York: Guilford.
Graham, S., & Harris, K. R. (1989). A components analysis of cognitive
for struggling writers (average weighted effect size = 1.02) strategy instruction: Effects on learning disabled students’ compositions
than for more typical writers (average weighted effect size and self-efficacy. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 353–361.
May/June 2009 [Vol. 102(No. 5)] 331

Graham, S., & Harris, K. R. (2003). Students with learning disabilities www.collegeboard.com/prod_downloads/writingcom/neglectedr.pdf
and the process of writing: A meta-analysis of SRSD studies. In H. L. National Commission on Writing (NCW). (2004, September). Writ-
Swanson, K. R. Harris, & S. Graham (Eds.), Handbook of learning dis- ing: A ticket to work . . . or a ticket out: A survey of business leaders.
abilities (pp. 383–402). New York: Guilford. Retrieved December 28, 2008, from http://www.writingcommission
Graham, S., Harris, K. R., & Mason, L. (2005). Improving the writing per- .org/prod_downloads/writingcom/writing-ticket-to-work.pdf
formance, knowledge, and motivation of struggling young writers: The National Commission on Writing (NCW). (2005, July). Writing: A powerful
effects of self-regulated strategy development. Contemporary Educational message from state government. Retrieved December 28, 2008, from http://
Psychology, 30, 207–241. www.writingcommission.org/prod_downloads/writingcom/powerful-
Graham, S., Harris, K. R., & Reid, R. (1992). Developing self-regulated message-from-state.pdf
learners. Focus on Exceptional Children, 24(6), 1–16. Persky, H. R., Daane, M. C., & Jin, Y. (2003). The nation’s report card:
Graham, S., & Perin, D. (2007a). Writing next: Effective strategies to Writing 2002 (NCES 2003–529). (U.S. Department of Education, Insti-
improve writing of adolescent middle and high school. Washington, DC: tute of Education Sciences, National Center for Education Statistics.)
Alliance for Excellence in Education. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Graham, S., & Perin, D. (2007b). A meta-analysis of writing instruction Pintrich, P., & Schunk, D. (1996). Motivation in education. Englewood
for adolescent students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 445–476. Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Greenwald, E., Persky, H., Ambell, J., & Mazzeo, J. (1999). National Rogers, L., & Graham, S. (2008). A meta-analysis of single subject design writ-
assessment of educational progress: 1998 report card for the nation and the ing intervention research. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 879–906.
states. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education. Saddler, B., Moran, S., Graham, S., & Harris, K. R. (2004). Preventing
Hammill, D., & Larsen, S. (1996). Test of Written Language (TOWL-3). writing difficulties: The effects of planning strategy instruction on the
Downloaded by [University of Massachusetts, Amherst] at 09:12 10 August 2012

Circle Pines, MN: AGS. writing performance of struggling writers. Exceptionality, 12, 3–17.
Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (1996). Making the writing process work: Strate- Schunk, D., & Zimmerman, B. (1998). Self-regulated learning: From teach-
gies for composition and self-regulation. Cambridge, MA: Brookline Books. ing to self-reflective practices. New York: Guilford.
Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (1999). Programmatic intervention research: Slavin, R., Madden, N., & Karweit, N. (1989). Effective programs for
Illustrations from the evolution of self-regulated strategy development. students at risk: Conclusions for practice and policy. In R. Slavin, N.
Learning Disability Quarterly, 22, 251–262. Karweit, & N. Madden (Eds.), Effective programs for students at risk (pp.
Harris, K. R., Graham, S., & Mason, L. (2006). Improving the writing, 21–54). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
knowledge, and motivation of struggling young writers: Effects of Stein, N., & Glenn, C. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in
Self-Regulated Strategy Development with and without peer support. elementary school children. In R. Freedle (Ed.), Advances in discourse
American Educational Research Journal, 43, 295–340. processes: Vol. 2. New directions in discourse processing (53–120). Nor-
Hogan, K., & Pressley, M. (Eds.). (1997). Scaffolding student instruction. wood, NJ: Ablex.
Cambridge, MA: Brookline Books. Urdan, T. (1997). Achievement goal theory: Past results, future direc-
National Commission on Writing (NCW). (2003, April). The neglected R: tions. In P. Pintrich & M. Maehr (Eds.), Advances in motivation and
The need for a writing revolution. Retrieved December 28, 2008, from http:// achievement (pp. 99–142). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

� � � � � � � �� � ������ ����
� �� � � �� � � ������ �������
������������������������������������������������������������������������������������
����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������
��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������
��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������
���������������������������

�������������������������
����������������������������������
��������������������������������������������������
������������������������������������������������������������������������
������������������������������������

������ ������� � ������


��� � ��� ������ �����������������
� �� ��������� ��� ����������
��� ������������� � � ��� ������������
�������������� ����������������
��������������������
��������������� ������������������������������������������������
����������������
�����������
���������������
Downloaded by [University of Massachusetts, Amherst] at 09:12 10 August 2012

���������������������������������������������������������������������������������
��������������������������������������������������������������������������������
��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������
���������������������������������������������������������������������������������
�������������������������������������������������������������������������������������
�����������������������������������������������������������������������������������
�������������������������������������������������������������������������������������

�������������������������
����������������������������������
��������������������������������������������������
������������������������������������������������������������������������
������������������������������������

�������������������������������������
������������������������������������������������

� ��� � � �� ����� � � ������


��� � ��� ������
� � � ������ � �� �� � ������ ����
��� ���� ���� ����� �� ��� ���� ��������
���� ��� � �� ��� � ��� ����� �� ������
�� ����� ����� ���

You might also like