Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 136 (2016) 6–19

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/obhdp

Motivation in organizational behavior: History, advances and prospects


Ruth Kanfer a,⇑, Gilad Chen b
a
School of Psychology, J.S. Coon Building, MC 0170, 654 Cherry St., Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, GA 30332, USA
b
Robert H. Smith School of Business, 4538 Van Munching Hall, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742-1815, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: In this article we selectively review major advances in research on motivation in work and organizational
Received 21 October 2015 behavior since the founding of Organizational Behavior and Human Performance (now Organizational
Revised 2 June 2016 Behavior and Human Decision Processes) 50 years ago. Using a goal-based organizing rubric, we highlight
Accepted 12 June 2016
the most impactful articles and summarize research progress over time related to understanding the
why, where, how, what, and when of motivation during goal choice and goal enactment. We also note
macro-level trends in motivation research published in this journal, including the shift away from pub-
Keywords:
lishing new, core theories of work motivation in favor of using new approaches published elsewhere to
Motivation
Self-regulation
examine key micro-regulatory processes involved in goal decisions and goal pursuit. We conclude with
Goals discussion of promising future research directions.
Motives Ó 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Goal choice
Goal pursuit

1. Introduction cles on goal setting (Latham & Locke, 1991; Locke, 1991) and the
role of self-efficacy and self-regulation (Bandura, 1991).
In 1966, Jim Naylor and George Briggs introduced the purpose As the 20th century drew to a close, the motivational research
of Organizational Behavior and Human Performance (OBHP, now landscape coalesced around the goal construct, prompting a broad
titled Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes reorganization of findings in terms of understanding the effects of
[OBHDP]) as a journal aimed at publishing significant research that person, context, and temporal variables in two goal-related subsys-
‘‘contribute(s) to our basic knowledge of human performance” tems – goal choice and goal pursuit (see Kanfer, 2012). In this
(Naylor & Briggs, 1966, p. 1). Over the past five decades, the journal expansive paradigm, motivation serves as an umbrella term that
has published seminal papers on the determinants, mechanisms, encompasses both the purpose and reasons underlying decision
and outcomes of motivation related to decision-making and per- processes and goal selection (that set the course of action), and
formance in work and achievement settings. New and influential the regulatory dynamics through which goals and other variables
theories of motivation have been introduced in the journal, includ- affect the allocation of an individual’s cognitive resources across
ing Alderfer’s ERG theory (1969), Ajzen’s Theory of Planned Behav- activities and over time for the purpose of goal attainment (i.e.,
ior (1991), Deci’s Cognitive Evaluation Theory (1972), Hackman goal pursuit; Vancouver, 2008). This meta-framework has also
and Oldham’s Job Characteristics Model (1976), and Locke’s Goal spawned new theories and research directed at more precise mea-
Model (1968). Other articles provide incisive reviews and critiques surement and understanding of person, social, and contextual
of these approaches that had considerable influence on the field influences on goal choice, goal construal, and behavioral intentions
(e.g., Mento, Steel, & Karren, 1987; Pritchard, 1969; Wahba & (Brockner & Higgins, 2001; Button, Mathieu, & Zajac, 1996; Chen &
Bridwell, 1976). In 1982, Campion and Lord introduced and tested Mathieu, 2008; da Motta Veiga & Turban, 2014; Grant et al., 2007),
a control systems model of motivation to explain how goals and the relationship between goal choice and goal pursuit (Gollwitzer,
performance feedback operate in tandem to affect motivation Heckhausen, & Ratajczak, 1990; Sun, Vancouver, & Weinhardt,
and behavior over time, and in 1986 Bandura and Cervone pub- 2014), motivational dynamics during goal pursuit (Fishbach &
lished empirical support for the role of self-efficacy in maintaining Choi, 2012; Seo & Ilies, 2009), and the motivational processes in
motivation during goal pursuit. A special issue on theories of cog- and of teams (Chen, Kanfer, DeShon, Mathieu, & Kozlowski, 2009;
nitive self-regulation in 1991 produced additional influential arti- Nahrgang et al., 2013).
In this article we provide a selective review of major advances
⇑ Corresponding author. in motivation related to work and organizational behavior since
E-mail addresses: rk64@prism.gatech.edu (R. Kanfer), giladchen@rhsmith.umd. the founding of the journal 50 years ago. In keeping with the cele-
edu (G. Chen). bration of the journal’s jubilee, we have organized our review in a

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2016.06.002
0749-5978/Ó 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
R. Kanfer, G. Chen / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 136 (2016) 6–19 7

way that highlights those articles in OBHP/OBHDP that have been


Socio-environmental Influences
most widely-cited and have had a major influence on the study of
“Where and When”
motivation in work and organizational psychology. The article is Culture Technology
organized into three sections. The first section sets the stage for Work Relaons
Leadership Job Design/Demands
the review by summarizing foundational principles in motivational Teams
science. Next, we highlight influential articles published in OBHP/ Dyadic Intra-Individual Influences
OBHDP and review progress by organizing theory and research “Why” “How”
Cognions
Implicit/Explicit
around four principal questions that have long-driven scientific Needs, Moves, Traits
efforts in motivational psychology: (1) Why do individuals allocate Affect/Emoons

resources to specific behaviors and courses of action, (2) What is


the influence of an individual’s environment on motivation – the
Goal Choice
‘‘where” and ‘‘when” questions, (3) How do goals and goal-linked
processes affect motivation and action, and (4) What resources “What”
do individuals employ for the purpose of goal accomplishment? Goal Pursuit

In the third and final section, we discuss the representation of


motivation theory and research in the journal, progress in each Fig. 1. A heuristic meta-model of work motivation and the focus of major
of the four topic areas, and promising future research directions. theoretical accounts.

2. A brief overview volitional (self-regulatory) processes required for goal accomplish-


ment (performance). The introduction of theories of goal pursuit in
Motivation in work and organizational psychology is concerned motivation psychology shifted attention away from determinants
with the energetic forces that originate both within as well as of goal selection and toward the operation of self-regulatory mech-
beyond an individual’s being that influence the initiation, direction, anisms and dynamic processes by which goals are enacted, modi-
intensity, and duration of action (cf. Pinder, 1998). Modern views fied, or abandoned.
typically portray motivation as a time-linked set of recursive and As ‘‘what” approaches gained traction during the late 20th cen-
reciprocal affective, behavioral, and cognitive processes and tury, motivation and decision-making researchers focused on
actions that are organized around an individual’s goals. An individ- increasingly different topics and questions. Motivational scientists
ual’s goal, defined as the mental representation of a desired out- within the broader organizational behavior domain focused on the
come, does not exist in isolation but rather within hierarchically processes by which goal attributes and the individual’s construal of
organized networks that are developed and modified over time the goal influenced planning and self-regulatory processes during
as a result of the continuous interplay between person, situation, goal pursuit and performance accomplishments. Over the past
and epigenetic forces (Powers, 1973). The resultant network struc- few decades, advances in measurement of implicit motives and
ture of goals contributes to both the stability and heterogeneity evidence for the influence of automatic processes during goal pur-
observed in motivated action. suit have further extended the study of motivation to incorporate
Over the past half-century there have been numerous accounts the impact of non-conscious influences on goal construal and goal
of work motivation. Although the development of a comprehensive pursuit.
integrative model lies beyond the scope of this article, our review At the same time, there have been significant advances over the
of the literature suggests that various conceptualizations may be past half-century in the ‘‘where and when” portion of the motiva-
broadly differentiated and loosely organized in terms of the pri- tional network. Theory and research in this segment of the field
mary issues they address and their position along a continuum of highlights the contextual variables and processes by which exter-
proximity to goal choice. As shown in Fig. 1, the most prominent nalities in the work setting influence an individual’s goals, engage-
theories of work motivation address the proximal, intra- ment, and behavior. Many, though not all of these approaches build
individual psychological forces, mechanisms, and processes that upon sense-making processes by which individuals interpret social
determine goal choice and action (i.e., the why, how, and what of events and work processes, and studies on the effects of these pro-
motivation). Within-person formulations in this segment of Fig. 1 cesses on trait activation, job attitudes, and affect. In addition to
derive from three distinct but related streams of research. ‘‘Why” research investigating the impact of job design and demands on
accounts of motivation may be traced back to early 20th century goals, new, integrative streams of research have emerged for
work on the identification of universal human motives and tenden- understanding the impact of work relations associated with differ-
cies. Although motive-based theories also posit a process by which ent patterns of leadership, team structures, and work-related inter-
motives influenced goal choice, the introduction of cognitive, personal processes (e.g., with co-workers, customers) on
information-processing approaches and expectancy-value theories motivation processes. Research findings in this area have been
to organizational psychology beginning in the 1950s riveted linked to a variety of work phenomena such as escalation of com-
research attention on the ‘‘how” question, including for example mitment, ethical decision making, negotiation outcomes, and pro-
the cognitive processes by which expectancies and outcomes are gress decisions during task goal striving (see Klein, Austin, &
integrated, and how best to conceptualize and assess affect. By Cooper, 2008). Although there has been less work directly linking
the mid-1960s, however, motivational theorists began to question broader constructs such as culture and technology to goal choice
basic tenets of expectancy-value models and their applicability for and action, we include these constructs in the figure to signify an
predicting performance on new, difficult, prolonged, or ill-defined important direction for future research.
career and task goals, such as becoming a neurosurgeon, or pursu-
ing a management promotion.
During the 1970s, social-cognitive theories (Bandura, 1977a, 3. A biography of journal contributions and progress in the field
1977b; Carver & Scheier, 1981) and theories of action regulation
(Heckhausen & Gollwitzer, 1987; Sun & Frese, 2013) emerged that 3.1. Person-oriented formulations: the ‘‘why” of motivation
addressed the gap between an individual’s goals and performance
(the ‘‘what” question). These approaches reconceptualized an The ‘‘why” question of motivation is typically studied from a
individual’s decision as a goal that instigated (when necessary) person-centric perspective, that focuses on the needs, motives,
8 R. Kanfer, G. Chen / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 136 (2016) 6–19

wants, and likes of individuals. These intra-psychic features pro- motivation associated with the satisfaction of higher-order
vide personal reasons for individual action. Needs and motives give motives related to sense of competence, autonomy, and control.
rise to explicit goals and action tendencies. Theorizing and In his article, Deci presented Cognitive Evaluation Theory (CET)
research in this area that has been highlighted in OBHP/OBHDP and reviewed prior empirical findings to support his new theory.
range from developmental approaches that focus on shifts in The essence of CET for employee motivation is that the application
motive salience over time (e.g., Alderfer, 1969) to formulations that of performance-contingent extrinsic rewards (e.g., pay) undermi-
emphasize the role of universal motives (e.g., Deci, 1972), and nes intrinsic motivation and performance when the extrinsic
motives that affect the construal of task goals (e.g., Dweck, 1986). reward is perceived as controlling (autonomy-reducing) rather
than informational (autonomy-supportive). Deci’s CET spurred
3.1.1. Early need approaches more than two dozen published studies in OBHP/OBHDP that pro-
Motivation research during the mid-20th century focused on vided positive (e.g., Erez, Gopher, & Arzi, 1990; Greenberger,
two issues: (1) how best to organize the numerous motives that Strasser, Cummings, & Dunham, 1989; Shapira, 1989), mixed
characterize human striving, and (2) what brings different needs/- (Farr, Vance, & McIntyre, 1977; Porac & Meindl, 1982; Shalley &
motives to the surface as the driving force for behavior. Maslow’s Perry-Smith, 2001), and negative (Boal & Cummings, 1981) results
Need Hierarchy Theory (1943, 1954) addressed both these issues for various portions of the theory.
(cf. Wahba & Bridwell, 1976). Maslow initially proposed five basic In the 40 years since Deci’s first publication of CET in OBHP,
human need categories (physiological, safety, love/belonging, controversy regarding the detrimental effects of extrinsic rewards
esteem, and self-actualization) that were arranged hierarchically, on intrinsic motivation has waxed and waned. Debates arose about
starting with the most basic physiological needs (e.g., food, water, the applicability of the findings to work settings (Kanfer, 1990),
sleep) and progressing to highest-level needs for self-actualization and how to interpret discrepant meta-analytic findings (Cerasoli,
(e.g., creativity, achieving individual potential). He further pro- Nicklin, & Ford, 2014). In 2000, Deci and his colleagues proposed
posed that individuals seek to satisfy unmet needs in a hierarchical a revised theory called Self-Determination Theory (SDT, Gagné &
fashion, seeking to satisfy lower-order ‘‘deficiency” needs before Deci, 2005; Ryan & Deci, 2000). The revised theory subsumed the
satisfying unmet higher-level ‘‘growth” needs. original formulation within a broader theory of self-
In 1969, Alderfer proposed and tested a modified theory in determination that could account for why extrinsic rewards did
OBHP that addressed some of early criticisms of the Maslow model. not always exert a negative effect on intrinsic employee motivation
Alderfer organized Maslow’s five need categories into three and performance. The SDT formulation posits six distinct types of
broader need groupings: existence, relatedness, and growth needs. motivation that vary in degree of self-determination, from Amoti-
Rather than imposing a strict hierarchical gratification of needs, vation (in which there is no self-regulation), through fully self-
Alderfer proposed two major mechanisms for movement across determined motivation (Intrinsic Motivation). Most interesting
need categories and tested a system in which satisfaction of are the four types of extrinsic motivation (external, introjected,
higher-order needs would increase the desire of growth needs. identified, and integrated). In these categories, the corrosive effect
Although Alderfer’s Existence-Relatedness-Growth (ERG) theory of the extrinsic reward depends on how the incentive is inter-
attracted attention among organizational psychologists as a viable preted with respect to the self and the regulatory processes that
alternative to Maslow’s model for understanding employee moti- are instigated as a consequence of that judgement. Thus, SDT
vation, a later critical review of the theory and evidence supporting allows for a range of outcomes associated with the application of
Maslow’s theory by Wahba and Bridwell (1976) greatly reduced extrinsic rewards. To date only one study on SDT has been pub-
scholarly interest in broad need approaches, as well as attempts lished in OBHDP (Grant, Nurmohamed, Ashford, & Dekas, 2011).
to organize diverse human motives. However, in partial support of the new SDT formulation, results
Theorizing and research on motives since the late 1970s dif- of a recent meta-analysis by Cerasoli et al. (2014) showed that
fered from earlier work driven by the Maslow and Alderfer models the detrimental effect of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation
in several ways. First, more recent investigations have not focused depends on both the nature of the performance objective (qualita-
on needs per se, but rather on the motives that develop and gain tive vs quantitative) and the nature of the reward structure (con-
salience as a function of culture, cognitive biology, and experience. tingent vs non-contingent).
One major stream of research has focused on the conditions that Although the original CET formulation has often been cited as
arouse fundamental motives to action, particularly ‘‘higher- arguing against the use of extrinsic rewards to enhance employee
order” motives, such as the desire for achievement, autonomy, motivation, Deci’s own statements in the 1972 OBHP article are
and power (e.g. McClelland, 1986). Second, current motive classifi- particularly noteworthy: ‘‘It is possible to pay workers and still
cation schemes tend to be goal-related and distinguish broadly have them intrinsically motivated . . .. It is not the money per se
between approach (growth) and avoidance (loss) motivational ori- which motivates performance, but rather it is the way that it is
entations (e.g., Crowe & Higgins, 1997), rather than motive tax- administered” (Deci, 1972, p. 227). Given that contingent pay
onomies organized around specific content. Third, motives are may be perceived as controlling and so diminish the positive
increasingly represented as both relatively enduring action prefer- impact of intrinsic motivation, Deci (1972) further suggests that
ences and emergent psychological states, suggesting a greater mal- ‘‘we [should] concentrate on structuring situations and jobs to
leability over the lifespan as a function of culture and experience. arouse intrinsic motivation” (Deci, 1972, p. 227). This is precisely
Finally, the Maslow and Alderfer theories address the role of expli- the approach taken in Hackman and Oldham’s (1976) Job Charac-
cit, or self-reported motives, whereas recent work has extended teristics Model, which we discuss later.
the study of motives to examine the role of implicit, or non-
conscious motives on motivation and performance (e.g., 3.1.3. Motivational orientation and goal construal theories
Tabernero & Wood, 1999). Findings across a range of subdisciplines in psychology provide
convergent evidence for the operation of two distinct motive sys-
3.1.2. Intrinsic motivation tems; an appetitive (approach-oriented) system and an avoidance
About the same time that Wahba and Bridwell (1976) published system. The appetitive system is concerned with the modulation of
their critique of Maslow’s theory and associated research, Deci action for the purpose of attaining pleasurable or desired out-
(1972) published a pioneering article in OBHP that focused on comes; in contrast the avoidance system is concerned with modu-
the relationship between extrinsic rewards (e.g., pay) and intrinsic lation of action for the purpose of avoiding unpleasurable
R. Kanfer, G. Chen / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 136 (2016) 6–19 9

outcomes. Over the past few decades, there has been renewed instantiated outside the individual’s awareness that, in turn, set
interest in understanding the antecedents of these motivational the stage for consciously-mediated goal choice (e.g., Andersen &
orientations, their impact on how individuals construe work goals, Chen, 2002; Chartrand & Bargh, 1996; Shah, 2003; Stajkovic,
and their effects on self-regulation and performance (Crowe & Locke, & Blair, 2006). Further research is needed to delineate how
Higgins, 1997; Dweck, 1986; Elliot & Harackiewicz; 1996; Kanfer significant others in the work environment and the social architec-
& Heggestad, 1997). ture of the workplace (e.g., team and multiteam structures) prime
The most well-known formulations in the work and organiza- or otherwise automatically influence goal construal and pursuit. A
tional literature are Dweck’s goal orientation theory and Higgins’ second area of promise pertains to research directed toward the
regulatory focus theory. Dweck’s formulation emphasizes the assessment and role that implicit motives play in motivation. Con-
implicit theories that individuals hold about their abilities and sistent with McClelland, Koestner, and Weinberger (1989) and
the influence of these theories on how individuals construe task Kehr (2004), recent work indicates that such conflicts may affect
goals. According to this model, individuals who hold an incremen- intrinsic motivation (Rawolle, Wallis, Badham, & Kehr, 2016), and
tal (or malleable) conception of their ability are posited to apply a goal pursuit (Gröpel & Kehr, 2014).
mastery or learning-oriented cognitive frame during goal pursuit. At the same time, there has been progress in understanding the
In contrast, individuals who hold a fixed conception of ability are processes by which universal motives, such as autonomy, become
posited to construe task goals using an avoidance perspective that salient motives that direct action. SDT, for example, links arousal of
highlights the demonstration of competency and avoidance of intrinsic motives to the self and provides a more precise under-
showing incompetence. The development and validation of indi- standing of the conditions in which individuals may react nega-
vidual differences measures of adult goal orientation by Button tively to the imposition of reward systems. Although many
et al. (1996) and VandeWalle (1997) clarified the multidimensional questions about this framework remain, the theory attests to the
structure of the goal orientation construct and stimulated research role that perceived discrepancies between the self and the environ-
over the past two decades in OBHDP and elsewhere examining the ment have for motivation and action.
effects of goal orientation on self-regulation and performance (e.g.,
Chen & Mathieu, 2008; da Motta Veiga & Turban, 2014; Kozlowski 3.2. Socio-environmental approaches: the ‘‘where” and ‘‘when” of
et al., 2001). Although findings over this period provide general motivation
support for the positive influence of learning goal orientation on
self-regulation and performance, results are mixed with respect Research on topics in this area consider how diverse attributes
to the negative effects of performance-prove and performance- of an individual’s environment, such as the physical and mental
avoid goal orientations (Payne, Youngcourt, & Beaubien, 2007). demands of one’s job, social interactions with colleagues and
In Regulatory Focus Theory (RFT), Crowe and Higgins (1997) supervisors, and the social structure of work influence motivation.
provide an alternative approach to understanding how the self Prominent work in this area over the past half-century includes
affects goal construal and striving processes. According to RFT, theories of job design, organizational justice, and social influences
individuals who are promotion-focused are oriented toward of leaders and work teams on motivation.
growth and development, and attainment of ideal self. In contrast,
individuals who are prevention-focused are oriented toward ought 3.2.1. Job design
aspirations, associated with duties and responsibilities. Individuals In 1976, Hackman and Oldham published one of their first
who engage in goals characterized by regulatory fit between the descriptions and comprehensive tests of Job Characteristics Theory
self and the situation (e.g., promotion focus during pursuit of in OBHP. Hackman and Oldham’s (1976) job characteristics theory
achievement goals) are proposed to experience fewer negative (JCT) specifies how job characteristics interact with individual dif-
emotions and more effective self-regulation than individuals who ferences in growth need strength to affect job satisfaction and
experience a misfit between their regulatory focus and the nature work motivation. Hackman and Oldham identified five key features
of the goal (Brockner & Higgins, 2001). Similar to Dweck, RFT: (1) of the immediate job context that were proposed to determine the
builds upon the approach-avoidance dichotomy to understand intrinsic motivating potential of the job; skill variety, task identity,
how differences in goal valence (promotion or prevention) affects task significance, autonomy, and feedback. According to JCT, these
motivational processing and (2) proposes that individual differ- characteristics interact with individual differences in growth need
ences in regulatory focus may be understood in terms of distal strength to affect three critical psychological states that serve as
individual differences in trait tendencies as well as in terms of the proximal determinants of affect and motivation; meaningful-
proximal motivational states. ness, responsibility, and knowledge of results. Between 1976 and
1983, OBHP published nine studies testing various aspects of the
3.1.4. Evaluative summary of ‘‘why” approaches theory (e.g., Evans, Kiggundu, & House, 1979; Hackman,
Over the past half-century researchers have moved away from Brousseau, & Weiss, 1976; Hackman, Pearce, & Wolfe, 1978;
taxonomic issues and developmental models of motivation (e.g., Kiggundu, 1983; Rousseau, 1977) as applied to individual and
Maslow) in favor of understanding how cognitive, affective, and group motivation and performance.
social information-processing mechanisms affect motive salience Although the JCT model proved practically useful for organiza-
and motivational processes. For example, the growing consensus tions seeking to increase employee motivation through job rede-
around the notion of co-existing approach and avoidance motiva- sign, conceptual and methodological criticisms of the theory
tional systems, rather than emphasis on stable motive hierarchies, mounted through the early 1980s (e.g., Pierce & Dunham, 1978;
has spurred a new generation of research on motivational orienta- Roberts & Glick, 1981). Principal among these concerns was the
tion. Research in these areas highlight the role of perceptual pro- validity of self-report measures used to assess objective task char-
cesses and identity in goal construal, and help to explain the acteristics. More recently, motivation researchers have pointed to
often observed variability in goal enactment, affect, and perfor- the narrowness of the model for work in the 21st century. Grant
mance among individuals with seeming similar outcome goals. and Parker (2009) argued that JCT neglects the social characteris-
Perhaps most importantly, new lines of inquiry are extending tics and context of work that derive from interdependent work
understanding of the reasons for action beyond consciously- roles. Morgeson and Humphrey (2006) identified five features of
mediated traits and motives. One area of particular promise per- the social context: social support, interactions outside the organi-
tains to the role of automatic perceptual and cognitive processes zation, initiated interdependence, received interdependence, and
10 R. Kanfer, G. Chen / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 136 (2016) 6–19

feedback from others. They also showed a significant relationship iors and regulatory resources, but a negative relationship
between these work features and subjective ratings of job perfor- between enactment of interpersonal justice behaviors and regula-
mance. Grant (2008) proposed and tested the notion that the rela- tory resources.
tional architecture of jobs may be designed to enhance intrinsic
and prosocial motivations by providing work opportunities to sat- 3.2.3. Social influences of teams and leaders on motivation
isfy motives related to affiliation and helping others (e.g., clients). Research has also examined social influences on motivation.
Grant and Parker (2009) further summarize early evidence for how Early work in psychology has focused on whether individuals exert
features of the social context affect performance through allocation different levels of effort when working in the presence of others
of effort and proactivity during goal pursuit. The extension of work (Triplett, 1898; Zajonc, 1965). Later work – including research pub-
design models that take fuller account of how social context and lished in OBHDP – has advanced theories and provided empirical
interpersonal relationships affect the character of goals and goal evidence regarding social loafing and the extent to which individ-
striving is an important advance in approaches to motivation in uals allocate more (or less) effort as they perform in collective
modern organizational settings. group settings (Loundt & Phillips, 2007; Miles & Greenberg,
1993; Mueller, 2012; Price, 1987; Weldon & Mustari, 1988). This
3.2.2. Equity/social exchange approaches research has suggested that working in group and team settings
Adams’ (1965) equity theory focuses on the universal motive for can sometime lead to motivational losses – especially when indi-
fairness in the distribution of outcomes. According to this theory, viduals lack accountability and responsibility for important out-
individuals value and seek fairness in employee-employer relation- comes. However, this research also suggests that, under certain
ships. Adams proposed that an individual’s perception of inequity conditions (e.g., when group members share common goals or
in the ratio of inputs to outcomes between herself and other have a common threat), group settings can also increase competi-
employees arouses an aversive psychological tension state and tiveness among individuals and provide individuals with greater
motivation to reduce the tension. Early studies of equity theory resources to accomplish their tasks.
examined the influence of perceived inequity in payment on per- Since the 1980s, organizational researchers have also become
formance. Findings from these studies suggest an asymmetry in increasingly interested in studying teams as important organiza-
inequity perceptions such that individuals are less motivated by tional units. Research has shown that team members’ motivation
perceptions of overpayment than underpayment inequity. Reviews to perform collective tasks can be increased when members are
by Pritchard (1969) and others note the difficulties in applying the assigned or share difficult and specific group goals (e.g., Crown &
theory to practice given the multiplicity of ways by which individ- Rosse, 1995; Durham, Knight, & Locke, 1997; Mulvey & Klein,
uals may reduce perceived inequity. Accordingly, more recent 1998). Moreover, research has suggested that individual motiva-
studies on social comparison processes have tended to focus on tional attributes, such as members’ regulatory focus (Beersma,
the person and contextual factors that influence perceptions of Homan, Van Kleef, & De Dreu, 2013) and epistemic motivation
outcome fairness (Sherf & Venkataramani, 2015), rather than the (or need for closure; Livi, Kruglanski, Pierro, Mannetti, & Kenny,
effects of perceived inequity on motivational variables and 2015) can influence team processes such as norm formation and
processes. team coordination. Finally, Chen and Kanfer (2006) formulated a
During the 1980s, however, burgeoning interest in organiza- multilevel theory of motivation in teams, according to which team
tional justice shifted research attention away from perceptions of and individual motivational processes are not only functionally
inequity in payment outcomes to the determinants and conse- similar (i.e., capture similar motivational constructs and influences
quences of perceptions of inequity in other work-related outcomes on performance), but also reinforce each other across levels. In
(e.g., promotions), organizational procedures and employee- support of Chen and Kanfer, Chen et al. (2009) found that team-
employer relationships (see Colquitt, Greenberg, & Zapata-Phelan, level allocation of collective effort promoted individual members’
2005). Meta-analytic findings of this work show that individuals allocation of effort toward accomplishing individual roles in the
who perceive outcomes, procedures, and relationships as unfair team, as well as individual members’ performance. Thus, there is
were more likely to engage in lower levels of job performance ample evidence (from research published in OBHDP and else-
and organizational citizenship behaviors, and/or higher levels of where) that individual-level motivation is influenced by group
counter-productive behavior (Colquitt, Conlon, Wesson, Porter, & and team processes.
Ng, 2001; Colquitt et al., 2013). In addition, theories of leadership have proposed that leaders
Although interest in the effects of justice perceptions on behav- can motivate employees by engaging in transformational behav-
ior continues, the theoretical explanation for the effects of injustice iors (such as inspiring employees to adopt more challenging goals;
on work behaviors has morphed from a motivational account Bass, 1985), by empowering employees on their jobs (e.g., Chen,
based on psychological tension reduction to explanations based Kirkman, Kanfer, Allen, & Rosen, 2007), and by developing mutu-
on principles of social exchange and moral accountability (e.g., ally trusting relationships with their employees (Graen & Uhl-
Lyons & Scott, 2012; Rupp, Shao, Jones, & Liao, 2014; Takeuchi, Bien, 1995). Empirical research has provided evidence for such
Yun, & Wong, 2011) that make relatively little contact with arguments. For example, Benjamin and Flynn (2006) found that
individual-level process theories of motivation. Several studies transformational leadership interacted with follower regulatory
point to the importance of motivational variables (such as regula- focus (locomotion or approach-oriented regulatory focus) to pre-
tory focus) in justice perceptions (Brebels, De Cremer, & Sedikides, dict follower willingness to exert extra effort. Zhang and Zhou
2008; Cropanzano, Paddock, Rupp, Bagger, & Baldwin, 2008; Li (2014) further found that followers of more empowering leaders
et al., 2011), and the conditions that increase justice motive sal- felt more efficacious in behaving creatively, especially when fol-
ience in goal choice (Johnson, Selenta, & Lord, 2006). Studies by lowers also trusted their leader, which in turn related positively
Judge, Scott, and Ilies (2006), Yang and Diefendorff (2009), and to employees’ creative performance
Ferris, Spence, Brown, and Heller (2012) also provide evidence
for the indirect effect of justice perceptions on behavior through 3.2.4. Evaluative summary of ‘‘where” and ‘‘when” approaches
trait, affective, and self variables. Recently, Johnson, Lanaj, and Early progress in this branch of work motivation psychology
Barnes (2014) examined the self-regulatory costs associated with was substantial and yielded important information about the link
engaging in different forms of justice behavior, and found a posi- between particular job characteristics, motives, and work motiva-
tive relationship between enactment of procedural justice behav- tion. As research in this stream matured and grew less productive
R. Kanfer, G. Chen / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 136 (2016) 6–19 11

during the late 20th century, the changing nature of work and ments of expectancy-value theory as predictors of behavioral
advances in affective neuroscience have encouraged new lines of intentions. In contrast to Vroom’s VIE model, however, TRA also
inquiry investigating social justice motives and work relations as takes into account the role of attitudes and social influences in
drivers of motivated action. To date, findings from social justice the form of subjective norms in forming an intention. Like Vroom’s
have also focused largely on the antecedents of adherence to jus- model, however, the episodic nature of the model does not for-
tice roles (Scott, Garza, Conlon, & Kim, 2014) or the effects of mally account for the gap between intention and behavior. In
unfairness perceptions on (counter-productive or deviant) behav- 1991, Ajzen introduced an important extension of TRA, namely
ior, rather than motivational processes per se. the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) in OBHDP. In contrast to
From a work motivation perspective, studies examining motiva- TRA, TPB includes measures of control beliefs and perceived behav-
tion in the context of teams, leadership, and co-worker relations ioral control as an additional determinant of the relationship
have yielded substantial insights into our understanding of socio- between intentions and behavior. TPA has received considerable
environmental influences on motivation. However, it is also note- attention in the social psychology literature. Meta-analytic find-
worthy that relatively little systematic research has been done ings provide general support for the model in predicting intentions
investigating the mechanisms by which ambient factors, such as and behavior (e.g., Ajzen, 1991; Armitage & Conner, 2001),
culture and technology, affect the instantiation of motives, goal although Armitage and Conner (2001) found a strong relationship
orientation, and affective reactions to goal progress. Given the between measures of perceived behavioral control and self-
increasing diversity of the workforce and growing use of technol- efficacy.
ogy in the workplace, further research is needed to better under- Articles published in OBHP (Schmidt, 1973; Zedeck, 1977) and
stand how the broader work environment impacts employee elsewhere identified a number of conceptual, methodological and
motivation. theoretical concerns with expectancy-based models of motivation
(e.g., Heneman & Schwab, 1972; Mitchell, 1974). Principal con-
3.3. Goal selection processes: the ‘‘how” of motivation cerns pertain to the inability of these models to account for inte-
gration of model elements (expectancy, instrumentality, and
Research on the ‘‘how” question delineates the decision-making valence), assumptions of rationality in decision-making, and the
process in which individuals choose and pursue course of action. mismatch between theory and the methodology used to test the
Although expectancy-value theories (e.g., Vroom, 1964) dominated theory. Although variations of basic expectancy-value models
this area during through the mid-to-late 20th century, formula- remain a staple component of many motivation theories, motiva-
tions that focused on goals, such as goal setting (e.g., Locke, tion research in the 21st century has increasingly focused on the
1968) and action theory (Gollwitzer, 1990) gained in popularity automatic and social processes that contribute to performance
toward the end of the 20th century. variability in organizational settings.

3.3.1. Expectancy-value theory 3.3.3. Goal setting


Vroom’s (1964) VIE theory is arguably one of the most influen- In his 1968 OBHDP article, ‘‘Toward a Theory of Task Motivation
tial work motivation theories of the 20th century. The theory pre- and Incentives,” Locke launched the outline of a new theory of
dicts an individual’s choice between tasks, jobs, and effort levels as motivation and provided results from diverse studies to support
a function of the integration of three sets of psychological vari- his central thesis that an individual’s conscious goals or intentions
ables: (1) expectancies, or the individual’s beliefs about the rela- function as the most proximal determinant of motivation and per-
tionship between levels of behavioral effort and performance, (2) formance. In contrast to expectancy-value based formulations,
instrumentalities, or the individual’s perception of the relationship Locke did not address how individuals form or select goals, but
between levels of performance and second-level outcomes, such as rather focused on the causal effects of goals on performance. In this
pay or promotion, and (3) valence, or the anticipated attractiveness seminal work, Locke defined a goal as what the individual is trying
of each secondary outcome. In 1966, Vroom published an influen- to achieve, and provided evidence for a significant positive rela-
tial article in OBHP that provided early support for his model with tionship between the difficulty of an individual’s behavioral inten-
respect to job choice among management trainees. His findings tions or goal and level of task performance. He also reviewed
showed a positive relationship between ratings of an organiza- studies showing that goals mediated the impact of other motiva-
tion’s perceived instrumentality for goal attainment and organiza- tional variables, including external incentives, knowledge of
tional choice. Interestingly, Vroom’s (1966) also provided early results, competition, and verbal reinforcers. Locke also suggested
evidence for differences in cognitive evaluation before and after that goal commitment was likely related to task persistence.
the job choice decision that served as early evidence for subse- Locke’s 1968 article stimulated intense research interest in goal
quent work by Gollwitzer (1990, 2003) on the differential process- setting as a potentially powerful motivational technique. Early
ing of information during goal choice (a decisional process) and research published in OBHDP further established the goal-
goal pursuit (an action process). Over the years, numerous studies performance relationship (e.g., Frost & Mahoney, 1976; Hamner
investigating the components and predictive validity of expectancy & Harnett, 1974; Mento et al., 1987; Pritchard & Curtis, 1973),
theories were published in OBHP/OBHDP (e.g., Hackman & Porter, the effects of goal setting at the group level (Forward & Zander,
1968; Lawler & Suttle, 1972, 1973; Pritchard, deLeo, & Von 1971), and explored the relationship between expectancy and
Bergen, 1976), with findings that provided general support for goals (Klein, 1991; Mento, Cartledge, & Locke, 1980). In 1981,
the original and revised models (e.g., Lawler & Suttle, 1973; see Locke, Shaw, Saari, and Latham extended the goal setting formula-
Mitchell, 1974, 1982), as well as stronger support for the model’s tion by proposing that goals affected action through four mecha-
efficacy in predicting job choice (i.e., decision-making) versus nisms: directing attention, mobilizing on-task effort, encouraging
motivation force or job performance (i.e., action). task persistence, and facilitating the development of effective per-
formance strategies.
3.3.2. Theory of planned behavior Goal setting research during the 1980s and 1990s increasingly
In social psychology, Fishbein and Ajzen’s (1975) Theory of Rea- focused on a series of issues related to the impact of goal attributes
soned Action (TRA) set the stage for extensive examination of the on goal striving. One line of research represented in the journal
relationships between attitudes, social influences and behavioral focused on the influence of goal origin (assigned vs self-set) and
intentions. Similar to Vroom’s model, TRA incorporates basic ele- participation in goal setting on individual performance (Austin,
12 R. Kanfer, G. Chen / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 136 (2016) 6–19

1989; Campbell & Gingrich, 1986; Earley & Kanfer, 1985; 3.3.5. Evaluative summary of ‘‘how” approaches
Hollenbeck & Brief, 1987; Sue-Chan & Ong, 2002) and at the team For much of the mid- to late-20th century, expectancy-value
level (Durham et al., 1997). Another line of inquiry examined the and goal setting models dominated the work motivation literature.
influence of feedback characteristics (e.g., Podsakoff & Farh, These formulations were intended as within-subject explanations
1989) and judgmental anchors on goal setting (Hinsz, Kalnbach, of goal choice and behavior over relatively short periods of time,
& Lorentz, 1997). However, the lion’s share of research in goal set- though many studies tested these models using between-subject
ting focused on the impact of goal setting on elements of goal striv- research strategies and examined behavior over longer periods of
ing, such as planning (Earley & Perry, 1987; Smith, Locke, & Barry, time (during which the value of various determinants changed).
1990) and resource allocations during learning (DeShon & Although tests of revised models show support for their use in pre-
Alexander, 1996; Kanfer & Ackerman, 1989). By the time of Mento dicting job choice and work effort in structured settings, there has
et al.’s 1987 OBHDP meta-analytic study of goal setting- been a broad shift in basic work motivation research away from
performance relations, over 70 studies had been reported on the these models toward research investigating the role of implicit
goal difficulty-performance relation. Mento et al.’s meta-analytic motives, cognitions, and affect on goal formation. Theorizing and
review of the goal setting literature showed robust support for findings by Gollwitzer and his colleagues on the role of post-goal
the notion that individuals who commit to difficult and specific implementation planning, the impact of affective experiences on
goals outperform individuals who adopt ‘‘do your best” task goals, non-consciously mediated behaviors (Wegge, van Dick, Fisher,
leading the authors to conclude that ‘‘it is time for a change in West, & Dawson, 2006; Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996), and recent
research emphasis toward underlying mechanisms which con- studies on non-conscious goal priming in goal setting (Stajkovic
tribute to these relationships” (Mento et al., 1987, p. 74), and sug- et al., 2006) suggest more research is needed to understand the
gesting that future research investigate the roles of self-efficacy role of emotional processing in goal choice.
and strategy development.
During and following the 1990s, the goal setting theoretical tent 3.4. Action/self regulation processes: the ‘‘what” of motivation
broadened as researchers examined the impact of goal construal on
the goal-performance relation in individuals (Drach-Zahavy & Erez, Theorizing and research on ‘‘what” questions address the
2002) and teams (Nahrgang et al., 2013) and the effects of perfor- strategies that individuals use to allocate and manage cognitive
mance goals on resource depletion and unethical behavior (Welsh and temporal resources during goal striving. Research on goal pur-
& Ordonez, 2014). Although the rate of published studies in OBHDP suit burgeoned during the latter part of the 20th century following
on the influence of goal setting on performance has slowed over the introduction of theories in psychology aimed at understanding
the past 15 years, two recently published studies in the journal the processes through which individuals sought to accomplish a
extended goal setting theory to understanding the effects of goal goal. Social cognitive formulations by Bandura (1977a, 1977b;
priming and subconscious goals on performance (Chen & Latham, Bandura & Cervone, 1986) and Kanfer and Hagerman (1981) con-
2014; Shantz & Latham, 2009). ceptualized goal pursuit in terms of the self-regulatory processes
by which individuals monitor, evaluate, and react to changes in
performance and goal progress. Carver and Scheier (1981) pre-
3.3.4. Theory of action phases sented a cybernetic control model that highlighted the role of
Whereas Locke’s initial theory focused on the causal influence action to reduce perceived goal-performance discrepancies. Goll-
of conscious goal attributes on performance, Gollwitzer et al. witzer’s model highlighted the role of planning implementation
(1990; Heckhausen & Gollwitzer, 1987) proposed a formulation during goal pursuit as a key determinant of the goal enactment
that highlighted differences in cognitive processing before and process. The introduction of these approaches in OBHDP by
after goal adoption, and the influence of post-goal planning inten- Campion and Lord (1982) and Bandura (Bandura, 1991; Bandura
tions on performance. According to the Theory of Action Phases & Cervone, 1986) was soon followed by an integration of goal set-
(and similar to assumptions underlying goal choice in ting and self-regulation models under the rubric of Goal Setting
Expectancy-Value theory), individuals are proposed to be delibera- Theory (Latham & Locke, 1991; Locke, 1991), prompting a new
tive and calculative during the pre-decisional, or goal selection generation of motivation research that continues today. OBHDP
phase. Key features of this deliberative mindset include greater published a number of studies documenting the determinants of
openness and recall of information provided by the environment self-efficacy judgments in task goal choice and group decision-
across all options and a balanced assessment of the desirability making (Henry & Sniezek, 1993; Phillips, 2001; Sanna & Mark,
of different goals. After making their decision and committing to 1995; Silver, Mitchell, & Gist, 1995; Tabernero & Wood, 1999;
a particular goal, however, individuals are proposed to shift to an Whyte, 1998) and the effects of self-efficacy as a key determinant
implemental mindset. In contrast to the deliberative mindset, cog- of goal choice and decision-making (Goncalo, Polman, & Maslach,
nitive processing during this phase narrows to include goal-related 2010; Klein & Kunda, 1994; Stone, 1994; Sue-Chan & Ong, 2002;
information and individuals demonstrate an array of cognitive Tasa & Whyte, 2005; Wood, Bandura, & Bailey, 1990), and during
judgment biases designed to support planning, and persistence, goal pursuit (Beck & Schmidt, 2012; Kozlowski et al., 2001; Seo &
including stronger positive beliefs in goal value, underestimation Ilies, 2009).
of risk to goal attainment, overestimation of the probability of goal
success, and underestimation of the time needed for goal accom- 3.4.1. Self-efficacy
plishment. Evidence over the past decade provides support for One important research direction in this integrative area of
the notion of differences in cognitive processing before and after study pertains to the critical role of self-efficacy in motivation.
a goal is adopted and the role of implementation planning in per- Self-efficacy judgments (Bandura, 1977a, 1977b) refer to the indi-
formance (see Gollwitzer & Oettingen, 2012). Findings to support vidual’s perceived capabilities for attaining specific goals or task
the Action Phases model are relatively recent, although the impli- outcomes. According to Bandura (1986), self-efficacy judgments
cations for practice appear promising. For example, recent meta- develop from the integration of information from four sources;
analytic findings by Webb, Gallo, Miles, Gollwitzer, and Sheeran performance feedback, vicarious experiences, previous perfor-
(2012) show a significant positive effect for the development of mance history, and social influence. Findings by Bandura and
emotion control implementation intentions, relative to no emotion Cervone (1986) and others (see Stajkovic & Luthans, 1998) provide
control conditions. support for notion of a generally positive relationship between
R. Kanfer, G. Chen / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 136 (2016) 6–19 13

self-efficacy and indices of task motivation. Using a control theory to identify key moderators of the resource depletion effect (e.g.,
perspective, however, Vancouver (2005, 2008, Vancouver & the effect of longevity on this phenomenon, particularly in the con-
Kendall, 2006; Vancouver, Thompson, & Williams, 2001) have text of goal pursuit over long periods of time).
argued that these findings are typically obtained using between-
subject rather than within-subject designs. Using a within-
3.4.3. Evaluative summary of ‘‘what” approaches
subject design, Vancouver and his colleagues showed a negative,
Research on the determinants and consequences of goal pursuit
rather than positive relationship between self-efficacy and
has burgeoned over the past few decades. An extensive body of
performance.
research grounded in social cognitive theories of motivation has
The inconsistent findings have yielded a lively debate on theory
accumulated for the cognitive processes involved in goal pursuit
and research design. Theoretical arguments for the inconsistency
and the role of self-efficacy in effective self-regulation. More recent
in findings focus on control theory vs. social cognitive explanations
work has employed an information-processing framework to
regarding the role of self-efficacy following goal-performance dis-
investigate the processes by which individuals modulate affect
crepancy reduction. Operational arguments point to differences in
and attention in the service of goal attainment (Johnson, Chang,
findings as a function of using between-subject versus within-
& Lord, 2006). Baumeister et al.’s (1998) theory of ego depletion,
subject designs. Attempts to reconcile this inconsistency by Sch-
while controversial and in need of further evaluation, has also
midt and his colleagues (Beck & Schmidt, 2012; Schmidt &
stimulated a number of important basic questions about how to
DeShon, 2009, 2010; Tolli & Schmidt, 2008) found that features
conceptualize self-regulation and the costs of self-control over
of the environment, such as prior task performance and perfor-
time.
mance ambiguity and change from average level of self-efficacy
moderate the within-person relationship between self-efficacy
and performance. 4. Summary and future research directions
Another important issue in goal pursuit pertains to what is reg-
ulated. During the 1980s, Naylor, Pritchard, and Ilgen (1980) and Our review of the motivation literature related to work and
Kanfer and Ackerman (1989) proposed models that recast motiva- organizational psychology over the past half century leads us to
tion in terms a resource allocation process of time and effort across form two broad conclusions. First, theory and research findings
activities. Integrating both goal choice and self-regulatory models, in motivation have focused largely on consciously-mediated pro-
Kanfer and Ackerman (1989) provided support for the viability of cesses (e.g., goal choice, self-regulation), predicated on cognitive
the resource perspective for understanding the differential effects formulations of mind and motives established mid-century. As this
of goal setting in the context of skill acquisition as a function of review attests, there is no question that this work has substantially
individual differences in cognitive abilities, task demands, and advanced our understanding of the key constructs (e.g., goals) and
resource allocations to off-task and self-regulatory processing. As mechanisms (e.g., self-efficacy) involved in motivation. At the
Beck and Schmidt (2012) note, the resource allocation model views same time, however, there has been increasing evidence to suggest
motivation as a ‘‘series of decisions about where to allocate that further progress in the field will require new or modified
resources and how many resources to allocate.” (p. 206), and pro- frameworks that take greater account of the individual’s experi-
vides a unifying framework for integrating theories of goal choice ences related to work. For example, as motivational scholars have
and goal pursuit. long noted, deliberative, consciously-mediated motivational pro-
cessing is the exception rather than the rule in daily life, with
3.4.2. Resource/ego depletion explicit modulation of one’s behavior most often required for situ-
The conceptualization of motivation during goal pursuit as a ations in which emotions and affect do not support the goal inten-
resource allocation process raises an important question about tion. In other words, how do situations and events encourage
the resource cost of engaging different forms of self-regulation. individuals to actively manage their work experiences and for
For example, during learning an individual may need to engage what purposes? Our review suggests a number of nascent and
in self-regulatory activities directed toward keeping disruptive, rapidly merging streams of research in different domains of psy-
negative emotions at bay. At other times, however, successful goal chology that have substantial potential for addressing this funda-
pursuit may involve engaging in self-regulatory strategies to mental question, including for example research investigating the
increase effort further, despite already high levels of performance role of non-conscious motives, and the role of emotions and affect
or increasing fatigue. In these instances individuals must often as they influence perception of context, instigate identity-based
employ self-control, a special form of self-regulation in which the processes, influence decision-making, and contribute to conflict
individual must allocate resources to task performance that during goal pursuit. In our opinion, while there is still much to
directly conflict with prevailing action tendencies. Baumeister be learned about the consciously-mediated motivational system
and his colleagues (Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Muraven, & Tice, (discussed below), future research efforts in this area will need
1998) have proposed that individuals possess a limited resource to take greater account of the role of non-conscious processes
capacity, and that self-control exerts a resource cost. During and to more precisely delineate how these processes affect goal
repeated task performance, failures in self-control arise from tem- choice and sustained goal pursuit.
porary resource depletion. A few studies in OBHDP provide support Further evidence for the shifting focus in motivation research
for this model with respect to unethical behavior (Gino, appearing in OBHP/OBHDP is provided by the steady decline in
Schweitzer, Mead, & Ariely, 2011) and daily work engagement the number of articles on the topic appearing in the journal over
(Lanaj, Johnson, & Barnes, 2014). However, evidence for the notion the past 50 years. Across more than 2500 articles published in
of self-control as a resource of limited capacity remains mixed, the journal between 1966 and September 2015, we found only
with meta-analytic findings by Hagger, Wood, Stiff, and 194 (or about 7.8%) contained the term motivation in the subject
Chatzisarantis (2010) showing strong support, but findings by term.1 More than half of these articles (N = 111) appeared in the
Carter, Kofler, Forster, and McCullough (2015) provide little sup-
1
port. Inzlicht, Schmeichel, and Macrae (2014) suggested further These values were obtained using the EBSCO host/Academic Search Complete
search database facility. First we searched for all OBHP/OBHDP publication entries
that ego depletion findings may be more parsimoniously explained between 1966 and September, 2015. We then searched for all journal entries that
based by changes in task motivation. Findings by Gino et al. (2011), used the word ‘‘motivation” in the subject term, and repeated the process again for
Lanaj et al. (2014) and others likewise suggest research is needed the time periods 1966–1985 and 1986–September 2015.
14 R. Kanfer, G. Chen / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 136 (2016) 6–19

20 years before the journal name change in 1985, with proportion- ceptions of goal progress, Koo & Fishbach, 2014), (3) the role of
ately fewer studies (N = 83) appearing during the 30 years after the affective events on cognitive processes during goal pursuit (e.g.,
journal name change. This trend is particularly interesting, as it cor- Seo & Ilies, 2009), and (4) how information processing influences
responds to the rise of OBHDP as a leading journal in judgment and the affect – motivation relationship (e.g., Forgas & George, 2001).
decision making since 1985 (see Edwards, 2002; Weber, 1998). As The goal paradigm has also stimulated renewed scholarly inter-
noted by Edwards in his 2002 editorial, ‘‘Several board members est in the ‘‘what” of motivation, and there is broad agreement that
expressed the belief that, while OBHDP had solidified its stature as purposive motivation involves the allocation of one’s cognitive
primary outlet for research in judgment and decision making, it resources across activities over time. Although few OBHDP articles
had drifted away from organizational behavior and psychology” directly address resource allocation processes, three topics cur-
(pp. 1–2). rently dominate the broader motivation literature. The first per-
In line with this sentiment, our review suggests that articles tains to delineating the independent and joint influence of
published in OBHP until the mid-1980s focused on core issues of person and contextual factors on resource allocation strategies in
person and contextual influences on motivation studied in the multiple goal environments (Kanfer & Kerry, 2012; Tolli &
fields of organizational behavior and organizational psychology. Schmidt, 2008). The second relates to Baumeister et al.’s (1998)
Seminal works on needs and explicit motives, goals and goal set- theory of ego depletion and concerns the development of better
ting, and job characteristics appeared during this period and stim- assessment tools for evaluating resource costs associated with
ulated hundreds of additional studies. From the mid-1980s self-control and other self-regulatory processes. The third pertains
through present, however, motivation research published in to understanding when and how implicit motives (that presum-
OBHDP has focused largely on the cognitive and affective processes ably do not require attentional resources) conflict with purposive
that underlie decision making and goal choice (e.g., goal processes, goals, and the potentially different patterns of goal pursuit associ-
goal orientation, regulatory focus, epistemic motivation, and moti- ated with different conflict patterns.
vational dynamics), rather than the broader context within which In contrast to OBHDP articles that continue to advance our
individuals work in organizations. In line with this trend, there understanding of ‘‘how” and (to a lesser extent) ‘‘what” occurs dur-
are fewer citations to articles published in OBHP/OBHDP, relative ing motivation, we found less attention given to the ‘‘why,”
to those published in Academy of Management Journal or Journal ‘‘where,” and ‘‘when” aspects of motivation. Research in the journal
of Applied Psychology, in large-scale and influential meta- continues to focus on the impact of a single trait or state variable
analyses of work design (Humphrey, Nahrgang, & Morgeson, (e.g., goal orientation), though there is accumulating evidence to
2007), leader-member exchange (Gerstner & Day, 1997), transfor- warrant closer attention to the role of motive complexes (Kanfer,
mational leadership (Judge & Piccolo, 2004), and team processes 2012). With the exception of work on intrinsic motivation, rela-
(LePine, Piccolo, Jackson, Mathieu, & Saul, 2008). tively few studies in the journal (e.g., Amit & Sagiv, 2013; Kim,
As such, on the one hand, OBHDP has become an important Van Dyne, Kamdar, & Johnson, 2013) have examined the antece-
translator of basic advances in psychology, particularly personality dents or consequences of other motive complexes (e.g., epistemic
and social psychology, as they may apply to organizations and motivation, prosocial motivation) on work goals, motivational tra-
work. On the other hand, we believe that the near exclusive focus jectories and performance patterns over time. Another promising
on intra-individual psychological processes may miss a greater area pertains to understanding how particular elements of a work
opportunity to link these processes to important contextual factors context (e.g., team configuration, leadership style) affect goal con-
in the person’s history, as well as to the organizational context strual and self-regulation processes during goal pursuit. Finally, a
within which work experiences and behaviors occur. As noted by nascent body of research in need of further research attention indi-
Kanfer, Chen, and Pritchard (2008), over 50 years of research on cates the importance of individual differences in future time orien-
work motivation (from middle of the 20th Century until today) tation and its interaction with features of the environment to affect
has uncovered a great deal of knowledge about the content of work individual and organizational outcomes, such as team performance
motivation (i.e., the ‘‘what” and ‘‘how” questions noted above); (Mohammed & Harrison, 2013); preferences (Kivetz & Tyler, 2007),
however, there is more to be learned about motivation from the work behavior (Kooij, Betts, & Kanfer, 2015), and retirement
perspectives of the context in which individuals work (i.e., the decision-making (Feldman, 2013).
‘‘where” and ‘‘when” questions), as well as from the perspective
of an individual’s experience of change over time (i.e., the ‘‘where” 4.1. Future research directions
and ‘‘why” questions).
Consistent with these observations, our review of the OBHP/ Our review of the decision science literature in OHBDP articles
OBHDP literature suggests that there has been substantial, but has also revealed research on topics that have yet to receive sub-
uneven progress in understanding the building blocks of motiva- stantial attention in the work motivation literature. A sampling
tion in work and organizational settings. By far the largest gain of these topic areas are described briefly below.
has been in the development and application of expectancy-value
theories, goal setting formulations, and dynamic formulations of 4.1.1. Temporal discounting
goal pursuit to better understand ‘‘how” individuals choose and Steel and Konig (2006) recently proposed a theory of motivation
sustain a course of action. Research on expectancy theory peaked that integrates prospect theory and hyperbolic discounting with
in the late 1970s and early 1980s. Locke’s (1968) landmark article expectancy-value and need theories, suggesting that people ‘‘tend
on the influence of goals on task motivation set in motion a torrent to overestimate low-probability events and underestimate high-
of research activity that continues until today (albeit at a less probability events” (p. 898). Their theory also suggests that adding
intense level), including over two dozen studies in OBHP/OBHDP the role of time (e.g., timing and/or duration of events) to motiva-
on the topic to date. The rise of the goal paradigm also sparked tion theories can enrich understanding of goal choice and goal
strong research interest in self-efficacy and self-regulatory pro- striving processes. However, few studies have been done to test
cesses. Research on these topics in OBHDP and other journals has the theory or to examine temporal discounting effects in job per-
highlighted the need for further research attention in four areas: formance. For example, recent findings by Shen, Fishbach, and
(1) the role of implicit motives and goal conflicts on regulatory pro- Hsee (2015) indicate that reward uncertainty may increase moti-
cessing (Gröpel & Kehr, 2014; Sripada, Swain, Ho, & Swain, 2014), vation, particularly when individuals focus on the process of goal
(2) identifying key determinants of regulatory dynamics (e.g., per- pursuit, which is more proximal to effort allocation. Another inter-
R. Kanfer, G. Chen / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 136 (2016) 6–19 15

esting application of discounting of delayed rewards pertains to changes in the nature of work and working over the next half-
motivation and behavior in groups (Charlton et al., 2013). Future century, we think it will be important to cast a wider net going for-
research is needed to better understand how multi-team system ward, and more closely evaluate how advances in more distant
and local team goals and priorities may differentially mitigate dis- subdisciplines, such as lifespan psychology, affective neuroscience,
counting effects at the individual level. and the broader organization sciences may inform research on
motivation in organized work settings. Research in lifespan psy-
chology, for example, provides evidence for important age-
4.1.2. Embodied cognition and motivation
related differences in cognitive abilities and work motives that
The past few decades have witnessed increased interest in
have been shown to have important consequences for motivation
embodied cognition, or the way in which bodily, automatic emo-
in the context of skill training and work engagement. However,
tional responses to various features of social environments con-
to integrate these and other developments in psychology and
tribute to decision making and action (Damasio, 1999). Although
related fields, motivation researchers may well need to expand
the bulk of research in this area to date has focused on the effects
beyond the standard, time-limited experimental paradigm, which
of emotions and actions on brain processes and their link to cogni-
has become commonplace in this journal. We strongly encourage
tive processes involved in action, the findings emerging from this
the use of multi-level, longitudinal field experiments, experience-
interdisciplinary paradigm have spurred new research related to
sampling studies, and intervention studies to allow for the evalua-
motivation in three promising areas. First, Oettingen and
tion of motivational and behavioral variability as a function of
Gollwitzer (2009) suggest that the embodied system may affect
time, work events, the individual’s history, and the social context
goal pursuit through its effects on implementation planning.
of action – determinants of motivation particularly important for
Wieber, Thurmer, and Gollwitzer (2015) integrated cognitive and
managing many modern organizational behavior problem spaces,
physiological findings on implementation planning effects to sug-
such as expatriation, work transitions, employee diversity, multi-
gest that action control through implementation intentions (that
team systems, and high-stakes work. We believe such a broaden-
incorporate embodied emotion regulation processes) may demand
ing is essential for maintaining the journal’s eminent role in the
less effort than action control through goal intentions. Further
field going forward.
research examining the impact of different implementation plans
on goal accomplishment is needed to evaluate this and related
hypotheses.
Acknowledgements
A second area of interest pertains to the impact of incentives.
Many organizations employ multiple and diverse types of incen-
The authors wish to thank Linn Van Dyne and the three review-
tive plans to promote work motivation, including for example
ers for their insightful suggestions on earlier versions of this
monetary incentives (e.g., bonus pay) and non-monetary incen-
manuscript.
tives (e.g., employee recognition, free food, or travel). Understand-
ing how individuals integrate these incentives and their role in
decision-making and motivational processing has theoretical and References
practical implications, particularly for human resource manage-
ment practices that seek to promote engagement and build new Adams, J. S. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.). Advances in
behavior patterns. Research by Braver and his colleagues (e.g., experimental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 267–299). New York, NY: Academic
Press.
Chiew & Braver, 2014; Krug & Braver, 2014) provides initial exper-
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human
imental and neuroscience evidence for how these incentives are Decision Processes, 50, 179–211.
integrated and their emotional influence on cognitive control in Alderfer, C. P. (1969). An empirical test of a new theory of human needs.
Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 4, 143–175.
task performance. Studies to examine the scale-up of these find-
Amit, A., & Sagiv, L. (2013). The role of epistemic motivation in individuals’ response
ings to the workplace represent an exciting and important new to decision complexity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
direction in work motivation. 121, 104–117.
A third area of interest in theory and research pertains to the Andersen, S. M., & Chen, S. (2002). The relational self: An interpersonal social–
cognitive theory. Psychological Review, 109, 619–645.
self and social identity as it affects motivation. Identity-based Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behavior: A
motivation (e.g., Oyserman, 2007, 2015) highlights the role of meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 471–499.
broad social factors, such as gender, social class, and ethnicity, on Austin, J. T. (1989). Effects of shifts in goal origin on goal acceptance and attainment.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 44, 415–435.
identity activation and the role of identity-congruence in behav- Bandura, A. (1977a). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
ioral choice and action. Although the theory has been focused to Bandura, A. (1977b). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change.
date largely on the effects of situationally-cued identity and Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory.
identity-congruence effects in educational, social and health con- Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
texts, findings by Lewis and Oyserman (2015) on the role of iden- Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of self-regulation. Organizational
tity congruence in retirement saving suggests new directions for Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 248–287.
Bandura, A., & Cervone, D. (1986). Differential engagement of self-reactive
future research investigating identity congruence in other work-
influences in cognitive motivation. Organizational Behavior and Human
related behaviors, such as innovation in teams. Decision Processes, 38, 92–113.
Bass, B. M. (1985). Leadership and performance beyond expectations. New York, NY:
Free Press.
5. A final observation Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is
the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,
1252–1265.
Overall, our review of the OBHP/OBHDP literature shows a Beck, J. W., & Schmidt, A. M. (2012). Taken out of context? Cross-level effects of
remarkable progress in motivation over the past half century, par- between-person self-efficacy and difficulty on the within-person relationship of
self-efficacy with resource allocation and performance. Organizational Behavior
ticularly with respect to understanding the basic psychological
and Human Decision Processes, 119, 195–208.
mechanisms involved in deliberate goal choice, self-regulation, Beersma, B., Homan, A. C., Van Kleef, G. A., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2013). Outcome
and decision-making and the influence of explicit motives and abil- interdependence shapes the effects of prevention focus on team processes and
ities on these processes. These advances have been made possible performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 121,
194–203.
largely by drawing on advances in the neighboring subdisciplines Benjamin, L., & Flynn, F. J. (2006). Leadership style and regulatory mode: Value from
of cognitive and social-personality psychology. As we contemplate fit? Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 100, 216–230.
16 R. Kanfer, G. Chen / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 136 (2016) 6–19

Boal, K. B., & Cummings, L. L. (1981). Cognitive evaluation theory: An experimental Durham, C. C., Knight, D., & Locke, E. A. (1997). Effects of leader role, team-set goal
test of processes and outcomes. Organizational Behavior and Human difficulty, efficacy, and tactics on team effectiveness. Organizational Behavior
Performance, 28, 289–310. and Human Decision Processes, 72, 203–231.
Brebels, L., De Cremer, D., & Sedikides, C. (2008). Retaliation as a response to Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American
procedural unfairness: A self-regulatory approach. Journal of Personality and Psychologist, 41, 1040–1048.
Social Psychology, 95, 1511–1525. Earley, P. C., & Kanfer, R. (1985). The influence of component participation and role
Brockner, J., & Higgins, E. T. (2001). Regulatory focus theory: Implications for the models on goal acceptance, goal satisfaction, and performance. Organizational
study of emotions at work. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 36, 378–390.
Processes, 86, 35–66. Earley, P. C., & Perry, B. C. (1987). Work plan availability and performance: An
Button, S. B., Mathieu, J. E., & Zajac, D. M. (1996). Goal orientation in organizational assessment of task strategy priming on subsequent task completion.
research: A conceptual and empirical foundation. Organizational Behavior and Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 39, 279–302.
Human Decision Processes, 67, 26–48. Edwards, J. R. (2002). The past, present, and future of Organizational Behavior and
Campbell, D. J., & Gingrich, K. F. (1986). The interactive effects of task complexity Human Decision Processes. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
and participation on task performance: A field experiment. Organizational 87, 1–4.
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 38, 162–180. Elliot, A. J., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). Approach and avoidance achievement goals
Campion, M. A., & Lord, R. G. (1982). A control systems conceptualization of the and intrinsic motivation: A mediational analysis. Journal of Personality and Social
goal-setting and changing process. Organizational Behavior and Human Psychology, 70, 461–475.
Performance, 30, 265–287. Erez, M., Gopher, D., & Arzi, N. (1990). Effects of goal difficulty, self-set goals, and
Carter, E. C., Kofler, L. M., Forster, D. E., & McCullough, M. E. (2015). A series of meta- monetary rewards on dual task performance. Organizational Behavior and
analytic tests of the depletion effect: Self-control does not seem to rely on a Human Decision Processes, 47, 247–269.
limited resource. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 796–815. Evans, M. G., Kiggundu, M. N., & House, R. J. (1979). A partial test and extension of
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: A control-theory the job characteristics model of motivation. Organizational Behavior and Human
approach to human behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag. Performance, 24, 354–381.
Cerasoli, C. P., Nicklin, J. M., & Ford, M. T. (2014). Intrinsic motivation and extrinsic Farr, J. L., Vance, R. J., & McIntyre, R. M. (1977). Further examinations of the
incentives jointly predict performance: A 40-year meta-analysis. Psychological relationship between reward contingency and intrinsic motivation.
Bulletin, 140, 980–1008. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 20, 31–53.
Charlton, S. R., Yi, R., Poerter, C., Carter, A. E., Bickel, W., & Rachlin, H. (2013). Now Feldman, Daniel C. (2013). Feeling lit it’s time to retire: A fit perspective on early
for me, later for us? Effects of group context on temporal discounting. Journal of retirement decisions. In M. Wang (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of retirement
Behavioral Decision Making, 26, 118–127. (pp. 280–292). New York: Oxford University Press.
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1996). Automatic activation of impression formation Ferris, D. L., Spence, J. R., Brown, D. J., & Heller, D. (2012). Interpersonal injustice and
and memorization goals: Nonconscious goal priming reproduces effects of workplace deviance: The role of esteem threat. Journal of Management, 38,
explicit task instructions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1788–1811.
464–478. Fishbach, A., & Choi, J. (2012). When thinking about goals undermines goal pursuit.
Chen, G., & Kanfer, R. (2006). Toward a systems theory of motivated behavior in Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 118, 99–107.
work teams. Research in Organizational Behavior, 27, 223–267. Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An
Chen, G., Kanfer, R., DeShon, R. P., Mathieu, J. E., & Kozlowski, S. W. J. (2009). The introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
motivating potential of teams: A test and extension of Chen & Kanfer’s (2006) Forgas, J. P., & George, J. M. (2001). Affective influences on judgments and behavior
model. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 110, 45–55. in organizations: An information processing perspective. Organizational
Chen, G., Kirkman, B. L., Kanfer, R., Allen, D., & Rosen, B. (2007). A multilevel study of Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 86, 3–34.
leadership, empowerment, and performance in teams. Journal of Applied Forward, J., & Zander, A. (1971). Choice of unattainable group goals and
Psychology, 92, 331–346. effects on performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 6,
Chen, X., & Latham, G. P. (2014). The effect of priming learning vs. performance 184–199.
goals on a complex task. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, Frost, P. J., & Mahoney, T. A. (1976). Goal setting and the task process: I. An
125, 88–97. interactive influence on individual performance. Organizational Behavior and
Chen, G., & Mathieu, J. E. (2008). Goal orientation dispositions and performance Human Performance, 17, 328–350.
trajectories: The roles of supplementary and complementary situational Gagné, M., & Deci, E. L. (2005). Self-determination theory and work motivation.
inducements. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 106, 21–38. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 331–362.
Chiew, K. S., & Braver, T. S. (2014). Dissociable influences of reward motivation and Gerstner, C. R., & Day, D. V. (1997). Meta-analytic review of leader–member
positive emotion on cognitive control. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral exchange theory: Correlates and construct issues. Journal of Applied Psychology,
Neuroscience, 14, 509–529. 82, 827–844.
Colquitt, J. A., Conlon, D. E., Wesson, M. J., Porter, C. O. L. H., & Ng, K. Y. (2001). Justice Gino, F., Schweitzer, M. E., Mead, N. L., & Ariely, D. (2011). Unable to resist
at the millennium: A meta-analytic review of 25 years of organizational justice temptation: How self-control depletion promotes unethical behavior.
research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 425–445. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 115, 191–203.
Colquitt, J. A., Greenberg, J., & Zapata-Phelan, C. P. (2005). What is organizational Gollwitzer, P. M. (1990). Action phases and mind-sets. In E. T. Higgins & R. M.
justice? A historical overview. In J. Greenberg & J. A. Colquitt (Eds.), Handbook of Sorrentino (Eds.). Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social
organizational justice (pp. 3–56). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum. behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 53–92). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.
Colquitt, J. A., Scott, B. A., Rodell, J. B., Long, D. M., Zapata, C. P., Conlon, D. E., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2003). Why we thought that action mindsets affect illusions of
Wesson, M. J. (2013). Justice at the millennium, a decade later: A meta-analytic control. Psychological Inquiry, 14, 259–267.
test of social exchange and affect-based perspectives. Journal of Applied Gollwitzer, P. M., Heckhausen, H., & Ratajczak, H. (1990). From weighing to willing:
Psychology, 98, 199–236. Approaching a change decision through pre- or postdecisional mentation.
Cropanzano, R., Paddock, L., Rupp, D. E., Bagger, J., & Baldwin, A. (2008). How Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 45, 41–65.
regulatory focus impacts the process-by-outcome interaction for perceived Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2012). Goal pursuit. In R. M. Ryan (Ed.), The Oxford
fairness and emotions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, handbook of human motivation (pp. 208–231). New York: Oxford University
105, 36–51. Press.
Crowe, E., & Higgins, E. T. (1997). Regulatory focus and strategic inclinations: Goncalo, J. A., Polman, E., & Maslach, C. (2010). Can confidence come too soon?
Promotion and prevention in decision-making. Organizational Behavior and Collective efficacy, conflict and group performance over time. Organizational
Human Decision Processes, 69, 117–132. Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 113, 13–24.
Crown, D. F., & Rosse, J. G. (1995). Yours, mine, and ours: Facilitating group Graen, G. B., & Uhl-Bien, M. (1995). Relationship-based approach to leadership:
productivity through the integration of individual and group goals. Development of leader-member exchange (LMX) theory of leadership over 25
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 64, 138–150. years: Applying a multi-level multi-domain perspective. Leadership Quarterly, 6,
da Motta Veiga, S. P., & Turban, D. B. (2014). Are affect and perceived stress 219–247.
detrimental or beneficial to job seekers? The role of learning goal orientation in Grant, A. M. (2008). Does intrinsic motivation fuel the prosocial fire? Motivational
job search and self-regulation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision synergy in predicting persistence, performance, and productivity. Journal of
Processes, 125, 193–204. Applied Psychology, 93, 48–58.
Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of Grant, A. M., Campbell, E. M., Chen, G., Cottone, K., Lapedis, D., & Lee, K. (2007).
consciousness. Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt College Publishers. Impact and the art of motivation maintenance: The effects of contact with
Deci, E. L. (1972). The effects of contingent and noncontingent rewards and controls beneficiaries on persistence behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human
on intrinsic motivation. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 8, Decision Processes, 103, 53–67.
217–229. Grant, A. M., Nurmohamed, S., Ashford, S. J., & Dekas, K. (2011). The performance
DeShon, R. P., & Alexander, R. A. (1996). Goal setting effects on implicit and explicit implications of ambivalent initiative: The interplay of autonomous and
learning of complex tasks. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision controlled motivations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
Processes, 65, 18–36. 116, 241–251.
Drach-Zahavy, A., & Erez, M. (2002). Challenge versus threat effects on the goal- Grant, A. M., & Parker, S. K. (2009). Redesigning work design theories: The rise of
performance relationship. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision relational and proactive perspectives. Academy of Management Annals, 3,
Processes, 88, 667–682. 317–375.
R. Kanfer, G. Chen / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 136 (2016) 6–19 17

Greenberger, D. B., Strasser, S., Cummings, L. L., & Dunham, R. B. (1989). The impact support as predictors of OCB. Organizational behavior and Human Decision
of personal control on performance and satisfaction. Organizational Behavior and Processes, 121, 231–245.
Human Decision Processes, 43, 29–51. Kivetz, Y., & Tyler, T. R. (2007). Tomorrow I’ll be me: The effect of time perspective
Gröpel, P., & Kehr, H. M. (2014). Motivation and self-control: Implicit motives on the activation of idealistic versus pragmatic selves. Organizational Behavior
moderate the exertion of self-control in motive-related tasks. Journal of and Human Decision Processes, 102, 193–211.
Personality, 82, 317–328. Klein, H. J. (1991). Further evidence on the relationship between goal setting and
Hackman, J. R., Brousseau, K. R., & Weiss, J. A. (1976). The interaction of task design expectancy theories. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 49,
and group performance strategies in determining group effectiveness. 230–257.
Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 16, 350–365. Klein, H. J., Austin, J. T., & Cooper, J. T. (2008). Goal choice and decision processes. In
Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. (1976). Motivation through the design of work: Test R. Kanfer, G. Chen, & R. D. Pritchard (Eds.), Work motivation: Past, present, and
of a theory. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 16, 250–279. future (pp. 101–150). New York: Routledge/Taylor Francis Group.
Hackman, J. R., Pearce, J. L., & Wolfe, J. C. (1978). Effects of changes in job Klein, W. M., & Kunda, Z. (1994). Exaggerated self-assessments and the preference
characteristics on work attitudes and behaviors: A naturally occurring quasi- for controllable risks. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 59,
experiment. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 21, 289–304. 410–427.
Hackman, J. R., & Porter, L. W. (1968). Expectancy theory predictions of work Koo, M., & Fishbach, A. (2014). Dynamics of self-regulation: How (un)accomplished
effectiveness. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 3, 417–426. goal actions affect motivation. Motivation Science, 1, 73–90.
Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2010). Ego depletion and Kooij, D. T., Betts, M., & Kanfer, R. (2015). Future time perspective: A systematic review
the strength model of self-control: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, and meta-analysis Unpublished manuscript : . Tilberg University.
495–525. Kozlowski, S. W. J., Gully, S. M., Brown, K. G., Salas, E., Smith, E. M., & Nason, E. R.
Hamner, W. C., & Harnett, D. L. (1974). Goal setting, performance and satisfaction in (2001). Effects of training goals and goal orientation traits on multidimensional
an interdependent task. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 12, training outcomes and performance adaptability. Organizational Behavior and
217–230. Human Decision Processes, 85, 1–31.
Heckhausen, H., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1987). Thought contents and cognitive Krug, M. K., & Braver, T. S. (2014). Motivation and cognitive control: Going beyond
functioning in motivational versus volitional states of mind. Motivation and monetary incentives. In E. Bijleveld & H. Aarts (Eds.), The psychological science of
Emotion, 11, 101–120. money (pp. 137–162). New York: Springer Science + Business Media.
Heneman, H. G., & Schwab, D. P. (1972). Evaluation of research on expectancy Lanaj, K., Johnson, R. E., & Barnes, C. M. (2014). Beginning the workday yet already
theory predictions of employee performance. Psychological Bulletin, 78, 1–9. depleted? Consequences of late-night smartphone use and sleep. Organizational
Henry, R. A., & Sniezek, J. A. (1993). Situational factors affecting judgments of future Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 124, 11–23.
performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 54, Latham, G. P., & Locke, E. A. (1991). Self-regulation through goal setting.
104–132. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 212–247.
Hinsz, V. B., Kalnbach, L. R., & Lorentz, N. R. (1997). Using judgmental anchors to Lawler, E. E., III, & Suttle, J. L. (1972). A causal correlational test of the need hierarchy
establish challenging self-set goals without jeopardizing commitment. concept. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 7, 265–287.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 71, 287–308. Lawler, E. E., III, & Suttle, J. L. (1973). Expectancy theory and job behavior.
Hollenbeck, J. R., & Brief, A. P. (1987). The effects of individual differences and goal Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 9, 482–503.
origin on goal setting and performance. Organizational Behavior and Human LePine, J. A., Piccolo, R. F., Jackson, C. L., Mathieu, J. E., & Saul, J. R. (2008). A meta-
Decision Processes, 40, 392–414. analysis of teamwork processes: Tests of a multidimensional model and
Humphrey, S. E., Nahrgang, J. D., & Morgeson, F. P. (2007). Integrating motivational, relationships with team effectiveness criteria. Personnel Psychology, 61,
social, and contextual work design features: A meta-analytic summary and 273–307.
theoretical extension of the work design literature. Journal of Applied Psychology, Lewis, N. A., Jr., & Oyserman, D. (2015). When does the future begin? Time and
92, 1332–1356. metrics matter, connecting present to future selves. Psychological Science, 26,
Inzlicht, M., Schmeichel, B. J., & Macrae, C. N. (2014). Why self-control seems (but 816–825.
may not be) limited. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. Li, A., Evans, J., Christian, M. S., Gilliland, S. W., Kausel, E. E., & Stein, J. H. (2011). The
tics.2013.12.009. effects of managerial regulatory fit priming on reactions to explanations.
Johnson, R. E., Chang, C.-H., & Lord, R. G. (2006). Moving from cognition to behavior: Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 115, 268–282.
What the research says. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 381–415. Livi, S., Kruglanski, A. W., Pierro, A., Mannetti, L., & Kenny, D. A. (2015). Epistemic
Johnson, R. E., Lanaj, K., & Barnes, C. M. (2014). The good and bad of being fair: motivation and perpetuation of group culture: Effects of need for cognitive
Effects of procedural and interactional justice behaviors on actors’ regulatory closure on trans-generational norm transmission. Organizational Behavior and
resources. Journal of Applied Psychology, 99, 635–650. Human Decision Processes, 129, 105–112.
Johnson, R. E., Selenta, C., & Lord, R. G. (2006). When organizational justice and the Locke, E. A. (1968). Toward a theory of task motivation and incentives.
self-concept meet: Consequences for the organization and its members. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 3, 157–189.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 99, 175–201. Locke, E. A. (1991). The motivation sequence, the motivation hub, and the
Judge, T. A., & Piccolo, R. F. (2004). Transformational and transactional leadership: A motivation core. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50,
meta-analytic test of their relative validity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 288–299.
755–768. Locke, E. A., Shaw, K. N., Saari, L. M., & Latham, G. P. (1981). Goal setting and task
Judge, T. A., Scott, B. A., & Ilies, R. (2006). Hostility, job attitudes, and workplace performance: 1969–1980. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 125–152.
deviance: Test of a multilevel model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 126–138. Loundt, R. B., & Phillips, K. W. (2007). Working harder with the out-group: The
Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. L. (1989). Motivation and cognitive abilities: An impact of social category diversity on motivation gains. Organizational Behavior
integrative/aptitude-treatment interaction approach to skill acquisition. and Human Decision Processes, 103, 214–224.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 657–690. Lyons, B. J., & Scott, B. A. (2012). Integrating social exchange and affective
Kanfer, R., Chen, G., & Pritchard, R. (2008). Work motivation: Forging new explanations for the receipt of help and harm: A social network approach.
perspectives and directions in the post-millennium. In R. Kanfer, G. Chen, & R. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 117, 66–79.
D. Pritchard (Eds.), Work motivation: Past, present, and future (pp. 601–632). Maslow, A. H. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychological Review, 50,
New York, NY: Routledge Academic. 370–396.
Kanfer, R. (1990). Motivation theory and industrial/organizational psychology. In M. Maslow, A. H. (1954). Motivation and personality. New York: Harpers.
D. Dunnette & L. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational McClelland, D. C. (1986). Human motivation. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman and
psychology. Theory in industrial and organizational psychology (Vol. 1, Company.
pp. 75–170). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. McClelland, D. C., Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self-attributed and
Kanfer, F. H., & Hagerman, S. M. (1981). The role of self-regulation. In L. P. Rehm implicit motives differ? Psychological Review, 96, 690–702.
(Ed.), Behavior therapy for depression: Present status and future directions Mento, A. J., Cartledge, N. D., & Locke, E. A. (1980). Maryland vs Michigan vs
(pp. 143–179). New York: Academic Press. Minnesota: Another look at the relationship of expectancy and goal difficulty to
Kanfer, R., & Heggestad, E. D. (1997). Motivational traits and skills: A person- task performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 25, 419–
centered approach to work motivation. Research in Organizational Behavior, 19, 440.
1–56. Mento, A. J., Steel, R. P., & Karren, R. J. (1987). A meta-analytic study of the effects of
Kanfer, R., & Kerry, M. (2012). Motivation in multiteam systems. In S. J. Zaccaro, M. goal-setting on task-performance: 1966 to 1984. Organizational Behavior and
A. Marks, & L. A. DeChurch (Eds.), Multiteam systems: An organization form for Human Decision Processes, 39, 52–83.
dynamic and complex environments. New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis. Miles, J. A., & Greenberg, J. (1993). Using punishment threats to attenuate social
Kanfer, R. (2012). Work motivation: Theory, practice, and future directions. In S. W. loafing effects among swimmers. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
J. Kozlowski (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of industrial and organizational Processes, 56, 246–265.
psychology (pp. 455–495). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Mitchell, T. R. (1974). Expectancy models of job satisfaction, occupational
Kehr, H. M. (2004). Integrating implicit motives, explicit motives, and perceived preference and effort: A theoretical, methodological, and empirical appraisal.
abilities: The compensatory model of work motivation and volition. Academy of Psychological Bulletin, 81, 1053–1077.
Management Review, 29, 470–499. Mitchell, T. R. (1982). Motivation – new directions for theory, research, and practice.
Kiggundu, M. N. (1983). Task interdependence and job design: Test of a theory. Academy of Management Review, 7, 80–88.
Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 31, 145–172. Mohammed, S., & Harrison, D. A. (2013). The clocks that time us are not the same: A
Kim, Y.-J., Van Dyne, L., Kamdar, D., & Johnson, R. E. (2013). Why and when do theory of temporal diversity, task characteristics, and performance in teams.
motives matter? An integrative model of motives, role cognitions, and social Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 122, 244–256.
18 R. Kanfer, G. Chen / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 136 (2016) 6–19

Morgeson, F. P., & Humphrey, S. E. (2006). The Work Design Questionnaire (WDQ): Seo, M., & Ilies, R. (2009). The role of self-efficacy, goal, and affect in dynamic
Developing and validating a comprehensive measure for assessing job design motivational self-regulation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
and the nature of work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 1321–1339. Processes, 109, 120–133.
Mueller, J. S. (2012). Why individuals in larger teams perform worse. Organizational Shah, J. (2003). The motivational looking glass: How significant others implicitly
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 117, 111–124. affect goal appraisals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 424–439.
Mulvey, P. W., & Klein, H. J. (1998). The impact of perceived loafing and collective Shalley, C. E., & Perry-Smith, J. E. (2001). Effects of social-psychological factors on
efficacy in group goal processes and group performance. Organizational Behavior creative performance: The role of informational and controlling expected
and Human Decision Processes, 74, 62–87. evaluation and modeling experience. Organizational Behavior and Human
Nahrgang, J. D., DeRue, D. S., Hollenbeck, J. R., Spitzmuller, M., Jundt, D. K., & Ilgen, D. Decision Processes, 84, 1–22.
R. (2013). Goal setting in teams: The impact of learning and performance goals Shantz, A., & Latham, G. P. (2009). An exploratory field experiment of the effect of
on process and performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision subconscious and conscious goals on employee performance. Organizational
Processes, 122, 12–21. Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 109, 9–17.
Naylor, J. C., & Briggs, G. E. (1966). Statement of editorial policy. Organizational Shapira, Z. (1989). Task choice and assigned goals as determinants of task
Behavior and Human Performance, 1, 1–2. motivation and performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Naylor, J. C., Pritchard, R. D., & Ilgen, D. R. (1980). A theory of behavior in Processes, 44, 141–165.
organizations. New York: Academic Press. Shen, L., Fishbach, A., & Hsee, C. K. (2015). The motivating-uncertainty effect:
Oettingen, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2009). Embodied goal pursuit. European Journal of Uncertainty increases resource investment in the process of reward pursuit.
Social Psychology, 39, 1210–1213. Journal of Consumer Research, 41, 1301–1315.
Oyserman, D. (2015). Pathways to success through identity-based motivation. New Sherf, E. N., & Venkataramani, V. (2015). Friend or foe? The impact of relational ties
York: Oxford University Press. with comparison others on outcome fairness and satisfaction judgments.
Oyserman, D. (2007). Social identity and self-regulation. In A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 128, 1–14.
Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 432–453). Silver, W. S., Mitchell, T. R., & Gist, M. E. (1995). Responses to successful and
New York: Guilford Press. unsuccessful performance: The moderating effect of self-efficacy on the
Payne, S. C., Youngcourt, S. S., & Beaubien, J. M. (2007). Meta-analytic examination relationship between performance and attributions. Organizational Behavior
of the goal orientation nomological net. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, and Human Decision Processes, 62, 286–299.
128–150. Smith, K. G., Locke, E. A., & Barry, D. (1990). Goal setting, planning, and
Phillips, J. M. (2001). The role of decision influence and team performance in organizational performance: An experimental simulation. Organizational
member self-efficacy, withdrawal, satisfaction with the leader, and willingness Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 46, 118–134.
to return. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 84, 122–147. Sripada, C., Swain, J. D., Ho, S. S., & Swain, J. E. (2014). Automatic goals and conscious
Pierce, J. L., & Dunham, R. B. (1978). The measurement of perceived job regulation in social cognitive affective neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain
characteristics: The job diagnostic survey versus the job characteristics Sciences, 156–157.
inventory. Academy of Management Journal, 21, 123–128. Stajkovic, A. D., Locke, E. A., & Blair, E. S. (2006). A first examination of the
Pinder, C. C. (1998). Work motivation in organizational behavior. Saddle River, NJ: relationships between primed subconscious goals, assigned conscious goals,
Prentice Hall. and task performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 1172–1180.
Podsakoff, P. M., & Farh, J.-L. (1989). Effects of feedback sign and credibility on goal Stajkovic, A. D., & Luthans, F. (1998). Self-efficacy and work-related performance: A
setting and task performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 240–261.
Processes, 44, 45–67. Steel, P., & Konig, C. J. (2006). Integrating theories of motivation. Academy of
Porac, J. F., & Meindl, J. (1982). Undermining overjustification: Inducing intrinsic Management Review, 31, 889–913.
and extrinsic task representations. Organizational Behavior and Human Stone, D. N. (1994). Overconfidence in initial self-efficacy judgments: Effects on
Performance, 29, 208–226. decision processes and performance. Organizational Behavior and Human
Powers, W. T. (1973). Feedback: Beyond behaviorism. Science, 179, 351–356. Decision Processes, 59, 452–474.
Price, K. H. (1987). Decision responsibility, task responsibility, identifiability, and Sue-Chan, C., & Ong, M. (2002). Goal assignment and performance: Assessing the
social loafing. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 40, mediating roles of goal commitment and self-efficacy and the moderating role
330–345. of power distance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 89,
Pritchard, R. D. (1969). Equity theory: A review and critique. Organizational Behavior 1140–1161.
and Human Performance, 4, 176–211. Sun, S. H., & Frese, M. (2013). Multiple goal pursuit. In E. A. Locke & G. P. Latham
Pritchard, R. D., & Curtis, M. I. (1973). The influence of goal setting and financial (Eds.), New developments in goal setting and task performance (pp. 177–194).
incentives on task performance. Organizational Behavior and Human New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
Performance, 10, 175–183. Sun, S., Vancouver, J. B., & Weinhardt, J. M. (2014). Goal choices and planning:
Pritchard, R. D., deLeo, P. J., & Von Bergen, C. W. (1976). A field experimental test of Distinct expectancy and value effects in two goal processes. Organizational
expectancy-valence incentive motivation techniques. Organizational Behavior Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 125, 220–233.
and Human Performance, 15, 355–406. Tabernero, C., & Wood, R. E. (1999). Implicit theories versus the social construal of
Rawolle, M., Wallis, M. S. v., Badham, R., & Kehr, H. M. (2016). No fit, no fun: The ability in self-regulation and performance on a complex task. Organizational
effect of motive incongruence on job burnout and the mediating role of intrinsic Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 78, 104–127.
motivation. Personality and Individual Differences, 89, 65–68. Takeuchi, R., Yun, S., & Wong, K. F. E. (2011). Social influence of a coworker: A test of
Roberts, K. H., & Glick, W. (1981). The job characteristics approach to task design: A the effect of employee and coworker exchange ideologies on employees’
critical review. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66, 193–217. exchange qualities. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 115,
Rousseau, D. M. (1977). Technological differences in job characteristics, employee 226–237.
satisfaction, and motivation: A synthesis of job design research and Tasa, K., & Whyte, G. (2005). Collective efficacy and vigilant problem solving in
sociotechnical systems theory. Organizational Behavior and Human group decision making: A non-linear model. Organizational Behavior and Human
Performance, 19, 18–42. Decision Processes, 96, 119–129.
Rupp, D. E., Shao, R., Jones, K. S., & Liao, H. (2014). The utility of a multifoci approach Tolli, A. P., & Schmidt, A. M. (2008). The role of feedback, causal attributions, and
to the study of organizational justice: A meta-analytic investigation into the self-efficacy in goal revision. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 692–701.
consideration of normative rules, moral accountability, bandwidth-fidelity, and Triplett, N. (1898). The dynamogenic factors in pacemaking and competition. The
social exchange. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 123, American Journal of Psychology, 9, 507–533.
159–185. Vancouver, J. B. (2005). The depth of history and explanation as benefit and bane for
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). When rewards compete with nature: The psychological control theories. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 38–52.
undermining of intrinsic motivation and self-regulation. In C. Sansone & J. M. Vancouver, J. B. (2008). Integrating self-regulation theories of work motivation into
Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal a dynamic process theory. Human Resource Management Review, 18, 1–18.
motivation and performance (pp. 13–54). New York: Academic Press. Vancouver, J. B., & Kendall, L. N. (2006). When self-efficacy negatively relates to
Sanna, L. J., & Mark, M. M. (1995). Self-handicapping, expected evaluation, and motivation and performance in a learning context. Journal of Applied Psychology,
performance: Accentuating the positive and attenuating the negative. 91, 1146–1153.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 64, 84–102. Vancouver, J. B., Thompson, C. M., & Williams, A. A. (2001). The changing signs in the
Schmidt, F. L. (1973). Implications of a measurement problem for expectancy theory relationships among self-efficacy, personal goals, and performance. Journal of
research. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 10, 243–251. Applied Psychology, 86, 605–620.
Schmidt, A. M., & DeShon, R. P. (2009). Prior performance and goal progress as VandeWalle, D. M. (1997). Development and validation of a work domain goal
moderators of the relationship between self-efficacy and performance. Human orientation instrument. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 57,
Performance, 22, 191–203. 995–1015.
Schmidt, A. M., & DeShon, R. P. (2010). The moderating effects of performance Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation. New York: Wiley.
ambiguity on the relationship between self-efficacy and performance. Journal of Vroom, V. H. (1966). Organizational choice: A study of pre- and postdecision
Applied Psychology, 95, 572–581. processes. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 1, 212–225.
Scott, B. A., Garza, A. S., Conlon, D. E., & Kim, Y. J. (2014). Why do managers act fairly Wahba, M. A., & Bridwell, L. G. (1976). Maslow reconsidered: A review of research
in the first place? A daily investigation of ‘‘hot” and ‘‘cold” motives and on the need hierarchy theory. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance,
discretion. Academy of Management Journal, 57, 1571–1591. 15, 212–240.
R. Kanfer, G. Chen / Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 136 (2016) 6–19 19

Webb, T. L., Gallo, I. S., Miles, E., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2012). Effective Whyte, G. (1998). Recasting Janis’s groupthink model: The key role of collective
regulation of affect: An action control perspective on emotion regulation. efficacy in decision fiascoes. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
European Review of Social Psychology, 23, 143–186. Processes, 73, 185–209.
Weber, E. U. (1998). From performance to decision processes in 33 years: A history Wieber, F., Thurmer, J. L., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2015). Promoting the translation of
of Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes under James C. Naylor. intentions into action by implementation intentions: Behavioral effects and
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 76, 209–222. physiological correlates. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. http://dx.doi.org/
Wegge, J., van Dick, R., Fisher, G. K., West, M. A., & Dawson, J. F. (2006). A test of 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00395.
basic assumptions of Affective Events Theory (AET) in call centre work. British Wood, R., Bandura, A., & Bailey, T. (1990). Mechanisms governing organizational
Journal of Management, 17, 237–254. performance in complex decision-making environments. Organizational
Weiss, H. M., & Cropanzano, R. (1996). Affective events theory: A theoretical Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 46, 181–201.
discussion of the structure, causes, and consequences of affective experiences at Yang, J., & Diefendorff, J. M. (2009). The relations of daily counterproductive
work. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummigs (Eds.). Research in organizational behavior workplace behavior with emotions, situational antecedents, and personality
(Vol. 18, pp. 1–74). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. moderators: A diary study in Hong Kong. Personnel Psychology, 62, 259–295.
Weldon, E., & Mustari, E. L. (1988). Felt dispensability in groups of coactors: The Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science, 149, 269–274.
effects of shared responsibility and explicit anonymity on cognitive effort. Zedeck, S. (1977). An information processing model and approach to the study of
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 41, 330–351. motivation. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 18, 47–77.
Welsh, D. T., & Ordonez, L. D. (2014). The dark side of consecutive high performance Zhang, X., & Zhou, J. (2014). Empowering leadership, uncertainty avoidance, trust,
goals: Linking goal setting, depletion, and unethical behavior. Organizational and employee creativity: Interaction effects and a mediating mechanism.
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 123, 79–89. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 124, 150–164.

You might also like