Amnesia, Consolidation, and Retrieval: Brooklyn College of The City University of New York

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Psyciwhikal Review

1973, Vol. 80, No. 1, 69-79

AMNESIA, CONSOLIDATION, AND RETRIEVAL 1


RALPH R. MILLER 2 AND ALAN D. SPRINGER
Brooklyn College of the City University of New York

A memory model is proposed in which information enters an initial structural-


chemical format within fractions of a second. Experimental amnesia is
viewed as a failure to retrieve information from long-term memory rather
than a failure of the information to be consolidated in long-term memory.
Consolidation and retrieval failure explanations of experimental amnesia
are found to differ primarily on the issue of recovery of memory. The
nature of experimental amnesia paradigms is such that the consolidation
position can never be supported; it may only be refuted by an occurrence
of recovery of memory or left as an unresolved issue by a lack of recovery.
The use of noncontingent stimuli to induce recovery from amnesia has been
successful in several instances, thereby supporting the retrieval failure hy-
pothesis. Success in producing amnesia for reactivated "old" memories
further corroborates the retrieval position. Arguments that effectively
delivered, high-intensity or high-dosage amnestic agents will disrupt consoli-
dation beg the question and are presently without any empirical support.
Two-process theories proposing both retrieval and consolidation failures are
presently neither necessary nor parsimonious.

Despite the numerous explanations of ex- of operations constituting memory. The


perimental amnesia proposed in recent years following comments are concerned with those
(e.g., Lewis, 1969; McGaugh, 1966; Spe- behavioral deficits that stem explicitly from
vack & Suboski, 1969), a few threads of memory failures as distinct from those defi-
consensus are detectable. Although all of cits caused by changes in performance vari-
the suggested hypotheses do not rely on the ables often seen in (a) studies examining
same model of memory, each of the models long intervals between acquisition and ad-
includes identifiable consolidation processes ministration of amnestic agents, (b*) short
(the establishment of an enduring neurologi- intervals between amnestic agent administra-
cal memory trace, sometimes referred to as tion and retention test, or (c) certain select
storage), retention processes (the passive behavioral tasks (e.g., Chorover & Schiller,
maintenance of this trace, also sometimes 1966; Spevack & Suboski, 1969).
referred to as storage), and retrieval pro-
cesses (the events by which this trace comes CONSOLIDATION
to influence subsequent behavior). All more Experimental amnesia induced by electro-
specifically defined processing functions can convulsive shock, hypothermia, metabolic in-
be subsumed under these three aspects of hibitors (e.g., puromycin, cycloheximide)
memory. Each of these three stages is essen- and various other amnestic agents within a
tial for proper functioning of memory. A retroactive interference paradigm tradition-
memory failure could, in effect, be attributed ally has been attributed to failures of the
to a disruption of any stage in the sequence consolidation process (Glickman, 1961; Mc-
1 Gaugh, 1966). The basic phenomenon cited
Preparation of this article and research re-
ported within were supported by United States
in support of this view is the often reported
Public Health Service Grant MH 19497 and Re- observation that the resultant amnesia is
search Foundation of the City University of New retrograde, that is, memory failure increases
York Grant 1385. as the interval between information input
Thanks are due Anthony Sclafani for his criti- and administration of the amnestic agent
cism of an earlier version of this article.
2
Requests for reprints should be sent to Ralph
decreases.
R. Miller, Department of Psychology, Brooklyn The early models suggesting that a dis-
College of the City University of New York, ruption of consolidation was the source of
Brooklyn, New York 11210. retrograde amnesia held that recently re-
69
70 RALPH R. MILLER AND ALAN D. SPRINGER

ceived information was uniquely encoded in lost. If retrieval is defined as all memory
short-term memory in the form of electro- processes necessary for the utilization of
chemically active, neural reverbatory circuits information already in passive storage, then
that were ultimately terminated by decay a retrieval failure implies that the informa-
and/or intereference processes (e.g., Hebb, tion is present but unavailable on the test
1949). The continued action of these cir- trial. On the basis of these definitions, a
cuits over hours (later minutes or seconds) potential distinction between retrieval and
was believed to give rise to a more perma- consolidation views of experimental amnesia
nent, long-term memory of an energetically is whether or not the memory impairment
passive, chemical-structural format. Elec- is permanent. Some specific models hypoth-
troconvulsive shock and hypothermia were esize further distinctions (e.g., Hebb, 1949;
thought to terminate short-term memory Weiskrantz, 1966), but the issue of perma-
processes before long-term memory could be nence remains primary. The retrieval ex-
established (McGaugh, 1966). Metabolic planation of experimental amnesia would be
inhibitors were believed to block the forma- established by recovering the unavailable
tion of long-term memory during the finite memory in the absence of any subsequent
duration of the relevant information in short- training. On the other hand, the consolida-
term memory (Barondes & Cohen, 1968). tion position could hardly be said to be
In both cases, the consolidation explanation proven by a failure of recovery from amnesia.
of experimental amnesia demands that the The possibility of recovery under untried
induced amnesia be permanent. circumstances always remains. Moreover,
Numerous recent experiments have placed a retrieval failure could theoretically be ir-
in question these explanations and their un- reversible. In the latter case, no behav-
derlying model of memory. For instance, ioral test would suffice to distinguish a re-
information received immediately prior to trieval failure from a consolidation failure,
electroconvulsive shock has been observed and the issue could not be resolved until
to be available for tens of minutes after elec- future technology provided us with tests that
troconvulsive shock (Geller & Jarvik, 1968b; are other than behavioral (e.g., biochemical).
McGaugh & Landfield, 1970; Miller & Clearly, behavioral studies of experimental
Springer, 1971). This suggests that either amnesia are, due to the nature of the para-
electroconvulsive shock fails to destroy all digm, poorly suited for establishing the
information uniquely encoded in short-term validity of consolidation theory.
memory or, alternatively, information enters Most studies have found amnesia to vary
long-term memory far more rapidly than is inversely with the interval between acquisi-
ordinarily assumed. As difficulties with the tion and administration of the amnestic agent
detailed consolidation explanation of the and, given a fixed interval between training
19SO's and early 1960's became apparent, and test trials, to be relatively independent of
many researchers turned to more general the interval between administration of the
models within the consolidation framework amnestic agent and the retention test (after
(e.g., McGaugh, 1968) while others sought correcting for effects of the amnestic agent
explanations of experimental amnesia in dis- on performance variables). These data indi-
ruption of the memory sequence subsequent cate that amnestic agents interfere with time-
to consolidation (e.g., Lewis, 1969). The dependent, memorial processes that are initi-
latter set of explanations is a variation on ated during or soon after acquisition and are
the retrieval failure theme. Few, if any, completed in relatively short order. This
researchers have suggested that retention constraint is consistent with the consolida-
(passive storage) is subject to disruption. tion failure explanation of experimental am-
Consolidation is ordinarily defined as the nesia since all initial entry into long-term
formation of permanent storage of the rele- storage must occur soon after training if
vant information; hence, a prevention of there is to be any retention displayed. How-
consolidation that is prolonged beyond the ever, the constraint is also consistent with a
duration of any temporary memory storage retrieval failure explanation as some retrie-
requires that the information be irrevocably val processes must occur during or soon after
AMNESIA, CONSOLIDATION AND RETRIEVAL 71
acquisition periods to permit later access to poral distinction between consolidation and
the relevant information. As an analogy, retrieval is an operational definition inde-
consider the acquisition of a new book by pendent of any behavioral consequences of
a library. Placing the book on a shelf may the operation, such as the permanence of am-
be viewed as equivalent to consolidation and nesia. Inasmuch as focusing on the perma-
making the appropriate entry in the card nence rather than the temporal distinction
catalog could be considered a retrieval func- between consolidation and retrieval is both
tion, one that must be performed soon after more common and more fruitful, this article
acquisition. Without the appropriate entry will adhere to the former definitions.
in the card catalog, the book may be physi-
cally present in the library but not available RECOVERY FROM AMNESIA
on request. The logical difficulty with consolidation
Returning to memorial processes, the re- explanations of experimental amnesia has
trieval view of experimental amnesia is that always been present. However, the short-
the relevant information is successfully con- comings of the consolidation position were
solidated within the central nervous system far less evident in prior years since there
and that the amnestic agent has impaired were no clear instances on record of recovery
the establishment or future functioning of (of the most recently acquired information)
the cataloging system necessary for retriev- from experimental amnesia. Considerable
ing the information from long-term storage clinical literature reporting spontaneous re-
at some later time, that is, during the reten- covery from traumatic amnesia, oldest mem-
tion test. The retrograde gradient for de- ories returning first, was ignored on the
gree of amnesia as a function of the interval grounds that there was no evidence of re-
between training and administration of the covery for the last few seconds preceding
amnestic agent is, according to the retrieval the traumatic event. The fact that most
position, a measure of the temporally de- memories did return was overshadowed by
creasing vulnerability of some cataloging the less well documented supposition that a
mechanism to the amnestic agent. Clearly few memories did not return spontaneously.
the cataloging mechanism is in itself a mem- The issue of recovery from experimental
ory of some sort; however, it should not be amnesia was first raised by Zinkin and Miller
confused with the fully elaborated memory (1967) who observed partial spontaneous
of the training event, which may exist inde- recovery from electroconvulsive shock-in-
pendently of catalog-type memories. Cata- duced amnesia when retention intervals
log-type memories are assumed not to be greater than the traditional 24 hours were
sources of primary information, but rather used in a repeated-tests paradigm. Subse-
means to facilitate access to primary in- quent studies have frequently failed to repli-
formation. One hypothesis to explain why cate this phenomenon, particularly when the
amnestic agents act preferentially on cata- possibly confounding repeated-tests aspect
loging processes rather than on consolidation of the experiment was removed (Herz &
is to assume that consolidation of primary Peeke, 1968; Luttges & McGaugh, 1967).
information occurs extremely rapidly, per- While spontaneous recovery from electro-
haps in fractions of a second, while catalog- convulsive shock-induced amnesia seems a
ing occurs over seconds, minutes, or even doubtful phenomenon, several recent studies
hours. Data and arguments supporting this have reported extensive spontaneous re-
hypothesis comprise much of the remainder covery from amnesia induced by the protein
of this article. synthesis inhibitor cycloheximide (e.g.,
As an alternative to the above schema, a Quartermain & McEwen, 1970).
few investigators have defined all events Koppenaal, Jagoda, and Cruce (1967)
occurring during acquisition as uniquely part initiated a second approach to the problem
of the consolidation process and all events of the permanence of electroconvulsive
occurring during testing as retrieval (Cher- shock-induced experimental amnesia. After
kin, 1970; Weiskrantz, 1966). While one training and amnesia-producing treatment,
can not dispute a definition, such a tem- they subjected their animals to a second
72 RALPH R. MILLER AND ALAN D. SPRINGER

training trial (reminder) with reinforcement with electroconvulsive shock can be largely
intensity sufficiently reduced from initial restored with scopolamine injected just prior
training such that animals receiving only to testing. Unfortunately, the permanence
the second training trial learned little or of the restored memory in this study was
nothing. The amnestic animals, however, not examined; however, even a temporary
displayed considerable recovery of memory. return of memory indicates that the informa-
Geller and Jarvik (1968a) successfully repli- tion was previously present although un-
cated this phenomenon. The difficulty with available. Another example is provided by
both of these examples of recovery is that Flexner and Flexner (1967) who, under
the initial training and amnesia-producing select circumstances, produced significant re-
treatment may have created an animal devoid covery from puromycin-induced amnesia in
of the relevant memory but with a superior mice by intracerebral injection of saline.
learning potential during the second training The explanations of these studies are far
trial. Lewis, Misanin, and Miller (1968) from clear. Davis, Thomas, and Adams
observed a significant return of memory (1971) theorize about the importance of
after correcting for this ambiguity by adding appropriate cholinergic levels for retrieval
control groups and administering the re- paralleling Deutsch's (1971) cholinergic
minder stimuli under noncontingent circum- model of spontaneous forgetting. However,
stances (outside of the training-test environ- all such recovery studies add weight to the
ment) that prohibited learning during the retrieval view of experimental amnesia which
reminder trial. Miller and Springer (1972a) postulates that the relevant information is
replicated this phenomenon and further present but unavailable.
demonstrated that the apparent recovery of The stimulus used to induce recovery of
memory was truly memorial in nature rather memory in most studies of facilitated re-
than a systemically induced phenomenon covery from electroconvulsive shock-induced
such as an alteration in activity level. amnesia has been the same aversive rein-
Quartermain, McEwen, and Azmitia forcer as was used in training. The question
(1970) reported studies corroborating the of stimulus specificity of the recovery agent
effectiveness of a noncontingent presentation with respect to the training reinforcer was
of the training reinforcer in restoring mem- recently raised by Springer and Miller
ory after either cycloheximide or electro- (1972). They used a one-trial passive
convulsive shock. They produced consider- avoidance task with ice water immersion as
able recovery of memory only when the the reinforcer. Rats receiving electrocon-
reinforcer (footshock) was presented within vulsive shock immediately after training dis-
an hour after an initial retention test. How- played essentially complete amnesia, but
ever, Miller and Springer (1972a) observed appreciable recovery of memory was ob-
extensive recovery even when a retention served if they were subjected to a noncon-
test was not given to the recovery group tingent footshock between electroconvulsive
prior to presentation of the noncontingent shock and the test trial. Control animals
reminder. The same study also observed indicated that the observed avoidance was
significant recovery from anmnesia over not due to changes in activity levels. Non-
electroconvulsive shock-reminder intervals of specificity of the recovery agent for electro-
up to 2 weeks (the longest interval ex- convulsive shock-induced amnesia is also
amined). Consequently, we are inclined to evident in the previously mentioned scopola-
reject the notion of an absolute critical period mine-induced recovery of memory reported
for recovery-inducing agents. by Davis, Thomas, and Adams (1971) and
Numerous other studies have found a di- is consistent with the prevention of cyclo-
verse group of stimuli and pharmaceutical heximide-induced amnesia achieved with
agents that induce partial or total recovery amphetamine and corticosteroids by Bar-
from experimental amnesia. Typical of the ondes and Cohen (1968), although in the
pharmacological studies are the experiments latter case these stimulants were effective
of Davis, Thomas, and Adams (1971) that only when injected soon after training and
indicate memory in rats rendered amnestic cycloheximide treatment.
AMNESIA, CONSOLIDATION AND RETRIEVAL 73

The results of the Springer and Miller propriately long exposure to the training
(1972) study suggested that the word re- environment as an effective reminder.
minder may be a misnomer for many stimuli The multitrial training used by Adams
that can induce recovery from amnesia. (1966) differs significantly from the one-
What this study did not make clear is trial training tasks that dominate the more
whether any stressor can act as a recovery recent literature. Consequently, to examine
agent regardless of the training task. Or Adams' effect in a contemporary context,
alternatively, is a stimulus similarity of the Miller, Springer, and Vega (1972) gave
internal physiological state produced by the one-trial aversive training in a shuttle box
typically aversive training reinforcer and the followed by immediate electroconvulsive
stressful recovery agent, thus permitting the shock and found that memory could be
latter state to act as a generalized reminder ? largely restored with brief exposure to the
To resolve this issue, Miller, Springer, and punishment compartment or somewhat longer
Vega (1972) used a one-trial appetitive task exposure to the start compartment. How-
and administered electroconvulsive shock im- ever, the data suggested that overexposure
mediately after training. Several parametric to either compartment not only did not
studies varying noncontingent recovery further improve retention but could actually
shock level failed to restore memory con- impair retention. Seemingly, after memory
trary to the high effectiveness of noncon- has been restored, it is vulnerable to extinc-
tingent shock in restoring memory of aver- tion like any normal memory. The potential
sive events. This suggested that the stress of apparatus cues to restore memories after
of recovery shock in Springer and Miller administration of an amnestic agent suggests
(1972) was specifically acting as a reminder that memory could reappear over repetitive
of earlier stressful events rather than as a test trials. In this light, the Zinkin and
reverser of all potential amnesias. Further Miller (1967) study, frequently ignored
experiments by Miller, Springer, and Vega exactly because of its multiple test trials,
(1972) found that appetitive memories could becomes worthy of reexamination. Perhaps
be largely restored after electroconvulsive these data are not an example of spontaneous
shock through either brief (1 minute) non- recovery but rather recovery facilitated by
contingent presentation of the appetitive re- intervening test trials that serve as re-
inforcer or somewhat longer exposure (three minders. Moreover, the shade of doubt
exposures of 1 minute each) to the training surrounding these data due to the failure
apparatus in the absence of the reinforcer. of others to replicate them (e.g., Luttges &
Looking for apparatus cues that could McGaugh, 1967) is eased by the recent suc-
restore aversive memories after electrocon- cess of Quartermain, McEwen, and Azmitia
vulsive shock brought to light a study by (1972) in restoring considerable memory
Adams (1966) in which amnesia for mul- with repeated test trials after either cyclo-
tiple-trial, aversive training in a shuttle box heximide or electroconvulsive shock treat-
was greatly attenuated through three ex- ment.
posures of 5 minutes each to the start com- Collectively, these recovery of memory
partment. Adams used a classical condition- studies indicate that at least some instances
ing explanation of electroconvulsive shock- of experimental amnesia are cases of re-
induced amnesia (US = electroconvulsive trieval failure and that many of the recovery
shock, UR — convulsion, CS = apparatus agents may reasonably be called reminder
cues, CR = partial covert convulsion) and stimuli. Until there is concrete evidence for
explained his data as an instance of extinc- two different sources of experimental am-
tion. Due to other data, the classical con- nesia, it would seem reasonable to assume
ditioning position has been discounted by the generality of the retrieval failure hypoth-
most researchers today (e.g., McGaugh & esis. However, an alternative to the unipro-
Petrinovich, 1966) ; however, consolidation cess retrieval hypothesis has been proposed.
theory offers no alternative explanation of The hypothetical argument has frequently
Adams' data. The retrieval position can been made that if the amnestic agent (e.g.,
interpret the phenomenon by regarding ap- electroconvulsive shock) were "effectively
74 RALPH R. MILLER AND ALAN D. SPRINGER

delivered" at sufficiently high intensities, perimental amnesia from the recovery of


then the resultant amnesia would reflect dis- memory data. Gherkin found that he could
rupted consolidation and would be perma- use reminder stimuli to recover significant
nent (e.g., King & Glasser, 1970). The amounts of memory after inducing amnesia
basis for this belief apparently lies in the with moderate but not strong doses of
greater degree of amnesia seen after (a) flurothyl. These data are complemented by
high-intensity electroconvulsive shock and several other studies in which weaker train-
( b ) transcorneal (as opposed to transpin- ing stimuli and/or stronger amnestic agents
nate) electroconvulsive shock electrode place- failed to yield the recovery of memory seen
ment (e.g., Ray & Barrett, 1969). The after stronger training stimuli and/or moder-
former effect is typically explained through ate amnestic agents. Gherkin argues that a
a traditional intensity argument. The latter "partial consolidation failure" could push
effect is thought to stem from transcorneal memory below an effective threshold and
electrodes delivering a greater percentage that a reminder stimulus may bring the
of the applied current to the central nervous memory into the suprathreshold range. The
system than transpinnate electrodes and/or problem with this explanation is that non-
the part of the brain receiving the greatest contingent reminder trials have been found
current concentrations being more suscepti- to be effective in restoring memory. Inas-
ble to the induction of amnesia. Insofar as much as a reminder trial cannot provide the
high- and low-electroconvulsive-shock cur- animal with any information by virtue of
rent intensity differ in degree rather than it being noncontingent, any memory ex-
kind, it appears to us more reasonable to pressed on the retention test must reflect
assume that differing depths of amnesia also information held within the animal since
differ only in degree at least until evidence initial training. Hence, this argument does
to the contrary is found. As for differences not counter the retrieval position. Further-
in amnesia resulting from variation in elec- more, it should be noted that experiments
trode placement, no data currently exist reporting a failure to obtain recovery from
suggesting that this is other than a conse- amnesia in certain circumstances have to
quence of diverse intensities reaching the date not clone any parametric study of the
brain. Diverse electrode sites may actually intensity and duration of the hypothetical
result in varying current intensities reaching recovery agent, and, consequently, the im-
the brain or may affect different neuro- plications of their null results are not com-
anatomical loci. Nevertheless, the fact re- pelling. Moreover, as was previously noted,
mains that differing electrode placements even if there were instances of compelling
yield amnesias that, to date, do not appear recovery failures, these would not distinguish
to differ in any fashion other than degree. between consolidation impairments and ir-
Consistent with this view, Miller and reversible retrieval failures.
Springer (1972b) demonstrated the effec-
tiveness of noncontingent reminder shock in REACTIVATION
reversing amnesia induced by transcorneal Reactivation within the experimental am-
electroconvulsive shock. In summary, there nesia literature refers to paradigms in which
appears to be no basis for believing that an animal is trained and then at a later time
increased current intensity or appropriate is presented noncontingently with a memory
electrode placement would prove capable of reactivator (ordinarily either the apparatus
disrupting consolidation when lesser con- cues or the reinforcer) followed immediately
vulsive currents and other convulsion-induc- with an amnestic agent. The time between
ing electrode placements apparently act on training and reactivation is usually 24 hours,
retrieval processes. If "appropriate" means an interval sufficient to insure consolidation
those conditions that by definition disrupt by most any standard and inadequate to
consolidation, then the question has been permit appreciable forgetting. Amnesia that
begged and does not merit further discussion. depends on the amnestic agent following
Gherkin (1972) has proposed another way soon after memory reactivation has been
to save the consolidation explanation of ex- observed in numerious instances with both
AMNESIA, CONSOLIDATION AND RETRIEVAL 75

appetitive and aversive reinforcers (e.g., and potentiate the memory loss caused by
Davis & Hirtzel, 1970; Davis & Klinger, an amnestic agent administered immediately
1969; DeVietti & Larson, 1971; Howard after detention. To cite a few examples,
& Meyer, 1971; Lewis, Bregman, & Mahan, Rbbustelli and Jarvik (1968) found that
1972; Misanin, Miller, & Lewis, 1968; detention caused amnesia in mice and, if
Schneider & Sherman, 1968). There have followed immediately by electroconvulsive
also been failures to obtain the reactivation shock, potentiated the amnestic effect of
phenomenon (Banker, Hunt, & Pagano, electroconvulsive shock. Davis and Klinger
1969; Dawson & McGaugh, 1969; Gold & (1969) and Davis and Hirtzel (1970), using
King, 1972; Jamieson & Albert, 1970); goldfish, observed that detention potentiated
however, none of the studies reporting null the amnesia produced by KC1, puromycin,
results distinguish themselves by making acetoxycycloheximide, and electroconvulsive
extensive parametric attempts to find the shock. And Geller, Robustelli, and Jarvik
optimal conditions for the phenomenon. (1971) found that posttraining detention
Clearly all of the variables controlling the of mice produces a memory deficit that is
phenomenon are not understood; however, additive to one produced by cycloheximide.
the pervasiveness of the effect across tasks, Considering detention as a performance
species, amnestic agents, and independent attenuating treatment in its own right, the
laboratories argues for the validity of the question arises as to whether detention is
phenomenon. merely an example of extinction (cessation
Considering the long training-electrocon- of previous reinforcement contingencies as-
vulsive shock interval used in reactivation sociated with training cues) and/or retro-
studies, consolidation failure appears to be active interference (due to irrelevant in-
a most unlikely explanation of the observed formation input) or whether it is a new
memory deficit. However, the administra- phenomenon in and of itself. Two variables
tion of an amnestic agent in conjunction that facilitate understanding of the detention
with select stimulus configurations at any effect are the place of detention and the delay
time prior to behavioral testing could, in between training and detention. Robustelli
principle, prevent the normal functioning of and Jarvik (1968) presented data suggesting
retrieval processes during the test trial. The that detention becomes less effective as an
occurrence of experimental amnesia pro- amnestic agent as the stimuli during deten-
duced at the time of reactivation further tion become increasingly divergent from
suggests that at least some experimental those of the training apparatus. This is
amnesia is due to a retrieval failure, a con- consistent with detention effects resulting
clusion in concurrence with the studies re- from extinction (to those conditioned stimu-
porting recovery from amnesia. Again, if lus elements present during detention) and
some experimental amnesia is due to re- retroactive interference (provided interfer-
trieval failure, parsimony compels us to ac- ence is assumed greatest when the irrelevant
cept all experimental amnesia as resulting information concerns aspects of the context-
from retrieval failure until evidence to the ual cues of the training situation). Calhoun
contrary is presented. Such evidence, due and Murphy (1966) presented data support-
to experimental design, cannot be provided ing this assumption for a passive avoidance
by any experimental amnesia paradigm used task similar to that used by Robustelli and
to date. Jarvik (1968). Similarly, the memory deficit
produced by detention increases with dimin-
DETENTION
ishing intervals between training and deten-
Despite apparent discrepancies in the para- tion (e.g., Robustelli & Jarvik, 1968). As
digms used, the recent studies of posttraining extinction and interference are most effective
detention are closely related to both re- soon after acquisition (Skinner, 1938), this
covery and reactivation phenomena. Several too is congruent with detention being re-
research groups have recently reported that garded as a combination of these two factors.
detaining an animal in the training apparatus Moreover, a spontaneous decrement in de-
after acquisition can both produce amnesia tention-induced deficits over a 24-hour, post-
76 RALPH R. MILLER AND ALAN D. SPRINGER

detention-retention interval (Robustelli, Gel- phenomenon produces sufficient amnesia to


ler, & Jarvik, 1969) is consistent with mask this effect.
spontaneous recovery from extinction. Fi-
nally, the poor memory displayed by animals THE STATE-DEPENDENT HYPOTHESIS
rendered amnestic with electroconvulsive The state-dependent hypothesis is one
shock and subsequently exposed to apparatus particular form of the retrieval failure view
cues for extended periods of time may be of experimental amnesia recently taken by
consistently viewed as a reminder-induced, several researchers. There is little doubt
memory increment followed by a deten- that treatments such as electroconvulsive
tion-induced, memory decrement (Miller, shock administered soon before acquisition
Springer, & Vega, 1972). yield memories more readily expressed in the
The potentiation of memory deficits pro- post-electroconvulsive shock state than the
duced by amnestic agents administered im- normal state (Gardner, Click, & Jarvik,
mediately after detention is thought by some 1972). However, the use of the state-
to be an interaction of detention and the dependent model to explain retroactive am-
amnestic agent (e.g., Davis & Hirtzel, 1970; nesia requires careful consideration.
Davis & Klinger, 1969) while others regard There are essentially two different state-
the two decremental effects as merely addi- dependent models. The first hypothesis
tive (e.g., Geller, Robustelli, & Jarvik, 1971). (DeVietti & Larson, 1971; Nielson, 1968)
However, other studies by the latter investi- assumes the subject learns in the "normal"
gators have yielded data suggesting a strong physiological state, and the subsequent am-
interaction (Robustelli, Geller, & Jarvik, nestic treatment creates an altered state that
1968). Conceivably these discrepancies are lasts for several days. During this time,
due to differences in species and amnestic retrieval of information concerning training
agents; however, it is possible that altera- should be hampered. The basic problem
tion in temporal parameters is responsible with this model is that it predicts spon-
for determining whether the results of the taneous recovery from experimental am-
tandem treatments are merely additive or nesia, an event that is rarely observed (e.g.,
reflect an interaction. Paradigms involving Luttges & McGaugh, 1967). Moreover, if
detention followed by an amnestic agent are the altered state is induced prior to acquisi-
essentially the same as the reactivation tion and amnesia-producing treatment, the
studies analyzed previously; only the dura- acquired information should be available
tion of detention is ordinarily greater than from the time of training until the normal
that used in reactivation studies. Detention state is reestablished. Miller, Malinowski,
can be viewed as serving to reactivate or Puk, and Springer (1972) failed to observe
maintain the activity of the relevant memory this predicted lack of amnesia on a 24-hour
immediately prior to administering the am- retention test.
nestic agent. For the present time, it ap- A second version of the state-dependent
pears reasonable to regard interactions of hypothesis (Thompson & Grossman, 1972;
detention and amnestic agents as further Thompson & Neely, 1970) assumes that
examples of reactivation rather than new the physiological state that must be present
phenomena in their own right. This appears for effective retrieval is the state present
consistent with detention serving as an ex- soon after acquisition, presumably the time
tinction period, for reactivation of a memory during which consolidation occurs according
seems a likely prerequisite for extinction to this model. Consequently, the memory
of that memory to occur. A reactivation is stored in an altered state produced by the
interpretation of the interaction of detention electroconvulsive shock and/or aversive re-
and amnestic agents offers an answer to inforcer and from which the animal is as-
the question of why doesn't the postdetention sumed to quickly recover. Tests very soon
amnestic agent cause amnesia for the extinc- after electroconvulsive shock do, in fact,
tion that occurs during detention. Possibly yield evidence of retention consistent with
amnesia for part or all of the detention period the model (e.g., McGaugh & Landfield,
does, in fact, take place, but the reactivation 1970; Miller & Springer, 1971). Moreover,
AMNESIA, CONSOLIDATION AND RETRIEVAL 77

returning the subject to the altered state as the time of consolidation, the only serious
shortly before a retention test has been seen question can be what are the temporal pa-
to restore the previously unavailable memory rameters ?
(Thompson & Neely, 1970). However, the In addition to interfering with retrieval,
restoration data supporting this seemingly electroconvulsive shock may reasonably be
successful model have been generated by a assumed to produce neural-electrical silence
paradigm similar to the one used in the pre- during the subsequent comatose state, or, at
viously described reminder studies with the least, to inject sufficient noise into the neural
stimuli affecting the return to the altered system to obliterate any information encoded
state serving as the recovery agent. The uniquely in electrochemical transmission
basic distinction between this state-depen- (unconsolidated memory). Therefore all in-
dent model and the reminder model is formation surviving electroconvulsive shock,
that the state-dependent position predicts whether immediately available or observable
that if the retention test is delayed a day or only after administration of a recovery agent,
more after the animal has been returned to must have already achieved a structural-
the altered physiological state for the second chemical format. In view of the recovery
time, memory should once again be unavail- data, this suggests extremely short consoli-
able. Miller, Malinowski, Puk, and Springer dation times. In fact, Lewis, Miller, and
(1972) examined this prediction and found Misanin (1969) report that under select
that memories once recovered do not fade circumstances (noncontingent familiarization
again over several days. Moreover, Miller, with the training environment prior to train-
Springer, and Vega (1972) found some ing) even a 500-millisecond interval between
effective recovery agents that do not subject training onset and electroconvulsive shock
the amnestic animal to the drastic change onset failed to yield amnesia. Lack of am-
in physiological state that scopolamine, foot- nesia following pretraining environmental
shock, and/or electroconvulsive shock pro- familiarization has also been obtained with
duce. Consequently, this second version of hypothermia as the potential amnestic agent
the state-dependent hypothesis is also found (Jensen & Riccio, 1970). These data argue
to be inadequate. Experimental amnesia for the occurrence of consolidation in inter-
appears to reflect a retrieval failure, but does vals of less than 500 milliseconds under these
not seem to be a dissociated learning phe- select circumstances and perhaps more gen-
nomenon. erally. Such consolidation intervals may
seem extremely short by traditional stan-
CONCLUSION dards, but numerous metabolic reactions in
The preceding arguments in support of a the nervous system are completed in equiva-
retrieval explanation of experimental am- lent temporal spans.
nesia are not meant to refute the concept of One phenomenon not readily understood
consolidation but rather to question its appli- in either a consolidation or a retrieval frame-
cability to the amnesia data. Assuredly, in- work is the short post-electroconvulsive
formation impinging on an organism is initi- shock interval during which memories ac-
ally represented by electrochemical, neural quired immediately prior to electroconvulsive
transmission. At much later periods of time, shock are available (e.g., McGaugh & Land-
this information is apparently stored in some field, 1970; Miller & Springer, 1971). Both
energetically passive, structural-chemical for- the consolidation and retrieval positions
mat (as, for example, evidenced by retention would be satisfied by hypothesizing that elec-
of all but the most recently acquired informa- troconvulsive shock either prevents consoli-
tion over a period of extreme hypothermia dation or future retrieval but does not de-
and consequent neural-electrical silence stroy short-term memory that is seen to be
(Lipp, 1964; Riccio, Hodges, & Randall, increasingly attenuated over time due to
1968)). Ignoring the possibility of more either decay or interference. This possi-
than two states of memory (short-term mem- bility, however, seems most unlikely if short-
ory and long-term memory), if the initial term memory is encoded in electrochemical
establishment of the latter state is defined neural transmission, for such a format would
78 RALPH R. MILLER AND ALAN D. SPRINGER

be highly vulnerable to electroconvulsive goldfish. Physiolof/y and Behavior, 1970, 5,


shock. The retrieval position can generate 1089-1092.
at least two more hypotheses. Perhaps there DAVIS, R. E., & KUNGER, P. D. Environmental
control of amnesic effects of various agents in
is an intermediate-term memory stage that goldfish. Physiology and Behavior, 1969, 4,
is passive, chemical-structural in format 269-271.
(hence postconsolidation), and is subject to DAWSON, R. G., & MCGAUGH, J. L. Electrocon-
decay or interference and consequently of vulsive shock effects on a reactivated trace:
rather limited size and does not require an Further examination. Science, 1969, 166, 525-
527.
operating catalog system to facilitate re- DEUTSCH, J. A. The cholenergic synapse and the
trieval. This hypothesis explains the phe- site of memory. Science, 1971, 174, 788-794.
nomenon, but at the expense of generating DEVIETTI, T. L., & LARSON, R. C. ECS effects:
another stage of memory. An alternative Evidence supporting state-dependent learning in
retrieval hypothesis is to assume that re- rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological
Psychology, 1971, 74, 407-415.
trieval processing is established as rapidly FLEXNER, J. B., & FLEXNER, L. B. Restoration of
as is consolidation, but the cataloging system expression of memory lost after treatment with
remains vulnerable for some time after its puromycin. Proceedings of the National Acad-
establishment. If electroconvulsive shock is emy of Sciences, 1967, 57, 1651-1654.
delivered during this vulnerable period, the GARDNER, E. L., CLICK, S. D., & JARVIK, M. E.
ECS dissociation of learning and one-way cross-
cataloging system is not immediately im- dissociation with physostigmine and scopolamine.
paired but is subject to increasing dysfunc- Physiology and Behavior, 1972, 8, 11-15.
tion over time. This hypothesis does not GELLER, A., & JARVIK, M. E. Electroconvulsive
require a third memory stage but appears to shock induced amnesia and recovery. Psycho-
place some rather unlikely constraints on nomic Science, 1968, 10, 15-16. (a)
the cataloging system. Further research is GELLER, A., & JARVIK, M. E. The time relations
of ECS induced amnesia. Psychonomic Science,
required to resolve this issue. 1968, 12, 169-170. (b)
GELLER, A., ROBUSTELLI, F., & JARVIK, M. E.
REFERENCES Cycloheximide induced amnesia: Its interaction
ADAMS, H. E. Extinction of the effect of one ECS. with detention. Psychopharmacologia, 1971, 21,
Psychonomic Science, 1966, 5, 295-296. 309-316.
BANKER, G., HUNT, E., & PAGANO, R. Evidence CLICK MAN, S. E. Perseverative neural processes
supporting the memory disruption hypothesis of and consolidation of the memory trace. Psy-
electro-convulsive shock action. Physiology and chological Bulletin, 1961, 58, 218-233.
Behavior, 1969, 4, 895-899. GOLD, P. E., & KING, R. A. Amnesia: Tests of
BARONDES, S. H., & COHEN, H. D. Arousal and the effect of delayed footshock-electroconvulsive
the conversion of "short-term" to "long-term" shock pairings. Physiology and Behavior, 1972,
memory. Proceedings of the National Academy 8, 797-800.
of Science, 1968, 61, 923-929. HEBB, D. O. The organisation of behavior. New
CALHOUN, W. H., & MURPHY, R. J. Pre- and York: Wiley, 1949.
post-trial stimulation: Effects on retention. HERZ, M. J., & PEEKE, H. V. S. ECS-produced
Psychonomic Science, 1966, 5, 435-436. retrograde amnesia: Permanence vs. recovery
GHERKIN, A. Retrograde amnesia: Impaired mem- over repeated testing. Physiology and Behavior,
ory consolidation or impaired retrieval? Com- 1968, 3, 517-521.
mulications in Behavioral Biology, 1970, 5, 183- HOWARD, R. L., & MEYER, D. R. Motivational
190. control of retrograde amnesia in rats: A replica-
GHERKIN, A. Retrograde amnesia in the chick: tion and extension. Journal of Comparative and
Resistance to the reminder effect. Physiology Physiological Psychology, 1971, 74, 37-40.
and Behavior, 1972, 8, 949-955. JAMIESON, J. L., & ALBERT, D. J. Amnesia from
CHOROVEK, S. L., & SCHILLER, P. H. Reexamina- ECS: The effect of pairing ECS and footshock.
tion of prolonged retrograde amnesia in one-trial Psychonomic Science, 1970, 18, 14-15.
learning; Journal of Comparative and Physio- JENSEN, R. A., & RICCIO, D. Effects of prior
logical Psychology, 1966, 61, 34-41. experience upon retrograde amnesia produced by
DAVIS, J. W., THOMAS, R. K., JR., & ADAMS, H. E. hypothermia, .Physiology and Behavior, 1970,
Interactions of scopolamine and physostigmine 5, 1291-1294.
with ECS and one trial learning. Physiology KING, R. A., & GLASSER, R. L. Duration of elec-
and Behavior, 1971, 6, 219-222. troconvulsive shock-induced retrograde amnesia
DAVIS, R. E., & HIRTZEL, M. S. Environmental in rats. Physiology and Behavior, 1970, S, 335-
control of ECS-produced retrograde amnesia in 340.
AMNESIA, CONSOLIDATION AND RETRIEVAL 79
KOPPENAAL, R. J., JAGODA, E., & CRUCE, J. A. F. QUARTERMAIN, D., & McEwEN, B. S. Temporal
Recovery from ECS-produced amnesia following characteristics of amnesia induced by protein
a reminder. Psychonomic Science, 1967, 9, 293- synthesis inhibitor: Determination by shock
294. level. Nature, 1970, 228, 677-678.
LEWIS, D. J. Sources of experimental amnesia. QUARTERMAIN, D., McEwEN, B. S., & AZMITIA,
Psychological Revieiv, 1969, 76, 461-472. E. C., JR. Amnesia produced by electrocon-
LEWIS, D. J., BREGMAN, N. J., & MAHAN, J. J., JR. vulsive shock or cycloheximide: Conditions for
Cue-dependent amnesia in rats. Journal of Com- recovery. Science, 1970, 169, 683-686.
parative and Physiological Psychology, 1972, 81, QUARTERMAIN, D., McEwEN, B. S., & AZMITIA,
243-247. E. C., JR. Recovery of memory following am-
LEWIS, D. J., MILLER, R. R., & MISANIN, J. R. nesia in the rat and mouse. Journal of Com-
Selective amnesia in rats produced by electro- parative and Physiological Psychology, 1972,
convulsive shock. Journal of Comparative and 79, 360-370.
Physiological Psychology, 1969, 69, 136-140. RAY, O. S., & BARRETT, R. J, Disruptive effects
LEWIS, D. J., MISANIN, J. R., & MILLER, R. R. of electroconvulsive shock as a function of CUP-
Recovery of memory following amnesia. Nature, rent level and mode of delivery. Journal of
1968, 220, 704-705. Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1969,
LIPP, J. A. Effect of deep hypothermia on the 67, 110-116.
electrical activity of the brain. Encephalography RICCIO, D. C., HODGES, L. A., & RANDALL, P. K.
and Clinical Neurophysiology, 1964, 17, 46-51. Retrograde amnesia produced by hypothermia
LUTTGES, M. W., & McGAUGH, J. L. Permanence in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiologi-
of retrograde amnesia produced by electrocon- cal Psychology, 1968, 66, 618-622.
vulsive shock. Science, 1967, 156, 408^-10. ROBUSTELLI, F., GELLER, A., & JARVIK, M, E.
McGAUGH, J. L. Time dependent processes in Potentiation of the amnesic effect of electrocon-
memory storage. Science, 1966, 153, 1351-1358. vulsive shock by detention. Psychonomic Science,
McGAUGH, J. L. A multi-trace view of memory 1968, 12, 85-86.
storage processes. Accademia Nasionale dei ROBUSTELLI, F., GELLER, A., & JARVIK, M. E.
Lincei, 1968, 109, 13-24. Incubation of a passive avoidance conditioned
McGAUGH, J. L., & LANDFIEI.D, P. W. Delayed response after anamnesic treatment. Proceed-
development of amnesia following ECS. Physi- ings of the 77th Annual Convention of the Ameri-
ology and Behavior, 1970, 5, 1109-1113. can Psychological Association, 1969, 4, 219-220.
McGAUGH, J. L., & PETRINOVICH, L. F. Neural ROBUSTELLI, F., & JARVIK, M. E. Retrograde am-
consolidation and electroconvulsive shock re- nesia from detention. Physiology and Behavior,
examined. Psychological Review, 1966, 73, 382- 1968, 3, 543-547.
387. SCHNEIDER, A .M., & SHERMAN, W. Amnesia:
MILLER, R. R., MALINOWSKI, B., PUK, G., & A function of the temporal relation of footshock
SPRINGER, A. D. State-dependent models of to electroconvulsive shock. Science, 1968, 159,
ECS-induced amnesia in rats. Journal of Com- 219-221.
parative and Physiological Psychology, 1972, in SKINNER, B. F. The behavior of organisms. New
press. York: Appleton-Century, 1938.
MILLER, R. R., & SPRINGER, A. D. Temporal SPEVACK, A. A., & SUBOSKI, M. D. Retrograde
course of amnesia in rats after electroconvulsive effects of electroconvulsive shock on learned re-
shock. Physiology and Behavior, 1971, 6, 229- sponses. Psychological Bulletin, 1969, 72, 66-76.
233. SPRINGER, A. D., & MILLER, R. R. Retrieval fail-
MILLER, R. R., & SPRINGER, A. D. Induced re- ure induced by electroconvulsive shock: Reversal
covery of memory in rats following electrocon- with dissimilar, training and recovery agents.
vulsive shock. Physiology and Behavior, 1972, Science, 1972, 177, 628-630.
8, 645-651. (a) THOMPSON, C. I., & GROSSMAN, L. B. Loss and
MILLER, R. R., & SPRINGER, A. D. Recovery from recovery of long-term memories after ECS in
amnesia following transcorneal electroconvulsive rats: Evidence for state-dependent recall. Jour-
shock. Psychonomic Science, 1972, 28, 7-9. (b) nal of Comparative and Physiological Psy-
MILLER, R. R., SPRINGER, A. D., & VEGA, D. C. chology, 1972, 78, 248-254.
Conditions for stimulus-induced recovery from THOMPSON, C. L, & NEELY, J. E. Dissociated
experimental amnesia. Paper presented at the learning in rats produced by electroconvulsive
meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, Hous- shock. Physiology and Behavior, 1970, S, 783-
ton, October 1972. 786.
MISANIN, J. R., MILLER, R. R., & LEWIS, D. J. WEISKRANTZ, L. Experimental studies of amnesia.
Retrograde amnesia produced by electroncon- In C. W. M. Whitty & 0. L. Zangwill (Eds.),
vulsive shock after reactivation of a consolidated Amnesia. London: Butterworths, 1966.
memory trace. Science, 1968, 160, 554-555. ' ZINKIN, S., & MILLER, A. J. Recovery of memory
NIELSON, H. C. Evidence that electroconvulsive after amnesia induced by electroconvulsive shock.
shock alters memory retrieval rather than mem- Science, 1967, 155, 102-104.
ory consolidation. Experimental Neurology,
1968, 20, 3-20. (Received September 11, 1972)

You might also like