Tee Kazantzis 2011

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/227585893

Collaborative Empiricism in Cognitive Therapy: A Definition and Theory for


the Relationship Construct

Article  in  Clinical Psychology Science and Practice · March 2011


DOI: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.2010.01234.x

CITATIONS READS

49 6,167

2 authors, including:

Nikolaos Kazantzis

163 PUBLICATIONS   3,029 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

International Study of the Development of Psychotherapists View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Nikolaos Kazantzis on 08 December 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Collaborative Empiricism in Cognitive Therapy:
A Definition and Theory for the Relationship Construct
John Tee and Nikolaos Kazantzis, School of Psychological Science, La Trobe University, Australia

Despite the consensus regarding the importance of col- 2008). However, there has been little theoretical analy-
laborative empiricism (CE) in Beck’s cognitive therapy, sis, and no definition or assessment of the construct.
absent are operational definitions, measures, or empiri- Although CE is frequently referred to as a distinctive
cal investigations of the construct. Existing research and defining characteristic of cognitive therapy, and as
has centered on constructs such as the working alliance
a core component of the therapeutic alliance (e.g.,
Beck, 1989; Bishop & Fish, 1999; Cross, 2007; Datti-
and has produced inconsistent findings. It is unclear
lio, 2000; Dudley & Kuyken, 2006; Friedberg & Clark,
whether CE is related to treatment outcomes, or
2006; Friedberg & Gorman, 2007; Kuyken, Padesky, &
whether it mediates other cognitive change processes.
Dudley, 2008; Merali & Lynch, 1997; Overholser,
This article argues that the core meaning of collabora-
1995; Rutter & Friedberg, 1999; Segal & Shaw, 1996;
tion in CE, ‘‘sharing the work,’’ is not captured by the Will, 1995), it remains unclear whether CE directly or
most frequently used construct of the therapeutic indirectly facilitates hypothesized mechanisms of cogni-
alliance in cognitive therapy research. A theory of CE, tive change and positive therapeutic outcomes. Existing
based on self-determination theory, is developed that discussion of CE in the literature has been limited to a
integrates the collaborative and empirical aspects brief outline of two components (collaboration and
of CE and addresses the motivational aspect of the empiricism). Absent are analyses of how these compo-
construct. nents interact, or a theory of how CE may benefit
Key words: cognitive, collaboration, collaborative clients in therapy.
empiricism, empiricism, relationship, therapy. [Clin This article will first review the current definition
Psychol Sci Prac 18: 47–61, 2011]
and assessment of CE in cognitive therapy. Second, the
article will provide a theoretical discussion to clarify
and elaborate on the nature and interaction between
A contradiction awaits the reader who reviews the lit- the elements of collaboration. Finally, the article will
erature on collaborative empiricism (CE). The past propose new components of CE that can form the
40 years have seen widespread agreement regarding the basis of a more fully articulated definition of the con-
central role of CE in cognitive therapy (Hollon & struct, which can also form the basis of an operational
Beck, 1979; Kuyken, Padesky, & Dudley, 2009; definition, and a new measure.
Madsen, 2009; Persons, 2008; Tryon & Misurell,
EXISTING DEFINITIONS OF COLLABORATIVE EMPIRICISM

Address correspondence to Nikolaos Kazantzis, School of The therapeutic collaboration was first outlined as a
Psychological Science, La Trobe University, Victoria 3086, means for identifying ‘‘raw data’’ and a means for
Australia. E-mail: N.Kazantzis@latrobe.edu.au. encouraging client identification, observation, and

 2011 American Psychological Association. Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc., on behalf of the American Psychological Association.
All rights reserved. For permissions, please email: permissionsuk@wiley.com 47
evaluation of introspective beliefs in Beck, Rush, Client: Gee, well, I don’t know…
Shaw, and Emery’s (1979) Cognitive Therapy for
Depression (see also Beck, 1967). These seminal works Perhaps they are tired … maybe, even they are
waiting for a lead from someone else? Is it possible
define CE as a basic strategy or form of interaction
they think I don’t make much of an effort?
underlying the relationship between client and thera-
pist in cognitive therapy (Hollon & Beck, 1979). Therapist: Those sound like some good possibilities.
Client and therapist actively collaborate to identify I really like how you are looking around the situa-
problem situations, and to design, implement, and tion from different perspectives. I wonder how
evaluate empirical tests of the client’s beliefs. A key could we figure out how much of the ‘‘not talking’’
objective is for CE to enable experimentation of is due to other people’s views of you … or any of
beliefs and behaviors outside therapy via the vehicle these other ideas you have generated?
of homework assignments (Beck, 1970, 1995; Kazant-
zis, MacEwan, & Dattilio, 2005). This collaborative Client: Hmmm. Maybe I could take more notice of
sharing of the work of experimental testing distin- what happens when I am socializing.…
guishes CE. For example, a therapist aiming to point
Therapist: Good, let’s figure out how to be specific
out errors in thought process through disputation
about how you will notice that.…
may be attempting to foster empiricism, without col-
laboration:
A true sharing of the work of identifying ideas to be
tested, and then devising means of testing them out,
Client: I don’t want to go to the dinner; people
clearly has a different presence in therapy. However,
never talk to me—they think I’m boring.
much of the literature following the early teaching in
CE is less than clear. While there is agreement that CE
Therapist: Sounds like you’re mindreading again.
is important for cognitive therapy (cf. Kuyken et al.,
When you’re at the dinner, notice whether people
2008, 2009; Persons, 2008; Tryon & Misurell, 2008),
actually seem bored, or whether you’re basing your
subsequent analysis and discussion have been largely
feeling on what other people think, or worse yet,
based on practical, commonsense grounds, rather than
what you think they’re thinking.
firm conceptual foundation. For instance, CE has been
The same lead from the client could be pursued by described as ‘‘embedded in every clinical action and
a therapist adherent to CE, as outlined in A. T. Beck’s decision’’ (Friedberg & McClure, 2002, p. 44), which
therapy, by taking the following approach: comprises the ‘‘cornerstone of CBT’’ and is ‘‘used
throughout the entire course of treatment’’ (Dattilio,
Client: I don’t want to go to the dinner; people
2000, p. 39). There is also confusion as to whether CE
never talk to me—they think I’m boring.
is a relationship attribute or technique, or both a pro-
Therapist: That sounds like a tricky situation for you cess and an approach (e.g., Gleeson & McGorry,
Would you like to talk about that some more—and 2004). In some cases, CE has been referred to as a
see what we can figure out here as a way to help? major therapeutic method and strategy in Beck’s ther-
apy (Abramson, Alloy, & Dykman, 1990; Bell, Grech,
Therapist: Ok, well if I am hearing you correctly, a Maiden, Halligan, & Ellis, 2005; Dattilio, 2000). In
concern that you have is that people think you are other cases, CE is referred to as a ‘‘philosophy which
boring, and this is connected to your conversations forms the foundation of cognitive therapy’’ (Wilkes,
with people. It might help us to find out under 1994, p. 309), as well as a ‘‘specific technique’’ and an
which situations this happens the most.… For the ‘‘atmosphere’’ that pervades the patient–therapist rela-
moment, I wonder what might be the reasons that tionship (Turkington & Siddle, 1998, p. 237). Thus, it
people (including you) might not talk much some- remains unclear whether CE can be considered a
times? requirement for the therapeutic relationship in Beck’s

CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE • V18 N1, MARCH 2011 48


cognitive therapy, or whether it can be better defined are extended to discussions of the alliance in cogni-
as a technique or a strategy (Hazlett-Stevens, 2008; van tive therapy. Incompatible definitions of CE include
Oppen, 2004; Waddington, 2002; Westefeld et al., comment that CE is referred to as the basis or ideal
2000). Importantly, as there is no measure or outline cognitive therapy alliance (Beck et al., 1985; Bishop &
of what constitutes more or less effective CE in the lit- Fish, 1999; Dobson & Shaw, 1988), whereas elsewhere
erature, clarifying the definition and its connection it is neither the relationship, nor a component of
with the therapeutic alliance is important for research, it, but a ‘‘stylistic fulcrum that permits the helping
training, and practice. alliance to thrive’’ (Stein, Kupfer, & Schatzberg, 2006,
p. 359).
COLLABORATIVE EMPIRICISM AND THE THERAPEUTIC If the concept of collaboration in Bordin’s (1979)
ALLIANCE working alliance fails to capture the meaning of col-
The therapeutic alliance can be broadly defined as the laboration in CE, it is possible that measures of the
alliance of a client’s reasonable side with a therapist’s alliance based on Bordin’s conception may similarly
working side (Gelso & Hayes, 1998; Horvath & fail to capture the meaning of collaboration in CE. It
Greenberg, 1994). There are reliable meta-analytic also follows that research studies that have employed
findings of a robust relationship between therapeutic working alliance measures may not have accounted
alliance and treatment outcomes in cognitive therapy for CE. This possibility is demonstrated here by
(Horvath & Symonds, 1991; Martin, Garske, & Davis, an analysis of the most commonly used measure of
2000; Shirk & Karver, 2003). This relationship holds the therapeutic relationship in cognitive therapy, the
across different therapies and disorders, despite whether Working Alliance Inventory (WAI; Horvath &
outcome or alliance is rated by therapist, client, or Greenberg, 1989, 1994).
observer (Martin et al., 2000; Shirk & Karver, 2003).
Bordin’s conception of the alliance has been the The Working Alliance Inventory
most often used in cognitive therapy research. Bordin The WAI is a 36-item questionnaire designed specifi-
described collaboration as a ‘‘working relationship’’ cally to measure Bordin’s conception of the working
between client and therapist (Bordin, 1979, 1994). At alliance (Horvath & Greenberg, 1989, 1994). It has
face value, this working relationship seems similar to been widely used in alliance research, across many
the meaning of collaboration in CE as ‘‘sharing the types of therapy (Horvath, 2001; Horvath & Green-
work.’’ However, Bordin conceived collaboration in berg, 1989, 1994; Horvath & Luborsky, 1993; Horvath
the working alliance as an ‘‘agreed-upon contract’’ & Symonds, 1991; Martin et al., 2000), and has good
between client and therapist regarding the tasks and psychometric properties (Hatcher & Gillaspy, 2006).
goals of therapy (Bordin, 1994, p. 254). According to However, analysis of the items in the WAI reveals that
Bordin, agreement is ‘‘the key process in building an no item attempts to measure collaboration, or indeed
initial, viable alliance’’ (Horvath & Greenberg, 1994, empiricism, as these concepts are used in cognitive
p. 1). However, collaboration in CE is not simply will- therapy. In the popular WAI revised short form, three
ing participation by the client nor agreement on tasks items in the 4-item Goal subscale refer to ‘‘agreement’’
or goals. Rather, the cognitive therapist aims to engage and ‘‘good understanding,’’ but collaboration is not
the client in a genuine sharing of the work of goal set- covered. The fourth item does refer to ‘‘collaboration
ting and creative authorship of therapeutic tasks, pro- in setting goals,’’ but this phrasing does not discrimi-
gressively encouraging the client to take the lead role nate the meaning of collaboration as ‘‘sharing the
in these activities as far as is practicable (Beck, 1995; work’’ that is central in CE. No items in either the
Beck, Emery, & Greenberg, 1985; Beck et al., 1979). Task or Bond subscales refer to collaboration. There-
The aim of this partnership is to actively share the fore, we argue that the most frequently used construct
work of empirically evaluating the client’s beliefs, not of the therapeutic alliance in cognitive therapy out-
simply to agree on a program of therapeutic tasks come research fails to capture the key aspects of CE as
and goals. However, problems in the definition of CE described in Beck’s cognitive therapy.

DEFINING COLLABORATIVE EMPIRICISM • TEE & KAZANTZIS 49


COLLABORATIVE EMPIRICISM AND THERAPIST COMPETENCE CTS and to discriminate between therapist adherence
The Cognitive Therapy Rating Scale and competence. The scale contains 25 items in five
Several scales have been designed to assess a therapist’s sections. Five items relate to the collaborative thera-
competence in cognitive therapy practice, in a specific peutic relationship. Collaboration is defined as ‘‘shared
session, with a specific client (Brosan, Reynolds, & responsibility for defining patient’s problems and
Moore, 2007; Brosan, Reynolds, & Moore, 2008; potential solutions; functioning as a team,’’ but as with
Dobson & Christensen, 2001; Kazantzis, 2003; Trepka, previous measures, it mainly misses the focus on
Rees, Shapiro, Hardy, & Barkham, 2004). The Cogni- empiricism. Item 23 refers to ‘‘asking for evidence ⁄
tive Therapy Scale (CTS; Young & Beck, 1980, 1988) alternative views: asked for patient’s evidence for
has been designed as a supervisor ⁄ independent observer maladaptive beliefs,’’ but does not emphasize collabora-
rating scale to assess the extent that cognitive therapy tion. Given that levels of inter-rater agreement were
is conducted in a manner consistent with its undergird- low for items measuring collaboration, guided discov-
ing theory and is being flexibly tailored to meet indi- ery, and ‘‘asking for evidence ⁄ alternative views’’
vidual client needs (cf. Kazantzis, Reinecke, & (adherence ICCs range 0.43–0.55, competence ICCs
Freeman, 2010). Included in the CTS is an item on range 0.48–0.53), it is possible that this lack of clarity
‘‘Collaboration.’’ The CTS rating manual specifies in the CE construct contributed to the low level of
three meanings of collaboration for this item: ensuring reliability on these items. Thus, as with existing mea-
compatible goals to avoid working against each other, sures of the therapeutic relationship, current measures
minimizing client resistance, and preventing misunder- of therapist competence in cognitive therapy cannot
standings. Although this sense of active teamwork is discriminate the meanings of collaboration or empiri-
compatible with the genuine sharing of the work of cism specific to CE without idiosyncratic interpretation
empirical evaluation involved in CE, whether ‘‘collab- of these terms by raters.
oration’’ is interpreted in the sense meant in CE, of a
creative and genuine sharing of the work, or simply Change Processes in Cognitive Therapy
agreement on tasks and goals, is a matter of interpreta- Collaborative empiricism has been hypothesized to act
tion by the individual rater. Further, neither the item as a change process in cognitive therapy (Dobson &
nor the rating manual specifies empirical investigation Dobson, 2009; Kuyken et al., 2008, 2009). Change in
as a focus for collaboration, but empirical investigation cognitive therapy has often been understood via the
is a key component of CE. cognitive mediation hypothesis, which proposes that
Other items on the CTS also refer to collaboration improvements in therapeutic outcome result from
(i.e., Agenda Setting, Feedback, Understanding, and direct change in clients’ cognitions (Beck, 1970;
Guided Discovery), but similarly do not distinguish the DeRubeis, Tang, & Beck, 2001). Belief change is
specific meanings of collaboration and empiricism hypothesized to be more likely if the rationale for
inherent in CE or provide clear guidance on the defi- change comes from clients’ identification of their own
nition of collaborative empiricism. Questions regarding introspective data, and collaborative experimentation,
the consistency of CTS ratings made by expert raters rather than from direct illustration by the therapist
have been published (Jacobson & Gortner, 2000; Shaw (Dattilio & Padesky, 1990). Self-efficacy may be lower
et al., 1999), so it is possible that greater attention and if the therapist is seen as the change agent, increasing
delineation of the ‘‘empiricism’’ element to CE could the probability of relapse (Horvath & Greenberg,
improve in the scale’s psychometric properties. A simi- 1994). Self-evaluation of beliefs, using experiments that
lar level of ambiguity regarding the CE construct exists are collaboratively designed, avoids the confrontation
in the revised CTS (e.g., Blackburn et al., 2001; Gor- implicit in directly challenging delusional beliefs and
don, 2007). hallucinations (Alford & Beck, 1994). In addition,
The Cognitive Therapy Adherence and Compe- collaborative empiricism reframes the process of
tence Scale (Barber, Liese, & Abrams, 2003) has been therapy as a shared, challenging, empirical experiment
designed to improve the psychometric properties of the (Klosko & Sanderson, 1999). This may increase the

CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE • V18 N1, MARCH 2011 50


psychological activation and a sense of hope, which tured collaboration of CE, combined with explicit
may be more important than specific cognitive or empirical testing of the client’s cognitions, may reduce
behavioral tasks (Cross, 2007). The atmosphere of curi- the impact of these biases or distortions in cognitive
osity modeled in CE may also foster flexible thinking, processing (Bieling & Kuyken, 2003).
encourage a broader examination of the client’s prob- CE may also reduce erroneous or biased recall on
lems, and allow the client and therapist to get at core the part of either client or therapist, by subjecting recall
subjective experiences (Friedberg & McClure, 2002). to ongoing empirical validation (Hollon, 1999).
In case conceptualization, the client’s unique experi- Reducing biased recall by the therapist may affect out-
ence guides how both the theory and empirically sup- comes by increasing the accuracy of the case conceptu-
ported principles of cognitive therapy are employed alization, which may then affect outcomes as discussed
with a particular individual in his or her unique cir- earlier. Collaborative empiricism may increase objectiv-
cumstances (Beck, 1995; Persons, 2008). Kuyken et al. ity in the session by reducing reliance on potentially
(2009) provided a helpful account of CE, as one of biased recall, and by providing the client with a sys-
three fundamental principles informing the case con- tematic method of objectively evaluating his or her
ceptualization process in cognitive therapy. In their beliefs (Hollon & Kriss, 1984). The merit of these
guide for practitioners, it was proposed that collabora- approaches is that they articulate mechanisms by which
tion and empiricism function in tandem as the driving case conceptualization may be related to CE.
force underlying the process of case conceptualization. This discussion provides some explanation of
Accordingly, the genuine collaboration inherent in CE ‘‘how’’ empirical investigation may affect therapeutic
aids the development of an accurate shared understand- outcomes, but it does not explain ‘‘why’’ a client may
ing of the client’s problem situation. Information cen- be motivated to engage in the arduous process of eval-
tral to understanding the client’s concerns and goals in uating his or her beliefs. Many clients, perhaps most,
their unique historical, social, developmental, and psy- come to therapy driven by an emotional pain they
chological contexts benefits from collaboration. The wish to relieve. However, as the motivational inter-
empirical aspect of CE then functions as a check on viewing literature underscores, clients often have con-
the accuracy of the information generated through the flicting motivations for therapy (Miller & Rollnick,
collaborative data-gathering process (Kuyken et al., 2002). Change may offer greater freedom or less pain
2009). Aspects of the conceptualization that do not fit but comes with its own costs. A theory that addresses
the client’s situation can be revised or discarded after this motivational dimension of CE is lacking.
empirical testing. The process of CE first increases the
range of information available to the case conceptuali- A THEORETICAL BASIS FOR COLLABORATIVE EMPIRICISM
zation and then filters out inaccurate information A theoretical foundation for CE that addresses the moti-
by testing it empirically. Therefore, CE mediates the vational aspect of the construct, and further articulates
relationship between the case conceptualization and the structure of its component parts, may be drawn from
therapeutic outcomes by increasing the relevance and self-determination theory (SDT). Self-determination
accuracy of the conceptualization. theory states that people have a fundamental propensity
Collaborative empiricism may also increase the toward growth, self-determination, and the resolution
accuracy of the case conceptualization by overcoming of psychological discrepancy (Deci & Ryan, 1980, 1985;
heuristic biases inherent in therapists’ interpretations of Ryan & Deci, 2000, 2002). The theory seeks to explain
their clients’ problem situations (Kuyken et al., 2009). how individuals self-regulate behavior and internalize
In developing a case conceptualization, therapists have extrinsic motivation to engage in autonomous action
working hypotheses about their client’s situation and (Markland, Ryan, Tobin, & Rollnick, 2005; Ryan &
interpret information arising in session in terms of these Deci, 2000; Vansteenkiste & Sheldon, 2006). It is also
hypotheses. Therapists, like anyone else, are susceptible concerned with the effect of environmental factors, such
to overreliance or misapplication of heuristics in deci- as directives or rewards, on intrinsic motivation to
sion making (Kahneman, 2003). However, the struc- engage in behaviors (Deci & Ryan, 2000).

DEFINING COLLABORATIVE EMPIRICISM • TEE & KAZANTZIS 51


Self-determination theory proposes that behaviors are tion of new behaviors and learning (Deci & Ryan,
regulated on a continuum of autonomy, ranging from 2008).
intrinsic (autonomous) regulation to introjected regula- Higher autonomous (intrinsic) motivation has also
tion, and then external regulation. External regulation been associated with better session outcomes, as rated
refers to behavior that is regulated by external factors, by either client or therapist, and lower psychopathol-
such as rewards or punishments. A direct attempt to per- ogy (Michalak, Klappheck, & Kosfelder, 2004), as well
suade a client to change a belief is an example of an as lower tension, less distraction, and higher positive
externally regulated motivation. Introjected regulation mood in session, and higher ratings of importance, sat-
refers to motivation that is internalized. In this case, the isfaction, and intention to continue with therapy
pressure to engage in a behavior comes from within the (Pelletier, Tuson, & Haddad, 1997). It has also been
person, for example, in the form of self-approval when found to predict symptom remission and post-
engaging successfully in the behavior, and shame or self- treatment depression severity, independently of whether
criticism when failing to engage successfully in the the therapeutic modality was interpersonal therapy,
behavior (Ryan & Deci, 2000). Empirical work suggests cognitive-behavior therapy, or pharmacotherapy with
that introjected regulation is more likely to lead to sus- clinical management (Zuroff et al., 2007). Similarly,
tained engagement in behavior than external regulation the self-reported willingness of clients to engage in
(Assor, Vansteenkiste, & Kaplan, 2009; Deci & Ryan, treatment for drug dependence has been shown to pre-
2000). However, it can be accompanied by significant dict therapist-rated engagement and post-treatment
ambivalence, as well as negative emotional states and abstinence (Zeldman, Ryan, & Fiscella, 2004). Thera-
inner conflict (Markland et al., 2005; Ryan, Rigby, & peutic goals that are valued as more personally impor-
King, 1993). At the other end of the continuum is tant or interesting are associated with lower negative
intrinsic motivation. Intrinsically motivated behaviors affect, higher positive affect, and higher self-esteem
are engaged in willingly and are experienced as reward- (Sheldon & Kasser, 1995). In addition, autonomous
ing of themselves (Deci & Ryan, 1980). regulation of behavior is performed with higher quality
Self-determination theory proposes that three key and attention (Deci & Ryan, 1980), is more stable over
needs underlie the innate tendency toward growth: time (Deci & Ryan, 1985), and is associated with more
competence, autonomy, and relatedness (Deci & Ryan, positive emotional states (Deci & Ryan, 1980; Ryan &
2000). That is, humans have an innate need to experi- Deci, 2002).
ence themselves as competent, autonomous actors who This relationship between intrinsic motivation and
are related to others in meaningful and satisfying rela- outcomes is also found in nonclinical settings. For
tionships. These factors work to increase the level of example, higher intrinsic motivation has been shown
intrinsic motivation associated with behavior. to predict weight loss (Williams, Grow, Freedman,
There is considerable empirical support for SDT in Ryan, & Deci, 1996), eating self-regulation (Mata
the social psychology literature. Of the three underly- et al., 2009), and greater needs satisfaction in volunteers
ing psychological needs proposed by SDT, most of the (Weinstein & Ryan, 2010).
existing research has been conducted on the concept of
autonomy. Autonomy in SDT involves the self- Self-Determination Theory and Collaborative Empiricism
endorsement of behavior, accompanied by a perception According to SDT, supporting the client’s sense of
of willingness or volition (Ryan & Deci, 2008). autonomy is required to augment competence and
Increased autonomy has been associated with support- relatedness and promote optimal motivation for change
ing autonomous choices (Moller, Deci, & Ryan, (Deci & Ryan, 2000; Markland et al., 2005). Self-
2006), goals that are self-endorsed (Koestner, Ryan, efficacy, the belief that one is competent to engage in
Bernieri, & Holt, 1984), and promoting choice (Deci, a behavior, is not sufficient to maximize motivation.
Eghrari, Patrick, & Leone, 1994). It leads to Clients may feel able to engage in behavior but not feel
increased perception of the self as the locus of causality motivated to do so. Increasing the level of autonomous
in therapy, with a corresponding increase in integra- regulation of a behavior is required to maximize

CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE • V18 N1, MARCH 2011 52


motivation to engage in the behavior (Deci & Ryan, CE and to actively exercise choice and creativity in the
1985; Reeve, 1998, 2002). design and evaluation of empirical tests of his or her
Self-determination theory can be used to explain beliefs. This genuine sharing of the work of therapy,
how CE moderates therapeutic outcomes and why cli- combined with the focus on the client’s intrinsic data
ents may be more motivated to engage in the process as the source and arbiter of empirical evaluation of
of belief change as a result of CE. The evidence beliefs, increases the client’s intrinsic motivation for
reviewed above suggests that at least four environmen- change.
tal conditions support clients’ autonomy: a meaningful
rationale for behavior change, minimal external contin- A DEFINITION OF COLLABORATIVE EMPIRICISM
gent reinforcers (rewards and punishments), active par- The concept of CE is founded in Beck’s (1967, 1976)
ticipation and exercising of choice, and acceptance and seminal cognitive theory and system of psychotherapy.
acknowledgment of negative feelings (Markland et al., In subsequent work, CE has been described as a com-
2005; Reeve, 1998, 2002). Collaborative empiricism bination of two components, collaboration and empiri-
directly influences the first three of these, and the cism. However, the relationship between these
fourth as part of general good cognitive therapy components remains unclear. As outlined earlier, SDT
practice. is able to clarify this relationship in terms of the key
In CE, a meaningful rationale for changing beliefs role of the client’s intrinsic data (see Table 1).
arises from the clients’ experience of the results of their When client and therapist collaborate to share the
own empirical tests of their beliefs. By definition, a work of therapy, but do not empirically evaluate the
good empirical test of the client’s beliefs is one in client’s cognitive content (quadrant II), the rationale
which alternative explanations for data resulting from for belief change is not grounded in the client’s evalua-
the experiment have been accounted for in the design tion of his or her own intrinsic data. According to
of the experiment (Beck, 1995). The elimination of SDT, this lack of intrinsic data results in lower intrinsic
alternative explanations of the results of experiments motivation, or even external motivation if data are
increases the validity of the data. The key point sug- sourced from the therapist. Lower intrinsic motivation
gested here is that CE also increases the degree of inter- is associated with lower motivation to change. Con-
nally autonomous relevance of the data for the client. versely, when intrinsic data are used in the empirical
Reasons for changing beliefs are ideally experienced as evaluation of the client’s beliefs, but the process is not
the client’s own reasons, rather than the therapist’s rea- genuinely collaborative (quadrant III), SDT suggests
sons. In CE, the client experiences this data as intrinsi- that the lack of collaboration will reduce the client’s
cally his or her own. The meaning of this intrinsic data sense of autonomy, competence, and efficacy and shift
is judged using criteria that the client decides upon the client’s locus of causality externally, again resulting
before the experiment is conducted. The client is the in lower intrinsic motivation to change. When collabo-
source of these criteria, and hence, the source of the ration and empiricism are combined in CE, SDT sug-
regulatory capacity of the criteria as a source of inter- gests that the resulting generation and evaluation of the
pretation of the data is internal to the client. client’s intrinsic data are likely to increase the client’s
Self-determination theory suggests that intrinsic data sense of efficacy, autonomy, internal locus of causality,
are more likely to be experienced as an internal and competence, and therefore intrinsic motivation for
autonomous source of regulation, leading in turn to change (see Figure 1).
increased motivation for the client to change his or her In summary, we suggest that SDT can provide a
belief. In addition, the process of collaboration mini- theoretical basis for the efficacy of CE in belief change.
mizes external contingent reinforcers, further increasing SDT highlights the key role of the client’s intrinsic data
internal motivation. Although CE is directive in the as the point of operation of the components of collabo-
sense that the client is directed to engage in the process ration and empiricism. In this view, CE encourages
of CE, the CE process itself is genuinely collaborative. autonomously motivated belief change by promoting
The client is encouraged to participate in all aspects of the acceptance and internalization of new beliefs, and

DEFINING COLLABORATIVE EMPIRICISM • TEE & KAZANTZIS 53


Table 1. Components of collaboration, empiricism, and intrinsic data

Low collaboration High collaboration

Low empiricism I. Low CE II. Increased autonomy but without


evaluation of beliefs via intrinsic data.
Lower intrinsic motivation for change
High empiricism III. External locus of causality, IV. Evaluation of client’s intrinsic data.
lower efficacy, diminished Increased autonomy, competence,
impact of intrinsic data intrinsic motivation

by increasing the meaningfulness of new beliefs for the associated with clients’ satisfaction with that session
client. (Eisenthal, Koopman, & Lazare, 1983). Clients are
The application of SDT to CE raises several more likely to return for therapy after an intake inter-
hypotheses for empirical examination. SDT suggests view in which the therapist shares the problem formu-
that the collaborative empirical investigation of clients’ lation and negotiates mutual therapeutic goals,
intrinsic data increases the core SDT variables of compared to an interview where neither is done
autonomy, competence, and relatedness, thereby (Tracy, 1977). Similarly, therapist–client discussion of
increasing motivation to engage in behavior change. goals and expression of thoughts and feelings are associ-
SDT also clarifies the functioning of collaboration and ated with observer ratings of ‘‘goodness’’ of therapy
empiricism in CE in terms of these core variables. sessions (Hoyt, 1980; Hoyt, Xenakis, Marmar, &
Before outlining a research agenda informed by these Horowitz, 1983). Simply specifying goals is enough to
hypotheses, existing empirical work on collaboration increase clients’ ratings of therapy helpfulness (Gold-
and empiricism will be reviewed. stein, Cohen, Lewis, & Struening, 1988). Client-rated
agreement with therapist and client-rated experience of
Empirical Evaluation of Collaborative Empiricism goal consensus at session 2 has been shown to predict
There has been no empirical investigation of CE in cog- the reduction in symptoms on the Symptom Check
nitive therapy research trials. However, research in other List (SCL; Derogatis, 1974), including up to six months
contexts has found a relationship between aspects of col- later (Dormaar, Dijkman, & de Vries, 1989). Higher
laboration in CE and therapeutic outcomes (Boardman, response congruence, in which therapist verbal
Catley, Grobe, Little, & Ahluwalia, 2006; Bordin, 1994; response directly addresses the subject of the client’s
Clemence, Hilsenroth, Ackerman, Strassle, & Handler, immediately preceding statement, is associated with
2005; Colson et al., 1988; Connor-Greene, 1993; higher likelihood of the client returning for therapy
Creed & Kendall, 2005; Diamond, Liddle, Hogue, & after the initial session (Duehn & Proctor, 1974).
Dakof, 1999; Hatcher, 1999; Hatcher & Barends, 1996; Finally, topic determination, the proportion of topic
Le Bloc’h, de Roten, Drapeau, & Despland, 2006). initiations subsequently followed by the other partici-
One such aspect of collaboration in CE is the fram- pant in therapy (client or therapist), predicts the con-
ing of the client as central to the process of shared tinuation of therapy past session 3 (Tracey, 1986).
decision making. Sharing decisions with the client These aspects of collaboration do not address the core
regarding the process of therapy, and developing a clear meaning of collaboration in CE. Nevertheless, shared
rationale and explanation for the course of a session are decision making, sharing the formulation, negotiation

Figure 1. Hypothetical relationships in the benefits of evaluating intrinsic data.

CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE • V18 N1, MARCH 2011 54


of therapeutic goals, response congruence, and topic A MEASURE OF COLLABORATIVE EMPIRICISM

determination are aspects of collaboration in CE for Empirical work has shown a relationship between
which there is empirical support. aspects of collaboration and therapeutic outcomes. This
Not all studies have found a relationship between suggests that CE, which incorporates these aspects of
aspects of collaboration and outcome. Beyebach and collaboration, may also be related to outcomes. Never-
Carranza (1997) failed to replicate the relationship theless, results of this research have been mixed, and
between topic determination and engagement reported much of this work has focused on collaboration as a
by Tracey (1986). However, nonengaged clients in the client factor, rather than a property of the dyad. Col-
Beyebach and Carranza study used more domineering laboration in CE goes beyond the meaning of collabo-
language with therapists, interrupted more often, and ration in these studies. Thus, while research to date
engaged in more conflict. It is possible that these provides qualified support for a relationship between
behaviors disrupted collaboration, resulting in lower aspects of CE and outcomes, research focusing specifi-
continuation of therapy (Tryon & Winograd, 2002). cally on CE is needed. For this, a measure is needed
Collaborative involvement has also been used as a that incorporates the aspects of collaboration discussed
measure of collaboration in therapy process. Collabora- earlier and operationalizes the meanings of collabora-
tive involvement is the mutual involvement of client tion and empiricism specific to CE.
and therapist in a helping relationship (Tryon & Wino- As with collaboration, there has been no specific
grad, 2002). The term ‘‘mutual involvement’’ suggests investigation of the empirical component of CE in
that this construct has a meaning similar to collabora- cognitive therapy trials. However, there has been some
tion in CE. However, studies using the construct have evaluation of one form of empirical evaluation of cli-
operationalized collaborative involvement as the com- ents’ beliefs in cognitive therapy in the form of behav-
pliance or cooperation of the client to the strategies of ioral experiments (BEs; Beck et al., 1979; Clark, Beck,
the therapist. For example, Schmidt and Woolaway- & Alford, 1999; Dobson & Dobson, 2009). Behavioral
Bickel (2000) operationalized collaborative involvement experiments are widely used and recommended in cog-
as homework completion. They found that therapists’ nitive therapy as a component of standard treatment for
ratings of compliance with homework predicted out- a range of disorders (Beck, 1995; Bennett-Levy et al.,
comes, but clients’ ratings of compliance did not. This 2004; Padesky & Greenberger, 1995). It has been sug-
suggests a relationship between clients’ compliance and gested that the very ubiquity of BEs in successful ther-
outcomes (see review in Kazantzis, Whittington, & apy attests to their value (Bennett-Levy et al., 2004).
Dattilio, in press), but is a long way from ‘‘mutual However, despite their importance in cognitive therapy,
involvement’’ in the sense of a genuinely collaborative only one study has directly investigated the efficacy of
sharing of the work indicative of CE. BEs in belief change (Bennett-Levy, 2003). In this
In an interesting study, O’Malley, Suh, and Strupp study, 27 trainee cognitive therapists compared the
(1983) found that client involvement at session 3 pre- effectiveness of BEs and automatic thought records in
dicted outcome, whereas involvement at sessions 1 and changing their own cognitions. The authors reported
2 did not. They concluded that client involvement that BEs produced significantly higher cognitive and
may not be simply a quality that the client brings to behavioral change compared with thought records.
therapy, but may be facilitated by qualities of the thera- Several trainees credited the higher efficacy of BEs to
pist. Windholz and Silberschatz (1988) found that ther- their experiential aspect, noting that BEs led to new
apist ratings of client involvement were related to cognitions that were more powerfully believed than
outcomes, but not client or observer ratings. It is sug- those which were understood rationally but not experi-
gested here that involvement may also be a function of enced in a BE.
the interaction between client and therapist, rather than There is some empirical work supporting a mediat-
a quality of one or the other in isolation. If true, the ing effect of CE on therapeutic outcomes. While there
genuine collaboration inherent in CE could be has been no direct investigation of the relationship
expected to increase client involvement. between case conceptualization and outcomes in

DEFINING COLLABORATIVE EMPIRICISM • TEE & KAZANTZIS 55


cognitive therapy, there is evidence that another type of the therapeutic relationship as conceived by Aaron T.
case conceptualization, the Core Conflictual Relation- Beck are accepted, serious questions can be asked of
ship Theme (CCRT) method, used in brief psychody- the empirical based on the therapeutic approach:
namic therapy, has good reliability and validity and is
related to therapeutic outcomes (Dobson & Dobson, Have previous studies missed an important determi-
2009; Luborsky & Crits-Christoph, 1998). This associa- nant of successful cognitive therapy outcome? If
tion has been interpreted in the literature as supporting training and evaluation of therapists’ practices in
a causal link between more accurate case conceptualiza- cognitive therapy trials have missed collaborative
tion and therapeutic outcomes (e.g., Bieling & Kuyken, empiricism, have the effects of the therapy been
2003). However, while some studies have found that diluted?
increased accuracy in conceptualization predicted symp-
tom reduction (Crits-Christoph, Barber, & Kurcias, A measure specific to CE is required to address
1993), other work has not found this effect (Crits-Chr- these issues. Existing theory provides a plausible expla-
istoph, Cooper, & Luborsky, 1988). These findings pro- nation for elements of the collaboration and empiricism
vide qualified support for the hypothesis that the components of CE, but remains piecemeal and ignores
accuracy of case conceptualization may be related to the motivational role of the construct. A new theory
outcomes in cognitive therapy. Further empirical work for CE has been proposed in this article, drawing on
is needed, focusing specifically on the role of CE in case well-validated social-cognitive theory, which integrates
conceptualization in cognitive therapy. the collaborative and empirical components of the con-
The lack of empirical investigation of the role of struct and its motivational aspect.
collaboration and empiricism in CE, as well as hypoth- Following from this article, the field would benefit
eses suggested by the application of SDT to CE above, from clinical discussion illustrating the role of CE in
suggests a research agenda for CE. An operational mea- effective cognitive therapy, empirical work utilizing
sure of CE is required to investigate the relationship systematic case studies, and prospective evaluations of
between CE and the theoretically derived variables sug- the relationship attribute as a determinant of therapeu-
gested in this article, as well as a component investiga- tic outcomes. Put simply, cognitive therapy researchers
tion of the CE construct. Development and evaluation are encouraged to collaborate on the empirical evalua-
of a measure of CE are currently being undertaken by tion of the CE construct.
the authors. One goal of the research program is to
facilitate an assessment of therapist competence in the REFERENCES
use of CE, as part of the training and supervision of Abramson, L. Y., Alloy, L. B., & Dykman, B. M. (1990).
practitioners in this important therapeutic process. Cognitive theory and therapy of emotional disorders: A
Another primary goal is to enable prospective treatment lay epistemic reconstruction? Psychological Inquiry, 1, 198–
outcome research to evaluate whether ‘‘sharing the 200.
work’’ of identifying and empirically evaluating clients’ Alford, B. A., & Beck, A. T. (1994). Cognitive therapy of
delusional beliefs. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 369–
beliefs can lead to better therapeutic outcomes.
380.
Assor, A., Vansteenkiste, M., & Kaplan, A. (2009). Identified
CONCLUSION
versus introjected approach and introjected avoidance
The present article reviewed definitions, provided a motivations in school and in sports: The limited benefits
theoretical discussion, and presented new components of self-worth strivings. Journal of Educational Psychology,
of collaborative empiricism. There is support in the 101, 482–497.
literature for the efficacy of aspects of collaboration Barber, J. P., Liese, B. S., & Abrams, M. J. (2003). Develop-
and empiricism in CE. However, no theoretical or ment of the Cognitive Therapy Adherence and Compe-
empirical work to date has defined or measured the tence Scale. Psychotherapy Research, 13, 205–221.
core elements of the construct. If the criticisms Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theo-
regarding the capability of existing measures to capture retical aspects. New York: Harper and Row.

CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE • V18 N1, MARCH 2011 56


Beck, A. T. (1970). Cognitive therapy: Nature and relation L. S. Greenberg (Eds.), The working alliance: Theory,
to behavior therapy. Behavior Therapy, 1, 184–200. research, and practice (pp. 13–37). Oxford: Wiley.
Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional Brosan, L., Reynolds, S., & Moore, R. G. (2007). Factors
disorders. New York: International Universities Press. associated with competence in cognitive therapists.
Beck, A. T. (1989). An interview with a depressed and suicidal Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 35, 179–190.
patient. In D. Wedding & R. J. Corsini (Eds.), Case studies Brosan, L., Reynolds, S., & Moore, R. G. (2008). Self eva-
in psychotherapy. (pp. 125–142). Itasca, IL: F. E. Peacock. luation of cognitive therapy performance: Do therapists
Beck, J. S. (1995). Cognitive therapy: Basics and beyond. New know how competent they are?. Behavioural and Cognitive
York: Guilford. Psychotherapy, 36, 581–587.
Beck, A. T., Emery, G., & Greenberg, R. L. (1985). Anxiety Clark, D. A., Beck, A. T., & Alford, B. A. (1999). Scientific
disorders and phobias: A cognitive perspective. New York: foundations of cognitive theory and therapy of depression. New
Basic Books. York: Wiley.
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Clemence, A., Hilsenroth, M., Ackerman, S., Strassle, C., &
Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford. Handler, L. (2005). Facets of the therapeutic alliance and
Bell, V., Grech, E., Maiden, C., Halligan, P. W., & Ellis, perceived progress in psychotherapy: Relationship
H. D. (2005). ‘Internet delusions’: A case series and theo- between patient and therapist perspectives. Clinical Psychol-
retical integration. Psychopathology, 38, 144–150. ogy and Psychotherapy, 12, 443–454.
Bennett-Levy, J. (2003). Mechanisms of change in cognitive Colson, D. D., Horwitz, L., Allen, J. G., Frieswyk, S. H.,
therapy: The case of automatic thought records and Gabbard, G. O., Newsom, G. E., et al. (1988). Patient
behavioral experiments. Behavioral and Cognitive Psychother- collaboration as a criterion for the therapeutic alliance.
apy, 31, 261–277. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 5, 259–268.
Bennett-Levy, J., Butler, G., Fennell, M., Hackmann, A., Connor-Greene, P. (1993). The therapeutic context: Precon-
Mueller, M., & Westbrook, D. (Eds.). (2004). Oxford guide ditions for change in psychotherapy. Psychotherapy: Theory,
to behavioral experiments in cognitive therapy. New York: Research, Practice, Training, 30, 375–382.
Oxford University Press. Creed, T. A., & Kendall, P. C. (2005). Therapist alliance-
Beyebach, M., & Carranza, V. E. (1997). Therapeutic inter- building behavior within a cognitive-behavioral treatment
action and dropout: Measuring relational communication for anxiety in youth. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy-
in solution-focused therapy. Journal of Family Therapy, 19, chology, 73, 498–505.
173–221. Crits-Christoph, P., Barber, J. P., & Kurcias, J. S. (1993).
Bieling, P. J., & Kuyken, W. (2003). Is cognitive case formu- The accuracy of therapists’ interpretations and the devel-
lation science or science fiction? Clinical Psychology: Science opment of the therapeutic alliance. Psychotherapy Research,
and Practice, 10, 52–69. 3, 25–35.
Bishop, W., & Fish, J. M. (1999). Questions as interventions: Crits-Christoph, P., Cooper, A., & Luborsky, L. (1988). The
Perceptions of Socratic, solution-focused, and diagnostic accuracy of therapists’ interpretations and the outcome of
questioning styles. Journal of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive dynamic psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Behavior Therapy, 17, 115–140. Psychology, 56, 490–495.
Blackburn, I.-M., James, I. A., Milne, D. L., Baker, C., Cross, L. W. (2007). Islands of health: Implications of aca-
Standart, S., Garland, A., et al. (2001). The revised demic psychology for psychotherapy technique and inte-
Cognitive Therapy Scale (CTS-R): Psychometric proper- gration. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 17, 251–273.
ties. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 29, 431–446. Dattilio, F. M. (2000). Cognitive-behavioral strategies. In J.
Boardman, T., Catley, D., Grobe, J., Little, T., & Ahluwalia, J. Carlson & L. Sperry (Eds.), Brief therapy with individuals
(2006). Using motivational interviewing with smokers: Do and couples (pp. 33–70). Phoenix, AZ: Zeig, Tucker and
therapist behaviors relate to engagement and therapeutic Theisen.
alliance? Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 31, 329–339. Dattilio, F. M., & Padesky, C. A. (1990). Cognitive therapy
Bordin, E. (1979). The generalizability of the psychoanalytic with couples. Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource
concept of the working alliance. Psychotherapy: Theory, Exchange.
Research & Practice, 16, 252–260. Deci, E. L., Eghrari, H., Patrick, B. C., & Leone, D. R.
Bordin, E. (1994). Theory and research on the therapeutic (1994). Facilitating internalization: The self-determination
working alliance: New directions. In A. O. Horvath & theory perspective. Journal of Personality, 62, 119–142.

DEFINING COLLABORATIVE EMPIRICISM • TEE & KAZANTZIS 57


Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1980). The empirical explora- others help children: Clinical supervision of child psychotherapy
tion of intrinsic motivational processes. In L. Berkowitz (pp. 109–122). Washington, DC: American Psychological
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 13, pp. Association.
39–80). New York: Academic Press. Friedberg, R. D., & Gorman, A. A. (2007). Integrating
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and psychotherapeutic processes with cognitive behavioral
self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum. procedures. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 37, 185–
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The ‘‘what’’ and the 193.
‘‘why’’ of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self- Friedberg, R. D., & McClure, J. M. (2002). Clinical practice of
determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227– cognitive therapy with children and adolescents. New York:
268. Guilford.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Facilitating optimal Gelso, C. J., & Hayes, J. A. (1998). The psychotherapy relation-
motivation and psychological well-being across life’s ship: Theory, research and practice. New York: Wiley.
domains. Canadian Psychology, 49, 14–23. Gleeson, J. F. M., & McGorry, P. D. (2004). Psychological
Derogatis, L. R. (1974). The Hopkins Symptom Checklist interventions in early psychosis. Chichester, UK: Wiley.
(HSCL). Behavioral Science, 19, 1–15. Goldstein, J. M., Cohen, P., Lewis, S. A., & Struening, E. L.
DeRubeis, R. J., Tang, T. Z., & Beck, A. T. (2001). Cogni- (1988). Community treatment environments: Patient vs.
tive therapy. In K. S. Dobson (Ed.), Handbook of cognitive staff evaluations. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 176,
behavioral therapies (2nd ed., pp. 349–392). New York: 227–233.
Guilford. Gordon, P. (2007). A comparison of two versions of the
Diamond, G. M., Liddle, H. A., Hogue, A., & Dakof, G. A. Cognitive Therapy Scale. Behavioural and Cognitive Psycho-
(1999). Alliance-building interventions with adolescents in therapy, 35, 343–353.
family therapy: A process study. Psychotherapy: Theory, Hatcher, R. (1999). Therapists’ views of treatment alliance
Research, Practice, Training, 36, 355–368. and collaboration in therapy. Psychotherapy Research, 9,
Dobson, K. S., & Christensen, A. (2001). Lessons from the 405–423.
conduct of clinical research trials. Behavior Therapist, 24, Hatcher, R., & Barends, A. (1996). Patients’ view of the
155–156. alliance in psychotherapy: Exploratory factor analysis of
Dobson, D. J. G., & Dobson, K. S. (2009). Evidence-based three alliance measures. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
practice of cognitive-behavioral therapy. New York: Guilford. Psychology, 64, 1326–1336.
Dobson, K. S., & Shaw, B. F. (1988). The use of treatment Hatcher, R., & Gillaspy, J. (2006). Development and valida-
manuals in cognitive therapy: Experience and issues. Jour- tion of a revised short version of the Working Alliance
nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 673–680. Inventory. Psychotherapy Research, 16, 12–25.
Dormaar, J., Dijkman, C. I., & de Vries, M. W. (1989). Hazlett-Stevens, H. (2008). Psychological approaches to general-
Consensus in patient-therapist interactions: A measure of ized anxiety disorder. New York: Springer.
the therapeutic relationship related to outcome. Psychother- Hollon, S. D. (1999). Rapid early response in cognitive
apy and Psychosomatics, 51, 69–76. behavior therapy: A commentary. Clinical Psychology: Sci-
Dudley, R., & Kuyken, W. (2006). Formulation in cognitive- ence and Practice, 6, 305–309.
behavioral therapy: ‘There is nothing either good or bad, Hollon, S. D., & Beck, A. T. (1979). Cognitive therapy
but thinking makes it so.’ In L. Johnstone & R. Dallos of depression. In P. C. Kendall & S. D. Hollon (Eds.),
(Eds.), Formulation in psychology and psychotherapy: Making Cognitive-behavioral interventions: Theory, research, and proce-
sense of people’s problems (pp. 17–46). New York: Routledge. dures (pp. 153–203). New York: Academic Press.
Duehn, W. D., & Proctor, E. K. (1974). No exit: Interac- Hollon, S. D., & Kriss, M. R. (1984). Cognitive factors in clini-
tional control of premature discontinuance. American Jour- cal research and practice. Clinical Psychology Review, 4, 35–76.
nal of Orthopsychiatry, 44, 201–202. Horvath, A. O. (2001). The alliance. Psychotherapy: Theory,
Eisenthal, S., Koopman, C., & Lazare, A. (1983). Process Research, Practice, Training, 38, 365–372.
analysis of two dimensions of the negotiated approach in Horvath, A. O., & Greenberg, L. S. (1989). Development
relation to satisfaction in the initial interview. Journal of and validation of the Working Alliance Inventory. Journal
Nervous and Mental Disease, 171, 49–54. of Counseling Psychology, 36, 223–233.
Friedberg, R. D., & Clark, C. C. (2006). Supervision of cog- Horvath, A. O., & Greenberg, L. S. (1994). The working
nitive therapy with youth. In T. K. Neill (Ed.), Helping alliance: Theory, research, and practice. New York: Wiley.

CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE • V18 N1, MARCH 2011 58


Horvath, A. O., & Luborsky, L. (1993). The role of the ther- Le Bloc’h, Y., de Roten, Y., Drapeau, M., & Despland, J.
apeutic alliance in psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and (2006). New, but improved? Comparison between first
Clinical Psychology, 61, 561–573. and revised version of the Helping Alliance Questionnaire.
Horvath, A. O., & Symonds, B. (1991). Relation between Schweizer Archiv fur Neurologie und Psychiatrie, 157, 23–28.
working alliance and outcome in psychotherapy: A meta- Luborsky, L., & Crits-Christoph, P. (1998). Understanding
analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38, 139–149. transference: The core conflictual relationship theme method (2nd
Hoyt, M. F. (1980). Therapist and patient actions in ‘‘good’’ ed.). New York: Basic Books.
psychotherapy sessions. Archives of General Psychiatry, 37, Madsen, W. M. (2009). Collaborative helping: A practice
159–161. framework for family-centered services. Family Process, 48,
Hoyt, M. F., Xenakis, S. N., Marmar, C. R., & Horowitz, 103–116.
M. J. (1983). Therapists’ actions that influence their per- Markland, D., Ryan, R. M., Tobin, V. J., & Rollnick, S.
ceptions of ‘‘good’’ psychotherapy sessions. Journal of Ner- (2005). Motivational interviewing and self-determination
vous and Mental Disease, 171, 400–404. theory. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 24, 811–831.
Jacobson, N. S., & Gortner, E. T. (2000). Can depression be Martin, D. J., Garske, J. P., & Davis, M. (2000). Relation of
de-medicalized in the 21st century: Scientific revolutions, the therapeutic alliance with outcome and other variables:
counter-revolutions and the magnetic field of normal sci- A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
ence. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 103–117. Psychology, 68, 438–450.
Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and Mata, J., Silva, M. N., Vieira, P. N., Carraça, E., Andrade,
choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, A. M., Coutinho, S. R., et al. (2009). Motivational ‘‘spill-
58, 697–720. over’’ during weight control: Increased self-determination
Kazantzis, N. (2003). Therapist competence in cognitive- and exercise intrinsic motivation predict eating self-regula-
behavioural therapies: Review of the contemporary tion. Health Psychology, 28, 709–716.
empirical evidence. Behaviour Change, 20, 1–12. Merali, N., & Lynch, P. (1997). Collaboration in cognitive-
Kazantzis, N., MacEwan, J., & Dattilio, F. M. (2005). A behavioral counselling: A case example. Canadian Journal of
guiding model for practice. In N. Kazantzis, F. P. Deane., Counselling, 31, 287–293.
K. R. Ronan, & L. L’Abate (Eds.), Using homework assign- Michalak, J., Klappheck, M. A., & Kosfelder, J. (2004). Per-
ments in cognitive behavior therapy (pp. 359–407). New sonal goals of psychotherapy patients: The intensity and
York: Routledge. the ‘‘why’’ of goal-motivated behavior and their implica-
Kazantzis, N., Reinecke, M. A., & Freeman, A. (2010). tions for the therapeutic process. Psychotherapy Research,
Concluding remarks. In N. Kazantzis, M. A. Reinecke, & 14, 193–209.
A. Freeman (Eds.), Cognitive and behavioral theories in clinical Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (2002). Motivational interview-
practice (pp. 363–367). New York: Guilford. ing: Preparing people to change. New York: Guilford.
Kazantzis, N., Whittington, C. J., & Dattilio, F. M. (2010). Moller, A. C., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2006).
Meta-analysis of homework effects in cognitive and Choice and ego-depletion: The moderating role of
behavioral therapy: A replication and extension. Clinical autonomy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32,
Psychology: Science and Practice, 17, 144–156. 1024–1036.
Klosko, J. S., & Sanderson, W. C. (1999). Depression: Clinical O’Malley, S. S., Suh, C. S., & Strupp, H. H. (1983). The
application of empirically supported psychotherapy. Northvale, Vanderbilt Psychotherapy Process Scale: A report on the
NJ: Jason Aronson. scale development and a process-outcome study. Journal of
Koestner, R., Ryan, R. M., Bernieri, F., & Holt, K. (1984). Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 581–586.
Setting limits on children’s behavior: The differential effects van Oppen, P. (2004). Cognitive therapy for obsessive-
of controlling versus informational styles on intrinsic moti- compulsive disorder. Clinical Case Studies, 3, 333–349.
vation and creativity. Journal of Personality, 52, 233–248. Overholser, J. C. (1995). Elements of the Socratic method:
Kuyken, W., Padesky, C. A., & Dudley, R. (2008). The sci- iV. Disavowal of knowledge. Psychotherapy: Theory,
ence and practice of case conceptualization. Behavioral and Research, Practice, Training, 32, 283–292.
Cognitive Psychotherapy, 36, 757–768. Padesky, C. A., & Greenberger, D. (1995). Clinician’s guide to
Kuyken, W., Padesky, C. A., & Dudley, R. (2009). Collabo- mind over mood. New York: Guilford.
rative case conceptualization: Working effectively with patients in Pelletier, L. G., Tuson, K. M., & Haddad, N. K. (1997).
cognitive-behavioral therapy. New York: Guilford. Client Motivation for Therapy Scale: A measure of

DEFINING COLLABORATIVE EMPIRICISM • TEE & KAZANTZIS 59


intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, and amotivation Stein, D. J., Kupfer, D. J., & Schatzberg, A. F. (Eds.). (2006).
for therapy. Journal of Personality Assessment, 68, 414–435. The American Psychiatric Publishing textbook of mood disorders.
Persons, J. B. (2008). The case formulation approach to cognitive- Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.
behavior therapy. New York: Guilford. Tracey, T. J. (1986). Interactional correlates of premature ter-
Reeve, J. (1998). Autonomy support as an interpersonal mination. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54,
motivating style: Is it teachable? Contemporary Educational 784–788.
Psychology, 23, 312–330. Tracy, J. J. (1977). Impact of intake procedures upon patient
Reeve, J. (2002). Self-determination theory applied to educa- attrition in a community mental health center. Journal of
tional settings. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Hand- Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 45, 192–195.
book of self-determination research (pp. 193–204). Rochester, Trepka, C., Rees, A., Shapiro, D. A., Hardy, G. E., & Bark-
NY: University of Rochester Press. ham, M. (2004). Therapist competence and outcome of
Rutter, J. G., & Friedberg, R. D. (1999). Guidelines for the cognitive therapy for depression. Cognitive Therapy and
effective use of Socratic dialogue in cognitive therapy. In Research, 28, 143–157.
L. VandeCreek & T. L. Jackson (Eds.), Innovations in clini- Tryon, W. W., & Misurell, J. R. (2008). Dissonance induc-
cal practice: A source book (pp. 481–490). Sarasota, FL: tion and reduction: A possible principle and connectionist
Professional Resource Exchange. mechanism for why therapies are effective. Clinical Psychol-
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory ogy Review, 28, 1297–1309.
and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social develop- Tryon, G. S., & Winograd, G. (2002). Goal consensus and
ment, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78. collaboration. In J. C. Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy rela-
Ryan, R. M., & Deci E. L. (2002). An overview of self- tionships that work: Therapist contributions and responsiveness to
determination theory: An organismic-dialectical perspec- patients (pp. 109–125). New York: Oxford University
tive. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self- Press.
determination research (pp. 3–36). Rochester, NY: Turkington, D., & Siddle, R. (1998). Cognitive therapy for
University of Rochester Press. the treatment of delusions. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment,
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2008). A self-determination 4, 235–241.
theory approach to psychotherapy: The motivational basis Vansteenkiste, M., & Sheldon, K. M. (2006). There’s nothing
for effective change. Canadian Psychology, 49, 186–193. more practical than a good theory: Integrating motiva-
Ryan, R. M., Rigby, S., & King, K. (1993). Two types of tional interviewing and self-determination theory. British
religious internalization and their relations to religious Journal of Clinical Psychology, 45, 63–82.
orientations and mental health. Journal of Personality and Waddington, L. (2002). The therapy relationship in cognitive
Social Psychology, 65, 586–596. therapy: A review. Behavioral and Cognitive Psychotherapy,
Schmidt, N. B., & Woolaway-Bickel, K. (2000). The effects 30, 179–192.
of treatment compliance on outcome in cognitive-behav- Weinstein, N., & Ryan, R. M. (2010). When helping helps:
ioral therapy for panic disorder: Quality versus quantity. Autonomous motivation for prosocial behavior and its
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 13–18. influence on well-being for the helper and recipient.
Segal, Z. V., & Shaw, B. F. (1996). Cognitive therapy. Amer- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 222–244.
ican Psychiatric Press Review of Psychiatry, 15, 69–90. Westefeld, J. S., Range, L. M., Rogers, J. R., Maples,
Shaw, B. F., Elkin, I., Yamaguchi, J., Olmsted, M., Vallis, M. R., Bromley, J. L., & Alcorn, J. (2000). Suicide: An
T., Dobson, K. S., et al. (1999). Therapist competence overview. The Counseling Psychologist, 28, 445–510.
ratings in relation to clinical outcome in cognitive therapy Wilkes, T. (1994). Cognitive therapy for depressed adolescents.
of depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, New York: Guilford.
67, 837–846. Will, T. E. (1995). Cognitive therapy and the MCMI-III. In
Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (1995). Coherence and con- P. D. Retzlaff (Ed.), Tactical psychotherapy of the personality
gruence: Two aspects of personality integration. Journal of disorders: An MCMI-III-based approach (pp. 90–110). Need-
Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 531–543. ham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Shirk, S., & Karver, M. (2003). Prediction of treatment out- Williams, G. C., Grow, V. M., Freedman, Z. R., Ryan,
come from relationship variables in child and adolescent R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1996). Motivational predictors of
therapy: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and weight loss and weight-loss maintenance. Journal of Person-
Clinical Psychology, 71, 452–464. ality and Social Psychology, 70, 115–126.

CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENCE AND PRACTICE • V18 N1, MARCH 2011 60


Windholz, M. J., & Silberschatz, G. (1988). Vanderbilt Psycho- methadone maintenance treatment. Journal of Social and
therapy Process Scale: A replication with adult outpatients. Clinical Psychology, 23, 675–696.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 56–60. Zuroff, D. C., Koestner, R., Moskowitz, D. S., McBride,
Young, J. E., & Beck, A. T. (1980). Cognitive Therapy Scale: C., Marshall, M., & Bagby, M. R. (2007). Autonomous
Rating manual. Unpublished manuscript, University of motivation for therapy: A new common factor in brief
Pennsylvania. treatments for depression. Psychotherapy Research, 17, 137–
Young, J. E., & Beck, A. T. (1988). Cognitive Therapy Scale. 147.
Unpublished manuscript, University of Pennsylvania.
Zeldman, A., Ryan, R. M., & Fiscella, K. (2004). Motiva- Received December 13, 2009; revised May 16, 2010;
tion, autonomy support, and entity beliefs: Their role in accepted May 25, 2010.

DEFINING COLLABORATIVE EMPIRICISM • TEE & KAZANTZIS 61

View publication stats

You might also like