Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Psychology of Sport and Exercise 12 (2011) 655e661

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Psychology of Sport and Exercise


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychsport

Passion and burnout in elite junior soccer players: The mediating role
of self-determined motivation
Thomas Curran a, *, Paul R. Appleton b, Andrew P. Hill a, Howard K. Hall a
a
York St John University, UK
b
University of Birmingham, UK

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Objectives: The purpose of the study was to examine the relationship between forms of passion
Received 15 September 2010 (harmonious and obsessive; Vallerand et al., 2003) and athlete burnout, and whether these relationships
Received in revised form are mediated by self-determined motivation. The proposed model posited that because harmonious
10 June 2011
passion originates from an authentic self, it will be positively associated with self-determined regulation.
Accepted 16 June 2011
Available online 25 June 2011
Conversely, because obsessive passion originates from ego-invested structures within the self, it will be
negatively associated with self-determined regulation. In turn, consistent with research examining the
relationship between motivation regulation and athlete burnout, self-determined regulation was
Keywords:
Harmonious passion
expected to be negatively associated with athlete burnout.
Obsessive passion Design: Cross-sectional survey.
Self-determination theory Method: This model was tested in 149 (M age ¼ 16.2, s ¼ 2.0, range ¼ 12e21) male junior athletes who
Behavioural regulation attended soccer academies in the UK. Participants completed the Passion Scale (Vallerand et al., 2003),
Sport the Sport Motivation Scale (Pelletier et al., 1995), and the Athlete Burnout Questionnaire (Raedeke &
Smith, 2001).
Results: Harmonious passion was negatively related to a latent athlete burnout factor, whereas obsessive
passion was unrelated to a latent athlete burnout factor. The relationship between harmonious passion
and burnout was fully mediated by self-determined regulation.
Conclusions: The results suggest that harmonious passion may offer some protection from burnout for
athletes due to higher levels of self-determined motivation.
Ó 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Burnout is a colloquial term that is frequently used in sport and only recently begun to examine the psychological mechanisms
by representatives of the media in their reporting (e.g. Dall, 2009). associated with athlete burnout. Consequently, research is required
Burnout is typically understood as the result of physical over- that examines the processes associated with burnout in elite junior
exertion and fatigue (see Brenner, 2007). However, contemporary athletes.
perspectives within the scientific community suggest the ante- One of the reasons why athlete burnout has received little
cedents of athlete burnout may be predominantly psychological systematic attention is that early research lacked a precise defini-
(Gould, Udry, Tuffey, & Loehr, 1996; Raedeke & Smith, 2001). Owing tion of the syndrome (e.g. Gould et al., 1996; Harlick & Mckenzie,
to the intense psychological demands placed upon developing 2000). Raedeke and Smith (2001, 2004) addressed this ambiguity
athletes, a number of researchers have proposed that elite junior by providing a definition and measure based on Maslach and
athletes may be especially susceptible to the development of Jackson’s (1986) research in an occupational context. Raedeke and
burnout (e.g., Appleton, Hall, & Hill, 2009; Gustafsson, Kentta, Smith (2001) consider athlete burnout to be an experiential
Hassmen, & Johansson, 2008; Hill, Hall, Appleton, & Kozub, 2008). syndrome consisting of three central symptoms; perceived physical
In spite of this, relatively little is known about the development of and emotional exhaustion, a reduced sense of athletic accom-
the syndrome in this group. This is because empirical research has plishment and sport devaluation. One important characteristic of
these symptoms is a lack of motivational quantity and quality (see
Eklund & Cresswell, 2007). In line with the predictions of self-
* Corresponding author. De Grey 110, Faculty of Health and Life Sciences, York St
determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985), athlete burnout has
John University, Lord Mayors Walk, York YO31 7EX, UK. Tel.: þ44 1904876239. been suggested to develop when motivation becomes increasingly
E-mail address: t.curran@yorksj.ac.uk (T. Curran). non-self-determined (lack of motivational quality) and amotivated

1469-0292/$ e see front matter Ó 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.06.004
656 T. Curran et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 12 (2011) 655e661

(lack of motivational quantity) (Eklund & Cresswell, 2007; Lemyre, for external reasons, behaviour is regulated out of choice. Introjected
Treasure, & Roberts, 2006). One important motivational antecedent and external regulations are controlled forms of extrinsic motivation
of this process may be the form of passion that energises partici- because behaviour is fuelled by pressures that emanate from internal
pation (Carbonneau, Vallerand, Fernet, & Guay, 2008; Vallerand, (i.e. ego enhancement) or external (i.e. rewards) contingencies. The
Paquet, Philippe, & Charest, 2010). final motivation on the self-determination continuum is amotivation,
which is an absence of motivational quantity. Within this framework,
Passion as an antecedent to athlete burnout the defining motivational features of athlete burnout are thought to
be lower levels of intrinsic motivation and higher levels of controlled
Passion is defined as a strong inclination towards an activity that and amotivation (Eklund & Cresswell, 2007). In accord, research
one enjoys, finds important and in which individuals invest time has found that higher athlete burnout is associated with less self-
and energy (Vallerand et al., 2003). Although passion ensures determined regulation and lower scores on the self-determination
willing participation, it has the potential to underpin both positive index (Lemyre et al., 2006; Lonsdale et al., 2009).
and negative outcomes depending on the type of passion exhibited. As behavioural regulation does not internalise into identity, it
Vallerand et al. (2003) propose that individuals internalise activi- resides at a more immediate level of engagement than passion
ties in two ways and the type of internalisation determines the (Vallerand, 2008). As a consequence, passion may influence the
passion that develops. Solely autonomous internalisation of an contextual-motivational processes that are central to understanding
activity is thought to result in harmonious passion. In contrast, athlete burnout (see Vallerand et al., 2006). Harmonious passion
partial or controlled internalisation leads to the development of emerges from the same autonomous internalisation as intrinsic
obsessive passion (Vallerand, 2008). While harmonious passion motivation and is indicative of a pattern of more self-determined
leads to positive outcomes such as eagerness, flow and post- regulation (c.f. Vallerand, 2008). In contrast, obsessive passion
participation replenishment (Hodgins & Knee, 2002), obsessive emerges from more controlled internalisation and features the ego-
passion is thought to lead to ego-investment, heightened self- investment and introjected affect indicative of less self-determined
awareness, and rigid task engagement (see Vallerand, 2008). regulation (Hodgins & Knee, 2002; Niemiec, Ryan, & Brown, 2008).
Differences between these forms of passion have also emerged in Clearly, the pattern of motivational regulation associated with
terms of their relationship with burnout. Specifically, obsessive harmonious passion is reflective of a motivational resiliency against
passion has been found to be positively related to burnout and burnout symptoms, whereas the pattern of motivational regulation
harmonious passion negatively related to burnout in occupational associated with obsessive passion is more consistent with the nature
settings (Carbonneau et al., 2008; Vallerand et al., 2010). of athlete burnout. Consequently, the degree to which obsessive and
Research has yet to examine the relationship between passion harmonious passion encapsulate self-determined motivation may
and athlete burnout. However, research in sport has supported the explain the relationship between passion and athlete burnout.
contention that harmonious and obsessive passions are likely to
have distinct motivational consequences (Vallerand et al., 2003, The present study
Vallerand, Rousseau, Grouzet, Dumais, & Grenier, 2006; Vallerand
et al., 2008). It is especially noteworthy that obsessive passion is To date, no research has examined the relationships between
associated with a number of psychological costs that are unrelated forms of passion and burnout in sport. Therefore, the first purpose
to harmonious passion. Obsessive passion, for example, has been of the current study was to examine the relationships between
found to be positively related to rigid persistence (Vallerand et al., harmonious and obsessive passion and athlete burnout. Consistent
2003), life conflict (Seguin-Lévesque, Laliberté, Pelletier, with research in the occupational domain (Carbonneau et al., 2008;
Blanchard, & Vallerand, 2003), rumination in the absence of the Vallerand et al., 2010), it was hypothesised that harmonious
activity (Ratelle, Vallerand, Mageau, Rousseau, & Provencher, 2004), passion would be negatively associated with athlete burnout and
chronic injury (Rip, Fortin, & Vallerand, 2006), negative affect obsessive passion would be positively associated with athlete
(Vallerand et al., 2003) and avoidance goals (Vallerand et al., 2008). burnout. The second purpose was to test whether the relationships
In contrast, harmonious passion is inversely related to negative between different forms of passion and athlete burnout would be
affect and cognition and positively related to life satisfaction, posi- mediated by self-determined motivation. The hypothesised rela-
tive affect and vitality (see Vallerand, 2008). Based on these find- tionships within the model are displayed in Fig. 1.
ings, one might expect types of passion to predict athlete burnout in
a similar manner to that which has been observed outside of sport. Method

The mediating role of self-determined motivation Participants and procedure

According to Eklund and Cresswell (2007), burnout can be char- One hundred and forty-nine (M age ¼ 16.2, s ¼ 2.0, range ¼
acterised by a distinct pattern of motivational regulation described 12e21) young male elite soccer players, playing for an average of
within self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985). Self-
determination theory conceptualises motivational regulation along
a continuum from self-determined (intrinsic) to non-self-determined HP RA
(extrinsic) to amotivation (Vallerand, 1997). This continuum is often
represented by a self-determination index that considers the relative
SDI ABO EX
contribution of the discrete motivational regulations within the
theory (e.g. Gillet, Vallerand, Amoura, & Baldes, 2010; Lonsdale,
OP D
Hodge, & Rose, 2009; Taylor, Ntoumanis, & Standage, 2008).
Intrinsic motivation is behavioural regulation that is fully self-
determined. Extrinsic motivation, on the other hand, consists of Fig. 1. Hypothesised path model of passion, self-determined motivation and athlete
burnout. Note; dashed lines indicate a hypothesised negative relationship; un-dashed
several forms of behavioural regulation, which differ in the degree of lines indicate a positive relationship; HP ¼ harmonious passion, OP ¼ obsessive
self-determination. Integrated and identified regulations are auton- passion, SDI ¼ self-determination index, ABO ¼ athlete burnout, RA ¼ reduced accom-
omous forms of extrinsic motivation because, though engagement is plishment, EX ¼ exhaustion, D ¼ devaluation.
T. Curran et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 12 (2011) 655e661 657

9.1 (s ¼ 2.9) years comprised the sample of this study. Prior to data scale consists of 15 items that assess three symptoms of burnout;
collection, ethical approval was provided by the research ethics sport devaluation (e.g. “I have negative feelings towards soccer”),
committee of a British University. Soccer clubs who took part in the reduced sense of accomplishment (e.g. “I am not achieving much in
study were then contacted to discuss the project and its objectives. soccer”) and emotional/physical exhaustion (e.g. “I am exhausted
Participants, or parents or guardians when applicable, were by the mental and physical demands of soccer”). Athletes respond
required to complete informed consent forms prior to participation according to the degree they experience each item over the last
in the study. Data collection took place within a classroom or month on a 5-point Likert scale (1 ¼ “almost never”, 5 ¼ “almost
training session setting in the presence of the lead author. The always”). Raedeke and Smith (2001) have provided initial support
questionnaire required approximately 20 minutes to complete. for the reliability of the instrument (a > .86) and studies have since
confirmed its utility as a valid and reliable measure of athlete
Instruments burnout (e.g. Cresswell & Eklund, 2005a, 2005b, 2005c; Lonsdale
et al., 2009).
Passion for sport
A 10 item version of the Passion Scale (adapted for soccer; Analytical strategy
Vallerand et al., 2003, study 4), with five items measuring harmo-
nious (HP; e.g. “soccer allows me to live a variety of experiences”) and Path analysis, with maximum likelihood estimation, was used to
five items measuring obsessive (OP; e.g. “I cannot live without test the hypothesised model. Two exogenous variables (obsessive
soccer”) passion was employed to measure athletes’ passion for and harmonious passion) and the mediator variable (self-deter-
soccer. Athletes responded to each item on a 7-point Likert scale mined motivation) were represented using measured variables. The
(1 ¼ “Do not agree at all”, 7 ¼ “Very strongly agree”). This passion endogenous variable, athlete burnout, was represented as a latent
scale has been widely used and proved valid and reliable in various factor with its symptoms as manifest variables. This approach is
domains including physical activity (e.g. Mageau, Vallerand, akin to past research that has attempted to examine athlete
Rousseau, Ratelle, & Provencher, 2005; Rousseau & Vallerand, burnout as a syndrome (e.g., Hill et al., 2008; Hill, Hall, Appleton, &
2008). Rousseau and Vallerand (2008), for example, found this Murray, 2010). A limitation of employing this mixed model
version of the passion scale to be highly reliable in a sample of older approach is that no error is modelled for the manifest variables.
adult exercisers (a ¼ .88 & .84 for harmonious and obsessive passion). However, this method was considered the most suitable because of
the relatively small sample size and the necessity of a minimum
Self-determined motivation case-to-parameter ratio for coefficient stability (5:1; Kline, 1998).
To assess athletes’ motivation in sport, the Sport Motivation Conventional criteria were used to assess the fit of the hypoth-
Scale (SMS; Pelletier et al., 1995) was employed. This inventory esised model with the observed data. Adequate fit was inferred
consists of 28 items assessing 7 subscales (4 items per subscale); when TLI and CFI > .90, SRMR & RMSEA < .10, c2/df < 3 (Hu &
intrinsic motivation to know (e.g. “I participate in soccer for the Bentler, 1995), whereas Hu and Bentler’s (1999) criteria were
pleasure it gives me to know more about my sport”), experience employed as evidence of good fit: TLI and CFI > .95, RMSEA < .06,
stimulation (e.g. “I participate in soccer for the pleasure I feel in SRMR < .08. The causal steps approach (see Holmbeck, 1997) was
living exciting experiences”), and accomplish (“I participate in used to compare full and partial mediation. In a separate analysis,
soccer because I feel a lot of personal satisfaction while mastering indirect effects with bias corrected bootstrapped (1000 resamples)
certain difficult training techniques”), identified regulation (e.g. “I confidence intervals were requested to assess the effect size and
participate in soccer because it is one of the best ways to maintain statistical significance of any mediation evidenced (Mackinnon,
good relationships with my friends”), introjected regulation (e.g. “I Lockwood, & Williams, 2004; Shrout & Bolger, 2002).
participate in soccer because it is absolutely necessary if one wants
to be in shape”), external regulation (e.g. “I participate in soccer Results
because I must do soccer to feel good about myself”) and amoti-
vation (e.g. “I participate in soccer but it’s not clear anymore; I don’t Preliminary analysis
really think my place is in soccer”). Athletes respond to how each
item corresponds to reasons they are presently practicing soccer on No missing cases or significant univariate outliers (p < .001;
a 7-point Likert scale (1 ¼ “does not correspond at all”, Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001) were observed in the data. However,
7 ¼ “corresponds exactly”). Pelletier et al. (1995) demonstrated multivariate non-normality was identified (Mardia’s normalised
initial reliability of the SMS (mean a ¼ .75) for all subscales and in coefficient ¼ 5.90; Kline, 1998). Since the data was multivariate
a thorough review, Pelletier and Sarrazin (2007) found the SMS to asymmetrical, in addition to the maximum likelihood estimation
be valid and reliable measure of sport motivation. method, path analysis was conducted using a bootstrap procedure
Following procedures outlined by Vallerand (2001), a self- consisting of 200 replication samples to estimate model fit and
determination index was calculated to reflect the athletes’ assess parameter stability (Efron & Tibshirani, 1998). Reported
perceptions of self-determined motivation. This index is obtained estimates are those derived from mean scores across the 200 iter-
by multiplying each subscale by an assigned weight in accordance ations. Reliability analysis (Cronbach’s a) revealed that the
with its location on the self-determination continuum. When measurement associated with the instruments used had adequate
calculating the self-determination index in the absence of inte- internal consistency (see table 2) (Loewenthal, 1996).
grated regulation, Vallerand (2001) recommends weights of 2 for
intrinsic motivation, 1 for identified regulation, 1 for introjected Descriptive analyses, bivariate and partial correlations
and external regulation, and 2 for amotivation. The product scores
were then summed to form an index of self-determination. The Using cut-off criteria for low and high experiences of burnout
higher the score, the more self-determined the motivation. (Raedeke, 1997; adapted by Hodge, Lonsdale, & Ng, 2008),
a frequency distribution revealed that fifty two athletes (35%)
Athlete burnout scored 2.30 or below for reduced accomplishment, 2.30 or below
To assess athlete burnout, the Athlete Burnout Questionnaire for emotional exhaustion, and 1.60 or below for devaluation (i.e.
(ABQ; Raedeke & Smith, 2001) was used. This multidimensional low burnout). Eight participants (5.4%) scored higher than 2.70 for
658 T. Curran et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 12 (2011) 655e661

reduced accomplishment, higher than 3.00 for emotional exhaus- burnout variable, respectively. Results indicated that this alterna-
tion and higher than 3.00 for devaluation on all burnout subscales tive model did not fit the data adequately (c2 ¼ 167.20 [8], p < .05;
(i.e. high burnout). While the majority of the sample scored low-to- c2/df ¼ 20.90; TLI ¼ .15; CFI ¼ .55; SRMR ¼ .22; RMSEA ¼ .38, 90%
moderate on burnout subscales, this is consistent with previous CI ¼ .32e.42).
research utilising similar elite junior samples (Appleton et al., 2009;
Hill et al., 2008) and elite adult samples (Cresswell & Eklund,
2005c; Hodge et al., 2008; Lonsdale et al., 2009). Furthermore, Assessment of mediation
the standard deviations associated with athlete burnout symptoms
indicate that there is sufficient variability in the data for the Mediation was assessed using a causal steps procedure
examination of relationships between study variables. (Holmbeck, 1997). The first step requires an adequate fit of a model
In line with previous research using the passion scale (e.g., that includes only direct paths between the predictor variables and
Vallerand et al., 2006), harmonious and obsessive passion were the outcome variable (M1). The second step requires that the full
highly positively correlated (r ¼ .73, p < .01). As a result, bivariate meditation model (no direct paths from passion to athlete burnout,
and partial correlations were calculated (Table 1). Results indicate only indirect paths via self-determined motivation; M2) provides
that, after controlling for harmonious passion, obsessive passion an adequate fit. All paths must be statistically significant. The final
was unrelated to the self-determination index and all symptoms of step requires a fit comparison between the full meditation model
athlete burnout. Having controlled for obsessive passion, harmo- (M2) and partial mediation model (direct paths from passion to
nious passion was positively associated with the self-determination athlete burnout entered in addition to indirect paths via self-
index and inversely associated with reduced accomplishment. determined motivation; M3). If there is no significant improve-
Bivariate correlations indicated that the self-determination index ment in fit between the full and partial mediation models following
was inversely associated with all symptoms of athlete burnout. the additional of these pathways, full mediation is demonstrated. If
the paths between the predictor and the outcome variables are
Path analysis reduced in M3 compared to M1 but still significant, partial medi-
ation is inferred.
Results from the path analysis are presented in Fig. 2. Fit indexes CFI and SRMR fit indices indicated that the model in step one
suggested the hypothesised model possessed an adequate fit to the (M1) demonstrated adequate fit (see Table 2). The path between
data (c2 ¼ 19.28 [8], p < .05; c2/df ¼ 2.41; TLI ¼ .94; CFI ¼ .97; obsessive passion and burnout was non-significant (g ¼ .07,
SRMR ¼ .06; RMSEA ¼ .10, 90% CI ¼ .04e.15) despite a high RMSEA p > .05), whereas the path between harmonious passion and
(a possible consequence of the combined influence of a small athlete burnout was significant (g ¼ .26, p < .01). Thus, only
degrees of freedom and sample size; Hu & Bentler, 1999). The path mediation with respect to harmonious passion and athlete burnout
coefficient between harmonious passion and self-determined is possible (Baron & Kenny, 1986). The full mediation model (M2)
motivation was statistically significant (g ¼ .43, p < .01). In demonstrated adequate fit and thus step two was met. With the
contrast, the path coefficient between obsessive passion and self- exception of the paths between obsessive passion and outcome
determined motivation was non-significant (g ¼ .04, p > .05). variables, all paths were significant in the hypothesised directions.
Finally, the path coefficient between self-determined motivation In the final step, the full mediation model (M2) was compared with
and the latent burnout factor was significant (b ¼ .64, p < .01). The the partial mediation model (M3). A chi-square difference test
model accounted for 18% of the variance in self-determined moti- revealed that the two models do not differ in their degree of fit
vation and 43% of the variance in the latent athlete burnout factor. (D1.23 (2), p > .05). Moreover, the direct effect between harmo-
Due to the cross-sectional design and the potential for multiple nious passion and athlete burnout was not significant in the partial
models to provide adequate fit for the data, a theoretically viable mediation model (M3) (b ¼ .01, p > .05). As a result, the analysis
alternative model was also tested. According to the dualistic model, supports the full mediation model with respect to harmonious
passions are assimilated in accord with the amount of autonomy passion (Holmbeck, 1997).
the environment provides (Vallerand et al., 2003). The same In a separate analysis, the effect size and statistical significance
process is thought to underpin contextual behavioural regulation of the indirect effect of harmonious passion and athlete burnout via
(Deci & Ryan, 1985) and thus it is possible that self-determined self-determined motivation was determined by examining 95% bias
motivation may precede passion. Based on this theoretical argu- corrected confidence intervals derived from 1000 bootstrapped
ment, an alternative model was tested in which self-determined resamples (Efron, 1988; Mackinnon et al., 2004). The bias corrected
motivation was the measured exogenous variable and was confidence intervals for the indirect effect of harmonious passion
assumed to be a positive and negative predictor of the measured on athlete burnout via self-determined motivation excluded zero
mediating variables; harmonious and obsessive passion respec- (ab ¼ .17, 95% BC CI ¼ .28 to .08). Therefore, the relationship
tively. Harmonious and obsessive passions were, in turn, assumed between harmonious passion and athlete burnout is fully mediated
to inversely and positively predict the latent endogenous athlete and indirect via self-determined motivation.

Table 1
Descriptive statistics and bivariate and partial correlation coefficients between passion, the self-determination index and symptoms of athlete burnout.

1 2 3 4 5 6 M (s) a
1. Harmonious passion e .32** .19* .10 .12 5.48 (.90) .69
2. Obsessive passion .73** e .02 .04 .06 .12 5.04 (1.2) .82
3. Self-determination index .42** .29** e 5.84 (3.5) e
4. Burnout e reduced accomplishment .31** .25** .54** e 2.15 (.67) .73
5. Burnout e exhaustion .08 .02 .39** .59** e 2.46 (.86) .87
6. Burnout e devaluation .27** .29** .54** .60** .57** e 1.63 (.73) .81

Note. Partial correlations appear above the diagonal and bivariate correlations appear below the diagonal. No Cronbach alpha is reported for the self-determination index
because this is a weighted composite score. The individual Cronbach alpha’s for discrete behavioural regulations were as follows; intrinsic motivation ¼ .89; identified
regulation ¼ .81; introjected regulation ¼ .72; external regulation ¼ .62; amotivation ¼ .82. M ¼ mean, s ¼ standard deviation; a ¼ Cronbach alpha. **p < .01, *p < .05.
T. Curran et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 12 (2011) 655e661 659

d1
d2
HP .43** .80** RA e1
.43
.18
-.65** .71**
.73** SDI ABO EX e2
-.04 .78**

OP D e3

Fig. 2. Results of path analysis: The mediating role of self-determined motivation on the relationship between passion and athlete burnout. Note; HP ¼ harmonious passion,
OP ¼ obsessive passion, SDI ¼ self-determination index, ABO ¼ athlete burnout, RA ¼ reduced accomplishment, EX ¼ exhaustion, D ¼ devaluation, e ¼ error term, d ¼ disturbance
term, **p < .01.

Discussion passion does not seem to actively mitigate the athlete burnout
syndrome. In accord with the findings of previous research, it
The current study had two purposes. The first was to examine appears that not all forms of passion for sport are adaptive (see
the relationship between passion and athlete burnout. It was Vallerand & Miquelon, 2007).
hypothesised that harmonious passion would be negatively asso- Although it is not clear why obsessive passion did not positively
ciated with athlete burnout, whereas obsessive passion would be associate with athlete burnout, it is possible that contextual
positively associated with athlete burnout. The second purpose was differences in the conceptualisation of burnout may, in part,
to examine whether self-determined motivation mediates the account for this finding. Occupational burnout contains a symptom
relationship between forms of passion and burnout. It was of depersonalisation (employed in Carbonneau et al., 2008),
hypothesised that self-determined motivation would mediate the whereas in sport this is replaced by devaluation (Raedeke & Smith,
relationships between passion and athlete burnout. Results 2001). Depersonalisation refers to a detachment from clients,
revealed that harmonious passion was inversely correlated with whereas devaluation refers to a decrease in the importance of sport
the latent athlete burnout factor. Contrary to expectations, obses- participation. Obsessive passion might be expected to lead to self-
sive passion was uncorrelated with the latent athlete burnout closure from intra-personal experience (i.e. depersonalisation).
factor. Self-determined motivation fully mediated the relationship However, due to an underpinning compulsive attraction to the
between harmonious passion and athlete burnout. However, no activity (Vallerand, 2008), this form of passion may be less likely to
mediation was found in the case of obsessive passion and athlete lead to any reduced sense of value associated with participation
burnout. (i.e. devaluation). In support of this possibility, obsessive passion
has been positively associated with activity valuation (Vallerand
Passion and athlete burnout et al., 2003, study 1) and high levels of behavioural investment
(Vallerand et al., study 3). Future research is required in order to
The finding that harmonious passion inversely correlated with examine potential contextual differences.
the latent athlete burnout variable extends research in this area by
replicating findings in an occupational context (Carbonneau et al.,
2008; Vallerand et al., 2010). It appears that this form of passion The mediating role of self-determined motivation
may ameliorate the burnout syndrome for athletes as it does in
other groups. Thus in a practical sense, the more one identifies with The assessment of mediation produced mixed findings. Self-
harmonious passion, the less likely they are to report symptoms of determined motivation fully explained the relationship between
athlete burnout. This discovery supports a growing body of harmonious passion and athlete burnout but no mediation was
research that has evidenced the adaptive role of harmonious evident in terms of obsessive passion. These findings extend
passion in sport (see Vallerand & Miquelon, 2007). Further, in the previous research by identifying an additional explanation for the
context of athlete burnout, this finding can also be considered to relationship between harmonious passion and burnout. Recently,
contribute to an understanding of potential mitigating constituents Vallerand et al. (2010) found that life satisfaction and conflict with
of the syndrome. other life spheres mediated the relationship between harmonious
With regard to obsessive passion, a positive association with passion and occupational burnout. It may be that the mediating
athlete burnout was expected in a manner that has been found in processes in the current study and those observed by Vallerand and
an occupational context (Carbonneau et al., 2008; Vallerand et al., colleagues are part of a larger process. Specifically, more self-
2010). Contrary to this expectation, obsessive passion was unre- determined motivation may facilitate positive affect (i.e. life satis-
lated to athlete burnout in the current study. Nevertheless, it is faction) and hinder negative cognition (i.e. conflict with other life
notable that obsessive passion did not inversely correlate with spheres), which in turn mitigates athlete burnout. Future research
athlete burnout. Therefore, unlike harmonious passion, obsessive may wish to examine this possibility.

Table 2
Fit of path models for mediation.

c2 (df) c2/df TLI CFI SRMR RMSEA (90% CI) Dc2 (df)
M1: Absence of mediator 16.41 (4)* 4.10 .89 .96 .06 .15 (.08e.22)
M2: Full mediation 19.30 (8)* 2.41 .94 .97 .06 .10 (.04e.15)
M3: Partial mediation 18.10 (6)* 3.00 .92 .97 .06 .12 (.05e.18) M2 vs M3 ¼ 1.23 (2) ns

Note: M1 ¼ in this model both components of passion have a direct pathway to athlete burnout; M2 ¼ in this model components of passion have only indirect pathways to
athlete burnout via self-determined motivation; M3 ¼ in this model both direct and indirect pathways from components of passion to athlete burnout are included. c2 ¼ chi
square; df ¼ degrees of freedom; TLI ¼ Tucker Lewis Index; CFI ¼ Comparative Fit Index; SRMR ¼ standardised root mean square residual; RMSEA ¼ root mean squared error of
approximation.*p < .05, ns ¼ non-significant.
660 T. Curran et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 12 (2011) 655e661

In the case of obsessive passion, the findings do not support and experimental designs are required to begin to determine the
the utility of self-determined motivation as an intervening validity of the current variable configuration.
variable. This was an unexpected finding since it is considered that Specific motivational regulations were omitted from the current
obsessive passion encompasses controlled motivational regulation study due to case-to-parameter restrictions. Future research would
(c.f. Vallerand, 2008) similar to that which characterises burnout benefit from an examination of the relationships between passion
(Lemyre et al., 2006). It is possible that despite obsessive passion and discrete forms of motivational regulation. This is particularly
being indicative of less self-determined motivation (e.g., introjec- important for determining the precise motivational valence of
tion), it does not encapsulate the most central antecedent of athlete obsessive passion and the degree to which obsessive passion
burnout; namely, amotivation (Cresswell & Eklund, 2005a, 2005b, encompasses the motivational features of athlete burnout. Finally,
2005c). Therefore, this form of passion may not fully capture the future research may wish to focus upon identifying other mediating
“motivational signature” of the burnout syndrome in sport (see processes, such as psychological need satisfaction and thwarting,
Eklund & Cresswell, 2007; Gould, 1996). Instead, obsessive passion which are predicted by passion and precede self-determined
may be best considered as an indicator of poorer motivational motivation (Vallerand et al., 2006). Modelling the cognitive
quality in tandem with high motivational quantity. This possibility (e.g., rumination), affective (e.g., satisfaction) and behavioural
is supported by Vallerand et al. (2003) who have found that (e.g., persistence) outcomes associated with passion would provide
obsessive passion energises high levels of behavioural investment, a particularly useful insight into the passioneathlete burnout
even when it is not sensible to do so. In other words, despite many relationship.
other negative outcomes relative to harmonious passion (see
Vallerand & Miquelon, 2007), obsessive passion may not neces-
Conclusion
sarily encompass a pattern of amotivation that predisposes athletes
to burnout. Longitudinal designs, and studies that examine the
Although burnout has been identified as a corrosive syndrome
mediating role of individual regulations, are required to examine
for sports participants (Gould et al., 1996), the mechanisms that
this conjecture.
explain its development in junior athletes are not clearly under-
stood. The current study extends understanding of athlete burnout
Alternative model testing
by suggesting that harmonious passion may provide some resil-
ience to the development of burnout. Furthermore, the current
In addition to the hypothesised model, an alternative model was
findings suggest that higher levels of self-determined motivation
tested. The alternative model proposed that self-determined
may explain the inverse relationship between harmonious passion
motivation is an antecedent of passion, as opposed to the reverse.
and burnout. This latter finding substantiates a growing body of
This was based on the possibility that forms of passion and
research that highlights the adaptive role of harmonious passion
behavioural regulation can be considered to emerge from the same
for athletes. In contrast, the research did not find support for the
processes of activity internalisation, and thus the relationship
hypothesised positive association between obsessive passion and
between passion and contextual motivation is not easily disen-
athlete burnout. As this relationship has been found outside of
tangled. Results indicated that the alternative model provided an
sport, future research is required to examine the possibility that
inadequate fit to the observed data. Consequently, the study
obsessive passion is unrelated to burnout in athletes.
provides initial support for suggestion that passions precede
behavioural regulation.
Though passion and behavioural regulation share internal- References
isation processes, they are distinguished by the degree to which
behaviour is internalised, and not the internalisation process per se. Appleton, P. R., Hall, H. K., & Hill, A. P. (2009). Relations between multidimensional
perfectionism and burnout in junior-elite male athletes. Psychology of Sport &
Whereas passion is internalised into one’s identity, behavioural
Exercise, 10, 457e465.
regulations operate at a more proximal level and do not constitute Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderatoremediator variable distinction in
identity space (see Vallerand, 2008). Accordingly, once assimilated, social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical consider-
ations. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 51, 1173e1182.
passions can be considered to precipitate contextual-motivational
Brenner, J. S. (2007). Overuse injuries, overtraining, and burnout in child and
processes (see Vallerand et al., 2006). In the case of athlete burnout, adolescent athletes. Paediatrics, 119, 1242e1245.
these findings should be considered to provide initial evidence that Carbonneau, N., Vallerand, R. J., Fernet, C., & Guay, F. (2008). The role of passion for
self-determined motivation can account for variance shared teaching in intra and interpersonal outcomes. Journal of Educational Psychology,
100, 977e987.
between harmonious passion and the syndrome. Future research is Cresswell, S. L., & Eklund, R. C. (2005a). Motivation and burnout among top amateur
now required to test the direction of these relationships over time. rugby players. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 37, 469e477.
Cresswell, S. L., & Eklund, R. C. (2005b). Motivation and burnout in professional
rugby players. Research Quarterly for Exercise & Sport, 76, 370e376.
Limitations and future research Cresswell, S. L., & Eklund, R. C. (2005c). Changes in athlete burnout and motivation
over a 12-week league tournament. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise,
The present study has a number of limitations. Firstly, because of 37, 1957e1966.
Dall, J. (2009, May 10th). Arsene warns of Theo burnout. Sky Sports News, . (Online).
the specific sample, the results cannot be generalised beyond the Available from. http://www.skysports.com/story/0,19528,12098_5308514,00.
context of young elite male soccer players. The replication of this html Accessed 12.05.09.
study to females and other sports would enhance the external Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human
behavior. New York: Plenum Press.
validity of the findings for the athletic population. In addition, Efron, B. (1988). Bootstrap confidence intervals: good or bad? Psychological Bulletin,
components of passion and self-determined motivation were 105, 293e296.
modelled in the absence of measurement error. This can lead to an Efron, B., & Tibshirani, R. J. (1998). An introduction to the bootstrap. Boca Raton: CRC
Press.
underestimation of direct and indirect effects (Shrout & Bolger,
Eklund, R. C., & Cresswell, S. L. (2007). Athlete burnout. In G. Tenenbaum, &
2002). The absence of a relationship between obsessive passion R. C. Eklund (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (3rd ed.). (pp. 621e641)
and self-determined motivation may have been a consequence of Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
taking this approach. Moreover, direct model comparison is not Gillet, N., Vallerand, R. J., Amoura, S., & Baldes, B. (2010). Influence of coaches’
autonomy support on athletes’ motivation and sport performance: a test of the
possible when models are not nested. Similarly, the cross-sectional hierarchal model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. Psychology of Sport &
design precludes any inference of causality. Therefore, longitudinal Exercise, 11, 155e161.
T. Curran et al. / Psychology of Sport and Exercise 12 (2011) 655e661 661

Gould, D. (1996). Personal motivation gone awry; burnout in competitive athletes. amotivation in sports: the sport motivation scale (SMS). Journal of Sport &
Quest, 48, 275e289. Exercise Psychology, 17, 35e53.
Gould, D., Udry, E., Tuffey, S., & Loehr, J. (1996). Burnout in competitive junior tennis Pelletier, L. G., & Sarrazin, P. (2007). Measurement issues in self-determination
players: I. A quantitative psychological assessment. The Sport Psychologist, 10, theory and sport. In M. S. Hagger, & N. L. D. Chatzisarantis (Eds.), Intrinsic
322e340. motivation and self-determination in exercise and sport (pp. 143e152). Cham-
Gustafsson, H., Kentta, G., Hassmen, P., & Johansson, M. (2008). A qualitative analysis paign, IL: Human Kinetics.
of burnout in elite Swedish athletes. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 9, 800e816. Raedeke, T. D. (1997). Is athlete burnout more than just stress? A sport commitment
Harlick, M., & McKenzie, A. (2000). Burnout in junior tennis: a research report. The perspective. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 19, 396e417.
New Zealand Journal of Sports Medicine, 28, 36e39. Raedeke, T. D., & Smith, A. L. (2001). Development and preliminary validation of an
Hill, A. P., Hall, H. K., Appleton, P. R., & Kozub, S. R. (2008). Perfectionism and athlete burnout measure. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 23, 281e306.
burnout in junior elite soccer players: the mediating influence of unconditional Raedeke, T. D., & Smith, A. L. (2004). Coping resources and athlete burnout: an
self-acceptance. Psychology of Sport & Exercise, 9, 630e644. examination of stress mediated and moderation hypothesis. Journal of Sport &
Hill, A. P., Hall, H. K., Appleton, P. R., & Murray, J. J. (2010). Perfectionism and burnout Exercise Psychology, 26, 525e541.
in canoe polo and kayak slalom athletes: the mediating influence of validation Ratelle, C. F., Vallerand, R. J., Mageau, G. A., Rousseau, F. L., & Provencher, P. (2004).
and growth-seeking. The Sport Psychologist, 24, 16e34. When passion leads to problematic outcomes: a look at gambling. Journal of
Hodge, K., Lonsdale, C., & Ng, J. Y. Y. (2008). Burnout in elite rugby: relationships Gambling Studies, 20, 105e119.
with basic psychological needs fulfilment. Journal of Sports Sciences, 26, Rip, B., Fortin, S., & Vallerand, R. J. (2006). The relationship between passion and
835e844. injury in dance students. Journal of Dance Medicine & Science, 10, 14e20.
Hodgins, H. S., & Knee, C. R. (2002). The integrating self and conscious experience. Rousseau, F. L., & Vallerand, R. J. (2008). An examination of the relationship between
In E. L. Deci, & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. passion and subjective well-being in older adults. International Journal of Aging
87e100). Rochester: University of Rochester Press. and Human Development, 66, 195e211.
Holmbeck, G. N. (1997). Toward terminological, conceptual, and statistical clarity in Seguin-Lévesque, C., Laliberté, M.-L. N., Pelletier, L. G., Blanchard, C. M., &
the study of mediators and moderators: examples from the child-clinical and Vallerand, R. J. (2003). Harmonious and obsessive passion for the internet: their
paediatric psychology literatures. Journal of Counselling & Clinical Psychology, 65, associations with couple’s relationships. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33,
599e610. 197e221.
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. (1995). Evaluating model fit. In I. R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and non-experimental
equation modelling: Concepts, issues, and applications (pp. 76e99). Thousand studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7,
Oaks, CA: Sage. 422e445.
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics. London: Allyn
analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation and Bacon.
Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 1e55. Taylor, I. M., Ntoumanis, N., & Standage, M. (2008). A self-determination theory
Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modelling (1st ed.). approach to understanding the antecedents of teachers’ motivational strategies
New York: Guilford Press. in physical education. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 30, 75e94.
Lemyre, P. N., Treasure, D. C., & Roberts, G. C. (2006). Influence of variability in Vallerand, R. J. (1997). Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic
motivation and affect on elite athlete burnout susceptibility. Journal of Sport & motivation. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology. San
Exercise Psychology, 28, 32e48. Diego: Academic Press.
Loewenthal, K. M. (1996). An introduction to psychological tests and scales. London: Vallerand, R. J. (2001). A hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in
UCL Press. sport and exercise. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Advances in motivation in sport and
Lonsdale, C., Hodge, K., & Rose, E. (2009). Athlete burnout in elite sport: a self- exercise (pp. 263e320). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
determination perspective. Journal of Sports Sciences, 27, 785e795. Vallerand, R. J. (2008). On the psychology of passion: in search of what makes
Mackinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., & Williams, J. (2004). Confidence limits for the people’s lives most worth living. Canadian Psychology, 49, 1e13.
indirect effect: distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivar- Vallerand, R. J., Blanchard, C. M., Mageau, G. A., Koestner, R., Ratelle, C., Leonard, M.,
iate Behavioral Research, 30, 99e128. et al. (2003). Les passions de l’ame: on obsessive and harmonious passion.
Mageau, G. A., Vallerand, R. J., Rousseau, F. L., Ratelle, C. F., & Provencher, P. J. (2005). Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 85, 756e767.
Passion and gambling: investigating the divergent affective and cognitive Vallerand, R. J., Mageau, G. A., Elliot, A., Dumais, M. A., Demers, M., & Rousseau, F. L.
consequences of gambling. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 35, 100e118. (2008). Passion and performance attainment in sport. Psychology of Sport &
Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. E. (1986). Maslach burnout inventory manual (2nd ed.). Palo Exercise, 9, 373e392.
Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press. Vallerand, R. J., & Miquelon, P. (2007). Passion for sport in athletes. In S. Jowett, &
Niemiec, C. P., Ryan, R. M., & Brown, K. W. (2008). The role of awareness and D. Lavallee (Eds.), Social psychology in sport (pp. 249e263). Champaign: Human
autonomy in quieting the ego: a self-determination theory perspective. In Kinetics.
H. A. Wayment, & J. J. Bauer (Eds.), Transcending self-interest: Psychological Vallerand, R. J., Paquet, Y., Philippe, F. L., & Charest, J. (2010). On the role of passion
explorations of the quiet ego (pp. 107e116). Washington, DC: American for work in burnout: a process model. Journal of Personality, 78, 289e312.
Psychological Association. Vallerand, R. J., Rousseau, F. L., Grouzet, F. M. E., Dumais, A., & Grenier, S. (2006).
Pelletier, L. G., Fortier, M. S., Vallerand, R. J., Tuson, K. M., Brière, N. M., & Blais, M. R. Passion in sport: a look at determinants and affective experiences. Journal of
(1995). Toward a new measure of intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, and Sport & Exercise Psychology, 28, 455e478.

You might also like