Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 22

^ Academy of Management Review

2001, Vol. 26. No. 3. 35&-376.

A TEMPORALLY BASED FRAMEWORK AND


TAXONOMY OF TEAM PROCESSES
MICHELLE A. MARKS
Florida International University

JOHN E. MATHIEU
University of Connecticut

STEPHEN J. ZACCARO
George Mason University

In this article we examine the meaning of team process. We first define team process
in the context of a multiphase episodic framework related to goal accomplishment,
arguing that teams are multitasking units that perform multiple processes simulta-
neously and sequentially to orchestrate goal-directed taskwork. We then advance a
taxonomy of team process dimensions synthesized from previous research and theo-
rizing, a taxonomy that reflects our time-based conceptual framework. We conclude
with implications for future research and application.

Much of, the work in organizations is com- such criteria as performance quality and quan-
pleted through teamwork: people working to- tity, as well as members' reactions.
gether to achieve something beyond the capa- With the growing interest in team research,
bilities of individuals working alone. Success is these conceptual models have rapidly gained
not only a function of team members' talents popularity as the foundation for hundreds of em-
and the available resources but also the pro- pirical studies. Yet there remains a need for
cesses team members use to interact with each greater conceptual clarity in our understanding
other to accomplish the work. Understanding the of teamwork, since there is still no unified con-
processes that employees use to work together ception of what team processes are and how
in teams will enable organizations to retool hu- they operate during team goal accomplishment.
man resource systems and managers to select, To further our ability to assess teamwork pro-
train, develop, and reward personnel for effec- cesses in studies of team effectiveness, we need
tive teamwork. a common conceptual and structural foundation
The realization that process plays a pivotal for the concept of team process.
role in team performance has led to a prolifera- Our purpose in this article is to advance fu-
tion of team studies in applied settings and re- ture studies of team effectiveness by taking a
search laboratories in the past twenty years. detailed look at the concept of team process
During this time, there has been increased at- along four major themes. First, we offer a defi-
tention on developing theoretical models of nition of team process aimed at reducing some
team effectiveness, with team processes occupy- of the current confusion in the literature over
ing a central role (e.g.. Gist, Locke, & Taylor, this ubiquitous term, and we discuss the types of
1987; Guzzo & Shea, 1992; Hackman, 1983). In variables that fall under the process umbrella,
these models researchers generally have as well as identify those that do not. Second, we
adopted an input-process-outcome (I-P-O) advance a temporally based conceptual model
framework. They view processes as mediating of team processes that, we believe, has wide-
mechanisms linking such variables as member, spread applicability to many team types, and
team, and organizational characteristics with we articulate how team processes operate in
recurring phases within an episodic framework
of team performance. Third, we introduce a new
This research was funded in part by the Airforce Office ior taxonomy of team process dimensions, arguing
Sponsored Research (AFSOR) Contract #F49620-98-1-0278. that there are ten critical processes clustered
356
2001 Marks, Mattiieu, and Zaccoro 357

into three higher-level categories, distinguish- Certain organizational contexts may place a
able on the basis of time (relatively speaking) premium on particular forms of teamwork
and content domain. This taxonomy is wed to (Guzzo & Shea, 1992), but here we address some-
the time-based model that we advance and what more general processes that have wide-
grounded heavily in prior work in this area. Fi- spread applicability. We delineate the multidi-
nally, we provide recommendations for using mensional nature of team process later, in the
this taxonomy to further team process assess- section on taxonomy development. For now, our
ment in research and practice. point is that teams use different types of pro-
cesses to convert inputs into outcomes. A review
of the process literature indicates that most au-
THE CONSTRUCT OF TEAM PROCESS thors believe the essence of the construct lies in
: Our review of the literature featuring investi- team interaction and that different forms of
gations or discussions of team process revealed team processes describe the types of interac-
surprisingly few definitions of the construct, and tions that take place among team members dur-
the ones provided typically were very general in ing the course of goal accomplishment. We
nature. For example, in a recent review of team should note, however, that the nature of the
effectiveness research, Cohen and Bailey define team activity performed might act as a bound-
team process as "interactions such as commu- ary condition for the points we advance below.
nication and conflict that occur among group More specifically, Sundstrom (1999) suggests
members and external others" (1997: 244). that work teams could be categorized into six
McGrath refers to team interaction process as types: (1) project, (2) production, (3) service,
"patterned relations" among team members (4) action/performing, (5) management, and (6)
(1984a: 11). Although these types of definitions parallel. As will become evident below, because
give readers a "feel" for what is meant by team of the temporal nature of activities performed,
process, they are not specific enough to provide our framework pertains best to the first four
clear guidance to researchers. We define team types of teams listed above and less to manage-
process as membeis' inteidependent acts that ment and parallel teams.
convert inputs to outcomes through cognitive,
verbal, and behavioial activities directed to-
PROCESSES VERSUS EMERGENT STATES
ward organizing taskwork to achieve collective
goals. Centrally, team process involves mem- Even with general agreement on the concep-
bers' interacting with other members and their tual meaning of team process, widespread con-
task environment. Team processes are the cerns exist regarding the selection and opera-
means by which members work interdepen- tionalization of process variables for use in
dently to utilize various resources, such as ex- research. One particular problem that has
pertise, equipment, and money, to yield mean- slowed the progression of the team process lit-
ingful outcomes (e.g., product development, rate erature is the diversity of variables that have
of work, team commitment, satisfaction). been selected as "processes" in tests of I-P-O
We, as have others, distinguish team pro- relationships. For example, variables such as
cesses from taskwork, defined as "a team's in- collective efficacy, potency, cohesion, and situ-
teractions with tasks, tools, machines, and sys- ational awareness have been used frequently to
tems" (Bowers, Braun, & Morgan, 1997: 90). represent process. We submit that these types of
Taskwork represents what it is that teams are constructs do not denote interaction processes
doing, whereas teamwork describes how they but, instead, tap qualities of a team that repre-
are doing it with each other. Taskwork is critical sent member attitudes, values, cognitions, and
to team effectiveness and depends heavily on motivations. We prefer to call these types of
member competence as well as team processes. variables "emergent states": constructs that
Team processes are used to direct, align, and characterize properties of the team that are typ-
monitor taskwork. Of course, this distinction ically dynamic in nature and vary as a function
of team context, inputs, processes, and outcomes.
may become blurry in practice, but our focus
here is on the team processes that enable teams Emergent states describe cognitive, motiva-
to orchestrate taskwork activities for goal ac- tional, and affective states of teams, as opposed
complishment. to the nature of their member interaction. Al-
358 Academy of Management Beview Iuly

though researchers have not typically classified dependent team contexts (e.g., fielding and
them as such, emergent states can be consid- throwing the baseball are critical taskwork com-
ered both team inputs and proximal outcomes. ponents involved in turning a double play, but
For example, teams with low cohesion (an emer- the coordination at second base is critical).
gent state) may be less willing to manage exist- Processes guide the execution of taskwork. Yet
ing conflict (the process), which, in turn, may the way in which taskwork is executed (e.g.,
create additional conflict that lowers cohesion quality and efficiency of work) certainly impacts
levels even further. This distinction is important, the need for processes to govern further team
because indices of emergent states are often activity.
intermingled with interactional process indica- In sum, the term teamwoik piocesses de-
tors (e.g., coordination), which results in serious scribes interdependent team activities that or-
construct contamination. Emergent states do not chestrate taskwork in employees' pursuit of
represent team interaction or team actions that goals. Teamwork processes are the vehicles that
lead toward outcomes. Rather, they are products transform team inputs to both proximal and
of team experiences (including team processes) longer-term outcomes. To avoid construct confu-
and become new inputs to subsequent pro- sion and to sharpen the conception of team pro-
cesses and outcomes. The point is that emergent cess, investigators must recognize the distinc-
states are not processes in and of themselves, tions among teamwork processes, taskwork,
because they do not describe the nature of mem- emergent states, and more permanent team
ber interaction. traits and characteristics.
In their review of applied team research, Co-
hen and Bailey (1997) make a related point by
distinguishing processes from team psychoso- A RECURRING PHASE MODEL OF TEAM
cial traits. Psychosocial traits are not explicitly PROCESSES ;,
defined in their article, but the authors provide The purpose of the recurring phase model of
examples of them: shared mental models, team processes is to outline how temporal fac-
norms, affect, and cohesion. In a model they tors impact team functioning. Although previous
present (Cohen 8f Bailey, 1997), psychosocial authors have offered valuable taxonomies oi
traits are predicted by inputs and processes, team processes, none to date have tied them
and they drive both subsequent processes and explicitly to a dynamic model of team effective-
outcomes. We choose to consider these vari- ness. Our framework is consistent with Zaheer,
ables emergent states rather than "traits" be- Albert, and Zaheer's (1999) recent call for more
cause of their mutable qualities. A trait is "a attention on time and how temporal intervals
relatively enduring characteristic" (Kerlinger, influence organizational processes. We argue
1986: 453) that has an air of permanency, that different team processes are critical at dif-
whereas states are more fluid and more easily ferent phases of task execution and that I-P-O
influenced by context. Some emergent states relationships occur over a series of related cy-
vary frequently, even in fairly short periods of cles. We advance this framework by (1) review-
time. For example, cockpit crews can phase in ing and synthesizing relevant background liter-
and out of situational awareness at different ature that describes how temporal influences
points during a single flight. Other states, such affect teams, (2) introducing the logic of epi-
as cohesion, are malleable in newly formed sodes as sequenced temporal units in which
teams but tend to remain fairly stable in those teams perform on their path to goal accomplish-
with a long history (Mullen & Copper, 1994). ment, and (3) presenting a recurring phase
Emergent states and other team traits and model and the notions of transition and action
characteristics serve as inputs and influence the phases to describe temporal influences on team
execution of teamwork processes and taskwork, processes.
which are likely to alter subsequent emergent
states, as well as teamwork and taskwork fur-
ther down the line. This cyclical pattem contin- Temporal Influences on Teams
ues until teams reach more distal team out- No work-related tasks are performed in a vac-
comes. Teamwork processes and taskwork often uum, unaffected by deadlines, time limits, or
seemingly co-occur, especially in highly inter- schedules. Work teams strive toward collective
2001 Marks, Mathieu, and Zaccaro 359

goals that incorporate time as a component ment is the "complex matching of bundles of
(Locke & Latham, 1990). Time factors such as activities to parficuiar periods of time" (1991:
project deadlines, synchronization of schedules, 163; emphasis added). We share McGrath's view
alignment of coordination efforts, and so forth that teams are typically engaged in the pursuit
dictate many aspects of team functioning, in- of multiple goals simultaneously; thus, several
cluding the strategies that are employed, the tasks are often being juggled at any one time.
pace of activities, and role assignments that The pursuit of multiple goals simultaneously
develop in order for the teams to perform suc- "creates an environment where members are
cessfully. Time-based rhythms act to shape how engaged in complex sequences of interdepen-
teams manage their behavior. However, there is dent tasks that comprise a larger project"
a paucity of research on how teams integrate (McGrath, 1991: 149). McGrath talks about time
temporal processes into their functioning. In the as an environmental driver, and we further this
vast majority of empirical studies, researchers with the notion that time is linked to goal ac-
have taken a rather static perspective when it complishment in an episodic framework. For
comes to examining team effectiveness simplicity's sake, we begin by explaining the
(McGrath, 1993). By this we mean that research- role of temporal influences on the achievement
ers have traditionally examined I-P-O relation- of a singie team goal, although later we elabo-
ships within a single task accomplishment pe- rate to point out the complexities that teams
riod. Even when authors observe teams over face when managing task accomplishment in
more extensive periods, in their analyses they the pursuit of multiple goals.
usually aggregate process data gathered over
time into a summary index that portrays direct
process->outcome relationships (e.g., Barry & Team Performance Episodes
Stewart, 1997). Thus, variance across time is col- Team performance trajectories most com-
lapsed into a static indicator of teamwork pro- monly consist of several I-P-O-type cycles that
cess as though it occurs at a single point in time. run sequentially and simultaneously. Our
The result is that temporal factors are elimi- framework is based on the idea that teams per-
nated from further examination. form in temporal cycles of goal-directed activity,
The framework advanced here is designed to called "episodes" (Weingart, 1997; Zaheer et al.,
explain how time relates to team goal attain- 1999). Episodes are distinguishable periods of
ment, rather than phases of a team life cycle or time over which peiiormance accrues and feed-
development (e.g., Tuckman, 1965). Some re- back is available (Mathieu &. Button, 1992). They
searchers have focused directly on the role of constitute the rhythms of task performance for
time as related to team goal-related activities teams, and they are marked by identifiable pe-
(e.g., Gersick, 1988, 1989; Kelly & McGrath, 1985). riods of action and transition periods between
However, there have been few efforts to synthe- actions. Episodes' durations stem largely from
size this body of work and to expand it to ex- the nature of the tasks that teams perform and
plain goal attainment with a dynamic model the technology that they employ, and from the
of team functioning. Kozlowski, Gully, Nason, manner in which members choose to complete
and Smith's (1999) team development model work. Episodes are most easily identified by
and McGrath's (1991) theory of time, interaction, goals and goal accomplishment periods. The
and performance (TIP) are two important excep- conclusion of one episode normally marks the
tions. Kozlowski et al. (1999) argue that team initiation of another, whether these are work
tasks cycle in intensity and that these cycles are orders, quarterly sales profits, or halves of a
used to develop learning skills at different sporting event (although there are variations on
stages of development. We build on their view this pattern—most notably, more complex ar-
rangements where episodes overlap). Episodes
that "team compilation" is "a sequence of modal
may vary substantially in their length and con-
phases and transition points" and that different
sistency, and they are often segmented into sec-
team activities occur in different phases of team
tions or subepisodes of more limited scope and
development (1999: 248-250). duration that contribute to the larger effort. Fur-
McGrath (1991) argues that teams simulta- ther, each episode has a valence, or relative
neously manage multiple bundles oi activities importance, attached to it that may heighten or
over time. A significant aspect of this manage-
360 Academy of Management Review July

weaken its salience to the team, given the myr- that I-P-O models are attached to episodes and
iad of demands the team faces. subepisodes, rather than the entire life cycle of
Up to this point we have discussed perfor- the team. Outcomes from initial episodes often
mance episodes as though teams pursue just become inputs for the next cycle. Processes are
one of them at a time. However, virtually all likely to vary in importance across episodes.
present-day work teams have to multitask in Adopting this episodic approach suggests
order to manage several performance episodes that teams are actively engaged in different
simultaneously (McGrath, 1991). Consequently, types of taskwork at different phases of task
they often work in multiple performance epi- accomplishment. Sometimes they are focused
sodes at a given point in time, each with its on activities related directly to goal accomplish-
constituent subgoals and episodes and with its ment, while at other times they are reflecting on
associated rhythms and sequence. Just as teams past performance and planning for future ac-
need to break down and sequence subepisode tion. We refer to these different emphases as
accomplishments, they must orchestrate multi- "action" and "transition phases." Action phases
ple episode interfaces. Moreover, the timing and are periods of time when teams are engaged in
duration of these episodes may often differ acts that contribute directly to goal accomplish-
markedly and cause even greater coordination ment (i.e., taskwork). Such actions may vary con-
demands. The primary challenge is for teams to siderably by team type. Surgical teams perform
develop and execute a multifaceted plan of operations, marketing teams develop advertis-
work that simultaneously m a n a g e s perfor- ing campaigns, and product development teams
mance gaps in each of their important perfor- coordinate design efforts. In contrast, transition
mance episodes. phases are periods of time when teams focus
The complexities described above are han- primarily on evaluation and/or planning activi-
dled by a host of team processes. First, there is a ties to guide their accomplishment of a team
premium on understanding the larger work en- goal or objective. These refer to the times when
vironment within which the team is operating, teams take inventory of how well they per-
developing appropriate strategies and contin- formed during the previous episode and prepare
gency plans, and specifying clear goals during for the upcoming episode. Teams compare cur-
transition phases. The role of communication is rent performance levels against goals and de-
heightened, especially during periods when rive performance gaps. Closing these gaps, in
members need to coordinate actions and to mon- combination with current and anticipated future
itor the environment and the team's progress. assignments, guides the development of future
Pressures and demands inevitably lead to con- performance goals and the strategies to achieve
fusion and conflict among members and can them.
erode their motivation, confidence, and morale. Figure 1 illustrates the temporal rhythm of
Processes are the means by which teams man- team task accomplishment by integrating the
age all of these concerns during multiepisodic central elements of our conceptual framework of
goal accomplishment. The types of processes team process. It depicts four different types of
that occur differ, in part, because of the partic- performance episodes that a team might exe-
ular activities that are being conducted at any cute while multitasking; these episodes vary in
given time during a performance episode. terms of their onset and cycle times. Task 1 de-
We now turn to a discussion of a model that picts a fairly fast cycle rhythm with cyclical
explains what types of processes are more likely transition and action phases. Task 2 illustrates a
to occur at different periods within performance much greater period of sustained activity before
episodes. goal accomplishment is evaluated. Task 3 falls
in between these two, whereas Task 4 repre-
Recurring Phase Model of Team Processes sents a cycle similar to the first but in which the
onset is delayed relative to the others. As Task 1
We introduce the notion of a recurring phase indicates, longer-term episodes are often seg-
model of team processes to delineate the role of mented into sections or subepisodes of more
process in performance episodes. We submit limited scope and duration that contribute to the
that, over time, team performance is best viewed larger effort. I-P-O cycles are nested in action
as a series of related I-P-O episodes. We assert and transition phases within episodes; thus.
2001 Marks, Mathieu, and Zaccaro 361

FIGURE 1
The Rhythm of Team Task Accomplishment

Taskl Transition

o
Task 2 Transition Action

Task 3 Transition Action Transition

o
Task 4 Transition Action Transition

Time

outputs generated from processes that occur phases axe not always separate periods and
during a transition phase, for example, become frequently blend into one another. Recall at the
inputs for the ensuing action phase. Figure 1 outset that we noted that team type may operate
shows that processes occur over and over again as an important boundary condition. Whereas
during team episodes, influenced by inputs (in- the temporal rhythms of project, production, ser-
cluding emergent states) and influencing proxi- vice, and action teams are fairly easy to discern,
mal outcomes (also including emergent states) those of managerial and parallel teams are less
within action and transition phases and across apparent. Further, product, production, service,
tasks and time as teams move toward goal ac- and action teams require taskwork activities
complishment. T .-;--:wn,-?;^:>-:?. ,;;:'c:s *:, •• ••: that follow from planning, strategy, goal setting,
The nature of team process changes as teams and other preparations. In contrast, the actual
move back and forth between action and transi- taskwork activities of managerial and many
tion phases. Teamwork processes that revolve parallel teams involve analyzing situations, for-
around planning and evaluation occur more fre- mulating strategies, setting goals, and so forth.
quently in transition phases. In contrast, coordi- Although we still believe that one can distin-
nation and monitoring processes are likely to guish between periods when a team decides
dominate the action phases of goal accomplish- how it will make decisions and the actual deci-
ment. Thus, the cyclical transition-action phases sion-making activities, the lines of demarcation
highlight the types of team processes needed at are less clear.
a given time. The frequency, length, and predict- In sum, we submit that identifying salient per-
ability of action and transition phase alterations formance episodes is critical to understanding
are functions of a variety of variables, such as not only what but when team processes become
team objectives, environment, expertise, norms, critical to goal accomplishment. The transition
and leadership. Even so, transition and action and action phases delineate when certain team
362 Academy of Management Review luly

processes are likely to be most salient. We fur- sions with those of earlier taxonomic and
ther submit that because the timing and rhythm empirical efforts. This illustrates how our work
of these episodes are somewhat arbitrary and dovetails with that of others.
idiosyncratic, a thorough team task analysis The taxonomy contains a hierarchical struc-
(Bowers, Baker, & Salas, 1994) is required to ture. The ten process dimensions are nested
identify them. In the following sections we dis- within three superordinate categories: (1) transi-
cuss the specific nature of the processes that tion phase processes, (2) action phase processes,
occur during transition and action phases of and (3) inteTpersonal processes. We argue that
team functioning. some processes are more likely to occur during
transition periods, whereas others are more
likely to occur during action periods. Interper-
TAXONOMY OF TEAM PROCESSES
sonal processes are expected to occur through-
The recurring phase model of team processes out transition and action phases, although, nat-
highlights the idea that process is multidimen- urally, the pertinent issues change at different
sional and that teams use different processes times. Figure 2 illustrates the process dimen-
simultaneously and over performance episodes sions as they occur within transition and action
in order to multitask effectively. Some processes phases.
transpire more frequently in action phases and Careful consideration was given to the level
others in transition periods. In prior taxonomies of specification of the dimensions. We have at-
of team processes (e.g., Fleishman & Zaccaro, tempted to fully represent the construct of pro-
1992; Prince & Salas, 1993), although providing a cess without leaving out critical components.
wealth of useful information, researchers have However, we wanted to avoid creating an ex-
not incorporated a multiphase perspective of haustive list of process variables too lengthy for
team processes. This perspective requires a dif- value in either research communities or the
ferent taxonomic structure in which processes field. The result is a categorization system con-
nested within transition and action phases are taining ten dimensions appropriate for teams
recognized. This taxonomy does not offer "new" across all contexts (to varying degrees). There
process dimensions per se but, rather, a new may be processes specific to one type of team
temporally based categorization system for ex- that are not included in our taxonomy. Each of
isting constructs that fit the definition of team the ten process dimensions refers to a general
process proposed in this article. type of activity that can be performed anywhere
from very well to very poorly.
Development of the Taxonomy of
Team Processes Previous Team Process Classifications
Our intention is to devise a taxonomy that is The idea of creating a classification system
broad enough to apply to different types of for team interaction is not new. There have been
teams yet specific enough to be easily under- several formal efforts to construct comprehen-
stood for applied and research purposes. We sive categorization schemes. Two particularly
created our taxonomy by (1) reviewing the ex- influential efforts include that of Prince and
tant literature on team processes (both concep- Salas (1993), who used literature reviews, criti-
tual models and empirical studies), (2) develop- cal incident interviews, and process ratings by
ing a framework of team processes to provide naval aviators to identify a set of seven critical
conceptual clarity, (3) using previous classifica- team skills, and that of Nieva, Fleishman,
tion efforts, and (4) integrating our applied ex- & Rieck (1978; later revised by Fleishman &
periences with teams to generate process di- Zaccaro, 1992), who developed a taxonomy of
mensions that are both distinct conceptually team performance functions. Although in nei-
and consistent with our theory of team pro- ther effort do the authors attend to a temporally
cesses. Table 1 displays our taxonomy with di- based framework, both works contain various
mension definitions. We view this taxonomy as process dimensions and were highly influential
a comprehensive effort that builds on and inte- in the construction of the present taxonomy.
grates previous work by many authors. In addi- There are other team interaction classification
tion. Table 1 cross-references our ten dimen- systems in which member utterances are sorted
2001 Marks, Mathieu, and Zaccaro 363

TABLE 1
Taxonomy of Team Processes
Process Dimensions Definition Previous Research on Tecon Processes
Transition processes
Mission analysis Interpretation and evaluation of the team's mission, Fleishman & Zaccaro (1992): Prince &
formulation and including identification of its main tasks as well as the Salas (1993)
planning operative environmental conditions and team resources
available for mission execution
Goal specification Identification and prioritization of goals and subgoals for Dickinson & Mclntyre (1997); Levins &
mission accomplishment Moreland (1990): O'Leary-Kelly,
Martocchio, & Frink (1994): Prussia &
Kinicki (1996); Saavedra, Early, & van
dyne (1993)
Strategy Development of altemative courses of action for mission Carmon-Bowers, Tannenbaum, Salas, &
formulation accomplishment Volpe (1995); Gladstein (1984);
Hackman (1983); Hackman & Oldham
(1980); Prince & Salas (1993); Stout,
Cannon-Bowers, Salas, & Milanovich
(1999); Weldon, lehn, & Pradhan (1991)

Action processes
Monitoring Tracking task and progress toward mission Cannon-Bowers, Tannenbaum, Salas, &
progress toward accomplishment, interpreting system information in Volpe (1995); Jentsch, Bamett, Bowers,
goals terms of what needs to be accomplished for goal & Salas (1999)
attainment, and transmitting progress to team members
Systems Tracking team resources and environmental conditions as Fleishman & Zaccaro (1992)
monitoring they relate to mission accomplishment, which involves
(1) intemal systems monitoring (tracking team resources
such as personnel, equipment, and other information
that is generated or contained within the team), and (2)
environmental monitoring (tracking the environmental
conditions relevant to the team)
Team monitoring Assisting team members to perform their tasks. Assistance Dickinson & Mclntyre (1997)
and backup may occur by (1) providing a teammate verbal feedback
behavior or coaching, (2) helping a teammate behaviorally in
carrying out actions, or (3) assuming and completing a
task for a teammate
Coordination Orchestrating the sequence and timing oi interdependent Brannick, Prince, Prince, & Salas (1992);
actions : -'• '••• - ••-•:;: Brannick, Roach, & Salas (1993);
Fleishman & Zaccaro (1992); Zalesny,
Salas, & Prince (1995)

Interpersonal processes
Conflict Preemptive conflict management involves establishing Carmon-Bowers, Tannenbaum, Salas, &
management conditions to prevent, control, or guide team conflict Volpe (1995); Gladstein (1984); Jehn
before it occurs. Reactive conflict management involves (1995); Pace (1990); Simons, Pellad, &
working through task and interpersonal disagreements Smith (1999); Simons & Peterson (2000);
among team members Smolek, Hoffman, & Moran (1999);
Tjosvold (1985); Van de Vliert, Euwema,
& Huismans (1995)
Motivation and Generating and preserving a sense of collective Fleishman & Zaccaro (1992)
confidence confidence, motivation, and task-based cohesion with
building regard to mission accomplishment
Affect Regulating member emotions during mission Carmon-Bowers, Tannenbaum, Salas, &
management accomplishment, including (but not limited to) social Volpe (1995)
cohesion, frustration, and excitement
364 Academy of Management Review July

FIGURE 2
Manifestation of Processes in Transition and Action Phases
Transition phase Action phase

Mission analysis

Goal specification

I
Strategy formulation and planning

Monitoring progress toward goals

1
Systems monitoring

Team monitoring and backup

Coordination

Conflict management

Motivating and confidence building

Affect management

into various categories: the interaction process episode. For instance, the process of planning
analysis (IPA; Bales, 1950), the system of multi- often requires team decision making and de-
ple-level observation of groups (SYMLOG; tailed preparation of team member actions for
Bales, 1980), and time-by-event-by-member pat- goal accomplishment.
tern observation (TEMPO; Futoran, Kelly, & • '-11

McGrath, 1989). Coding systems such as IPA,


SYMLOG, and TEMPO classify statements that Transition Phase Processes
team members make while communicating, but As mentioned earlier, transition phases are
they fall short in identifying the processes that periods of time when teams focus primarily on
teams engage in during task performance. Be- evaluation and/or planning activities to guide
cause category membership is determined by their accomplishment of a team goal or objec-
the meaning of single member statements, there tive. The processes of mission analysis, goal
is no synthesis of the verbal interaction among specification, and strategy formulation and
team members to understand the processes that planning typically occur during time set aside
occur. Detecting processes often requires a more for analysis, evaluation, and future direction
macro observation of the verbal exchanges and (e.g., staff meetings, retreats, after-action re-
behaviors that take place during a particular views).
2001 Marks, Mathieu, and Zaccaro 365

Mission analysis. Mission analysis is the in- the day if weather conditions change or equip-
teipietation and evaluation of the team's mis- ment becomes unavailable (Tesluk & Mathieu,
sion, including identification of its main tasks as 1999).
well as the operative environmental conditions Whereas effective goal specification leads to
and team iesources available for mission execu- challenging, attainable goals that are aligned
tion. The process of interpreting a mission with the larger organizational vision and with
within the given performance context occurs collective strategies, ineffective goal specifica-
cognitively, as team members interpret their tion has debilitating effects on collective perfor-
charge within the boundaries of team abilities, mance. Poorly conceptualized goals may be
resources, and time constraints. This process overly general, vague, conflicting, ambiguous,
also includes verbal discussion, to ensure that unattainable, impractical, or not valued by team
all members have a shared vision of the team's members. These types of goals do not stimulate
purpose and objectives. Mission analysis blends effective strategies, timelines, and collective ac-
two foci: backward evaluation and forward vi- tivities for effective performance. A team that
sioning. The backward visioning aspect in- disregards the goal specification stage entirely
cludes diagnosing previous performance and ends up with no shared understanding of the
interpreting the causes of success and failure. team's purpose.
Previous research has revealed that to the ex- Strategy formulation and planning. Strategy
tent that teams better understand the underly- formulation and planning refer to the develop-
ing causes of previous performance, they can ment of alternative courses of action for mission
better prepare for future efforts (Blickensderfer, accomplishment. This involves decision making
Cannon-Bowers, & Salas, 1997). The forward vi- about how team members will go about achiev-
sioning aspect of mission analysis concerns ing their missions, discussion of expectations,
how the team interprets its charge for the future relay of task-related information, prioritization,
in the context of ongoing events. Teams that fail role assignment, and the communication of
to conduct thorough mission analyses will be plans to all team members (Hackman & Oldham,
undermined by changing circumstances or rele- 1980; Stout, Cannon-Bowers, Salas, & Milano-
gated to operating in a purely reactive mode. vich, 1999). Good strategy development includes
Worse yet, teams that abbreviate or omit mis- consideration of situational and time con-
sion analysis activities run the risk of misguid- straints, team resources, member expertise, and
ing their attention and efforts until it is too late the changing nature of the environment. The
to recover (Gersick, 1988). resulting strategies contain information about
Goal specification. Goal specification refers member roles and responsibilities, the order
to the identification and prioiitization of goals and timing of actions, and how task-related ac-
and subgoals for mission accomplishment. This tivities should be executed. Poor strategy devel-
is the process that teams go through to develop opment occurs when teams are unable to de-
and assign overall mission goals and subgoals velop plans for successful goal accomplishment.
that indicate what and how much must be ac- This results in ineffective strategies (if there are
complished by a specified time and within cer- strategies in place at all) that force teams to rely
tain quality standards. For instance, a snow re- completely on past experience or improvise as
moval team might set a goal of plowing 100 they perform, which can be exceedingly difficult
for complex and novel tasks.
percent of the highways and 50 percent of the
neighborhood streets in the county by the end of We have further classified the strategy and
the day. This process usually occurs during tran- planning dimension into three subdimensions:
sition phases and in conjunction with mission (1) deliberate planning, (2) contingency plan-
analysis and strategy development. Ideally, ning, and (3) reactive strategy adjustment. De-
goals are aligned with strategies, and timelines liberate planning refers to the formulation and
are associated with mission accomplishment. transmission of a principal course of action fox
However, goals might also have to be specified mission accomplishment. This is a chief activity
(or respecified) during action phases because of of transition periods occurring at the beginning
a team's inability to fully anticipate all situa- of episodes. For example, hotel catering teams
tional contingencies. For example, a snow re- meet each afternoon to develop a strategy that
moval team may have to respecify its goals for details the order in which next-day events will
366 Academy ol Management Review July

be handled, as well as the member assignments Either because of unpredictable situations or


for each catering job. These decisions are based faulty original plans, teams should have the
on the information that is currently available, ability to change strategies during action peri-
including event size, time and complexity, mem- ods. This process results in new rules for collec-
ber capabilities, event importance, and location. tive goal attainment in the current performance
It is a premeditated process (Weldon, 1998) and context. For example, a new and swiftly trans-
is consistent with the conceptualization of plan- ferable computer virus can cause an organiza-
ning and strategy development most commonly tion's computer support team to restrategize im-
found in the team literature. mediately. Viruses are unpredictable and
Contingency planning refers to the a priori demand immediate attention, requiring the
formulation and transmission of alternative computer service team to change foci immedi-
ately, based on a newly emerging set of contex-
plans and strategy adjustments in response to
tual parameters. Unlike primary strategizing or
anticipated changes in the performance envi-
contingency planning, reactive strategy adjust-
ronment. We differentiate contingency planning
ment occurs during action phases, when teams
from deliberate planning to highlight the need
must adapt to unexpected and abrupt environ-
to prepare ahead of time for anticipated chang-
mental changes (Meyer, 1982; Weldon, 1998) or to
ing events. This includes specifying alternative
emerging feedback that a current strategy is
courses of action and rules that teams will use
ineffective. '^ "' ^ ' . • ; " •.'"- "
at the appropriate time if/when needed. For in-
stance, catering teams may devise a contin-
gency plan for action that would take effect if an Action Phase Processes
event should fall behind schedule, or if there is
Action phases are periods of time when teams
risk of last-minute cancellation for a particular
conduct activities leading directly to goal ac-
event. Good contingency planning rests on "if/
complishment. In the sections below we discuss
then logic" tied specifically to various "trigger
four process dimensions that occur most com-
events." The need to have a contingency plan
monly during action phases: monitoring
increases for teams that perform in dynamic and
progress toward goals, systems monitoring,
unpredictable situations. They are the formation
team monitoring and backup responses, and co-
of "Plan Bs" that range from small tactical shifts ordination activities.
to the development of entire alternative strate-
Monitoring progress toward goals. Monitoring
gies for goal accomplishment. Importantly, con- progress toward goals is defined as tracking
tingency planning is primarily a transition pe- task and progress toward mission accomplish-
riod activity in which alternative courses of ment, interpreting system information in terms
action are laid out a priori and pursued on the of what needs to be accomplished for goal at-
basis of intermediate outcomes (i.e., the trigger tainment, and transmitting progress to team
events). members. This involves providing feedback to
When unexpected events occur during the ac- the team on its goal accomplishment status so
tion phases, or when errors are detected in a that members can determine their progress and
team's initial strategy, an unforeseen need their likelihood of success within a given period
emerges for strategic change. Reactive strategy of time. Teams assess in real time the discrep-
adjustment is the alteration of existing strategy ancies between their goals and their current
or plans in response to unanticipated changes in situation (Austin & Vancouver, 1996). This func-
the performance environment and/or perfor- tions as a means of self-regulation by alerting
mance feedback. In effect, it signals the initia- teams when performance gaps emerge or when
tion of a transitory subepisode evoked to redi- they veer off in a different direction. For exam-
rect a team's activity. When a team encounters a ple, product development teams will monitor
change in the performance environment for how well they are progressing on a given task to
which neither the original strategy nor the con- determine whether to work overtime, call in ad-
tingency plan appears appropriate, it has to in- ditional help, or adjust their strategies or goals.
vent a new plan. An effective team decides "on Monitoring goals includes not only detecting
the fly" to reconsider, abandon, or adjust the progress but transmitting that progress to team
original plan. members. Statements about goal progress con-
2001 Maiks, Mathieu, and Zaccaro 367

tain information about how well the team has other team members so that decisions about ap-
implemented its task strategy, as well as sug- propriate responses can take place. Likewise,
gestions for how the team should alter its goals, pilot crews rely on extensive panels of instru-
plans, activities, or effort level in order to in- mentation that must be checked regularly to de-
crease effectiveness or avoid problems (Gaddy tect flight problems.
& Wachtel, 1992; Weidon et al., 1991). Poor goal Teams that work in less dynamic environ-
monitoring occurs when teams drift, procrasti- ments may set aside specific time periods for
nate, or stray off task and lose track of their monitoring internal and external environments
purpose for extensive periods of time. In addi- (e.g., surveys to assess buyer markets, weekly
tion, teams that are unaware of their progress meetings to review resource allocations for
cannot provide themselves with appropriate projects). Poor systems monitoring is evidenced
performance feedback (e.g., speed up, slow in some teams by erroneous interpretation of
down, get back on track, locate more resources). critical internal and environmental elements.
Monitoring goals occurs with varying frequency, For example, failure to attend to a weather
depending on the nature of the team. Teams that storm warning might leave construction teams
work in fast-paced environments (e.g., military exposed to dangerous elements.
command and control, firefighters) monitor their Team monitoring and backup responses.
progress frequently during action phases. For Team monitoring and backup is defined as as-
other teams, progress monitoring occurs more sisting team members to perform their tasks,
periodically, sometimes taking place during which may occur by (1) providing a teammate
transition periods that are used specifically for verbal feedback or coaching, (2) assisting a
intermittent progress reports. teammate behaviorally in carrying out actions,
Systems monitoring. Systems monitoring re- or (3) assuming and completing a task for a
fers to tracking team resources and environmen- teammate. This dimension includes the provi-
tal conditions as they relate to mission accom- sion of feedback and task-related support and
plishment; it involves (1) internal systems the seeking of help from teammates when nec-
monitoring, tracking team resources such as per- essary. For team monitoring and backup to oc-
sonnel, equipment, and other information that is cur effectively, teammates need to be informed
generated or contained within the team, and of others' role assignments in order to identify
(2) environmental monitoring, tracking the envi- what type of assistance is required at a partic-
ronmental conditions relevant to the team. Effec- ular time. Often, team members watch out for
tive teams manage their environments, both in- one another, render assistance when required,
ternal and external to the teams themselves, by and warn of possible problems or dangerous
observing changes that occur as they perform. circumstances. For instance, in an airplane
They do this by monitoring critical information cockpit, copilots must stay abreast of the pilot's
internal (e.g., equipment, resources) and exter- actions in order to detect or compensate for crit-
nal (e.g., constituents, weather patterns, econ- ical lapses in judgment or oversight. A failure to
omy, news events) to the team. This process is monitor teammates and to provide backup ren-
similar to what some have referred to as "situ- ders the entire team susceptible to a single
ational assessment," in the sense that the pro- shortcoming. If teammates are not looking out
cess of monitoring critical internal and external for, or willing to help out, each other, the team
systems, along with the effective communica- will fail when any one member fails.
tion of this information among team members, Team monitoring is primarily a cognitive op-
leads to situational awareness Gentsch et al., eration in which team members observe the ac-
1999). tions of their teammates and watch for errors or
Effective teams that work in dynamic environ- performance discrepancies. When a team mem-
ments continually monitor systems. Moreover, ber identifies the need to provide help, backup
they rely heavily on technology (e.g., machines, behavior in the form of suggestive or corrective
displays, counters) to facilitate this process. For feedback (verbal and/or behavioral) is provided
example, an operating room team keeps con- to assist the team member and get performance
stant watch of the heart-monitoring machine back on track (Dickinson & Mclntyre, 1997).
during open heart surgery. Any changes on the Coordination activities. We define coordina-
monitor are detected and then communicated to tion activities as fhe process of orchestrating the
368 Academy of Management Beview Iuly

sequence and timing of interdependent actions. teams, is a function of the conflict management
This refers to the management of synchronous process, which involves how the team handles
and/or simultaneous activities, and involves in- conflict situations that have arisen or have the
formation exchange and mutual adjustment of potential to arise. Research on conflict resolu-
action (Brannick et al., 1993) in order to align the tion targets what we refer to as "reactive conflict
pace and sequencing of team member contribu- management." This involves techniques for re-
tions with goal accomplishment. This feature of ducing or facilitating conflict that has emerged
teamwork is closely intertwined with the task- during the team's performance cycle. Some tech-
work required of the team. Coordination often niques for reactive conflict management include
occurs during action phases (e.g., telecommuni- identification of the parameters of conflict be-
cations service teams working interdependently tween team members (Pace, 1990), problem solv-
during a new community installation) but also ing, compromising, openness and flexibility,
during transition phases (e.g., integrating work and willingness to accept differences of opin-
schedule demands with inventories, technician ions.
availability, and customer service mandates). Preemptive conflict management focuses spe-
The more interdependent the tasks, the more cifically on reducing or controlling the nature of
teams rely on coordination as a central process team conflict before it occurs. The establishment
for effective functioning (Tesluk, Mathieu, Zac- of norms for cooperative rather than competitive
caro, & Marks, 1997). Teams experiencing "com- approaches to conflict resolution (Tjosvold,
munication breakdowns" and those that get "out 1985), team contracts or charters that specify a
of sync" are likely to be experiencing problems priori how team members agree to handle diffi-
with their coordination process. This is what cult situations (Smolek et al., 1999), and the de-
Steiner (1972) refers to as "process loss" due to velopment of team rules and norms about the
coordination, when teams fall below their esti- nature and timing of conflict may be vehicles for
mated productivity level. curtailing the destructive aspects of conflict be-
fore they occur.
Motivating/confidence building. Motivating
Interpersonal Processes
and confidence building involve generating and
The final three dimensions—conflict manage- preserving a sense of collective confidence, mo-
ment, motivating/confidence building, and af- tivation, and task-based cohesion with regard to
fect management—represent processes teams mission accomplishment. This includes encour-
use to manage interpersonal relationships. We aging team members to perform better or to
note that interpersonal processes occur through- maintain high levels of performance. Teams mo-
out both transition and action phases, and typ- tivate members by communicating their beliefs
ically lay the foundation for the effectiveness of about team ability (e.g., pep talks), competence
other processes. We describe processes that on particular tasks, and feedback on team suc-
govern interpersonal activities, rather than the cess. They may also rely on imagery or model-
emergent states that often emanate from such ing techniques to illustrate the capabilities that
experiences. teams like themselves have for particular situ-
Conflict management. Working in teams pro- ations.
vides an interpersonal context in which conflicts Interestingly, we have often observed teams
may occur and attempts to manage them are act in ways that are demotivational to team
made (Jehn, 1995). We have defined two types of members. Negative comments about the team's
conflict management processes that can be (lack of) competence or that of individual mem-
used to resolve or minimize conflict: (1) preemp- bers can reduce confidence levels and task co-
tive conflict management involves establishing hesiveness. Just as teams can enhance working
conditions to prevent, control, or guide team con- relationships and performance by boosting their
flict before it occurs, and (2) reactive conflict confidence level, so, too, they can hamper them
management involves working through task, by deflating themselves. Debilitating team pro-
process, and interpersonal disagreements cesses can spiral teams into a vicious cycle that
among team membeis. We believe the degree to drags down both team confidence and perfor-
which conflict emerges, and eventually inter- mance over time (Lindsley, Brass, & Thomas,
feres with (or enhances) the productivity of work 1994). In addition, processes such as social loaf-
2Q01 Marks, Mathieu, and Zaccaro 369

ing (Latane, Williams, & Harkins, 1979) and three higher-level categories. It is also aligned
shirking Qones, 1984) typically occur when low with the recurring phase model introduced ear-
motivation levels reduce the amount of effort lier in the article, in that two of the three super-
expended by members on the team task, thus ordinate categories (i.e., transition and action
lowering collective performance. processes) correspond directly with the phases
Affect management. Affect management in- of performance episodes. As we have stated, the
volves regulating member emotions during mis- lower-order dimensions within each of these
sion accomplishment, including (but not limited categories may occur at any time, although they
to) social cohesion, frustration, and excitement. are more frequent in their respective phases.
It refers to the process of calibrating team mem- Interpersonal processes occur throughout epi-
ber emotional levels (George, 1990), which can sodes with regularity and are the kinds of fac-
be inflated (or deflated) because of task condi- tors that can rally or derail teams at any point.
tions (e.g., failure, temporal stress), personal fac- For example, the ability to garner resources,
tors (e.g., animosity among members), or situa- to pull together, and to squelch petty conflicts
tional factors (e.g., job security concerns). may well enable a team to rise to an occasion
Techniques involved in regulating emotions and to reach performance levels well beyond
may involve attempts to calm members down, what a composite of their knowledge, skills, and
control frustration levels, boost team morale and abilities would predict they could achieve. Al-
cohesiveness among members, and provide em- ternatively, breakdowns in interpersonal rela-
pathy. A telecommunications service and repair tions can subvert even the best-organized strat-
team deals frequently with stress-producing sit- egy development session, or can lead to a
uations, such as dealing with irate customers failure to monitor and back up teammates or to
and making weather-induced emergency re- other types of coordination breakdowns.
pairs. The team can effectively handle the situ- Whereas the transition and action phase pro-
ation by actively working to calm down frus- cesses have a natural temporal rhythm and re-
trated members or by sending in another person lationship to one another, the interpersonal pro-
to deal with the angry customer. In contrast, the cesses can work as an attribute or liability
team could ineffectively manage member affect throughout goal accomplishment episodes.
by ignoring, isolating, or fueling angry team-
mates.
FUTURE IMPUCATIONS
Traditional team-building interventions have
targeted affect management by focusing on the Teams traditionally have been thought of as
regulation of team member emotions. For exam- pursuing one task at a time to reach a single,
ple, traditional T groups put members into con- collective goal. However, the primary argu-
frontational environments to deal with inter or ments of the framework and taxonomy we ad-
intrapersonal issues (Patten, 1981). Exercises vance here are that most teams work on multi-
have been developed to manage the affect gen- ple goals simultaneously and engage in
erated from team conflict (Harrison, 1983) and to multitask processing. The idea that teams per-
improve relations among team members (Bech- form in recurring transition and action phases,
hard, 1983). Team activities such as joking, and that they use different^jrocesses during dif-
relaxing, and complaining may also be consid- ferent points in time, challenges the way we
ered forms of affect management, if imple- have been thinking about team effectiveness.
mented in a manner that builds cohesion, Consistent with Zaheer et al.'s (1999) call for
breaks tension, vents frustration, or manages more attention on time in the study of organiza-
stressful situations. However, it is also possible tional issues, we believe that researchers and
that such activities, if managed ineffectively, practitioners should consider a team's temporal
may lead to increased negative affect, wasted rhythms in measurements and evaluations of
time, and performance problems. teamwork processes and effectiveness. A team
task analysis is a necessary first step (Bowers et
al., 1994) in exposing the temporal rhythms of
Summary of Taxonomy team performance episodes, as well as the mul-
This taxonomy offers a two-tiered classifica- titasking activities that teams pursue. In line
tion system that arranges ten processes into with McGrath's (1991) point about how teams
370 Academy of Management Review July

simultaneously manage multiple bundles of ac- opment, and environmental monitoring, be-
tivities over time, researchers should think cause these processes help teams to better un-
about how teams allocate resources to multiple derstand both challenging and unstable
tasks over performance episodes. performance situations.
In the following sections we discuss more spe- However, researchers who are primarily inter-
cifically the implications of the team process ested in predicting team effectiveness defined
framework and taxonomy for future process as team longevity or satisfaction (Hackman &
measurement, new research, and practice. Morris, 1975) should consider assessing interper-
sonal processes. Interpersonal processes are
more likely to influence team cohesion over
Process Taxonomy As a Guide for time, which is a primary antecedent of team
Process Measurement longevity and satisfaction. Thus, investigators
We hope researchers will use the framework who want to explain team turnover rates, team
and taxonomy to further refine future research commitment, affect, efficacy, and satisfaction
on team processes. Specifically, this work is in- might target such processes as conflict and af-
tended to shape future conceptualization of both fect management and confidence building.
the scope and boundaries of team process, as The ten-dimension taxonomy also implies
well as to serve as a guide for measuring pro- that gaining a more complete understanding of
cess constructs in forthcoming studies of team how processes contribute to team effectiveness
effectiveness. Our hope is that this work will necessitates the measurement of more than one
provide researchers not only with guidance in process variable. However, we recognize that
the selection of appropriate process variables measuring ten process dimensions is not often
but with thoughts about how and when to mea- practical or necessary. When the research goal
sure them so that we can learn not only what but is to examine a broad range of processes, we
when team processes influence team effective- suggest representing each superordinate cate-
ness. The framework and taxonomy can help gory (transition, action, and interpersonal) with
researchers with three critical issues that arise a process dimension most relevant to the re-
when planning studies to capture teamwork search context. When the research goal is a tar-
processes. geted focus on a certain type of teamwork, mea-
(1) What teamwork processes should be as- suring one or more dimensions within a
sessed? When the goal is to predict team effec- superordinate category is recommended. Re-
tiveness defined as performance quality and ef- searchers should evaluate the particular
ficiency, transition and action processes should tradeoffs between depth and breadth of process
be targeted, because they have the greatest po- measurement versus the added complexity in
tential to impact the rate and caliber of task- research time and measurement. The taxonomy
work. Researchers interested in predicting team can serve as a good starting point for consider-
outcomes such as product development time ation of what processes are most critical, sa-
and quality, decision accuracy, response time, lient, or challenging in a particular environ-
customer service quality, amount of sales, or ment.
manufacturing errors might consider assessing (2) What measurement strategy should be
such transition processes as goal specification employed? There is a variety of strategies avail-
and strategy formulation, as well as action pro- able for process assessment (see Weingart, 1997,
cesses like monitoring progress toward goals. for an excellent review). Studies in which re-
Those studying team tasks that require high searchers have examined multiple processes
member interdependency could measure pro- typically have involved use of a single measure-
cesses such as coordination and team monitor- ment strategy (e.g.. Campion, Medsker, & Higgs,
ing and backup behavior (Tesluk et al., 1997), 1993; Hyatt & Ruddy, 1997), either survey meth-
because they describe techniques that team odology or behavioral observation. However,
members use to synchronize each other's activ- certain measurement strategies are more appro-
ities. Those interested in teams that operate priate for some processes than others. For exam-
within complex, dynamic, and unpredictable en- ple, processes such as strategy formulation,
vironments might choose to focus more on such goal specification, coordination, and backup are
dimensions as mission analysis, strategy devel- generally observable, lending themselves to the
2001 Marks, Mathieu, and Zaccaro 371

use of observer ratings. Mission analysis and er than systems monitoring). Making the same
environmental monitoring may be less observ- observations during start-up or shut-down oper-
able, so techniques such as in-baskets, inter- ations, however, would be most illuminating.
views, and self-report questionnaires may pro- This type of approach will enable researchers
vide alternate methods to examine these to answer questions about when teamwork is
processes. We encourage researchers interested most critical for team effectiveness, which then
in assessing multiple processes within the con- has significant implications for when team
text of a single study to consider (when feasible) training or other interventions should be intro-
employing multiple measurement techniques duced.
that best align with the dimensions of interest In the laboratory environment, team tasks can
(see Tesluk et al., 1997). be designed to unfold over time and to elicit
In addition, researchers should consider less different processes by embedding trigger events
traditional and more creative methods of captur- in the task environment (e.g., the TARGETS
ing teamwork processes that would be sensitive methodology; see Dwyer, Fowlkes, Oser, Salas,
to time-based dynamics. Williams and Alliger & Lane, 1997). For example, if a researcher uses
(1994) employed a design in the study of affec- a three-month-long team business simulation to
tive state-behavior relationships in which they study the relationship among strategy develop-
used daily diaries and behavioral checklists. In ment, environmental monitoring, and corporate
this way, employees could be asked to write financial performance, he or she should con-
teamwork process information in journals either sider when strategies and monitoring are most
daily or when critical teamwork events oc- likely to occur and target measurement at that
curred. Team researchers could supplement tra- point. An experimental platform could be de-
ditional survey and observational data collec- signed to place a premium on strategy develop-
tion designs with handheld computers, ment within the first week of performance, fol-
videotapes, archival measures (e.g., e-mail and lowed by pivotal environmental instability
videoconferencing records, performance traces, (requiring monitoring) in weeks four and seven.
and so forth), and other methods to collect real- Strategy development should be measured dur-
time data. The point is that not all process di- ing or following week one, and monitoring dur-
mensions should necessarily be targeted, and ing weeks four and seven.
different ones may be better assessed using dif- In sum, our recommendations go beyond the
ferent measurement techniques (see Tesluk et typical lament for longitudinal research and use
al., 1997, for a detailed outline of team process of multiple sources of measurement. Yes, longi-
measurement strategies). tudinal designs are important, but it is not suf-
(3) When should team processes be assessed? ficient to simply collect all measures of interest
The recurring phase model introduced earlier periodically, say quarterly, via a survey. We ar-
depicts team performance as composed of a se- gue that particular types of measures should be
ries of episodes, each containing cyclical I-P-O gathered at appropriate times and using mea-
linkages. Currently, we rely on single or aggre- sures that are most suitable for the nature of the
gate measurement of teamwork to show rela- construct(s) being examined—all based on the
tionships between overall teamwork quality (or knowledge garnered from a time-sensitive team
quantity) and performance. Longitudinal and task analysis. In practice, such a strategy is
time sampling approaches that align team pro- likely to offer the greatest payoff in terms of
cess measurement with performance episodes insight gained from the efforts invested in data
(Zaheer et al., 1999) will permit an examination collection.
of how teams work together as they move
through different phases of goal accomplish-
Process Framework and Taxonomy AB a Basis
ment. For example, teamwork such as affective
for Practical Applications
management is absolutely critical for nuclear
power plant control teams during planned and The heavy reliance on teams in applied set-
unexpected shutdowns (Gaddy & Wachtel, 1992). tings has created an increased need for tools
Observation of a nuclear operating crew's team- and interventions targeted specifically at opti-
work processes during normal operations would mizing team effectiveness (Stevens & Campion,
probably show very little teamwork activity (oth- 1994). The conceptual framework and taxonomy
372 Academy of Managemenf Review July

introduced here have implications for the struc- be provided with conflict management skills.
turing of teams, a s well a s the design and ap- Other teams that depend heavily on backup be-
plication of h u m a n resource management sys- havior can receive focused training in team
tems. Primarily, they bring to the forefront the monitoring and coaching behavior.
importance of considering how time impacts A time-sensitive team task analysis, used in
teamwork and that various teamwork processes conjunction with the framework presented here,
a r e more a n d less likely to occur at different should also provide concrete prescriptions of
points in the performance cycle. Thus, a logical when feedback should be available to teams at
first step is to understand a team's temporal different points in time. For example, feedback
rhythms a n d episodes a n d then to consider entailing a qualitative shift in team strategy
what, when, a n d how teamwork processes con- (Gersick, 1989) should be presented during tran-
tribute to critical performance outcomes. sition phases, when teamwork is focused mainly
To do so, we suggest conducting a traditional on guiding team efforts through strategy formu-
team-level task analysis (Bowers et al., 1994) lation, mission analysis, and goal specification.
e m b e d d e d within a temporal framework in or- In transition periods, teams are more prepared
der to decipher team performance rhythms crit- to consider changing environmental influences,
ical to goal accomplishment within a particular revisit plans, and realign goals associated with
team context, noting what behaviors and activ- adopting new strategies. However, feedback
ities occur together during a single performance containing new strategies introduced in the
episode—in other words, tracking how teams middle of action phases runs the risk of derail-
work together over time to obtain collective ing ongoing goal accomplishment processes.
goals. For example, a time-sensitive team task Action-phase feedback should pertain largely to
a n a l y s i s carried out for a certain company's re- monitoring team progress, systems, members,
tail sales t e a m s might detail planning meetings and coordination. In general, team-level feed-
that occur primarily at the beginning of each back should be delivered based on the type of
month, followed by periods of active customer information necessary at different points in the
site visits a n d weekly progress and coordination team performance cycle and the types of team-
meetings. The focus of the planning meetings is work processes that occur during these times.
to develop goals, to analyze particular facets of Selection, placement, and training. Several re-
the environment that help them (e.g., strong searchers have advocated that team member
economy) or hinder them (e.g., competition), and selection, placement, and training be based on
then to develop appropriate strategies to meet a set of team competencies (Cannon-Bowers et
those goals. Customer site visits require coordi- al., 1995) or KSAs (Stevens & Campion, 1994) nec-
nation, monitoring each other's customer loads, essary for effective teamwork. The framework
a n d affect management when sales are not go- and taxonomy support the idea that rather than
ing well. This information, used alongside the looking for uniform teamwork skills among all
taxonomy, gives rise to a number of practical members, employers should be asking what
applications regarding team feedback, perfor- teamwork skills are necessary at what stages of
mance appraisal, staffing, and training. the team performance cycle. In other words, how
Team appraisals and feedback. The taxonomy can teams align their KSAs with teamwork skills
could also be used to conduct feam process ap- necessary at different points in time? Some in-
praisals, where particular teams would be eval- dividuals may be most suited for managing the
uated on their ability to conduct each of the strategy development process, whereas others
processes identified as critical by a team task may be more talented for action-phase team-
analysis. The resulting team process profile work activities. Along these lines, people with
would delineate teamwork strengths and weak- particular KSAs may be chosen for a particular
nesses. This information could then be used to team setting because they are capable of lead-
provide teams with customized feedback and ing that team through transition processes,
interventions, as well as to identify specific de- whereas others might be selected for their abil-
velopmental and training needs. For example, ity to contribute to action or interpersonal pro-
rather than rely on generic team-building inter- cesses.
ventions as a universal solution for all process- For instance, we often hear of the superior
related problems, teams in constant discord can "clubhouse presence" (e.g., affect management.
2001 Marks, Mathieu, and Zaccaro 373

ability to motivate) of certain players to a base- order process dimensions that map onto three
ball team's overall effectiveness. Teamwork pro- higher-order categories. This content domain is
files that delineate the range of processes that embedded within the context of a time-based
occur across different episodes in the team goal framework that stipulates the role of process in
accomplishment cycle open the door to a host of team effectiveness. By introducing both a frame-
tailored training solutions for teamwork issues. work and related taxonomy of team processes,
These solutions range from team-building inter- we aim to clarify current confusion in the liter-
ventions designed to foster interpersonal ature on the conceptualization and delineation
processes to the use of technology (e.g., online of process variables. Additionally, we hope to
displays, communication software) that is im- spark a more systematic and comprehensive ap-
plemented to remedy or enhance the teamwork proach to the measurement of team processes.
needs of particular teams in context (Bikson, The result should be a better understanding of
Cohen, & Mankin, 1999). teamwork that contributes to the development of
Further, consideration of the surrounding con- more effective teams in organizations.
text in which teams perform uncovers a new set
of process taxonomy applications. For example,
teams that strategize in relatively stable and
REFERENCES
predictable environments should be taught for-
mal planning skills, whereas those that perform Austin. I. T., & Vancouver, J. B. 1996. Goal constructs in
in turbulent and unpredictable environments psychology: Structure, process, and content. Psycholog-
ical Bulletin, 120: 338-375.
should receive strategy training that empha-
sizes problem diagnosis and impromptu strat- Bales. R. F. 1950. Interaction process analysis: A method lor
the study of small groups. Cambridge, MA: Addison-
egy adjustment. Wesley.
In sum, our framework emphasizes two often-
Bales, R. F. 1980. SYMLOG case study kit. New York: Free
practiced strategies and also illustrates why Press.
they are effective. First, creating a well-
Barry, B., & Stewart, G. L. 1997. Composition, process, and
balanced constellation of KSAs in the team, performance in self-managed groups: The role of per-
whether accomplished through selection, place- sonality. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82: 62-78.
ment, and/or training, creates an enabling con- Bechhard, R. 1983. Optimizing team building efforts. In W. L.
dition whereby human resources are available French, V. Bell, Ir., & R. A. Zawacki (Eds.), OrganiiaHonal
for the varied tasks at hand. Second, the move development: Theoiy. research, and practice: 152-157.
toward self-directed or empowered teams sug- Piano, TX: Business Publications.
gests that the members themselves are perhaps Bikson, T. K., Cohen, S. G., & Mankin, D. 1999. Information
best positioned to align those talents to the technology and high performance teams. In E. Sund-
changing requirement dynamics over the course strom & Associates (Eds.), Supporting work team effec-
tiveness: 215-245. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
of episodes. Shifting the orientation from look-
ing for uniformly high "team work skills" to Blickensderfer, E., Cannon-Bowers, J. A., & Salas, E. 1997.
Training teams to self-correct: An empirical investiga-
managing team human resources as a constel- tion. Paper presented at the 12th annual meeting of the
lation of talents should help to enable teams to Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology,
be successful throughout the changing pres- St. Louis.
sures of performance episodes. Bowers, C. A., Baker, D. P., & Salas, E. 1994. Measuring the
importance of teamwork: The reliability and validity of
job/task analysis indices for team-training design. Mil-
Conclusions itary Psychology. 6: 205-214.
Despite McGrath's (1984b) call for the serious Bowers, C. A., Braun, C. C, & Morgan, B. B., Jr. 1997. Team
workload: Its meaning and measurement. In M. T. Bran-
study of team process, there is still no concep- nick, E. Salas, & C. Prince (Eds.), Team performance and
tual framework of team process, no agreed-upon measurement: Theory, methods, and applications: 85-
definition or set of process dimensions and chal- 108. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
lenges associated with its measurement. Here, Brannick, M. T., Prince, A., Prince, C, & Salas. E. 1992. Team
through the development of a conceptual frame- coordination: Measuring an eiusive construct. Paper pre-
work of team processes as they relate to team sented at the meeting of the Human Factors Society,
effectiveness, we have identified the content do- Atlanta.
main for team process. It includes ten lower- Brannick, M. T., Roach, R. M., & Salas, E. 1993. Understanding
374 Academy of Management Beview Iuly

team performance: A multimethod study. Human Perior- interaction processes, and group performance effective-
mance, 6: 287-308. ness: A review and proposed integration. In L. Berkowitz
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, vol.
Campion, M. A., Medsker, G. J., & Higgs, A. C. 1993. Relations 8: 45-99. New York: Academic Press.
between work group characteristics and effectiveness:
Implications for designing effective work groups. Per- Hackman, J. R., & Oldman, G. R. 1980. Work redesign. Read-
sonnel Psychology, 46: 823-850. ing, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Cannon-Bowers, I. A., Tannenbaum, S. I., Salas, E., & Volpe, Hackman, R. 1983. A normative model of work team effec-
C. E. 1995. Defining competencies and establishing team tiveness. Technical report No. 2. New Haven, CT: Yale
training requirements. In R. A. Guzzo, E. Salas, & Asso- School of Organization and Management.
ciates (Eds.), Team ellectiveness and decision maJcing in Harrison, R. 1983. When power conflicts trigger team spirit.
organizations.- 333-380. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
In W. L. French, V. Bell, Jr., & R. A. Zawacki (Eds.), Organ-
Cohen, S. G., & Bailey, D. E. 1997. What makes teams work: izationai development: Theory, research, and practice:
Group effectiveness research from the shop floor to the 183-191. Piano, TX: Business Publications.
executive suite. Joumal of Management, 23: 239-290.
Hyatt D. E., & Ruddy, T. M. 1997. An examination of the
Dickinson, T. L., & Mclntyre, R. M. 1997. A conceptual frame- relationship between work group characteristics and
work for teamwork measurement. In M. T. Brannick, performance: Once more into the breech. Personnel Psy-
E. Salas, & C. Prince (Eds.), Team performance and mea- chology. SO: 5b3S85. v-rv. .:;;.;o,..;;j;:;; - • ^ =; .
surement: Theory, methods, and applications: 19-43.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Jehn, K. A. 1995. A multimethod examination of the benefits
and detriments of intragroup conflict. Administrative
Dwyer, D. J., Fowlkes, J. E., Oser, R. L., Salas, E., & Lane, N. E. Science QuarteWy, 40: 256-282.
1997. Team performance measurement in distributed en-
vironments: The TARGETS methodology. In M. T. Bran- Jentsch, F., Barnett, J., Bowers, C. A., & Salas, E. 1999. Who is
nick, E. Salas, & C. Prince (Eds.), Teani per/ormance and flying this plane anyway? What mishaps tell us about
measurement: Theory, methods, and applications: 137- crew member role assignment and air crew situational
153. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. awareness. Human Factors, 41(1): 1-14.
Fleishman, E. A., & Zaccaro, S. J. 1992. Toward a taxonomy of Jones, G. R. 1984. Task visibility, free riding, and shirking:
team performance functions. In R. W. Swezey & E. Salas Explaining the effect of structure and technology on
(Eds.), Teams; Their training and performance: 31-56. employee behavior. Academy of Management Beview,
Norwood, NJ: Ablex. 9: 684-895.
Futoran, G. C, Kelly, J. R., & McGrath, J. E. 1989. TEMPO: A Kelly, J. R., & McGrath, J. E. 1985. Effects of time limits and
time-based system for the analysis of group interaction task types on task performance and interaction of four-
process. Basic and App/ied Social Psychology, 10: 211- person groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
232. chology, 49: 39S-407.
Gaddy, C. D., & Wachlel, J. A. 1992. Team skills in nuclear Kerlinger, F. N. 1986. Foundations of behavioral research.
power plant operations. In R. W. Swezey & E. Salas Fort Worth, TX: Holt, Rinehart 8f Winston.
(Eds.), Teams: Their training and performance: 379-398. Kozlowski, S. W. J., Gully, S. M., Nason, E. R., & Smith, E. M.
Norwood, NJ: Ablex. 1999. Developing adaptive teams: A theory of compila-
George, J. 1990. Personality, affect, and behavior in groups. tion and performance across levels and time. In D. R.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 75: 107-118. Ilgen & E. D. Pulakos (Eds.), The changing nature of work
Gersick, C. J. G. 1988. Time and transition in work teams: performance: Implications for staffing, personnel ac-
Toward a new model of group development. Academy of tions, and development: 240-292. San Francisco: Jossey-
Management Journal, 31: 9-41. Bass.
Gersick, C. J. G. 1989. Marking time: Predictable transitions Latane, B., Williams, K., & Harkins, S. 1979. Many hands
in task groups. Academy of Management Joumal, 32: make light the work: The causes and consequences of
274-309. social loafing. Joumal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 37: 822-832.
Gist, M. E., Locke, E. A., & Taylor, M. S. 1987. Organizational
behavior: Group structure, process, and effectiveness. Levine, J. M., & Moreland, R. L. 1990. Progress in small group
Joumal of Management, 13: 237-257. research. Annual review of psychology, 41: 585-634.
Gladstein, D. 1984. Groups in context: A model of task group Lindsley, D. H., Brass, D. J., & Thomas, J. B. 1994. Efficacy-
effectiveness. Administrative Science Quarterly, 29: performance spirals: A multilevel perspective. Academy
499-517. of Management Joumal, 20: 645-678.
Guzzo, R. A., & Shea. G. P. 1992. Group performance and Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. 1990. A theory of goal setting and
intergroup relations in organizations. In M. D. Dunnette task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
& L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook ol industrial and organ- Mathieu, J. E., & Button, S. B. 1992. An examination of the
izational psychology (2nd ed), vol. 3: 289-313. Palo Alto, relative impact of normative information and self-
CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. efficacy on personal goals and performance over time.
Hackman, J. R., & Morris, C. G. 1975. Group tasks, group Joumal of Applied Social Psychology, 22: 1758-1775.
2001 Marks, Mathieu, and Zaccaro 375

McGrath, J. E. 1984a. Groups: Interaction and peHormance. Stevens, M. J., & Campion, M. A. 1994. The knowledge, skill,
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. and ability requirements for teamwork: Implications for
McGrath, J. E. 1984b. Studying groups at work: Ten critical human resource management. Journal oi Management,
needs for theory and practice. In P. Goodman OEd.), De- 20: 503-S30.
signing effective work groups: 362-391. San Francisco: Stout, R. J., Cannon-Bowers, J. A., Salas, E., & Milanovich,
Jossey-Bass. D. M. 1999. Planning, shared mental models, and coor-
McGrath, J. E. 1991. Time, interaction, and performance (TIP): dinated performance: An empirical link is established.
A theory oi groups. Small Group Research, 22: 147-174. Human Factors, 41(1): 61-71.
McGrath, J. E. 1993. Introduction: The JEMCO workshop- Sundstrom, E. 1999. The challenges of supporting work team
description of a longitudinal study. Small Group fle- effectiveness. In E. Sundstrom & Associates (Eds.), Sup-
search, 24: 285-306. ,;. ;• : •: porting worfc team eiieciiveness: 3-23. San Francisco:
Jossey-Bass.
Meyer, A. D. 1982. Adapting to environmental jolts. Adminis-
trative Science Quarterly, 27: 515-537. Tesluk, P., & Mathieu, J. E. 1999. Overcoming roadblocks to
effectiveness: Incorporating management of perfor-
Mullen, B., & Copper, C. 1994. The relation between group mance barriers into models of work group effectiveness.
cohesiveness and performance: An integration. Journal Journal oi Applied Psychology, 84: 200-217.
of Applied Psychology, 115: 210-227.
Tesluk, P., Mathieu, J. E., Zaccaro, S. J., & Marks, M. A. 1997.
Nieva, V. F., Fleishman, E. A., & Rieck, A. M. 1978. Team Task and aggregation issues in the analysis and as-
dimensions: Their identity, their measurement, and their sessment of team performance. In M. T. Brannick,
relationships. Technical report. Washington, DC: AHRO. E. Salas, & C. Prince (Eds.), Team performance and mea-
O'Leary-Kelly, A. M., Martocchio, J. J., & Frink, D. D. 1994. A surement: Theory, methods, and applications: 197-224.
review of the influence of group goals on group perfor- Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Eribaum Associates.
mance. Academy o/Management/ournai, 37: 1285-1301. Tjosvold, D. 1985. Implications of controversy research for
Pace, R. C. 1990. Personalized and depersonalized conflict in management. Journal oi Management, 11: 21-37.
small group discussions. SmaJ] Group Research, 21: Tuckman, B. W. 1965. Developmental sequence in small
79-98. groups. Psychological Bulletin, 63: 384-389.
Patten, T. H., Jr. 1981. Organizationai development througii Van de Vliert, E., Euwema, M. C & Huismans, S. E. 1995.
teambuilding. New York: Wiley. Managing conflict with a subordinate or superior: Effec-
Prince, C, & Salas, E. 1993. Training research for teamwork tiveness of conglomerated behavior. Journal oi Applied
in the military aircrew. In E. L. Wiener, B. G. Kanki, & Psychology, 80: 271-281.
R. L. Helmreich (Eds.), Cocipit research management: Weingart, L. R. 1997. How did they do that? The ways and
337-366. Orlando, FL: Academic Press. means of studying group process. Research in Organi-
Prussia, G. E., & Kinicki, A. J. 1996. A motivational investiga- zational Behavior, 19: 189-239.
tion of group effectiveness using social-cognitive the- Weldon, E. 1998. Strategy formation in empowered work
ory. Journal oi Applied Psychology, 81: 187-198. groups. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
Saavedra, R., Early, P. C, & van dyne, L. 1993. Complex Academy of Management, San Diego.
interdependence in task performing groups. Journal of Weldon, E., Jehn, K. A., & Pradhan, P. 1991. Processes that
Applied Psychology, 78: 61-72. mediate the relationship between a group goal and
Simons, T. L, Pellad, L. H., Sc Smith, K. A. 1999. Making use of improved group performance. Journal oi Personality and
difference: Diversity, debate, and decision comprehen- Social Psychology, 61: 555-569.
siveness in top management teams. Academy ot Man- Williams, K. J., & Alliger, G. M. 1994. Role stressors, mood
agement Journal 42: 662-673. spillover, and perceptions of work-family conflict in em-
Simons. T. L., & Peterson, R. S. 2000. Task conflict and rela- ployed parents. Academy oi Management Journal, 37:
tionship conflict in top management teams: The pivotal 837-868.
role of intragroup trust. Journal oi Applied Psychology, Zaheer, S., Albert, S., & Zaheer, A. 1999. Time scales and
85: 102-111. organizational theory. Academy oi Management Re-
Smolek, J., Hoffman, D., & Moran, L. 1999. Organizing teams view, 24: 725-741.
for success. In E. Sundstrom (Ed.), Supporting work team Zalesny, M. D., Salas, E., & Prince, C. 1995. Conceptual and
effectiveness: 24-62. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. measurement issues in coordination: Implications for
Steiner, I. D. 1972. Group processes and productivity. New team behavior and performance. Research in Personnel
York: Academic Press. and Human Resources Management, 13: 81-115.

Michelle A. Marks is an assistant professor of psychology at Florida Intemational


University. She received her Ph.D. in industrial/organizational psychology from
George Mason University. Her current research interests focus on team and multiteam
training and effectiveness, workgroup conflict, and mentoring relationships.
John E. Mathieu is a professor of management at the University of Connecticut. He
376 Academy of Management Review July

received his Ph.D. at Old Dominion University and has published over fifty articles
and chapters mostly in the areas of micro- and meso-organizational behavior. His
current research interests include models of training effectiveness, team and mul-
titeam processes, and cross-level models of organizational behavior.
Stephen I. Zaccaro is an associate professor of psychology at George Mason Univer-
sity. He received his Ph.D. in social psychology from the University of Connecticut. He
has numerous publications on work attitudes, leadership, and team effectiveness. His
current research interests include executive leadership, leader development, team
processes, and leader-team relationships.

t.;s.J.^:.- -.'•::-.''<''.[^W:^^:y':^^.

You might also like