Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

180

10 Internally Directed Attention in


Creative Cognition
Mathias Benedek

Attention is a fundamental cognitive function experiences or future plans (Baird, Smallwood,


needed to select and focus on relevant informa- & Schooler, 2011). These stimulus-independent
tion. The current focus of attention is determined thoughts can be spontaneous or goal-directed.
by our goals (i.e., what we want to pay attention Spontaneous stimulus-independent thought
to) and by stimulus salience (i.e., what attracts includes the phenomenon of mind-wandering,
our attention; Corbetta & Schulman, 2002). An where thoughts are unintentionally drawn away
effective interplay between these modes of top- from a task (Smallwood & Schooler, 2015). In
down and bottom-up control is crucial to all contrast, goal-directed stimulus-independent
forms of cognitive performance (Kane, Bleckley, thought includes activities like deliberate plan-
Conway, & Engle, 2001). We can further distin- ning or idea generation (Christoff, 2013).
guish between externally directed attention and There are different theoretical views on the
internally directed attention: while external atten- role of attention in creative cognition. Creativity
tion refers to a focus on sensory information, has been associated with concepts like defocused
internal attention refers to a focus on self-gener- attention (Martindale, 1999; Mendelsohn, 1976),
ated mental representations (Chun, Golomb, & broad attention (Kasof, 1997; Rowe, Hirsh, &
Turk-Browne, 2011). Due to the limited capacity Anderson, 2007), flexible attention (Vartanian,
for conscious information processing, exter- 2009; Zabelina & Robinson, 2010), reduced
nal and internal attention are competing mental latent inhibition (Carson, Peterson, & Higgins
states. We can typically only focus on external or 2003; Zabelina, O’Leary, Pornpattananangkul,
internal information at one time, but attention can Nusslock, & Beeman, 2015), and attentional
shift swiftly between external and internal worlds. control (Benedek, Jauk, Sommer, Arendasy, &
Importantly, externally and internally directed Neubauer, 2014; Wiley & Jarosz, 2012). Most of
cognition are thought to involve different brain these accounts do not explicitly refer to the role
mechanisms (Dixon, Fox, & Christoff, 2014). of internal versus external attention, but, if any-
Cognitive science has traditionally focused thing, suggest a higher permissiveness to external
on the study of externally directed cognition. information outside the main focus of attention
Following the influential notions of behavior- (Gabora, 2002; Zabelina, Saporta, & Beeman,
ism, empirical investigations have been largely 2015). The underrepresentation of internal atten-
interested in behavior attributed to externally tion in accounts of creativity seems at odds with
presented stimuli. Recent research, however, its relevance for creative thought. Core pro-
suggests that we spend 25%–50% of our wak- cesses in creative thinking such as imagination
ing time engaged in thoughts unrelated to and the forging of novel combinations between
what is going on around us (Kane et al., 2007; remotely associated concepts (Benedek, Könen,
Killingsworth & Gilbert, 2010). During these & Neubauer, 2012; Koestler, 1964; Mednick,
states our attention is directed to internal trains of 1962) typically involve an internal focus of atten-
thought that are commonly concerned with past tion rather than attention to sensory information.

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Leicester, on 29 Jan 2018 at 00:14:05, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.011
181

Internally Directed Attention in Creative Cognition 181

The role of internal attention seems particu- means of the EEG. EEG recordings assess elec-
larly relevant to the process of individual idea trocortical oscillations that can be divided into
generation. For instance, when we are engaged in different frequency bands. The dominant fre-
writing, and look for a good way to express our quency band in the human brain represents brain
thoughts, we may be found sitting with eyes wide oscillations from 8 to 13 Hz, and is called alpha
open, but barely acknowledging what is going activity (Klimesch, 2012). EEG alpha activity is
on around us. Our eyes may be directed toward typically highest in states of rest and when eyes
the screen or paper, but our attention is directed are closed, and alpha activity is reduced during
to internal processes, such as retrieving relevant many cognitive tasks (von Stein & Sarntheim,
content from memory, and mental simulations 2000). Colin Martindale, a pioneer in the neu-
of potential next sentences. Once we are satis- roscientific investigation of creativity, hypoth-
fied with an idea, we will refocus on the external esized that primary process cognition in the
world in order to write down those lines and make creative process should be reflected in reduced
sure they read well. Writing is just one example level of cortical arousal and hence higher alpha
activity where the more creative part is certainly activity (Martindale & Armstrong, 1974; cf.
associated with an internal focus of attention. The Kris, 1952). Martindale indeed found that EEG
same process is consistently evident in empirical alpha activity was higher during the imagination
research on divergent thinking. After prompt- of a creative story as compared to a later period
ing participants with a task cue, they commonly when a story was elaborated and written down
engage in idea generation for minutes without a (Martindale & Hasenfus, 1978). Another study
need to process any external information (e.g., showed that more-creative people show higher
Benedek, Mühlmann, Jauk, & Neubauer, 2013), alpha activity than less-creative people during
because all relevant information is self-generated. various cognitive tasks (Martindale & Hines,
Because internal attention is relevant to crea- 1975). These initial findings suggested a posi-
tive thought, and characterized by distinct brain tive association between EEG alpha activity and
mechanisms, this chapter aims to explore the creativity.
role of internal attention in creative cognition More recent EEG studies have also measured
and neuroscience. Most of the pertinent research changes in alpha activity between a task and a
has been conducted by means of the electroen- pre-task reference period, which allows discov-
cephalogram (EEG) and functional magnetic ery of whether alpha activity actually increases
resonance imaging (fMRI). As these two lines of or decreases during a task. Task-related alpha
research have developed largely independently, increases are called alpha synchronization,
I will first consider relevant EEG research, fol- whereas task-related decreased are called
lowed by fMRI research, and finally look for alpha desynchronization (Pfurtscheller, 1999).
potential common ground related to internal This method revealed that alpha activity is not
attention in creative cognition. only relatively higher during tasks with higher
creative task demands (as already observed by
Martindale & Hasenfus, 1978), but actually
increases during creative thought relative to a
Creativity and Internal
pre-task resting baseline especially at frontal
Attention in EEG Research
brain regions (Fink, Benedek, Grabner, Staudt,
Research on Creativity and EEG Alpha & Neubauer, 2007; Jaarsveld et  al., 2015;
Activity Schwab, Benedek, Papousek, Weiss, & Fink,
The first investigations of the brain activation 2014). Specifically, alpha synchronization was
associated with creativity were carried out by found to be higher when generating creative uses

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Leicester, on 29 Jan 2018 at 00:14:05, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.011
182

182 m. b enedek

for objects versus generating words for given were assumed to indicate a more diffuse and
suffixes (Fink et al., 2007), and when generating coarse semantic coding of information, charac-
creative versus common uses and associations terized by the activation of larger semantic maps
(Jauk, Benedek, & Neubauer, 2012). that serve to facilitate more remote associations
A number of other studies have corroborated (Fink et al., 2007; cf. Jung-Beeman, 2005).
the creativity–alpha association from different
perspectives. The generation of highly creative EEG Alpha Activity as Indicator of
ideas was associated with higher EEG alpha Internal Attention
activity than the generation of less-creative ideas Task-related EEG alpha increases have not only
(Fink & Neubauer, 2006, 2008; Grabner, Fink, been observed during creative thinking, but
& Neubauer, 2007). Creative people were found also in other fields of research. For example,
to show higher task-related alpha synchroniza- EEG studies on memory reported “paradoxi-
tion than less-creative people (Fink et al., 2009; cal” alpha effects as well: alpha synchroniza-
Fink, Graif, & Neubauer, 2009; Jaušovec, 2000). tion was observed in tasks with high demands
Finally, EEG alpha activity was shown to be on short-term memory (Klimesch, Doppelmayr,
increased by creativity training (Fink, Grabner, Schwaiger, Auinger, & Winkler, 1999) and to
Benedek, & Neubauer, 2006) and cognitive increase as a function of memory load (Jensen,
stimulation (Fink, Schwab, & Papousek, 2011). Gelfand, Kounios, & Lisman, 2002). Other lines
Together, these findings provide strong empiri- of research suggested that alpha activity might
cal support for an association between creativity be related to attentional demands. Ray and Cole
and EEG alpha activity (for reviews, see Dietrich (1985) compared EEG alpha activity between
& Kanso, 2010; Fink & Benedek, 2013, 2014). cognitive tasks that did not require process-
Regarding topographical effects, creativity was ing of external stimuli (e.g., mental arithmetic
generally characterized by alpha synchronization or the visualization of an imaginary walk) and
in frontal brain regions and in posterior regions other tasks that did (e.g., visual detection tasks).
of the right hemisphere, and by reduced alpha Performance of the sensory-independent tasks
desynchronization in occipital brain regions. elicited much higher alpha activity than the sen-
EEG alpha has originally been viewed as an sory-intake tasks, especially in parietal brain
indicator of cognitive inactivity, whereas task- regions. Similarly, Cooper, Croft, Dominey,
related alpha decreases were thought to indicate Burgess, and Gruzelier (2003), presented partic-
active brain regions during a given task. Thus, ipants either series of visual, auditory, or haptic
researchers were quite surprised to find a robust stimuli (i.e., external condition), or asked them
association between creativity and EEG alpha to imagine these stimuli after being trained to
increases, as this seemed to imply that creativity- do so (i.e., internal condition). The internal con-
related cognition is essentially associated with dition was again associated with higher alpha
decreases of brain activation. Different explana- activity than the external condition, and this was
tions have been offered for this apparent discrep- true across all sensory modalities. Moreover,
ancy. It was proposed that alpha increases do alpha activity increased further with task diffi-
not reflect deactivation but rather reduced corti- culty within internal tasks. The findings by Ray
cal arousal, possibly indicating a state of defo- and Cole (1985) and by Cooper and colleagues
cused attention (Martindale, 1999). Similarly, (2003) suggest that EEG alpha activity reflects
alpha increases in frontal regions were seen increased internally directed attention rather
to represent reduced executive control, which than reduced cortical activity or arousal.
was termed hypofrontality (Dietrich, 2003). This inference poses the question whether
Moreover, right hemispheric alpha increases the relationship between creativity and EEG

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Leicester, on 29 Jan 2018 at 00:14:05, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.011
183

Internally Directed Attention in Creative Cognition 183

alpha activity can also be explained by inter- the task, which is just the normal case used in all
nal attention demands. We sought to address previous research that had reported alpha syn-
this question in an EEG study, which indepen- chronization during divergent thinking. Perhaps
dently manipulated the level of creativity-related this discrepancy was due to the specific diver-
demands and of internal attention demands gent thinking task chosen in this study. Creative
in four experimental conditions (Benedek, sentence generation requires the processing of
Bergner, Könen, Fink, & Neubauer, 2011). All four letters that can be used in any sequence, and
conditions presented nouns consisting of four therefore arguably implies higher sensory intake
letters (e.g., POST). Low creative task demands than previously used divergent thinking tasks,
were realized with a convergent thinking task, such as the alternate uses task, which presents
which asked participants to find correct anagram only one single concept (e.g., “brick”) that can
solutions (e.g., “STOP”). Higher creative task easily be kept in mind and does not require any
demands were realized with a divergent think- reprocessing during task performance.
ing task (namely, creative idea generation task), We tested this potential explanation in another
which asked participants to generate creative EEG study, which now employed two divergent
four-word sentences using the four letters as ini- thinking tasks:  one sensory-intake task (creative
tials in any order (e.g., “Oldies sometimes pro- sentence generation) and one sensory-independent
voke tears”). Additionally, the level of internal task (alternate uses task; Benedek, Schickel, Jauk,
attention demands was manipulated by a con- Fink, & Neubauer, 2014). Stimuli were again
ditional masking of the stimulus words. In half masked in half of the trials for both tasks. It was
of the trials the stimulus was masked shortly hypothesized that stimulus masking should have
after their presentation (0.5 s), thereby enforc- a stronger effect on the sensory-intake task than
ing increased internal attention demands as the on the sensory-independent task because it only
task had to be completed in the mind’s eye. The affects the process of bottom-up stimulus process-
findings of this study were straightforward: task- ing. In fact, EEG alpha activity was found to be
related alpha activity was higher in conditions increased in posterior regions of the right hemi-
of high internal attention demands as compared sphere in all cases, except for the sensory intake
to unmasked task performance that granted task performed in the unmasked condition. In
continuous sensory access to the stimuli. While other words, alpha increases were observed in all
alpha activity varied as a function of attentional conditions involving high internally directed atten-
demands, it was largely independent from the tion, such as in the sensory-independence task
type of task. Convergent and divergent thinking (for both masking conditions) and in the sensory-
did not differ notably in alpha activity, except intake task when the task had to be completed in
for slightly higher parietal alpha activity dur- the mind’s eye due to stimulus masking.
ing divergent thinking. These findings support These two experimental EEG studies may help
the view that EEG alpha activity during crea- to clarify the role of alpha activity in creative
tive thought reflects increased internal attention thought. Alpha activity was found to be specific
demands rather than other creativity-related cog- to internal attention demands rather than specific
nitive processes. to creative task demands (Benedek et al., 2011).
One specific finding of this study was quite As a consequence, alpha levels may vary even
puzzling though:  Why was alpha synchroniza- between creative thinking tasks: those which are
tion during divergent thinking only observed intrinsically sensory-independent are accompa-
in the internal attention condition but not in the nied with higher alpha activity than those which
external attention condition? In the external con- require regular processing of external informa-
dition, the stimuli remained visible throughout tion (Benedek, Schickel, et al., 2014).

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Leicester, on 29 Jan 2018 at 00:14:05, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.011
184

184 m. b enedek

The presented findings suggest that pro- might signify that they are better able to enter
nounced alpha activity during creative thought and maintain states of deep absorption during
is explained by the internal nature of creative creative thought. Sustained attention on relevant
cognition. It is interesting to apply this internal (internal) content while gating out irrelevant sen-
attention interpretation of alpha activity to other sory input is commonly seen as a sign of execu-
findings of the creativity–alpha relationship. tive control (Engle, 2002; Kane et  al., 2001).
For example, the finding that more-creative The importance of executive control for creative
ideas are preceded by higher alpha activity than thought has been repeatedly demonstrated (e.g.,
less-creative ideas suggests that the generation Nusbaum & Silvia, 2011; Silvia, 2015). Studies
of highly creative ideas may be accompanied consistently found that executive abilities in
by deeper levels of absorption during creative terms of working memory capacity, cognitive
thought. Being fully engaged with internal cog- inhibition and intelligence predict higher per-
nitive processes, and not distracted by external formance in creative idea generation (Benedek,
interference, may enable the generation of more Franz, Heene, Neubauer, 2012; Benedek, Jauk,
elaborate and vivid mental representations, et  al., 2014) including metaphor production
which may ultimately result in more creative (Beaty & Silvia, 2013) and humor production
ideas. Relatedly, the occurrence of spontane- (Kellner & Benedek, 2017). While executive
ous insights in creative problem-solving was control can contribute to creative thought in
also shown to be preceded by increases of alpha many ways (e.g., Beaty, Silvia, Nusbaum, Jauk,
activity in the right posterior–occipital cortex & Benedek, 2014; Benedek & Jauk, in press),
(Jung-Beeman et al., 2004). This finding was a most general way would be to facilitate sus-
proposed to reflect a strong temporary inward tained internal attention during the cognitively
focus, or a “mental blink” (Kounios & Beeman, demanding process of creative idea generation.
2009, p. 212), and thus could be seen as a Taken together, there is substantial evidence
moment of “in-sight” as the solution appears that EEG alpha activity (particularly at right
in the mind’s eye. This interpretation was sup- posterior–parietal brain regions) indicates states
ported by an eye-tracking study, which showed of internally directed attention. While many
that solving problems with insight is accom- cognitive tasks are accompanied by decreases
panied by longer blinks and more gaze aver- of alpha activity, increases in alpha activity
sion (Salvi, Bricolo, Franconeri, Kounios, & increases are observed in cognitive activi-
Beeman, 2015). Moreover, idea generation was ties that require sustained internally directed
found to be accompanied by longer and more attention. Conforming to this interpretation of
frequent blinks, reduced microsaccade activity, EEG alpha activity, the consistent association
and a reduced angle of eye vergence reflecting between creativity and alpha activity can be
visual disaccommodation (Benedek, Stoiser, explained by central role of internal attention
Walcher, & Körner, 2017; Walcher, Körner, & for creative thought.
Benedek, 2017). Hence, alpha activity appears
to serve as a sensitive indicator of transient Functional Mechanisms Underlying
changes in the internal focus over time, and the EEG Alpha Activity
actual level of internal focus may predict the Let us go one step further and explore the
outcome of creative thought. functional role of EEG alpha activity for inter-
We can also consider implications of the nal attention. EEG alpha activity was initially
internal attention interpretation of alpha activ- assumed to represent a state of cortical idling
ity on individual differences findings. When (Pfurtscheller, Stancák, & Neuper, 1996), but
creative people show higher alpha activity, this more recent theoretical accounts assign it a

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Leicester, on 29 Jan 2018 at 00:14:05, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.011
185

Internally Directed Attention in Creative Cognition 185

more active role in human cognition. EEG alpha Creativity and Internal
is proposed to reflect active top-down inhibi- Attention in fMRI Research
tion of task-irrelevant brain regions (Jensen &
Mazaheri, 2010; Klimesch et  al., 1999). The Default Mode Network Activation as
inhibition of irrelevant sensory processing may an Indicator of Internal Attention?
help to shield internal representations from inter- The investigation of the neural correlates of
ference. This active inhibition view is supported creative cognition by means of fMRI was met
by EEG studies examining alpha activity in with much enthusiasm in the beginning of this
visuo-spatial attention tasks. For example, alpha millennium, but was followed by disenchant-
activity was found to increase in occipital cortex ment when the first reviews were unable to
regions contralateral to the position of stimuli identify consistency across findings (Arden,
that should be ignored (Händel, Haarmeier, Chavez, Grazioplene, & Jung, 2010; Dietrich
& Jensen, 2011; Rihs, Michel, & Thut, 2007; & Kanso, 2010). This inconsistency may in part
Worden, Foxe, Wang, & Simpson, 2000). Recent be explained by the heterogeneity of tasks that
evidence suggests that right-posterior alpha shared the label of creativity, but not necessar-
increases are also observed when people try to ily common cognitive processes. More recent
ignore auditory stimuli (Dubé, Payne, Sekuler, reviews focusing on more closely defined activi-
& Rotello, 2013). ties (e.g., creative idea generation, or musical
The top-down modulation of sensory pro- improvisation) reveal a more consistent picture
cessing may be achieved by phase coupling (Beaty, 2015; Fink & Benedek, 2014; Gonen-
between prefrontal and posterior regions in Yacoovi et al., 2013).
the alpha band range. Sauseng et  al. (2005) As one of the more consistent findings, MRI
showed that active manipulation of information studies on creativity commonly demonstrate
in memory was associated with increased func- brain activation in regions pertaining to the
tional coupling between prefrontal and occipi- default mode network (DMN; Benedek, Beaty,
tal regions potentially reflecting top-down et  al., 2014; Fink et  al., 2014; Jauk, Neubauer,
control by anterior over posterior brain regions. Dunst, Fink, & Benedek, 2015; Jung et  al.,
Moreover, when prefrontal function was per- 2010; Takeuchi et al., 2011). The DMN mainly
turbed by means of TMS, top-down modulation consists of regions along the anterior and pos-
in a selective-attention task decreased, particu- terior midline, the posterior inferior parietal
larly in people with stronger frontoposterior cortex and the medial temporal lobe, and this
functional connectivity (von Stein, Chiang, network deactivates in many cognitive tasks but
& König 2000; Zanto, Rubens, Thangavel, & shows increased activation during periods of rest
Gazzaley, 2011). The active role of EEG alpha (Buckner, Andrews-Hanna, & Schacter, 2008;
activity in internal cognition may include selec- Gusnard & Raichle, 2001). The DMN is consist-
tive attention and maintenance of internal rep- ently activated in many active cognitive tasks
resentations in working memory (Klimesch, as well, including autobiographical memory
2012; Palva & Palva, 2007). Higher alpha retrieval, prospection, mental navigation, theory
activity is thought to correspond to increasingly of mind, and mind wandering (Fox, Spreng,
reduced attention to external stimuli (Jensen, Ellamil, Andrews-Hanna, & Christoff, 2015;
Bonnefond, & VanRullen, 2012) and highest Spreng, Mar, & Kim, 2009). Many of these
alpha levels are associated with a complete tasks do not require much processing of exter-
break-down of sensory processing as observed nal information but are achieved with an internal
during loss of consciousness (Supp, Siegel, focus of attention, suggesting that the DMN rep-
Hipp, & Engel, 2011). resents an “introspective network” (Pinho, Ullén,

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Leicester, on 29 Jan 2018 at 00:14:05, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.011
186

186 m. b enedek

Castelo-Brancod, Fransson, & de Manzano, although these occipital regions actually showed
2015). On the other hand, it is also often empha- reduced activation during internal attention. This
sized that the DMN is particularly involved in activation pattern suggests that the right IPL may
constructive, self-generated thought based on be involved in downregulating visual process-
content retrieved from memory. Because most ing during internally focused attention (Sarter,
internal cognition tasks crucially depend on Givens, & Bruno, 2001). Such a top-down mech-
such generative processes, it is hard to keep anism is in line with the perceptual decoupling
apart attention-specific and task-specific pro- hypothesis (Smallwood et al., 2011; Smallwood
cesses when comparing brain activation between & Schooler, 2015), which assumes that focused
internal and external cognition. Thus, the ques- internal attention requires that attention is decou-
tion remains as to what extent DMN activation is pled from perceptual input. Thus, the right aIPL
actually specific to internally directed attention, may exert top-down control over occipital regions
or to constructive, self-generated thought that is in order to suppress early visual information pro-
prevalent in internally directed cognition. cessing, which serves to shield ongoing internal
We examined the brain mechanisms associ- processes from sensory interference.
ated with internal attention in creative thought Considering the contrast of divergent think-
in a recent fMRI study (Benedek et  al., 2016) ing versus convergent thinking, divergent think-
that employed essentially the same experimen- ing was associated with higher brain activation
tal design as in one of our previous EEG studies in left frontal cortex and in regions attributed to
(Benedek et  al., 2011). The study used a diver- the DMN including the posterior cingulate cor-
gent and a convergent thinking task, and we tex, the middle temporal gyrus, and the angular
assumed that the divergent thinking task (gen- gyrus. This finding underscores the relevance of
erating creative four-word sentences) involved the DMN together with executive networks for
considerably higher levels of self-generated creative thought. The coupling of default and
thought processes than the convergent thinking executive networks has been shown to be char-
task (finding correct anagram solutions), because acteristic for domain-general and artistic forms
it required participants to produce novel content of creative thought (Beaty, Benedek, Silvia, &
going beyond the four stimulus letters. Internal Schacter, 2016; Jung, Mead, Carrasco, & Flores,
attention demands were experimentally manipu- 2013). Moreover, higher creative potential is
lated by masking the stimulus in half of the tri- associated with higher coupling of these net-
als immediately after it was read. This design works during creative thought and at rest (Beaty,
was used to disentangle brain activation associ- Benedek, Kaufman, & Silvia, 2015; Beaty
ated either with internal attention or with more et  al., 2014; Spreng, Stevens, Chamberlain,
creative, self-generated thought processes, and Gilmore, & Schacter, 2010). The findings of
clarify the role of DMN with respect to these this study suggest that the DMN is not an indi-
conditions. The study revealed that internally cator of internal attention per se, but is rather
directed attention was associated with reduced specific to constructive, self-generated thought,
activation in extended regions of the visual net- which is central to most internal cognition tasks
work, and with increased activation of the right (Andrews-Hanna, Smallwood, & Spreng, 2014).
anterior inferior parietal cortex (aIPL; specifi-
cally the posterior part of the right supramar-
ginal gyrus), bilateral lingual gyrus and left
Imagery
cuneus. Interestingly, the right aIPL also showed Another cognitive activity that appears relevant
increased functional connectivity with bilateral to internal attention and creativity is imagery.
occipital cortex regions during internal attention, Mental imagery occurs when perceptual

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Leicester, on 29 Jan 2018 at 00:14:05, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.011
187

Internally Directed Attention in Creative Cognition 187

information is retrieved from memory to re- past experiences related to given objects or
establish a previous experience in the form of to imagine likely future experiences for the
seeing it with the mind’s eye or hearing it with same object. Future events should be novel but
the mind’s ear (Kosslyn, Ganis, & Thompson, plausible and participants were asked to imag-
2001; Pearson, Naselaris, Holmes, & Kosslyn, ine a specific scene with all contextual details
2015). Imagery may not be an essentially crea- from an individual perspective. They found
tive process as long as it concerns recollection, that recalling past experiences from episodic
but as soon as it involves the imagination of memory and imagining future ones recruited
something new, it certainly represents a crucial similar brain regions. The core network of com-
capacity underlying creative thought (Finke, mon regions between recall and imagination
1996; Jung, Flores, & Hunter, 2016). included posterior cingulate cortex, medial pre-
The neural foundations of mental imagery have frontal cortex, hippocampal regions, and infe-
been extensively studied with fMRI and positron rior parietal cortex, many of which represent
emission tomography (PET). Participants are core regions of the default mode network (see
typically shown an image and are later asked to also Jung et  al., 2016). Schacter et  al. (2012)
imagine this image again in order to answer ques- concluded that imagination and future thinking
tions about it. Visual imagery was found to share crucially rely on memory-based simulations,
many neural systems with visual perception where information (particularly from episodic
(Kosslyn, Thompson, & Alpert, 1997). One of memory) is used to imagine, simulate, and pre-
the key regions for visual imagery is the primary dict future events. Imagining future events for
visual cortex (Brodman area 17). Impairing the objects bears obvious conceptual similarity to
function of this region by means of transmag- the divergent thinking task asking for creative
netic stimulation impaired the performance in an object uses. Therefore, it may not be surpris-
imagery task (Kosslyn et  al., 1999). Similar to ing that divergent thinking performance is cor-
the study of visual imagery, the brain activation related with the amount of episodic detail in
underlying auditory imagery has been examined reporting future events (Addis, Pan, Musicaro,
by comparing the imagination of music to actual & Schacter, 2016), and that future thinking
listening conditions. The brain activation during and divergent thinking share neural substrates
auditory imagery clearly overlapped with the including key regions of the DMN (Gonen-
brain activation for auditory perception includ- Yacovi et al., 2013; Spreng et al., 2009).
ing the bilateral superior temporal gyrus and Visual imagery activates visual processing
the left supramarginal gyrus (Zatorre, Halpern, areas, auditory imagery activates auditory pro-
Perry, Meyer, & Evans, 1996). Thus, imagery cessing areas, and future thinking activates areas
in the visual and auditory domain recruits brain associated with episodic memory retrieval such
regions associated with perceptual processing of as the DMN. In other words, we find clear over-
visual and auditory stimuli, respectively. lap between recall and imagery within domains,
What about more complex forms of imagery but these studies do not reveal a brain region
that go beyond mere mental reproduction and that is common to imagery across domains. The
involve the production of novel mental rep- existence of such a common brain region might
resentations (i.e., imagination)? There has have been expected because these imagery tasks
been a lot of interest in the neural correlates are thought to share some cognitive processes
of prospection, and how envisaging the future such as an internal focus of attention. One poten-
differs from recalling the past (Schacter et  al., tial reason is that imagery in terms of the repro-
2012). In one of the first studies, Addis, Wong, duction of previously experienced stimuli is not
and Schacter (2007) asked participants to recall as cognitively demanding as the production of

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Leicester, on 29 Jan 2018 at 00:14:05, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.011
188

188 m. b enedek

novel representations and ideas. In fact, musi- between creativity and alpha activity reflects the
cal imagery often happens even involuntarily intrinsic role of internal attention in creative cog-
(Beaty et  al., 2013). Therefore, imagery may nition (Benedek et al., 2011; Benedek, Schickel
not require sustained internally focused atten- et al., 2014; Fink & Benedek, 2013, 2014). EEG
tion to an equal extent as goal-directed creative alpha activity is currently conceived as a brain
thought. Together these findings point to strik- mechanism that exerts top-down control in order
ing conceptual and neurophysiological associa- to suppress the processing of task-irrelevant sen-
tions between perception and imagery, as well sory information, and shield ongoing internal
as between imagery of previous experiences and processes from interference. The active role of
the imagination of novel representations. alpha activity for internal cognition may further
include the maintenance of object representa-
tions during working memory processing (Jensen
Summary and Conclusions
et al., 2012; Klimesch, 2012; Palva & Palva, 2007)
Creative cognition is commonly characterized by Default mode activity is also prevalent in
sustained internally directed attention to self-gen- internally directed cognition, including sponta-
erated thought, such as mental simulations based neous forms such as mind-wandering (Fox et al.,
on content retrieved from memory. Thus, under- 2015), and goal-directed forms such as plan-
standing the neuroscience of creativity requires ning or creative thought (Spreng, Mar, & Kin,
an understanding of the brain mechanisms asso- 2009; Gonen-Yaacovi et  al., 2013). However,
ciated with internal attention in creative thought. unlike EEG alpha activity, DMN activity seems
This chapter reviewed relevant research in this not to be related to internal attention per se, but
field and put a particular emphasis on the role to self-generated thought and mental simula-
of EEG alpha activity and default mode network tion (Andrews-Hanna et  al., 2014; Schacter
activity. Alpha and DMN activity share a similar et al., 2012). Rather, increased internal attention
history in neuroscience research. Both have ini- demands have been linked to increased activa-
tially been mistaken as idling, resting, or default tion in the right anterior inferior parietal cortex,
activity by an empirical tradition that focused on and widely reduced activation in the visual net-
externally directed cognition (e.g., Pfurtscheller works (Benedek et al., 2016; Sarter et al., 2001).
et al., 1996; Raichle et al., 2001). Today, it is The topography of this fMRI activation pat-
fully acknowledged that the brain is highly active tern strikingly corresponds to alpha synchro-
in the absence of stimulus-driven activity. As a nization effects in internal cognition that are
consequence of this awareness, EEG alpha and typically most pronounced in right inferior pari-
DMN activity were ennobled to represent inter- etal cortex and occipital regions during creative
nally directed cognition or stimulus-independent thought (Fink & Benedek, 2013, 2014). EEG and
thought rather than resting state activity. It was fMRI research thus provide converging evidence
also hypothesized that EEG alpha activity may for the important role of the right inferior pari-
directly correspond to processes of the default etal cortex for sustained internal attention. The
mode network (Mok, 2014). As we have seen, right inferior parietal cortex may exert top-down
however, these views are only partially supported control over occipital regions via connectivity in
by empirical research. the alpha frequency range (Sauseng et al., 2005;
EEG research has accumulated substantial Klimesch, 2012), as observed by increased
evidence that alpha activity, particularly in the alpha levels in right parietal and occipital brain
right parietal cortex, is indeed associated with regions (Benedek, Schickel, et al., 2014; Fink &
internally directed attention (Cooper et al., 2003; Benedek, 2014) as well as increased functional
Ray & Cole, 1985). The consistent association connectivity between right parietal cortex and

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Leicester, on 29 Jan 2018 at 00:14:05, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.011
189

Internally Directed Attention in Creative Cognition 189

the visual network (Benedek et al., 2016). The Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2007).
suppression of early visual processing may serve Remembering the past and imagining the
to decouple attention from perceptual input and future: Common and distinct neural substrates
maintain a coherent train of thought (Smallwood during event construction and elaboration.
Neuropsychologia, 45, 1363–1377.
& Schooler, 2015).
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Smallwood, J., & Spreng,
While internal attention may be ubiquitous
R. N. (2014). The default network and self-
in creative thought, an important question is
generated thought: Component processes,
whether it is actually specific to creativity. Is dynamic control, and clinical relevance.
internal attention just a basic precondition for Annuals of the New York Academy of Sciences,
creative thought that simply needs to be given 1316, 29–52.
(like open eyes are required for reading), or is Arden, R., Chavez, R. S., Grazioplene, R., &
there potentially a closer relationship between Jung, R. E. (2010). Neuroimaging creativity:
internal attention and creativity? Creative peo- A psychometric review. Behavioral Brain
ple and creative ideas are associated with higher Research, 214, 143–156.
EEG alpha activity, suggesting at least a graded Baird, B., Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2011).
relationship between internal attention and Back to the future: Autobiographical planning
and the functionality of mind-wandering.
creativity. It may not be enough that attention
Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1604–1611.
is directed to internal processes, but the actual
Beaty, R. E. (2015). The neuroscience of musical
depth and stability of internal attention over
improvisation. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral
time may be relevant. Creative thought involves Reviews, 51, 108–117.
the generation of complex mental representa- Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Kaufman, S. B., & Silvia,
tions that need to be maintained over extended P. J. (2015). Default and executive network
periods of time for simulation and elabora- coupling supports creative idea production.
tion. The effectivity of this process may well Scientific Reports, 5, 10964.
depend on the capacity to stay deeply absorbed Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., & Schacter,
in self-generated thoughts, despite the constant D. L. (2016). Creative cognition and brain
exposition of potentially interfering sensory network dynamics. Trends in Cognitive
stimulation. Future research in this field should Sciences, 20, 87–95.
Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Wilkins, R. W., Jauk,
take advantage of available physiological indi-
E., Fink, A., Silvia, P. J., … Neubauer,
cators of internally versus externally directed
A. C. (2014). Creativity and the default
attention as provided by the neurosciences to
network: A functional connectivity analysis of
advance our understanding of how internal the creative brain at rest. Neuropsychologia,
attention contributes to creative cognition. 64, 92–98.
Beaty, R. E., Burgin, C. J., Nusbaum, E. C., Kwapil,
T. R., Hodges, D. A., & Silvia, P. J. (2013).
Acknowledgment Music to the inner ears: Exploring individual
differences in musical imagery. Consciousness
This work was supported by the Austrian Science and Cognition, 22, 1163–1173.
Fund (FWF): P29801-B27. Beaty, R. E., & Silvia, P. J. (2013). Metaphorically
speaking: Cognitive abilities and the production
of figurative speech. Memory and Cognition, 41,
255–267.
References
Beaty, R. E., Silvia, P. J., Nusbaum, E. C., Jauk, E.,
Addis, D. R., Pan, L., Musicaro, R., & Schacter, D. & Benedek, M. (2014). The role of associative
L. (2016). Divergent thinking and constructing and executive processes in creative cognition.
episodic simulations. Memory, 24, 89–97. Memory & Cognition, 42, 1186–1195.

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Leicester, on 29 Jan 2018 at 00:14:05, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.011
190

190 m. b enedek

Benedek, M., Beaty, R., Jauk, E., Koschutnig, K., internally versus externally directed cognition.
Fink, A., Silvia, P. J., … Neubauer, A. C. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1092. doi:10.3389/
(2014). Creating metaphors: The neural basis fpsyg.2017.01092
of figurative language production. NeuroImage, Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter,
90, 99–106. D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network. Annals
Benedek, M., Bergner, S., Könen, T., Fink, A., of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 1–38.
& Neubauer, A. C. (2011). EEG alpha Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M.
synchronization is related to top-down (2003). Decreased latent inhibition is associated
processing in convergent and divergent thinking. with increased creative achievement in high-
Neuropsychologia, 49, 3505–3511. functioning individuals. Journal of Personality
Benedek, M., Franz, F., Heene, M., & Neubauer, and Social Psychology, 85, 499–506.
A. C. (2012). Differential effects of cognitive Christoff, K. (2013). Thinking. In K. N. Ochsner &
inhibition and intelligence on creativity. S. M. Kosslyn (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of
Personality and Individual Differences, 53(4), cognitive neuroscience (Vol. 2, pp. 318–333).
480–485. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.014 Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Benedek, M., & Jauk, E. (in press). Spontaneous Chun, M. M., Golomb, J. D., & Turk-Browne, N. B.
and controlled processes in creative cognition. (2011). A taxonomy of external and internal
In K. C. R. Fox, & K. Christoff (Eds.), The attention. Annual Review Psychology, 62,
Oxford handbook of spontaneous thought: Mind 73–101.
wandering, creativity, dreaming, and clinical Cooper, N. R., Croft, R. J., Dominey, S. J. J., Burgess,
disorders. Oxford: Oxford University Press. A. P., & Gruzelier, J. H. (2003). Paradox
Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Beaty, R. E., Fink, A., lost? Exploring the role of alpha oscillations
Koschutnig, K., & Neubauer, A. C. (2016). during externally vs. internally directed
Brain mechanisms associated with internally attention and the implications for idling and
directed attention and self-generated thought. inhibition hypotheses. International Journal of
Scientific Reports, 6, 22959. Psychophysiology, 47, 65–74.
Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Sommer, M., Arendasy, M., & Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of
Neubauer, A. C. (2014). Intelligence, creativity, goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention
and cognitive control: The common and in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3,
differential involvement of executive functions 201–215.
in intelligence and creativity. Intelligence, Dietrich, A. (2003). Functional neuroanatomy of
46, 73–83. altered states of consciousness: The transient
Benedek, M., Könen, T., & Neubauer, A. C. (2012). hypofrontality hypothesis. Consciousness and
Associative abilities underlying creativity. Cognition, 12, 231–256.
Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Dietrich, A., & Kanso, R. (2010). A review of EEG,
Arts, 6, 273–281. ERP, and neuroimaging studies of creativity and
Benedek, M., Mühlmann, C., Jauk, E., & Neubauer, insight. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 822–848.
A. C. (2013). Assessment of divergent thinking Dixon, M. L., Fox, K. C. R., & Christoff, K. (2014).
by means of the subjective top-scoring A framework for understanding the relationship
method: Effects of the number of top-ideas between externally and internally directed
and time-on-task on reliability and validity. cognition. Neuropsychologia, 62, 321–330.
Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Dubé, C., Payne, L., Sekuler, R., & Rotello, C. M.
Arts, 7, 341–349. (2013). Paying attention to attention in
Benedek, M., Schickel, R. J., Jauk, E., Fink, A., & recognition memory. Insights from models and
Neubauer, A. C. (2014). Alpha power increases electrophysiology. Psychological Science, 24,
in right parietal cortex reflects focused internal 2398–2408.
attention. Neuropsychologia, 56, 393–400. Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity
Benedek, M., Stoiser, R., Walcher, S., & Körner, as executive attention. Current directions in
C. (2017). Eye behavior associated with Psychological Science, 11, 19–23.

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Leicester, on 29 Jan 2018 at 00:14:05, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.011
191

Internally Directed Attention in Creative Cognition 191

Fink, A., & Benedek, M. (2013). The creative brain: Finke, R. A. (1996). Imagery, creativity, and
Brain correlates underlying the generation of emergent structure. Consciousness and
original ideas. In O. Vartanian, A. S. Bristol, Cognition, 5, 381–393.
& J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), Neuroscience of Fox, K. C. R., Spreng, R. N., Ellamil, M., Andrews-
creativity (pp. 207–232). Cambridge, MA: Hanna, J. R., & Christoff, K. (2015). The
MIT Press. wandering brain: Meta-analysis of functional
Fink, A., & Benedek, M. (2014). EEG alpha power neuroimaging studies of mind-wandering
and creative ideation. Neuroscience and and related spontaneous thought processes.
Biobehavioral Reviews, 44, 111–123. NeuroImage, 111, 611–621.
Fink, A., Benedek, M., Grabner, R. H., Staudt, B., Gabora, L. (2002). Cognitive mechanisms underlying
& Neubauer, A. C. (2007). Creativity meets the creative process. In T. Hewett & T. Kavanagh
neuroscience: Experimental tasks for the (Eds.), Proceedings of the Fourth International
neuroscientific study of creative thinking. Conference on Creativity and Cognition
Methods, 42, 68–76. (pp. 126–133). Loughborough: Loughborough
Fink, A., Grabner, R. H., Benedek, M., & Neubauer, University.
A. C. (2006). Divergent thinking training Gonen-Yaacovi, G., de Souza, L. C., Levy, R.,
is related to frontal electroencephalogram Urbanski, M., Josse, G., & Volle, E. (2013).
alpha synchronization. European Journal of Rostral and caudal prefrontal contribution
Neuroscience, 23, 2241–2246. to creativity: A meta-analysis of functional
Fink, A., Grabner, R. H., Benedek, M., Reishofer, imaging data. Frontiers in Human
G., Hauswirth, V., Fally, M., … Neubauer, A. Neuroscience, 7, 465.
C. (2009). The creative brain: Investigation of Grabner, R. H., Fink, A., & Neubauer, A. C. (2007).
brain activity during creative problem solving Brain correlates of self-rated originality of
by means of EEG and fMRI. Human Brain ideas: Evidence from event-related power and
Mapping, 30, 734–748. phase-locking changes in the EEG. Behavioral
Fink, A., Graif, B., & Neubauer, A. C. (2009). Brain Neuroscience, 121, 224–230.
correlates underlying creative thinking: EEG Gusnard, D. A., & Raichle, M. E. (2001). Searching
alpha activity in professional vs. novice dancers. for a baseline: Functional imaging and
NeuroImage, 46, 854–862. the resting human brain. Nature Reviews
Fink, A., Koschutnig, K., Hutterer, L., Steiner, Neuroscience, 2, 685–694.
E., Benedek, M., Weber, B., … Weiss, E. Händel, B. F., Haarmeier, T., & Jensen, O. (2011).
M. (2014). Gray matter density in relation Alpha oscillations correlate with the successful
to different facets of verbal creativity. Brain inhibition of unattended stimuli. Journal of
Structure and Function, 219, 1263–1269. Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 2494–2502.
Fink, A., & Neubauer, A. C. (2006). EEG alpha Jaarsveld, S., Fink, A., Rinner, M., Schwab,
oscillations during the performance of verbal D., Benedek, M., & Lachmann, T. (2015).
creativity tasks: Differential effects of sex and Intelligence in creative processes: An EEG
verbal intelligence. International Journal of study. Intelligence, 49, 171–178.
Psychophysiology, 62, 46–53. Jauk, E., Benedek, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2012).
Fink, A., & Neubauer, A. C. (2008). Eysenck Tackling creativity at its roots: Evidence
meets Martindale: The relationship between for different patterns of EEG alpha activity
extraversion and originality from the related to convergent and divergent modes
neuroscientific perspective. Personality and of task processing. International Journal of
Individual Differences, 44, 299–310. Psychophysiology, 84, 219–225.
Fink, A., Schwab, D., & Papousek, I. (2011). Jauk, E., Neubauer, A. C., Dunst, B., Fink, A., &
Sensitivity of EEG upper alpha activity to Benedek, M. (2015). Gray matter correlates
cognitive and affective creativity interventions. of creative potential: A latent variable voxel-
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 82, based morphometry study. NeuroImage, 111,
233–239. 312–320.

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Leicester, on 29 Jan 2018 at 00:14:05, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.011
192

192 m. b enedek

Jaušovec, N., 2000. Differences in cognitive processes Kellner, R., & Benedek, M. (2017). The role
between gifted, intelligent, creative, and average of creative potential and intelligence for
individuals while solving complex problems: An humor production. Psychology of Aesthetics,
EEG study. Intelligence, 28, 213–237. Creativity, and the Arts, 11, 52–58. doi:10.1037/
Jensen, O., Bonnefond, M., & VanRullen, R. (2012). aca0000065
An oscillatory mechanism for prioritizing Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A
salient unattended stimuli. Trends in Cognitive wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science,
Sciences, 16, 200–206. 330, 932–932.
Jensen, O., Gelfand, J., Kounios, J., & Lisman, J. E. Klimesch, W. (2012). Alpha-band oscillations,
(2002). Oscillations in the alpha band (9–12 Hz) attention, and controlled access to stored
increase with memory load during retention in information. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16,
a short-term memory task. Cerebral Cortex, 12, 606–617.
877–882. Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Schwaiger, J.,
Jensen, O., & Mazaheri, A. (2010). Shaping Auinger, P., & Winkler, T. (1999). “Paradoxical”
functional architecture by oscillatory alpha alpha synchronization in a memory task.
activity: Gating by inhibition. Frontiers in Cognitive Brain Research, 7, 493–501.
Human Neuroscience, 4, 186. Koestler, A. (1964). The act of creation. New York,
Jung, R. E., Flores, R. A., & Hunter, D. (2016). A NY: Macmillan.
new measure of imagination ability: Anatomical Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. L.
brain imaging correlates. Frontiers in (2001). Neural foundations of imagery. Nature
Psychology, 7, 496. Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 635–642.
Jung, R. E., Mead, B. S., Carrasco, J., & Flores, R. A. Kosslyn, S. M., Pascual-Leone, A., Felician, O.,
(2013). The structure of creative cognition in the Camposano, S., Keenan, J. P., Ganis, G., …
human brain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, Alpert, N. M. (1999). The role of area 17 in
7, 330. visual imagery: Convergent evidence from PET
Jung, R. E., Segall, J. M., Bockholt, H. J., Flores, R. and rTMS. Science, 284, 167–170.
A., Smith, S. M., Chavez, R. S., & Haier, R. J. Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., & Alpert, N.
(2010). Neuoroanatomy of creativity. Human M. (1997). Neural systems shared by visual
Brain Mapping, 31, 398–409. imagery and visual perception: A positron
Jung-Beeman, M. (2005). Bilateral brain processes emission tomography study. NeuroImage, 6,
for comprehending natural language. Trends in 320–334.
Cognitive Sciences, 9, 512–518. Kounios, J., & Beeman, M. (2009). The Aha!
Jung-Beeman, M., Bowden, E. M., Haberman, J., moment: The cognitive neuroscience of insight.
Frymiare, J. L., Arambel-Liu, S., Greenblatt, Current Directions in Psychological Science,
R., … Kounios, J. (2004). Neural activity when 18, 210–216.
people solve verbal problems with insight. PLoS Kris, E. (1952). Psychoanalytic explorations in art.
Biology, 2, 500–510. New York, NY: International Universities Press.
Kane, M. J., Bleckley, K. M., Conway, A. R., & Martindale, C. (1999). Biological bases of creativity.
Engle, R. W. (2001). A controlled-attention In R. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity
view of working-memory capacity. Journal of (pp. 137–152). Cambridge: Cambridge
Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 169–183. University Press.
Kane, M. J., Brown, L. H., McVay, J. C., Silvia, P. Martindale, C., & Armstrong, J. (1974). The
J., Myin-Germeys, I., & Kwapil, T. R. (2007). relationship of creativity to cortical activation
For whom the mind wanders, and when and its operant control. Journal of Genetic
an experience-sampling study of working Psychology, 124, 311–320.
memory and executive control in daily life. Martindale, C., & Hasenfus, N. (1978). EEG
Psychological Science, 18, 614–621. differences as a function of creativity, stage of
Kasof, J. (1997). Creativity and breadth of attention. the creative process, and effort to be original.
Creativity Research Journal, 10, 303–315. Biological Psychology, 6, 157–167.

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Leicester, on 29 Jan 2018 at 00:14:05, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.011
193

Internally Directed Attention in Creative Cognition 193

Martindale, C., & Hines, D. (1975). Creativity Ray, W. J., & Cole, H. W. (1985). EEG alpha reflects
and cortical activation during creative, attentional demands, and beta activity reflects
intellectual, and EEG feedback tasks. Biological emotional and cognitive processes. Science,
Psychology, 3, 71–80. 228, 750–752.
Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the Rihs, T. A., Michel, C. M., & Thut, G. (2007).
creative process. Psychological Review, 69, Mechanisms of selective inhibition in visual
220–232. spatial attention are indexed by α-band
Mendelsohn, G. A. (1976). Associative and EEG synchronization. European Journal of
attentional processes in creative performance. Neuroscience, 25, 603–610.
Journal of Personality, 44, 341–369. Rowe, G., Hirsh, J. B., & Anderson, A. K. (2007).
Mok, L. W. (2014). The interplay between Positive affect increases the breadth of
spontaneous and controlled processing in attentional selection. Proceedings of the
creative cognition. Frontiers in Human National Academy of Sciences, 104, 383–388.
Neuroscience, 8, 663. Salvi, C., Bricolo, E., Franconeri, S. L., Kounios,
Nusbaum, E. C., & Silvia, P. J. (2011). Are intelligence J., & Beeman, M. (2015). Sudden insight is
and creativity really so different? Fluid associated with shutting out visual inputs.
intelligence, executive processes, and strategy use Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22,
in divergent thinking. Intelligence, 39, 36–45. 1814–1819.
Palva, S., & Palva, J. M. (2007). New vistas for Sarter, M., Givens, B., & Bruno, J. P. (2001).
α-frequency band oscillations. Trends in The cognitive neuroscience of sustained
Neurosciences, 30, 150–158. attention: Where top-down meets bottom-up.
Pearson, J., Naselaris, T., Holmes, E. A., & Kosslyn, Brain Research Reviews, 35, 146–160.
S. M. (2015). Mental imagery: Functional Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M.,
mechanisms and clinical applications. Trends in Pecherstorfer, T., Freunberger, R., & Hanslmayr,
Cognitive Sciences, 19, 590–602. S. (2005). EEG alpha synchronization and
Pfurtscheller, G. (1999). Quantification of functional coupling during top-down processing
ERD and ERS in the time domain. In G. in a working memory task. Human Brain
Pfurtscheller, & F. H. Lopes da Silva (Eds.), Mapping, 26, 148–155.
Event-related desynchronization. Handbook Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., Hassabis, D., Martin,
of electroencephalography and clinical V. C., Spreng, R. N., & Szpunar, K. K.
neurophysiology (Vol. 6, pp. 89–105). (2012). The future of memory: Remembering,
Amsterdam: Elsevier. imagining, and the brain. Neuron, 76, 677–694.
Pfurtscheller, G., Stancák Jr., A., & Neuper, A. C. Schwab, D., Benedek, M., Papousek, I., Weiss, E.
(1996). Event-related synchronization (ERS) M., & Fink, A. (2014). The time-course of
in the alpha band – An electrophysiological EEG alpha power changes in creative ideation.
correlate of cortical idling: A review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 310.
International Journal of Psychophysiology, Silvia, P. J. (2015). Intelligence and creativity are
24, 39–46. pretty similar after all. Educational Psychology
Pinho, A. L., Ullén, F., Castelo-Brancod, M., Review, 27, 599–606.
Fransson, P., & de Manzano, Ö. (2015). Smallwood, J., Brown, K. S., Tipper, C., Giesbrecht,
Addressing a paradox: dual strategies for B., Franklin, M. S., Mrazek, M. D., …
creative performance in introspective and Schooler, J. W. (2011). Pupillometric evidence
extrospective networks. Cerebral Cortex, 26, for the decoupling of attention from perceptual
3052–3063. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhv130 input during offline thought. PLoS ONE, 6,
Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., e18298.
Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2015). The science
L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. of mind wandering: Empirically navigating
Proceedings of the National Academy of the stream of consciousness. Annual Review of
Sciences, 98, 676–682. Psychology, 66, 487–518.

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Leicester, on 29 Jan 2018 at 00:14:05, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.011
194

194 m. b enedek

Spreng, R. N., Mar, R. A., & Kim, A. S. N. (2009). Consciousness and Cognition, 53, 165–175.
The common neural basis of autobiographical doi:10.1016/j.concog.2017.06.009
memory, prospection, navigation, theory of Wiley, J., & Jarosz, A. F. (2012). Working memory
mind, and the default mode: A quantitative capacity, attentional focus, and problem solving.
meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Current Directions in Psychological Science,
Neuroscience, 21, 489–510. 21, 258–262.
Spreng, R. N., Stevens, W. D., Chamberlain, J. P., Worden, M., Foxe, J. J., Wang, N., & Simpson, G.
Gilmore, A. W., & Schacter, D. L. (2010). V. (2000). Anticipatory biasing of visuospatial
Default network activity, coupled with the attention indexed by retinotopically specific
frontoparietal control network, supports goal- α-band electroencephalography increases over
directed cognition. NeuroImage, 53, 303–317. occipital cortex. Journal of Neuroscience,
Supp, G. G., Siegel, M., Hipp, J. F., & Engel, A. 20(6), RC63.
K. (2011). Cortical hypersynchrony predicts Zabelina, D. L., O’Leary, D., Pornpattananangkul,
breakdown of sensory processing during N., Nusslock, R., & Beeman, M. (2015).
loss of consciousness. Current Biology, 21, Creativity and sensory gating indexed by the
1988–1993. P50: Selective versus leaky sensory gating
Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Hashizume, H., Sassa, Y., in divergent thinkers and creative achievers.
Nagase, T., Nouchi, R., & Kawashima, R. Neuropsychologia, 69, 77–84.
(2011). Failing to deactivate: The association Zabelina, D. L., & Robinson, M. D. (2010).
between brain activity during a working Creativity as flexible cognitive control.
memory task and creativity. NeuroImage, 55, Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the
681–687. Arts, 4, 136–143.
Vartanian, O. (2009). Variable attention facilitates Zabelina, D., Saporta, A., & Beeman, M. (2015).
creative problem solving. Psychology of Flexible or leaky attention in creative people?
Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 57–59. Distinct patterns of attention for different types
Von Stein, A., Chiang, C., & König, P. (2000). of creative thinking. Memory & Cognition, 144,
Top-down processing mediated by interareal 488–498.
synchronization. Proceedings of the National Zanto, T. P., Rubens, M. T., Thangavel, A., &
Academy of Sciences, 97, 14748–14753. Gazzaley, A. (2011). Causal role of the
Von Stein, A., & Sarnthein, J. (2000). Different prefrontal cortex in top-down modulation of
frequencies for different scales of cortical visual processing and working memory. Nature
integration: From local gamma to long range Neuroscience, 14, 656–663.
alpha/theta synchronization. International Zatorre, R. J., Halpern, A. R., Perry, D. W., Meyer,
Journal of Psychophysiology, 38, 301–313. E., & Evans, A. C. (1996). Hearing in the
Walcher, S., Körner, C., & Benedek, M. (2017). mind’s ear: A PET investigation of musical
Looking for ideas: Eye behavior during imagery and perception. Journal of Cognitive
goal-directed internally-focused cognition. Neuroscience, 8, 29–46.

Downloaded from https://www.cambridge.org/core. University of Leicester, on 29 Jan 2018 at 00:14:05, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at
https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316556238.011

You might also like