Implicit Causality and Discourse Focus: The Interaction of Text and Reader Characteristics in Pronoun Resolution

You might also like

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 26

Journal of Memory and Language 42, 545–570 (2000)

doi:10.1006/jmla.1999.2695, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on

Implicit Causality and Discourse Focus: The Interaction of Text and


Reader Characteristics in Pronoun Resolution

Debra L. Long and Logan De Ley


University of California, Davis

We conducted this study to examine how pronoun resolution is affected by the implicit causality
inherent in certain verbs. In three probe-recognition experiments, we found that the implicit causality
effect depended on important characteristics of the reader and of the stimuli. First, only skilled
readers showed an early effect of implicit causality on pronoun resolution, responding faster to names
that matched the causal bias of the verb than to names that did not match. Implicit causality influenced
less skilled readers’ performance at the end of the sentence, when they integrated information from
the two clauses. Second, the implicit causality effect exhibited by skilled readers was limited to NP 2
verbs, those verbs in which the implied cause of the event was the grammatical object of the sentence.
In order to explore why the effect was limited to NP 2 verbs, we examined a small corpus of natural
texts. We found that NP 2 verbs were better predictors of the subsequent mention of the implied cause
than were NP 1 verbs. © 2000 Academic Press
Key Words: implicit causality; pronoun resolution; reading skill; discourse focus; reading
comprehension.

An important goal of research in text com- connected in terms of the situation or context to
prehension is to specify the nature of readers’ which a text refers. These relations often in-
text representations and how various combina- volve inferences about the causes and conse-
tions of text, task, and reader characteristics quences of events, the spatial arrangements of
affect the processes that construct them. Re- objects, the procedure for performing a task, or
searchers generally agree that readers represent the goals and intentions of individuals.
a text in the form of a discourse model Readers are aided in finding referential rela-
(Garnham, 1981, 1987; Greene, McKoon, & tions among entities in their discourse model by
Ratcliff, 1992; Grosz, 1981; Grosz & Sidner, a variety of syntactic, semantic, and pragmatic
1986). Discourse models are representations of devices (Greene et al., 1992; Grosz, 1981;
the entities (e.g., people, objects, places) and Grosz & Sidner, 1986). These devices function
events in a text and the relations among them. to establish one (or a small set) of entities as the
Readers construct a discourse model by inte- focus of the reader’s attention. Entities that are
grating each sentence in a text into their devel- focused in the discourse model are easily acces-
oping representation. This involves discovering sible and available for subsequent reference.
two classes of relations. Referential relations Focus can be seen in the ease with which an
reflect knowledge that two text elements refer to entity can be referenced by a pronoun. Pronouns
the same entity. These relations often involve are used readily to refer to focused entities, but
readers’ knowledge about anaphors, linguistic are seldom used to refer to entities that have
forms that refer to entities (antecedents) intro- been backgrounded by subsequent events. In the
duced earlier in a text. Coherence relations re- current study, we examined how pronoun reso-
flect the knowledge that ideas in a text are lution was affected by one possible focus de-
vice, the implicit causality inherent in certain
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Debra
Long, Department of Psychology, One Shields Avenue,
verbs, and how pronoun resolution was affected
University of California, Davis, Davis, CA 95616. E-mail: by the interaction of implicit causality and read-
dllong@ucdavis.edu. ing skill.
545 0749-596X/00 $35.00
Copyright © 2000 by Academic Press
All rights of reproduction in any form reserved.
546 LONG AND DE LEY

IMPLICIT CAUSALITY AND ing comprehension of a pronoun that refers to


DISCOURSE FOCUS that individual.
Psychologists have been very interested in WHEN DOES IMPLICIT CAUSALITY
the extent to which certain verbs, in particular, HAVE ITS EFFECT?
those that exhibit “implicit causality,” act as
Previous research has clearly demonstrated
focus devices. Implicit causality is a property of
that readers use knowledge about implicit cau-
transitive verbs in which one or the other of the
sality to help resolve ambiguous pronouns in
verb’s arguments is implicated as the underly-
sentences. The time course by which readers
ing cause of the action or attitude (Au, 1986; use such knowledge, however, is much less
Brown & Fish, 1983; Garvey & Caramazza, clear (Garnham, Traxler, Oakhill, & Gerns-
1974; Garvey, Caramazza, & Yates, 1975; bacher, 1996; Greene & McKoon, 1995; Mc-
Greene & McKoon, 1995). Some of these verbs Donald & MacWhinney, 1995). In this section,
appear in sentence contexts in which the initia- we describe two proposals for understanding
tor is the grammatical subject of the sentence readers’ use of implicit causality.
and the reactor is the object. These include Implicit causality may have its effect when
verbs such as annoy, charm, and infuriate, often readers encounter the verb. That is, implicit
called NP 1 verbs. For example, the subject of causality may operate to make the individual
the verb annoy performs some action or has who is the implied cause of an event the most
some characteristic that evokes a response from activated entity in the reader’s discourse model
the grammatical object (e.g., John annoyed Paul and the likely referent of a subsequent pronoun.
because John wouldn’t sit still). In contrast, NP 2 This proposal is based on the assumption that
verbs appear in contexts in which the initiator is readers use many sources of information to de-
the grammatical object and the reactor is the termine how entities in the discourse model are
subject, such as admire, praise, and thank. For related and they do so as early in the compre-
example, the object of the verb praise performs hension process as possible. This assumption
some action that evokes a response from the plays a central role in a text-processing frame-
grammatical subject (e.g., John praised Paul work described by Sanford and Garrod (1998),
because Paul did all of his homework). called the Scenario Mapping and Focus (SMF)
Researchers have found that implicit causal- theory.
ity can play an important role in the interpreta- According to SMF, the reader’s primary goal
tion of pronouns. For example, Caramazza, is to develop a contextualized representation,
Grober, Garvey, and Yates (1977) had partici- one in which linguistic input is mapped onto
pants read sentences such as “John telephoned relevant world knowledge, at the earliest possi-
Bill because he wanted some information” and ble moment. Incoming language is analyzed
decide whether the pronoun referred to the first against background knowledge structures: rep-
or to the second person in the sentence. Car- resentations of recent text input and general
amazza et al. found that participants identified world knowledge. Some of these representa-
referents for the pronouns that were consistent tions are in explicit focus. Explicit focus tracks
with the verbs’ implicit causality. In addition, currently relevant discourse entities: representa-
responses were faster when the subordinate tions of foregrounded characters, objects,
clause was consistent with the verb’s bias than places, and so on. Other representations are in
when it was inconsistent. Similar results have implicit focus; implicit focus tracks currently
been found using sentence-completion and relevant scenarios: representations of actions,
-generation tasks (Au, 1986; Brown & Fish, events, and so on. Representations in explicit
1983; Garvey & Caramazza, 1974; Garvey, and implicit focus are distinct, but are related by
Caramazza, & Yates, 1975). Such findings are mappings between discourse entities and the roles
consistent with the claim that implicit causality that these entities play in activated scenarios.
focuses the reader’s attention on the individual Scenario Mapping and Focus Theory can be
who is the cause of the action, thereby facilitat- used as a framework for understanding how
IMPLICIT CAUSALITY AND DISCOURSE FOCUS 547

implicit causality might focus the reader’s at- its effect on pronoun resolution at the end of a
tention on the implied cause of an event. Con- sentence. Readers will construct a representa-
sider the sentence, “John telephoned Bill be- tion of the first clause (e.g., “John telephoned
cause he wanted some information.” When Bill”) and set it aside when they encounter the
readers comprehend the first clause, the dis- connective “because.” Then, readers will con-
course entities “John” and “Bill” are in explicit struct a representation of the second clause and
focus. Moreover, these entities are mapped onto reactivate their representation of the first. When
a representation of the scenario “telephoning” in readers integrate the two clauses, they will as-
implicit focus. The scenario includes roles rel- sign a referent to the pronoun based on the
evant to telephoning, the initiator of the phone meaning of the connective “because” and their
call (i.e., John) and the recipient of it (i.e., Bill). knowledge about the implied cause of the event.
The scenario also includes other schemalike In summary, implicit causality may have its
information about a telephoning event, includ- effect when readers comprehend the pronoun
ing a typical instrument (e.g., telephone) and because the implied cause is in implicit focus
typical events (e.g., the initiator dials a tele- and readily accessible in the discourse model.
phone, a telephone rings). To the extent that the Alternatively, implicit causality may have its
initiator plays a central role in a scenario, such effect at the end of the sentence when readers
as telephoning, that individual should be easily integrate information from the two clauses. Re-
accessible in the reader’s discourse model. cently, Garnham et al. (1996) reported results
Therefore, readers should find it easy to under- that are relevant to these alternatives. They con-
stand a connective that explicitly suggests a causal ducted their experiments using two-clause sen-
connection between the two clauses (i.e., because) tences: a main clause that contained either an
and to determine that the implied cause of the NP 1 or an NP 2 verb and a subordinate clause
event is the referent of a subsequent pronoun. that was either congruent or incongruent with
An alternative account places the locus of the verb’s bias. Consider the following sen-
implicit causality later in the comprehension tences:
process. Readers may use knowledge about the
(a) David approached Brian after
implied cause to resolve a pronoun, not when
school because he wanted some
they encounter it, but later when they integrate
advice. (NP 1/congruent)
their representations of the two clauses. Such an
(b) David approached Brian after
account is described by Millis and Just (1994).
school because he looked friendly.
They have proposed a model of the influence of
(NP 1/incongruent)
connectives on the comprehension of complex
(c) Sherry envied Evette all the time
sentences, called the Connective Integration
because she had a fast car. (NP 2/
Model (CIM). According to CIM, readers inter-
congruent)
pret a connective like “because” as a cue to
(d) Sherry envied Evette all the time
integrate information across two clauses. When
because she had no money. (NP 2/
readers encounter a connective in a sentence,
incongruent)
their representation of the first clause is set aside
in working memory while they construct a rep- One of their hypotheses was that implicit cau-
resentation of the second. Subsequently, readers sality might have a focus effect, increasing the
reactivate their representation of clause 1 and accessibility of the argument that is consistent
the two clausal representations are integrated. with the verb’s bias. According to this hypoth-
Integration may involve modifying the reader’s esis, the effect should be seen as soon as the
representation of one or both clauses and elab- main clause has been processed, consistent with
orating their representation with inferences to the Scenario Mapping and Focus theory. Thus,
make it consistent with the meaning of the con- David should be more accessible than Brian
nective. when readers encounter the pronoun in (a) and
The CIM can be used as a framework for in (b), whereas Evette should be more accessi-
understanding how implicit causality may have ble than Sherry at the pronoun in (c) and in (d).
548 LONG AND DE LEY

Given the greater accessibility of the implied tween a referent and contextually relevant world
cause of the event, readers should be able to knowledge depends on statistical regularities in
assign a referent to the pronoun as soon as they the language. For example, when readers com-
encounter it. prehend the verb “write,” they automatically
Their second hypothesis was that implicit integrate an instrument of the verb (e.g., pen)
causality might have its effect at the end of the into their discourse representation only if the
sentence, a prediction that is consistent with the instrument is the preferred semantic default.
Connective Integration Model. Rather than fo- Similarly, we might expect the implied cause of
cus the reader’s attention on the implied cause, an event to be focused in the readers discourse
implicit causality may affect the ease with model only to the extent that it is usually the
which readers can integrate information in the topic of continued discourse. Garnham et al.
two clauses. At integration, the implied cause (1996) mentioned such regularities as one of the
may be more accessible than other arguments, reasons why implicit causality may not have an
but only when the subordinate clause is congru- immediate focus effect. It may be counterpro-
ent with the verb’s bias [e.g., David in (a) and ductive to focus attention on the implied cause
Evette in (c)]. of the event when it may not be the subject of a
Garnham et al. (1996) tested their hypotheses subsequent clause. For example, a subordinate
using a probe task in which participants read clause that describes the consequence rather
sentences and verified the presence of a probe than the cause of an event may contain no
name. The probe names were presented at var- reference to the individual who is the implied
ious points in the sentence to determine whether cause. Thus, implicit causality may have an
they were differentially activated as a function early effect on pronoun resolution only to the
of the verb in the main clause. The results extent that verbs with implicit causality are re-
supported the integration hypothesis. When the liably followed by mention of explicit causes.
probe name appeared immediately after the pro- We return to this issue later when we describe
noun, participants exhibited a first-mention ef- our analysis of a corpus of natural language.
fect; the first name that appeared in the sentence A second reason implicit causality may not
was verified faster than the second name (see have an immediate effect on pronoun resolution
Gernsbacher, 1990, for a complete description is that readers may be limited in their ability to
of the first mention effect). This effect was not use such information early in comprehension.
modified by verb bias. When the probe name Computing causal relations can require consid-
appeared at the end of the sentence, participants erable processing resources (Long & Golding,
exhibited a congruency effect, responding faster 1993; McKoon & Ratcliff, 1992; Oak-
when the subordinate clause was congruent hill, 1994). If resolving a pronoun requires a
with the verb’s bias than when it was incongru- complex causal inference, readers may not have
ent. Interestingly, Garnham et al. found no re- the processing resources available to make the
liable pronoun referent effect. That is, partici- inference until the end of the sentence. Indeed,
pants responded no faster to the referent of the Garnham et al. (1996) suggest that implicit cau-
pronoun than to the nonreferent; responses to sality effects may be reliable only when a main
both names were faster following the congruent event and a subordinate clause can be connected
than following the incongruent ending. by a relatively simple inference. One implica-
Garnham et al.’s (1996) results suggest that a tion of this is that the locus of implicit causality
verb’s implicit causality has no immediate ef- effects may differ across readers. In particular,
fect on pronoun resolution; rather, its effect implicit causality may have an early effect on
occurs when readers integrate a subsequent pronoun resolution in readers who have suffi-
clause into their existing representation. It may cient resources to make use of such information
be premature, however, to conclude that im- on-line during comprehension. We examined
plicit causality has no immediate focus effect. the locus of implicit causality effects among
According to the Scenario Mapping and Focus readers of different skill levels in Experiments 1
theory, making an immediate connection be- through 3.
IMPLICIT CAUSALITY AND DISCOURSE FOCUS 549

EXPERIMENT 1 ers because they have done so more frequently


in the past. Thus, less skilled readers show
We hypothesized that implicit causality may
sensitivity to these constraints in tasks that al-
have an early effect on pronoun resolution, but
low sufficient computation time (e.g., plausibil-
only among particularly skilled readers. We
ity judgments), but not in more demanding on-
based this hypothesis on two sets of findings
line tasks (e.g., reading time). The situation may
about the comprehension abilities of these read-
be similar when readers are faced with an am-
ers. One set suggests that skilled readers are
biguous pronoun. They may have knowledge
more likely than other readers to elaborate their
about the relative plausibility of alternative ref-
discourse models with knowledge-based infer- erents of the pronoun (based on knowledge
ences (Long & Golding, 1993; Lee-Sammons & about the implicit causality of a verb), but only
Whitney, 1991; Long, Oppy, & Seely, 1994, skilled readers may use this information on-line
1997; Oakhill, 1983, 1984; Oakhill, Yuill, & to resolve the ambiguity.
Donaldson, 1990). For example, Long and her Our methodology was similar to that used in
colleagues have found that skilled, but not less previous studies of implicit causality (Garnham
skilled readers, (a) make inferences about the et al. 1996; McKoon, Greene, & Ratcliff, 1993).
topics of sentences (Long et al., 1994, 1997), (b) Participants read sentences depicting a causal
form memory connections among stories that relation between a main event and a subordinate
share the same theme (Long et al., 1997), and clause (e.g., “Paul annoyed Harry because he
(c) make inferences about the goals of charac- wouldn’t sit still”). We used a probe-verifica-
ters who perform actions in stories (Long & tion task to assess the relative accessibility of
Golding, 1993). Thus, skilled readers may be the verbs’ arguments immediately before and
more likely than less skilled readers to elaborate after the pronoun in the sentences. One impor-
their discourse model with information about tant difference between the current experiment
the implied cause of an event. and the Garnham et al. (1996) experiments de-
A second set of findings suggests that skilled scribed above is that we constructed two-clause
readers are more likely than other readers to use sentences in which the second clause was al-
knowledge about the statistical regularities in ways congruent with the implied causality of
language to resolve local ambiguities (Pearl- the verb in the main clause. Thus, in the context
mutter & MacDonald, 1995; Van Petten, Weck- of this experiment, a verb’s implied causality
erly, McIsaac, & Kutas, 1997). Pearlmutter and reliably predicted the referent of a subsequent
MacDonald (1995) examined how readers who pronoun.
differed in their working memory capacity used The focus hypothesis predicts that the indi-
such knowledge to resolve syntactic ambigu- vidual who initiates the main event should be
ities. They found that both high- and low-ca- more accessible in the discourse model than the
pacity readers were sensitive to statistical regu- individual who reacts to it. In addition, the
larities in an off-line rating task. For example, implied cause should be the default referent of
both groups understood that the verb cooked the subsequent pronoun. Thus, readers should
was more likely to have a past-tense, intransi- respond faster when the probe name is consis-
tive interpretation in the context of an inanimate tent with the verb’s bias than when it is incon-
noun phrase (e.g., the soup) than an animate sistent. This pattern may be found, however,
noun phrase (e.g., the chef). However, high- only among readers who have the resources to
capacity readers were more likely to use this use causal information early in the comprehen-
information on-line to resolve a syntactically sion process.
ambiguous verb than were low-capacity read-
ers. Pearlmutter and MacDonald argued that Method
reading skill influences the speed with which Participants. Participants were 120 under-
readers can compute contextual constraints. graduate psychology students who received
Skilled readers compute these constraints more course credit for their participation. We admin-
rapidly and efficiently than do less skilled read- istered the vocabulary and comprehension sec-
550 LONG AND DE LEY

tions of the Nelson–Denny Reading Test (Form verbs that exhibited inconsistent causal attribu-
F) and identified a group of skilled and less tions in our normative data.
skilled readers based on their test performance We included a set of 48 questions about the
(the top and bottom third of our participants, sentences to encourage sentence comprehen-
respectively). Skilled readers had scores that sion. Each question was a true/false paraphrase
ranged from 75 to 98% (M ⫽ 85%, n ⫽ 40). based on information in the preceding sentence.
Less skilled readers had scores that ranged from Half of the test questions followed the experi-
42 to 64% (M ⫽ 57, n ⫽ 40). All participants mental sentences and half followed the filler
spoke English as their first language and none sentences. In addition, half of the test sentences
had a diagnosed reading or learning disability. were true about the preceding sentence and half
Materials. We selected 57 NP 1 and 36 NP 2 were false.
verbs from those used in previous studies of We created four lists of stimulus sentences
implicit causality (Au; 1986; Brown & Fish, and test probes. Each list contained 96 sen-
1983; Garvey & Caramazza, 1974; Garvey et tences (all of the experimental and filler sen-
al., 1975; Greene & McKoon, 1995; McKoon et tences) and 48 test questions. In each set of
al., 1992). We collected sentence-completion materials, half of the experimental sentences
data for these verbs from a separate group of 40 appeared with test probes that were NP 1 names
participants. Participants were asked to com- and half with probes that were NP 2 names. The
plete sentence fragments of the following type: filler sentences appeared with names that were
NP 1 verb ⫹ ed NP 2, because (e.g., “Ann an- not mentioned in the sentences. Half of all
noyed Pam because ”). We scored the probes appeared immediately before the pro-
completions as to whether the subject of the noun and half appeared immediately after it.
subordinate clause was consistent or inconsis- The probe conditions (i.e., name and position)
tent with the verb’s bias. We then selected the were counterbalanced across material sets. Each
verbs with the highest consistency scores. This material set was preceded by 10 additional sen-
resulted in a set of 24 NP 1 verbs (NP 1 was the tences and associated test probes. These sen-
tences were used in practice trials to familiarize
subject of the subordinate clause in 84% of the
participants with the task.
completions) and 24 NP 2 verbs (NP 2 was the
Procedure. Participants were randomly as-
subject in 81% of the completions).
signed to one of the four material sets (10
We constructed 48 experimental and 48 filler
skilled and 10 less skilled readers per set). The
sentences. The experimental sentences appear in
sentences were presented in random order on
Appendix A. These sentences are similar to the
PCs by means of a word-by-word presentation
congruent sentences used by Garnham et al.
procedure. All words were presented in the cen-
(1996). They were constructed using the follow-
ter of the screen for 300 ms with a 150-ms
ing frame: name verb ⫹ ed name, because pro-
interval between words. 1 Each trial began with
noun verb ⫹ ed noun phrase (“Ann annoyed Pam
a warning signal, a row of asterisks presented
because she kept fidgeting in class”). The names
for 1 s in the middle of the screen. The warning
used in the sentences were common American signal was followed by a 1-s pause and then the
first names. In each sentence, the two names were first word of the sentence. The test probes ap-
either stereotypically female or stereotypically peared in capital letters near the top of the
male. Across sentences, half of the names were screen. When a test probe was presented before
female and half were male. These names also the pronoun, it appeared 150 ms after the offset
served as the test probes for the experimental of the word immediately preceding the pronoun.
sentences. All sentences ended with a subordinate When it was presented after the pronoun, it
clause that was consistent with the verb’s causal
1
bias. The filler sentences were constructed in the We have found in previous studies that this is a com-
fortable reading rate for both our skilled and less skilled
same way. The verbs in these sentences were readers (Long & Golding, 1993; Long et al., 1994). Graham
similar to those used in the experimental sen- et al. (1996) used similar timing parameters in their study (a
tences; the majority were selected from the set of constant of 300 ms plus 16.667 ms per character).
IMPLICIT CAUSALITY AND DISCOURSE FOCUS 551

TABLE 1

Experiment 1: Probe Response Times (in Milliseconds) and Percentage Correct Responses (in Parentheses)

Reading skill

Skilled Less skilled

Probe before pronoun Probe after pronoun Probe before pronoun Probe after pronoun

NP 1 name
NP 1 verb 901 (99.1) 909 (99.6) 965 (96.1) 960 (96.2)
NP 2 verb 931 (97.8) 986 (99.6) 973 (96.2) 969 (96.3)
NP 2 name
NP 1 verb 928 (96.1) 923 (94.7) 1014 (95.8) 1004 (96.0)
NP 2 verb 911 (96.9) 913 (97.0) 1003 (95.9) 1000 (96.0)

appeared 150 ms after the pronoun’s offset. Accuracy. The percentage of correct re-
Participants were asked to decide whether the sponses and response times to the probe words
test probe had appeared in the sentence. They appear in Table 1. Our analysis revealed two
pressed a key labeled YES if the probe had reliable effects. Participants were more accurate
appeared and one labeled NO if it had not in their responses to NP 1 names (M ⫽ 97.6)
appeared. than to NP 2 names (M ⫽ 96.1), F 1(1,78) ⫽
Half of the sentences were followed by com- 14.09, MS e ⫽ .42; F 2(1,46) ⫽ 19.65, MS e ⫽
prehension questions. When this occurred, the 1.97, and skilled readers responded more accu-
word TEST appeared for 1 s in the middle of the rately (M ⫽ 97.6) than did less skilled readers
screen. This was followed by a comprehension (M ⫽ 96.1), F 1(1,78) ⫽ 6.03, MS e ⫽ .67;
question. Participants pressed YES if the test F 2(1,46) ⫽ 10.01, MS e ⫽ .94.
item was true about the immediately preceding Response times. All errors and latencies more
sentence and pressed NO if it was false. than 3 standard deviations from a participant’s
mean were treated as missing data (6.6% of the
Results and Discussion data) and excluded from the analyses. Our anal-
We performed 2(skill) ⫻ 2(bias) ⫻ 2(posi- yses revealed reliable skill ⫻ name, bias ⫻
tion) ⫻ 2(name) ANOVAs on the accuracy and name, and skill ⫻ bias ⫻ name interactions,
response-time data. All analyses were per- F 1(1,78) ⫽ 7.44, MS e ⫽ 12316; F 2(1,46) ⫽
formed with participants treated as a random 12.70, MS e ⫽ 2249 and F 1(1,78) ⫽ 9.41, MS e ⫽
variable (F 1) and again with items treated as the 7478; F 2(1,46) ⫽ 5.62, MS e ⫽ 1833 and
random variable (F 2). Skill (skilled vs less F 1(1,78) ⫽ 3.86, MS e ⫽ 7478; F 2(1,46) ⫽ 3.81,
skilled readers) was a between-participant and MS e ⫽ 2249, respectively.
between-item factor and bias (NP 1 vs NP 2 We examined these interactions by perform-
verb), position (before vs after the pronoun), ing separate analyses of skilled and less skilled
and name (NP 1 vs NP 2) were within-participant readers’ response times. Skilled readers’ re-
and within-item factors. All effects were tested sponses to NP 1 names were affected by verb
at a significance level of p ⬍ .05 unless other- bias and by probe position. When the probe
wise indicated. appeared after the pronoun, skilled readers ex-
Comprehension. We analyzed skilled and hibited an implicit causality effect (i.e., faster
less skilled readers’ responses to the compre- response times to the name that was the implied
hension test questions. Skilled readers per- cause than to the other name in the sentence).
formed better than did less skilled readers (M ⫽ Interestingly, this effect was limited to the NP 2
92% and M ⫽ 86%, respectively), F(1,78) ⫽ verbs. Responses to NP 1 names were slower in
8.86, MS e ⫽ 13.01. the context of NP 2 verbs (M ⫽ 986) than in the
552 LONG AND DE LEY

context of NP 1 verbs (M ⫽ 909), F 1(1,78) ⫽ match the verb’s bias rather than facilitated
17.28, F 2(1,46) ⫽ 9.57. Responses to these responses that matched. This finding is incon-
names were also slower than responses to NP 2 sistent with previous results reported by Mc-
names that appeared with either NP 1 verbs Donald and MacWhinney (1995), who found an
(M ⫽ 923), F 1(1,78) ⫽ 13.36; F 2(1,46) ⫽ increase in the accessibility of NP 2 names pre-
12.98) or NP 2 verbs (M ⫽ 913), F 1(1,78) ⫽ sented immediately after the pronoun; however,
16.66; F 2(1,46) ⫽ 10.01. When the probe ap- it is consistent with Gernsbacher’s claims about
peared before the pronoun, skilled readers ex- the mechanisms involved in referential access
hibited no reliable differences. In contrast, less (Gernsbacher, 1989, 1990). She argues that both
skilled readers exhibited only one reliable ef- referents and nonreferents are active when read-
fect. They responded faster to NP 1 names (M ⫽ ers encounter an ambiguous pronoun. Pronoun
967) than to NP 2 names (M ⫽ 1005), resolution, therefore, does not involve enhanc-
F 1(1,78) ⫽ 11.60, F 2(1,46) ⫽ 12.67. ing the activation of the referent; the referent is
These results suggest that implicit causality already active. Rather, pronoun resolution in-
can have an effect relatively early in the com- volves suppressing the nonreferent. This is the
prehension process, but only in certain circum- pattern that we observed for the NP 2 verbs.
stances. First, the effect depended on reading Responses to NP 1 names were slower after the
skill; implicit causality had an effect on skilled, pronoun than before it, whereas time to respond
but not on less skilled, readers’ performance. to NP 2 names did not change as a function of
Second, the effect was limited to the NP 2 verbs. probe position.
Skilled readers responded faster to NP 2 than to A second interesting characteristic of our data
NP 1 names presented after the pronoun, concerns the asymmetrical effect of implicit
whereas they showed no differences in the con- causality; the effect occurred with NP 2, but not
text of NP 1 verbs. with NP 1, verbs. This asymmetry would be ex-
One explanation for less skilled readers’ fail- pected if NP 2 verbs were better predictors of the
ure to show an implicit causality effect is that pronoun’s referent. However, the asymmetry
they have less knowledge about implicit causal- was not found in our normative data. We found
ity than do skilled readers. We examined this strong effects of implicit causality for both
possibility by asking another group of skilled types of verbs when we asked participants to
and less skilled readers to perform a pronoun- complete sentence fragments and when we
identification task. We presented each experi- asked skilled and less skilled readers to identify
mental sentence up through the pronoun (e.g., the referents of the pronouns. Our response-
“Ann annoyed Pam because she . . .”) and had time results suggest that pronoun resolution
participants identify the pronoun’s referent. may be more robust with NP 2 than with NP 1
Again, we used Nelson–Denny scores to iden- verbs. We examined this issue further in Exper-
tify skilled and less skilled readers (M ⫽ 82%, iment 2.
n ⫽ 20 and M ⫽ 54%, n ⫽ 20 for skilled and
less skilled readers, respectively). We found no EXPERIMENT 2
differences in the groups’ performance. Both
were likely to select NP 1 as the referent of the Skilled readers’ responses to probe names in
pronoun in the context of an NP 1 verb (72 and Experiment 1 were influenced by implicit cau-
73%, skilled and less skilled readers, respec- sality, but only when the verb’s causality was
tively) and NP 2 as the referent in the context of directed toward the grammatical object of the
an NP 2 verb (70 and 72%, skilled and less sentence. Our goal in Experiment 2 was to
skilled readers, respectively). examine this asymmetry. We asked whether
More interesting than the response-time dif- implicit causality effects occur with NP 1 verbs
ferences between skilled and less skilled readers when additional information from the sentence
was the way in which skilled readers manifested context supports NP 1 as the referent of the pro-
the implicit causality effect. Implicit causality noun. If so, then an implicit causality effect may
slowed responses to the argument that did not be found for NP 1 verbs by testing the activation
IMPLICIT CAUSALITY AND DISCOURSE FOCUS 553

TABLE 2

Experiment 2: Probe Response Times (in Milliseconds) and Percentage Correct Responses (in Parentheses)

Reading skill

Skilled Less skilled

Probe before pronoun Probe after sentence Probe before pronoun Probe after sentence

NP 1 name
NP 1 verb 874 (96.5) 898 (98.7) 969 (97.6) 982 (97.2)
NP 2 verb 907 (97.4) 988 (97.8) 966 (99.5) 977 (98.1)
NP 2 name
NP 1 verb 888 (96.1) 911 (93.9) 992 (95.2) 1019 (92.9)
NP 2 verb 895 (95.6) 912 (92.5) 996 (93.8) 1002 (93.3)

of the verbs’ arguments at the end of the sen- ever, this difference was not reliable, F(1,78) ⫽
tence. 2.17, MS e ⫽ 15.55, p ⫽ .14.
Experiment 2 was identical to Experiment 1 Accuracy. Participants were more accurate in
except for the position of the test probes; an response to NP 1 names (M ⫽ 97.85) than to NP 2
end-of-sentence probe replaced the after-the- names (M ⫽ 94.16), F 1(1,78) ⫽ 14.25, MS e ⫽
pronoun probe. If pronoun resolution is affected .21 and F 2(1,46) ⫽ 14.45, MS e ⫽ 2.67.
by additional supporting context, then we Response times. Errors and outliers consti-
should find similar effects for NP 1 and NP 2 tuted 7.8% of the data. Skilled readers exhibited
verbs. Readers should respond faster to the faster response times (M ⫽ 909) than did less
name that is consistent with the verb’s bias than skilled readers (M ⫽ 988), F 1(1,78) ⫽ 3.88,
to the name that is inconsistent with its bias. MS e ⫽ 214538 and F 2(1,46) ⫽ 14.14, MS e ⫽
1777. In addition, readers responded faster to
METHOD before-the-pronoun probes (M ⫽ 936) than to
Participants. Participants were 112 under- end-of-sentence probes (M ⫽ 961), F 1(1,78) ⫽
graduates who received course credit for their 8.7, MS e ⫽ 12155 and F 2(1,46) ⫽ 4.45, MS e ⫽
participation. We identified groups of skilled 2700.
and less skilled readers using Nelson–Denny The analysis also revealed reliable skill ⫻
scores as described in Experiment 1 (range ⫽ name and skill ⫻ bias ⫻ name interactions,
72 to 99%; M ⫽ 82%; n ⫽ 40 and range ⫽ 31 F 1(1,78) ⫽ 9.43, MS e ⫽ 13427; F 2(1,46) ⫽
to 62%; M ⫽ 55%; n ⫽ 40, respectively). 11.38, MS e ⫽ 1777 and F 1(1,78) ⫽ 3.68, MS e ⫽
Materials and procedure. Materials and pro- 8806; F 2(1,46) ⫽ 4.42, MS e ⫽ 1777, respec-
cedure were the same as those used in Experi- tively. Follow-up analyses revealed a pattern of
ment 1 with one exception. We replaced the response times very similar to that in Experi-
after-the-pronoun probe with an end-of-sen- ment 1. When the probe appeared at the end of
tence probe. the sentence, skilled readers exhibited an im-
plicit causality effect, but only in the context of
Results and Discussion NP 2 verbs. Responses to NP 1 names were
The data were analyzed as described in Ex- slower in the context of NP 2 verbs (M ⫽ 988)
periment 1. Mean response times and accuracy than in the context of NP 1 verbs (M ⫽ 891),
scores appear in Table 2. F 1(1,78) ⫽ 11.10 and F 2(1,46) ⫽ 9.79. Re-
Comprehension. Skilled readers performed sponses to these names were also slower than
somewhat better on the comprehension test responses to NP 2 names that appeared with ei-
questions than did less skilled readers (M ⫽ ther NP 1 verbs (M ⫽ 911), F 1(1,78) ⫽ 14.28
95% and M ⫽ 93% correct, respectively); how- and F 2(1,46) ⫽ 4.52, or NP 2 verbs (M ⫽ 912),
554 LONG AND DE LEY

F 1(1,78) ⫽ 16.54 and F 2(1,46) ⫽ 13.16. When noun, 200 ms after the pronoun, and at the end
the probe appeared before the pronoun, skilled of the sentence. They found a robust first-men-
readers exhibited no reliable response time dif- tion effect at all delays in sentences that con-
ferences. In contrast, less skilled readers tained NP 1 verbs. However, in the context of
showed a reliable first-mention effect, but no NP 2 verbs, the first-mention effect disappeared
effect of implicit causality. They responded immediately after the pronoun and at the end of
faster to NP 1 names (M ⫽ 974) than to NP 2 the sentence. In the following experiment, we
names (M ⫽ 1002) irrespective of verb bias, address possible explanations for the asymmet-
F 1(1,78) ⫽ 4.42 and F 2(1,46) ⫽ 4.13. rical effect of implicit causality.
The pattern of response times in this experi-
ment replicated our earlier findings. Again, we EXPERIMENT 3
found that verb bias directed the reader’s atten- Our experiments and those conducted by
tion to the implied cause of the event, but only McKoon et al. (1993) were similar in that par-
when the reader was a skilled comprehender. ticipants read sentences in which the subordi-
We also replicated the asymmetrical effect of nate clause immediately followed the NP 2 name
implicit causality observed in Experiment 1. (e.g., “Matt congratulated Scott because he won
Considered together, Experiments 1 and 2 sug- the race”). An early effect of implicit causality
gest that pronoun resolution is more robust for was found for NP 2 verbs in both studies. In
NP 2 than for NP 1 verbs. Although previous contrast, Garnham et al. (1996) used sentences
studies of implicit causality provide no ready in which the subordinate clause followed a filler
explanation for the difference between the two phrase (e.g., “Jane congratulated Mary on the
verb types, two previous studies have reported beach because she won the contest”) and found
results consistent with the asymmetry that we no early effect of implicit causality. In order to
observed here (McKoon et al., 1993; McDonald determine whether this difference in materials
& MacWhinney, 1995). McKoon et al. (1993) contributed to the inconsistency between our
examined the differential activation of NP 1 and results and those reported by Garnham et al., we
NP 2 names when a subordinate clause was ei- added filler phrases to all of the sentences used
ther consistent or inconsistent with the bias of in this experiment.
the verb in the main event. In two of their We also changed our materials to determine
experiments, readers responded to the test whether our previous results were affected by
probes at their own pace (Experiments 1 and 3). the reliability with which implicit causality pre-
In an additional two experiments, readers were dicted the pronoun’s referent. Experiments 1
encouraged to make very fast responses (Exper- and 2 were similar to McDonald and MacWhin-
iments 2 and 4). McKoon et al. examined the ney’s (1995) experiment in that we used sen-
extent to which readers exhibited a matching tences in which the second clause was always
effect, responding faster to the name that was congruent with the implied causality of the verb
consistent with the verb’s bias than to the name in the main clause. In Experiment 3, we manip-
that was inconsistent. When readers responded ulated congruency such that half of the sen-
at their own pace, McKoon et al. found a match- tences were followed by clauses that were con-
ing effect for both NP 1 and NP 2 verbs. When gruent with the verb’s bias and half were
readers were encouraged to respond quickly, followed by clauses that were incongruent with
they continued to show a matching effect for the verb’s bias. Test probes appeared after the
NP 2 verbs, but no longer showed the effect for pronoun or at the end of the sentence. By adding
NP 1 verbs. filler phrases and manipulating congruency, we
McDonald and MacWhinney (1995) also ob- replicated the conditions in Garnham et al.
served an asymmetrical effect of implicit cau- (1996, Experiment 3).
sality in an experiment using a cross-modal
priming paradigm. They presented probes at Method
four points in the sentence: after the NP 2 name Participants. Participants were 120 under-
in the first clause, immediately after the pro- graduates who received course credit for their
IMPLICIT CAUSALITY AND DISCOURSE FOCUS 555

TABLE 3

Experiment 3: Probe Response Times (in Milliseconds) and Percentage Correct Responses (in Parentheses)

Skilled readers Less skilled readers

Congruent Incongruent Congruent Incongruent

Probe after Probe after Probe after Probe after Probe after Probe after Probe after Probe after
pronoun sentence pronoun sentence pronoun sentence pronoun sentence

NP 1 name
NP 1 verb 892 (99.8) 916 (97.5) 900 (99.5) 1028 (96.7) 948 (97.8) 1017 (95.1) 953 (97.7) 1034 (95.2)
NP 2 verb 988 (98.7) 997 (96.0) 999 (95.0) 1020 (96.7) 931 (96.2) 1007 (94.2) 939 (96.5) 1046 (94.7)
NP 2 name
NP 1 verb 911 (97.6) 923 (95.0) 914 (99.2) 1039 (95.0) 1011 (98.3) 1005 (95.2) 999 (97.5) 1097 (95.2)
NP 2 verb 912 (99.2) 920 (94.3) 901 (97.5) 1041 (95.8) 1016 (96.7) 1011 (97.5) 989 (96.7) 1042 (95.8)

participation. We identified groups of skilled reliable effects in our analysis of the accuracy
and less skilled readers using Nelson–Denny scores. Our analysis of the response times re-
scores as described in Experiments 1 and 2 vealed reliable effects of congruency and posi-
(range 74 to 97%; M ⫽ 83%; n ⫽ 40 and tion. Participants were faster to respond to
range ⫽ 28 to 62%; M ⫽ 50%; n ⫽ 40, respec- probes in congruent sentences (M ⫽ 963) than
tively). in incongruent sentences (M ⫽ 996),
Materials and procedure. Each of the exper- F 1(1,78) ⫽ 12.90, MS e ⫽ 26646; F 2(1,46) ⫽
imental and filler sentences used in Experiments 32.50, MS e ⫽ 3738. Participants were also
1 and 2 was altered by adding a two- to three- faster to respond to probes that appeared after
word filler phrase before the connective. In ad- the pronouns (M ⫽ 950) than to those that
dition, we wrote an incongruent ending for each appeared at the end of the sentences (M ⫽
sentence. The experimental sentences appear in 1008), F 1(1,78) ⫽ 28.87, MS e ⫽ 28927;
Appendix A. We then created eight lists of F 2(1,46) ⫽ 112.63, MS e 3738. These effects
stimulus sentences and test probes. Each list were modified by the following interactions:
contained 96 sentences (48 experimental and 48 skill ⫻ bias [F 1(1,78) ⫽ 3.61, MS e ⫽ 25585,
filler sentences). Congruency (congruent vs in- p ⫽ .06; F 2(1,46) ⫽ 5.31, MS e ⫽ 4610], skill ⫻
congruent ending), probe name (NP 1 vs NP 2 name [F 1(1,78) ⫽ 5.37, MS e ⫽ 44680;
name), and probe position (after-the-pronoun vs F 2(1,46) ⫽ 14.72, MS e ⫽ 4610], bias ⫻ name
end-of-sentence) were counterbalanced across [F 1(1,78) ⫽ 3.12, MS e ⫽ 24084, p ⫽ .08;
material sets. The procedure was the same as F 2(1,46) ⫽ 6.68, MS e ⫽ 3738], congruency ⫻
that used in Experiments 1 and 2. position [F 1(1,78) ⫽ 11.60, MS e ⫽ 21276;
F 2(1,46) ⫽ 9.76, MS e ⫽ 3738], skill ⫻ posi-
Results and Discussion tion ⫻ name [F 1(1,78) ⫽ 6.33, MS e ⫽ 21120;
The data were analyzed as described in Ex- F 2(1,46) ⫽ 9.03, MS e ⫽ 4610], congruency ⫻
periment 1. Mean response times and accuracy position ⫻ name [F 1(1,78) ⫽ 5.54, MS e ⫽
scores appear in Table 3. 30717; F 2(1,46) ⫽ 7.95, MS e ⫽ 3738], and
Comprehension. Skilled readers were more skill ⫻ bias ⫻ congruency ⫻ name [F 1(1,78) ⫽
accurate in response to the comprehension test 3.89, MS e ⫽ 21494; F 2(1,46) ⫽ 10.54, MS e ⫽
questions than were less skilled readers (M ⫽ 4610]. The skill ⫻ bias ⫻ position ⫻ name and
91 and M ⫽ 90% correct, respectively); how- the skill ⫻ bias ⫻ position ⫻ name ⫻ congru-
ever, this difference was not reliable, F ⬍ 1. ency interactions were marginally reliable in
Accuracy and response times. Errors and out- both the participants and items analyses
liers constituted 5.4% of the data. We found no [F 1(1,78) ⫽ 3.40; MS e ⫽ 28699, p ⫽ .08;
556 LONG AND DE LEY

F 2(1,46) ⫽ 2.78, MS e ⫽ 4610, p ⫽ .10 and pronoun, they showed no effect of congruency
F 1(1,78) ⫽ 3.30, MS e ⫽ 68963, p ⫽ .07; (all Fs ⬍ 1); however, they were faster to test
F 2(1,46) ⫽ 3.55, MS e ⫽ 4610, p ⫽ .06, respec- probes in congruent sentences (M ⫽ 1010) than
tively]. in incongruent sentences (M ⫽ 1056) when the
We analyzed skilled and less skilled readers’ probes appeared at the end of the sentence. This
response times separately and found results sim- latter effect was reliable in the participants, but
ilar to those reported in Experiments 1 and 2. not in the items, analysis [F 1(1,78) ⫽ 7.30;
When the test probe appeared after the pronoun, F 2(1,46) ⫽ 1.47].
skilled readers exhibited an implicit causality Changing our materials to be consistent with
effect, but no effect of congruency. (Note that a those used previously by Garnham et al. (1996)
congruency effect would be surprising at this had little effect on skilled readers’ performance.
point because readers had not yet encountered These readers continued to exhibit an early ef-
the incongruent information.) Responses to NP 1 fect of implicit causality, but only in the context
names were slower in the context of NP 2 verbs of NP 2 verbs. This occurred even though filler
(M ⫽ 994) than in the context of NP 1 verbs material intervened between the main clause
(M ⫽ 896), F 1(1,78) ⫽ 7.75; F 2(1,46) ⫽ 7.74. and the subordinate clause and even though the
Responses to these names were also slower than implied cause of the verb did not reliably pre-
responses to NP 2 names that appeared with ei- dict the pronoun’s referent. Less skilled readers,
ther NP 1 verbs (M ⫽ 913) or NP 2 verbs (M ⫽ in contrast, showed a late effect of congruency,
907), F 1(1,78) ⫽ 6.15; F 2(1,46) ⫽ 8.67 and replicating Garnham et al.’s results. Like
F 1(1,78) ⫽ 7.11; F 2(1,46) ⫽ 26.08. When the Garnham et al., we found no pronoun-referent
probe appeared at the end of the sentence, effect. Less skilled readers responded to both
skilled readers responded faster to probes in NP 1 and NP 2 names faster following the con-
congruent sentences (M ⫽ 939) than in incon- gruent than the incongruent ending.
gruent sentences (M ⫽ 1032), F 1(1,78) ⫽ As we mentioned previously, the asymmetry
21.23; F 2(1,46) ⫽ 23.31. In addition, they between NP 1 and NP 2 verbs is not consistent
showed a different effect of implicit causality in with the sentence-continuation and pronoun-
the two types of sentences. When the sentence identification data presented here and in other
ending was congruent with the verb’s bias, studies of implicit causality (Au, 1986; Brown
skilled readers showed the same pattern as they & Fish, 1983; Garvey & Caramazza, 1974).
did when the test probe appeared after the pro- Why then do we find skilled readers exhibiting
noun. They were slower to NP 1 names in the an implicit causality effect in the response time
context of NP 2 verbs (M ⫽ 997) than in the data for NP 2, but not for NP 1, verbs? One pos-
context of NP 1 verbs (M ⫽ 916), F 1(1,78) ⫽ sibility is that NP 1 and NP 2 verbs are used
6.22; F 2(1,46) ⫽ 7.24. They were also slower to differently in natural language and this is re-
these names than to NP 2 names that appeared flected in on-line measures, such as reading
with either NP 1 verbs (M ⫽ 923) or NP 2 verbs time or probe verification, but not in off-line
(M ⫽ 920), F 1(1,78) ⫽ 5.17; F 2(1,46) ⫽ 24.50 measures, such as pronoun identification or sen-
and F 1(1,78) ⫽ 5.60; F 2(1,46) ⫽ 4.81. When tence continuation. We examined this possibil-
the sentence ending was incongruent with the ity by analyzing how these verbs were used in a
verb’s bias, skilled readers showed no effect of small corpus of natural language.
referent name, Fs ⬍ 1.
Less skilled readers showed reliable effects CORPUS ANALYSIS
of position and name, responding faster to after- One explanation for the asymmetrical effect
the-pronoun probes (M ⫽ 973) than to end-of- of implicit causality that we observed in the
sentence probes (M ⫽ 1032), F 1(1,78) ⫽ 13.68; previous experiments is that NP 2 verbs are bet-
F 2(1,46) ⫽ 56.63, and responding faster to NP 1 ter predictors of a subsequent pronoun’s refer-
names (M ⫽ 984) than to NP 2 names (M ⫽ ent than are NP 1 verbs. In this study, we exam-
1021), F 1(1,78) ⫽ 3.33, p ⫽ .07; F 2(1,46) ⫽ ined the use of NP 1 and NP 2 verbs in a small
12.61. When the test probe appeared after the corpus of natural text. We used the Internet to
IMPLICIT CAUSALITY AND DISCOURSE FOCUS 557

access the archives for two newspapers pub- frequency for each verb in the initial and in the
lished in San Francisco, California. The ar- final corpus.
chives contain staff-written articles and “Letters For each example in our final corpus, two
to the Editor” published in recent years. We independent judges recorded information about
searched the archives to find examples of the the verb’s use. (One judge was the first author;
NP 1 and NP 2 verbs used in this study. the other was a graduate student in the linguis-
We coded several pieces of information from tics program at the University of California,
the corpus. First, we coded information about Davis.) First, we coded information about the
the clause in which each verb appeared, includ- clause in which the verb appeared (clause 1).
ing information about its grammatical form This included information about the sentence
(e.g., tense, voice) and information about its type (active vs passive voice) and information
arguments (e.g., animacy). Second, we coded about the animacy of the verb’s arguments. Sec-
information about the subsequent clause (or ond, we coded information about the clause (or
sentence), including information about the ana- sentence) that followed the verb (clause 2). We
phor (e.g., pronoun, explicit name, relative pro- focused on the subject of this clause. We deter-
noun) and information about the anaphor’s ref- mined whether the subject was an anaphor; if
erent (e.g., NP 1, NP 2, other). Finally, we coded so, we recorded whether it was a pronoun, a
information about the relation between the two relative pronoun, or an explicit noun phrase. In
clauses (e.g., cause, consequence, etc.), includ- addition, we recorded information about the ref-
ing information about the connective (if any) erent of the subject anaphor: NP 1, NP 2, or other.
that appeared between them. The subject anaphor was always coded as NP 1 if
it referred to the first noun phrase in the preced-
Method ing clause and was always coded as NP 2 if it
referred to the second noun phrase. We fol-
Materials. Our corpus was obtained from the lowed the same procedure in both active and
archives of the San Francisco Chronicle and the passive sentences. Finally, we coded for the
San Francisco Examiner (http://www.sfgate. relation between the two clauses. We recorded
com/). These are two daily newspapers that information about any connective that appeared
serve communities in the metropolitan area of between the two clauses (e.g., and, because,
San Francisco. The archives contain all staff- when, then, so). We also recorded information
written articles and “Letters to the Editor” pub- about the semantic relation between the clauses.
lished in the past 5 years. We accessed the Based on an initial survey of the corpus, we
archives via the Internet to examine those arti- identified four types of relations: (a) clause 2
cles and letters published between January 1, was the cause of the event in clause 1 (cause),
1996 and December 31, 1998. Specifically, we (b) clause 2 was the consequence of the event in
used the NP 1 and NP 2 verbs from the previous clause 1 (consequence), (c) clause 2 provided
experiments to search the archives for informa- additional information about NP 1 (NP 1-elabora-
tion about how these verbs were used. tive), and (d) clause 2 provided additional in-
Procedure. We searched the archives for ex- formation about NP 2 (NP 2-elaborative). All
amples of the NP 1 and NP 2 verbs, using the other types of relations were coded as other.
verb ⫹ ed form. This gave us an initial corpus Appendix B contains examples from our final
of 16417 contexts (7481 NP 1 and 8936 NP 2 corpus. Included are examples of NP 1 and NP 2
contexts). For each verb, we randomly sampled verbs followed by subject referents that are con-
from the initial corpus, selecting each context in sistent with their bias and examples that are
which the verb was used in its transitive form. inconsistent with their bias. Also included are
We continued this procedure until we had iden- examples of NP 1 and NP 2 verbs followed by
tified 100 examples of the verb or until we had causes and examples of the verbs followed by
exhausted the entire sample for that verb. This consequences.
constituted our final corpus (1777 NP 1-verb and Cause and consequence relations were iden-
2022 NP 2-verb contexts). Table 4 contains the tified using a question–answering procedure de-
558 LONG AND DE LEY

TABLE 4

Frequency of Occurrence in the Initial and in the Final Corpus

NP 1 verbs NP 2 verbs

Initial corpus Final corpus Initial corpus Final corpus

aggravated 288 100 admired 378 100


agitated 89 18 adored 125 100
amazed 280 46 appreciated 334 100
amused 158 31 assisted 132 53
angered 217 100 blamed 1132 100
annoyed 123 62 comforted 107 100
apologized to 351 100 commended 119 100
bored 298 11 congratulated 111 100
charmed 116 100 consoled 64 41
cheated 124 100 deplored 35 19
concerned 552 100 despised 118 100
confessed to 360 91 detested 109 100
deceived 119 100 dreaded 152 47
disappointed 1330 39 envied 121 100
exasperated 79 31 feared 754 100
fascinated 195 80 honored 939 47
flabbergasted 85 69 liked 1373 100
frightened 246 100 mourned 102 88
humiliated 117 69 noticed 1097 100
infuriated 128 100 praised 691 100
inspired 1319 100 reassured 105 100
intimidated 225 100 resented 107 100
peeved 51 30 thanked 180 100
scared 631 100 valued 551 27

scribed by Graesser and Clark (1985). They pendix. When clause 1 in (11) is phrased as a
have found that answers to why questions reveal what-happened-next question (i.e., “What hap-
causal information about the goals of actions, pened after a man living in a nearby apartment
states that initiate goals, and the causal anteced- frightened the person away?”), clause 2 yields a
ents of events. In contrast, answers to what- sensible answer (i.e., “He called the police who
happened-next questions reveal information arrived only minutes later”). If we determined
about the consequences of actions and events. that clause 2 was not a sensible answer to either
In order to identify causes, we phrased clause 1 question, then we asked whether it provided
as a why-question and judged whether clause 2 elaborative information about one of the verb’s
was a sensible answer to the question. If so, it arguments in clause 1 (e.g., information about
was coded as a cause. Consider example (1) occupation, role, location). If so, we coded it as
from the Appendix. When clause 1 in (1) is either NP 1-elaborative or NP 2-elaborative; if
phrased as a why question (i.e., “Why did Allen not, then we coded it as other.
Edwards exasperate Pitino?”), clause 2 yields a
sensible answer (i.e., “because he came back Results and Discussion
slowly from an ankle injury”). We followed a Table 5 contains descriptive information
similar procedure for identifying consequences. about the examples in our final corpus. Agree-
We phrased clause 1 as a what-happened-next ment between the two judges across the coding
question and judged whether clause 2 was a categories was high, ranging from 86 to 100%.
sensible answer. If so, we coded it as a conse- All disagreements were resolved by discussion.
quence. Consider example (11) from the Ap- Only those categories containing at least 2% of
IMPLICIT CAUSALITY AND DISCOURSE FOCUS 559

TABLE 5 is consistent with the implicit causality of the


Analysis of Verbs Appearing in Their Transitive Form: verbs. The implicit causality of a verb is re-
Classification as a Function of Verb Type (in Percentages) versed when it is written in the passive voice.
For example, admired is an NP 2 verb and in the
Verb type active voice the implied cause is NP 2 (e.g., John
admired Paul because he was so talented); thus,
NP 1 verbs NP 2 verbs
(n ⫽ 1777) (n ⫽ 2022) we would have coded a subsequent reference to
the implied cause as NP 2; however, in the pas-
Clause 1 sive voice, the implied cause is NP 1 voice (e.g.,
Sentence type Paul was admired by John because he was so
Active voice 65.1 87.6 talented); thus, we would have coded a subse-
Passive voice 34.8 12.4
Animacy of the verb’s
quent reference to the implied cause as NP 1
arguments even though admired is an NP 2 verb. Our un-
NP 1 animate 72.7 92.5 derestimate of the subsequent mention of the
NP 2 animate 61.7 50.2 implied cause may have been greater for NP 1
Clause 2 than for NP 2 verbs because NP 1 verbs were
Subject anaphor
Pronoun 33.8 44.2
more likely than NP 2 verbs to appear in the
Relative pronoun 15.2 14.0 passive voice. We examined this possibility by
Explicit NP 51.1 41.8 analyzing examples in the active and the passive
Subject referent voice separately.
NP 1 47.7 35.4 Table 6 contains descriptive information
NP 2 50.3 63.9
Relation between clauses
about the contexts in which the NP 1 and NP 2
Connective verbs appeared in the transitive form/active
None 77.9 78.5 voice. Information about the referent of the ana-
And 9.8 11.7 phor in clause 2 (subject referent) and informa-
Because 2.1 4.0 tion about the semantic relation between the two
Relation type
Cause 26.8 41.3
clauses is presented as a function of the type of
Consequence 31.0 14.7 connective that appeared between the two
NP 1-elaborative 12.7 8.6 clauses.
NP 2-elaborative 14.4 13.7 When no connective appeared between the
two clauses, the referent of the anaphor in
clause 2 was more likely to be NP 2 than NP 1 in
the corpus are presented in the table. One im- the context of an NP 2 verb (76.7% of the 1400
portant difference between the contexts contain- examples vs 22.8% of the examples) [␹ 2(1) ⫽
ing the NP 1 and NP 2 verbs is that we found 82.16, p ⬍ .001]. Thus, the referent was gener-
more examples of NP 2 than NP 1 verbs appear- ally consistent with the implicit causality of NP 2
ing in the transitive form, active voice (87.6% verbs. This was not the case for NP 1 verbs. In
vs 65.1% of the corpus, respectively) [␹ 2(1) ⫽ the absence of a connective, verb bias did not
74.41, p ⬍ .001]. In addition, NP 2 verbs were reliably predict the referent of the anaphor in
more likely than NP 1 verbs to appear in contexts clause 2. We also found NP 2 verbs were more
in which the subject of the subsequent clause likely than NP 1 verbs to be followed by causes
was consistent with the verb’s implicit causality [␹ 2(1) ⫽ 143.25, p ⬍ .001], whereas NP 1 verbs
[␹ 2(1) ⫽ 61.40, p ⬍ .001]. NP 2 verbs were also were more likely than NP 2 verbs to be followed
more likely than NP 1 verbs to be followed by by consequences [␹ 2(1) ⫽ 143.89, p ⬍ .001],
causes of the event in the preceding clause NP 1-elaborations [␹ 2(1) ⫽ 25.18, p ⬍ .001], or
[␹ 2(1) ⫽ 38.82, p ⬍ .001]. NP 2-elaborations [␹ 2(1) ⫽ 4.58, p ⫽ .03].
It should be noted that the information in When the connective and appeared between
Table 5 is collapsed across sentence type (active clause 1 and 2, the pattern for NP 1 and NP 2
vs passive voice). Thus, we may be underesti- verbs was fairly similar. The referent of the
mating the frequency that subsequent reference anaphor in clause 2 was more likely to be NP 1
560 LONG AND DE LEY

TABLE 6 consistent with verb bias; NP 1 was likely to be


Analysis of Verbs Appearing in Their Transitive Form/ the referent in the context of NP 1 verbs and NP 2
Active Voice: Classification as a Function of Verb Type and was likely to be the referent in the context of
Connective (in Percentages) NP 2 verbs. This effect, however, was stronger
for NP 2 than for NP 1 verbs [␹ 2(1) ⫽ 12.45, p ⬍
Verb type
.001]. The presence of the connective because
NP 1 verbs NP 2 verbs was also predictive of the semantic relation
Connective (n ⫽ 1157) (n ⫽ 1771) between the two clauses. In almost every exam-
ple, clause 2 was the cause of the event in clause
None n ⫽ 871 n ⫽ 1400 1; we found no effect of verb type.
Subject Referent Table 7 contains descriptive information
NP 1 40.6 22.8
NP 2 58.4 76.7
about the contexts in which NP 1 and NP 2 verbs
Relation between clauses appeared in their transitive form/passive voice.
Cause 20.0 48.5 When no connective appeared between the two
Consequence 36.9 11.7 clauses, the subject referent was generally con-
NP 1-elaborative 10.7 5.1 sistent with the verb’s bias [␹ 2(1) ⫽ 4.01, p ⫽
NP 2-elaborative 18.0 14.6
“And” n ⫽ 138 n ⫽ 214
.05]. NP 2 was more likely to be the referent than
Subject referent NP 1 in the context of an NP 1 verb, whereas NP 1
NP 1 74.6 87.4 was more likely to be the referent than NP 2 in
NP 2 23.2 12.2 the context of an NP 2 verb. NP 1 verbs were
Relation between clauses more likely than NP 2 verbs to be followed by
Cause 0.0 8.1
Consequence 59.8 34.1
consequences [␹ 2(1) ⫽ 7.20, p ⬍ .01]; we found
NP 1-elaborative 22.5 14.0 no effect of verb type on the other semantic
NP 2-elaborative 0.0 4.7 relations.
“Because” n ⫽ 31 n ⫽ 78 When the connective and appeared between
Subject referent clauses 1 and 2, the referent of the anaphor in
NP 1 74.2 0.3
NP 2 25.8 96.2
clause 2 was more likely to be NP 1 than NP 2 in
Relation between clauses the context of both NP 1 and NP 2 verbs. This
Cause 96.8 97.4 was somewhat more likely for NP 2 than for NP 1
Consequence 0.0 0.0 verbs [␹ 2(1) ⫽ 5.91, p ⫽ .02]. NP 1 verbs were
NP 1-elaborative 0.0 0.0 more likely than NP 2 verbs to be followed by
NP 2-elaborative 0.0 0.0
causes [␹ 2(1) ⫽ 4.27, p ⫽ .04], whereas NP 2
verbs were more likely than NP 1 verbs to be
followed by consequences [␹ 2(1) ⫽ 19.88, p ⬍
than NP 2 in the context of both NP 1 and NP 2 .001]. This is the same pattern that we observed
verbs. This was particularly true for NP 2 verbs, for verbs that appeared in the active voice (see
␹2(1) ⫽ 7.57, p ⫽ .006. We also found that NP 2 Table 6). We found no effect of verb type on
verbs were more likely than NP 1 verbs to be elaborations of NP 1 and NP 2.
followed by causes [␹ 2(1) ⫽ 5.96, p ⫽ .02] and Finally, when the connective because ap-
elaborations of NP 2 [␹ 2(1) ⫽ 6.65, p ⫽ .01], peared between clauses 1 and 2, the referent of
whereas NP 1 verbs were more likely than NP 2 the anaphor in clause 2 was generally consistent
verbs to be followed by consequences [␹ 2(1) ⫽ with the verb’s bias, NP 2 in the context of NP 1
12.20, p ⬍ .001] and elaborations of NP 1 verbs and NP 1 in the context of NP 2 verbs
[␹ 2(1) ⫽ 4.17, p ⫽ .04]. [␹ 2(1) ⫽ 9.35, p ⬍ .01]. We found no effect of
The connective because did not appear in our verb type on the semantic relation between
corpus very often, but in the few examples in clauses 1 and 2.
which it did appear, we still found that NP 2 Our analysis suggests differences in the way
verbs were somewhat better predictors of a sub- that NP 1 and NP 2 verbs are used in natural
sequent referent than were NP 1 verbs. The ref- contexts. These differences are consistent with
erent of the anaphor in clause 2 was generally the asymmetry that we observed in our re-
IMPLICIT CAUSALITY AND DISCOURSE FOCUS 561

TABLE 7 more likely than NP 1 verbs to be followed by a


Analysis of Verbs Appearing in Their Transitive Form/ reference to the implied cause of the event. An
Passive Voice: Classification as a Function of Verb Type entirely different pattern of results was found
and Connective (in Percentages) when the context included the connective and.
NP 1 was generally the referent of the subse-
Verb type
quent anaphor, irrespective of verb bias.
NP 1 NP 2 verbs Although our findings from the corpus anal-
Connective (n ⫽ 620) (n ⫽ 251) ysis correspond to the asymmetry that we ob-
served in Experiments 1–3, there are several
None n ⫽ 512 n ⫽ 187 reasons to interpret our data with caution. First,
Subject referent our final corpus of NP 1 and NP 2 verbs was
NP 1 41.0 52.9
NP 2 58.2 46.5
small, containing a total of 3799 examples of
Relation between clauses verbs used in the past-tense, transitive form.
Cause 39.8 34.0 The size of the corpus was even smaller when
Consequence 16.5 7.5 we considered only those contexts in the active
NP 1-elaborative 10.2 17.6 voice (2928 examples). Second, our corpus
NP 2-elaborative 18.0 23.0
“And” n ⫽ 36 n ⫽ 23
came from a single genre of text, articles, and
Subject referent letters that appeared in the newspaper. There
NP 1 77.8 95.7 may be systematic differences in the way that
NP 2 22.2 0.0 these verbs are used across genres. Indeed, we
Relation between clauses observed differences in how these verbs were
Cause 16.7 0.0
Consequence 30.6 78.3
used across newspaper articles on different top-
NP 1-elaborative 27.7 21.7 ics. Verbs such as aggravated, confessed, and
NP 2-elaborative 5.6 0.0 cheated appeared predominantly in articles on
“Because” n⫽7 n⫽9 crime, whereas verbs such as admired, com-
Subject referent forted, and mourned appeared in articles about
NP 1 28.6 100.0
NP 2 71.0 0.0
death (the deaths of Diana, Princess of Wales
Relation between clauses and Mother Teresa, in particular). These differ-
Cause 100.0 100.0 ences may result from the semantics of the
Consequence 0.0 0.0 verbs and generalize across other genres, but
NP 1-elaborative 0.0 0.0 some of these differences may be specific to
NP 2-elaborative 0.0 0.0
newspapers.
One final reason for caution in interpreting
these results is that the verbs in our corpus
sponse-time data. NP 2 verbs were more likely almost never appeared with the connective be-
than NP 1 verbs to appear in the grammatical cause. In most of our examples, the clause con-
form that we used in Experiments 1–3 (transi- taining the NP 1 or NP 2 verb was followed by
tive form/active voice). In addition, NP 2 verbs another sentence. Although the verbs some-
were generally better predictors of subsequent times appeared in a main clause followed by a
mention than were NP 1 verbs. When the verbs subordinate clause, the subordinate clause was
appeared in the past tense, transitive, or active typically introduced by a relative pronoun (e.g.,
voice and when the verbs appeared without a who) and not by a connective. This may be one
connective, NP 2 verbs were more likely than reason why we found differences in the use of
NP 1 verbs to be followed by a subject anaphor NP 1 and NP 2 verbs. When a subordinate clause
that was consistent with the implied causality of is introduced by a relative pronoun, the pronoun
the verb. In addition, NP 2 verbs were more typically refers to the most recently mentioned
likely to be followed by causes than were NP 1 antecedent. This antecedent is likely to be the
verbs. When contexts included the connective implied cause when the main event contains an
because, both NP 1 and NP 2 verbs were likely to NP 2 verb in the active voice (e.g., “The world
be followed by causes, but NP 2 verbs were still mourned Mother Teresa who was buried in Cal-
562 LONG AND DE LEY

cutta today”) In contrast, when a relative pro- experiments, always responding faster to NP 1
noun refers to the implied cause in the case of than to NP 2 names.
an NP 1 verb, the verb is likely to appear in the In Experiment 3, we replicated an earlier
passive voice (e.g., Sales recruits were charmed experiment by Garnham et al. (1996, Experi-
by Chen Wang, who shared Horatio Alger hom- ment 3) to determine whether our findings de-
ilies and old Chinese proverbs.). This may ex- pended on the reliability with which verb bias
plain why NP 1 verbs were more likely to be predicted the referent of the subsequent pro-
followed by causes when they appeared in the noun. When we compared the results of the two
transitive form/passive voice than when they experiments, we found that our less skilled
appeared in the transitive form/active voice. readers exhibited the same pattern of response
In summary, we found that NP 2 verbs were times as did participants in Garnham et al.’s
better predictors of the subsequent mention of experiment. When the probe name appeared at
the implied cause of an event than were NP 1 the end of the sentence, readers exhibited a
verbs. This may be related to the finding that congruency effect, responding faster when the
NP 2 verbs also appeared more often in the tran- subordinate clause was congruent with the
sitive, active voice. Although our findings are verb’s bias than when it was incongruent. The
consistent with the asymmetry that we observed performance of these readers is consistent with
in Experiments 1–3, additional studies are predictions from the Connective Integration
needed to examine the use of such verbs in a Model (Millis & Just, 1994). Less skilled read-
wider range of contexts. ers use knowledge about the implied cause of an
event to resolve a pronoun, but not when they
GENERAL DISCUSSION encounter it. Rather, this knowledge is used
Does implicit causality have an effect on later when they integrate their representations of
discourse focus early in the comprehension pro- the two clauses. The similarity between the per-
cess; that is, soon after readers encounter the formance of our less skilled readers and the
verb? Our results suggest that this question has performance of participants in Garnham et al.’s
no simple “yes” or “no” answer. Rather, the experiment is interesting. It suggests that less
answer depends on a complex interaction be- skilled readers have no particular deficit in their
tween the type of verb in the sentence and the knowledge about implicit causality. They use
comprehension skill of the reader. In three ex- knowledge about implicit causality to resolve
periments, we found that implicit causality af- an ambiguous pronoun, but do so later in the
fected readers’ comprehension of a subsequent comprehension process than do skilled readers.
pronoun, but only when the reader was a skilled Differences between our results and those
comprehender and only when the sentence con- reported by Garnham et al. (1996) are found
tained an NP 2 verb. In Experiment 1, we com- only in the behavior of our skilled readers. First,
pared response times to NP 1 and NP 2 names we found a much earlier effect of implicit cau-
presented before and after the pronoun. We sality than did Garnham et al. Once again, this
found that skilled readers’ responses to the NP 1 effect was dependent on verb type. When the
names were affected by the implicit causality of probe appeared after the pronoun, skilled read-
the verb contained in the sentence. When the ers responded slowly to NP 1 names in the con-
sentence contained an NP 2 verb, skilled readers text of NP 2 verbs, irrespective of congruency.
responded slower to NP 1 names presented after When the probe appeared after the sentence,
the pronoun than before it. When the sentence skilled readers again responded slowly to NP 1
contained an NP 1 verb, we found no difference names in the context of NP 2 verbs, but only in
in response times before and after the pronoun. the congruent sentences. Second, we found no
Skilled readers showed a similar pattern of re- first-mention effect in the behavior of our
sponse times when we replaced the after-the- skilled readers. We are unaware of any previous
pronoun test probes with end-of-sentence studies examining the influence of reading skill
probes (Experiment 2). In contrast, less skilled on the first-mention effect. It may be that the
readers exhibited a first-mention effect in both effect is less robust in skilled than in less skilled
IMPLICIT CAUSALITY AND DISCOURSE FOCUS 563

readers. Alternatively, skilled readers may show tant role in pronoun resolution. It is also con-
a reliable first-mention effect, but not at the sistent with the individual differences that we
points in sentence processing that we tested. observed. Gernsbacher has presented consider-
The first-mentioned participant may be more able evidence that the ability to suppress acti-
accessible than the second-mentioned partici- vated, but irrelevant, information underlies in-
pant as skilled readers comprehend the first dividual differences in comprehension skill
clause, but both participants may be readily (Gernsbacher & Faust, 1991; Gernsbacher &
accessible when readers begin processing the Robertson, 1995; Gernsbacher, Varner, &
second clause. Faust, 1990).
In both Experiments 1 and 2, we found no We were not surprised to find that implicit
evidence for an implicit causality effect before causality was manifested as a suppression effect
the pronoun in the sentence. This finding can be in our data; however, we were very surprised to
interpreted in one of two ways. First, the pro- find that the effect was limited to the NP 2 verbs.
cess by which the implied cause of an NP 2 verb The asymmetry between NP 1 and NP 2 verbs
becomes focused may be initiated at any point appears to be a robust finding. We observed it in
after readers encounter the verb (e.g., when they three experiments and McKoon et al. (1993)
comprehend the verb or when they comprehend and McDonald and MacWhinney observed a
the connective because), but the process may similar effect in their experiments. One expla-
not be complete until later in processing. Thus, nation for the asymmetrical effect of implicit
the implicit causality effect may not be apparent causality is that NP 2 verbs may be better pre-
before readers encounter the pronoun. Second, dictors of the subsequent mention of the implied
the process may be initiated when readers com- cause than are NP 1 verbs. We presented prelim-
prehend the pronoun. This possibility is consis- inary evidence in our corpus analysis that the
tent with Greene et al.’s (1992) “pronoun-as- implied cause was the subject of a subsequent
cue” framework. According to this framework, clause more often when the verb in the main
a pronoun serves as a memory cue to the most event was NP 2 biased than when it was NP 1
likely entity in the reader’s discourse model. biased.
The morphological and syntactic features of the As we discussed earlier, the data from our
pronoun (e.g., gender, number, grammatical corpus analysis should be interpreted cau-
role) are matched automatically to the semantic tiously. Our corpus was relatively small and
features of entities in the discourse model. The based on a single genre of text. However, in a
accessibility of these entities is determined by recent study of NP 1 and NP 2 verbs in a very
syntactic, semantic, and pragmatic features. If large corpus of natural text, Burgess, Livesay,
the degree of match between the pronoun and and Lund (1998) also observed an asymmetry in
one of the entities is higher than the match the verbs’ use. Burgess and his colleagues have
between the pronoun and other entities, the ref- developed a computerized text-analysis system,
erent for the pronoun is automatically identified. the Hyperspace Analog to Language (HAL),
If the matching process fails to identify a unique that derives word representations from a large
referent, then pronoun resolution will be de- text corpus of 300 million words (Burgess et al.,
layed or the reader may invoke a problem- 1998; Burgess & Lund, 1997; Lund & Burgess,
solving strategy to select a referent. Implicit 1996). The corpus contains conversations from
causality may be one of the factors that affects a variety of Internet news groups. HAL con-
the matching process, leading to the early iden- structs a matrix of co-occurrence values for the
tification of the pronoun’s referent. 70,000 most frequently occurring words in the
Implicit causality appeared to have its effect corpus. The co-occurrence values are computed
on the behavior of skilled readers by decreasing within a 10-word moving window and are in-
the accessibility of entities other than the im- versely proportional to the number of words
plied cause. As we mentioned previously, this is separating a specific pair. Rows in the matrix
consistent with Gernsbacher’s (1990) claim that encode information about the extent to which a
the mechanism of suppression plays an impor- word was preceded by other words in the cor-
564 LONG AND DE LEY

pus; columns encode information about the ex- explain why skilled readers exhibited an im-
tent to which a word was followed by other plicit causality effect in the context of NP 2
words. Complete knowledge about a word is verbs, but not in the context of NP 1 verbs.
represented by the concatenation of a word’s However, this correspondence between subse-
row and column vector. These vectors (with a quent mention in the corpus data and the acces-
length of 140,000 values) can be viewed as the sibility of the referents in the reaction-time data
coordinates of points in a high-dimensional se- does not necessarily mean that one caused the
mantic space, with each word occupying one other. Both may be due to some other underly-
point. Using this representation, differences be- ing factor. For example, we found grammatical
tween two words’ co-occurrence vectors can be differences between the verbs that could be
measured as the distance between the high- responsible for asymmetries in both the corpus
dimensional points defined by their vectors. and the reaction-time data. NP 2 verbs were
Burgess et al. (1998) examined the vector more likely than NP 1 verbs to appear in the
representations of 15 NP 1 and 13 NP 2 verbs. past-tense/transitive/active voice. HAL’s vector
(Their corpus included 8 of the NP 1 and 9 of the representations appear to be sensitive to the
NP 2 verbs used in our study.) They applied a grammatical contexts in which words are used
multidimensional scaling (MDS) procedure to (Burgess & Lund, 1997). Thus, NP 2 verbs may
the verbs’ co-occurrence vectors to obtain a cluster more closely because they are more
spatial representation of the distances between likely than NP 1 verbs to appear in the same gram-
them. They found that the NP 2 verbs were well matical form. The grammatical differences be-
differentiated from the NP 1 verbs; however, the tween the verbs may also influence pronoun res-
NP 1 verbs were only marginally differentiated olution, perhaps by affecting the ease with which
from the NP 2 verbs. In other words, the NP 2 information can be integrated across clauses.
verbs were clustered more closely in the high- Whichever factor is responsible for differ-
dimensional space than were NP 1 verbs. In ad- ences in the contextual use of NP 1 and NP 2
dition, they found some subcategorization verbs, skilled readers appear to be sensitive to it.
within each verb type. For example, the NP 2 They resolve a pronoun referent to the implied
verbs, loathe, despise, hate, abhor, and detest, cause of an event earlier in comprehension
formed a cluster. Likewise, the NP 1 verbs, de- when the pronoun follows an NP 2 verb than
ceive, annoy, repel, and disappoint, formed when it follows an NP 1 verb. The differences
their own cluster. Interestingly, these subclasses that we observed in the performance of our
of NP 1 and NP 2 verbs were quite distant in the skilled and less skilled readers may be due to
space even though both sets contain words with processing differences between the two groups.
negative connotations. Skilled readers, who tend to have faster and
Burgess et al.’s (1998) results, in combina- more accurate word-recognition skills than do
tion with the results of our corpus analysis, less skilled readers, may have more resources
suggest that the contextual use of NP 1 and NP 2 available to elaborate their discourse represen-
verbs is somewhat different. The contexts in tations with appropriate world knowledge, in-
which NP 2 verbs are used appear to be more cluding knowledge about the implied cause of
similar than are the contexts in which NP 1 verbs the event (Perfetti, 1985; Long et al., 1994,
are used. The asymmetry in the verbs’ use may 1997). Processing differences alone, however,
be due to differences in the verbs’ implicit cau- do not explain why skilled readers show an
sality. We found that NP 2 verbs were more early effect of implicit causality in the context
likely than NP 1 verbs to be followed by the of NP 2, but not in the context of NP 1, verbs.
cause of the event and were more likely to be Alternatively, differences in skilled and less
followed by a reference to the entity who was skilled readers’ performance may be related to
the implied cause (i.e., NP 2). differences in reading experience. Skilled read-
We have suggested that NP 2 verbs are better ers, who generally have more reading experi-
predictors of the subsequent mention of an im- ence than do less skilled readers, may have
plied cause than are NP 1 verbs and that this may encoded more information about the contextual
IMPLICIT CAUSALITY AND DISCOURSE FOCUS 565

use of individual words, perhaps in the manner (16) Jenny fascinated Sara (at the concert) because she
suggested by lexicalist accounts of language was a wonderful musician.
(17) Mike flabbergasted Frank (at work) because he
comprehension (e.g., MacDonald, Pearlmutter,
made the coffee.
& Seidenberg, 1994). This might lead skilled (18) June frightened Kay (in the car) because she ran a
readers to expect the implied cause to be the red light.
referent of a subsequent pronoun when the main (19) Joel humiliated Alex (at the party) because he
event contains an NP 2 verb used in its transitive, wanted a scapegoat.
active voice, but not when it contains an NP 1 (20) Cindy infuriated Brenda (at work) because she was
always late for meetings.
verb in this form. (21) Steve inspired Bill (this year) because he volun-
In summary, our findings suggest that the teered at a homeless shelter.
locus of implicit causality effects depend on (22) Jill intimidated Gwen (before the game) because she
important characteristics of the stimuli and of was so athletic.
the reader. Implicit causality appears to play a (23) Randy peeved Jerry (yesterday afternoon) because
role in pronoun resolution, but only among he made such a mess.
(24) Becky scared Nancy (last night) because she lost
skilled readers and only for those verbs that control of her temper.
exhibit NP 2 causality.
Verb Bias ⫽ NP 2
APPENDIX A (1) Mark admired James (last year) because he was such
a good student.
Experimental Sentences for (2) Beth adored Joy (for years) because she was the
Experiments 1, 2, and 3 nicest person in the world.
Congruent sentences appeared in all experiments (infor- (3) Richard appreciated Charles (last week) because he
mation in parentheses appeared in Experiment 3 only). was such a good listener.
Incongruent sentences appeared in Experiment 3. (4) Sharon assisted Debra (at the office) because she had
an urgent deadline.
Congruent Sentences (5) Glen blamed Phil (after the game) because he missed
Verb Bias ⫽ NP 1 the goal.
(1) John aggravated Dick (last night) because he played (6) Claire comforted Grace (last week) because she
the stereo too loud. broke up with her boyfriend.
(2) Kate agitated Jane (at the movies) because she (7) Eric commended Andy (at the ceremony) because he
smacked her gum continuously.
saved the child’s life.
(3) Walter amazed Ronald (on vacation) because he went
(8) Amy congratulated Helen (yesterday afternoon) be-
skydiving.
cause she won the race.
(4) Sandra amused Carol (at the party) because she told
(9) Sam consoled Ken (last week) because he lost his dog.
funny jokes.
(10) Eve deplored Faith (for many years) because she
(5) Rick angered Scott (at the park) because he was cruel
was a mean-spirited gossip.
to animals.
(11) Gary despised Alan (for several years) because he
(6) Ann annoyed Pam (this morning) because she kept
was rude to everyone.
fidgeting in class.
(7) David apologized to Peter (after work) because he (12) Joanne detested Cathy (for years) because she stole
was sorry for his mistake. money from a church.
(8) Janet bored Ellen (at dinner) because she told the (13) Paul dreaded Jeff (at work) because he said such
same story three times. cruel things.
(9) Tom charmed Jack (at the interview) because he was (14) Liz envied Marge (last year) because she won the
so debonair. state lottery.
(10) Sue cheated Gail (yesterday afternoon) because she (15) Matthew feared Albert (at school) because he was
wanted to win. such an aggressive person.
(11) Donald concerned Robert (at school) because he (16) Donna honored Lucy (at the event) because she
hadn’t been to class in a week. deserved the award.
(12) Tina confessed to Lisa (the next day) because she (17) Greg liked Ted (at work) because he was so easy to
felt guilty. get along with.
(13) Luke deceived Dean (at the trial) because he didn’t (18) Peg mourned Dawn (after the accident) because she
want to be convicted. died much too young.
(14) Lynn disappointed Ruth (last week) because she (19) Danny noticed Gary (at the concert) because he was
didn’t come to the party. wearing such bright colors.
(15) Brian exasperated Harry (last night) because he (20) Patty praised Linda (after the meeting) because she
dropped the roast on the floor. had made a good presentation.
566 LONG AND DE LEY

(21) Neal reassured Bruce (after the game) because he Verb Bias ⫽ NP 2
needed an ego boost. (1) Mark admired James last year because he needed to
(22) Joan resented Bev (during childhood) because she admire someone.
was the favorite daughter. (2) Beth adored Joy for years because she adored almost
(23) Edward thanked Douglass (yesterday morning) be- everyone.
cause he had washed the car. (3) Richard appreciated Charles last week because he
(24) Diane valued Gina (these days) because she was thought it was appropriate.
such a good friend. (4) Sharon assisted Debra at the office because she
wanted to be useful.
Incongruent Sentences (5) Glen blamed Phil after the game because he needed
Verb Bias ⫽ NP 1 to blame someone.
(1) John aggravated Dick last night because he was eas- (6) Claire comforted Grace last week because she
ily aggravated. wanted to help.
(2) Kate agitated Jane at the movies because she was (7) Eric commended Andy at the ceremony because he
already feeling anxious. thought it was proper.
(3) Walter amazed Ronald on vacation because he was (8) Amy congratulated Helen yesterday afternoon be-
cause she liked the performance.
amazed so easily.
(9) Sam consoled Ken last week because he wanted to be
(4) Sandra amused Carol at the party because she was
of assistance.
easily entertained.
(10) Eve deplored Faith for many years because she
(5) Rick angered Scott at the park because he was al-
deplored most people.
ready feeling angry.
(11) Gary despised Alan for several years because he
(6) Ann annoyed Pam this morning because she got
was jealous.
annoyed so easily.
(12) Joanne detested Cathy for years because she de-
(7) David apologized to Peter after work because he
tested almost everyone.
demanded an apology.
(13) Paul dreaded Jeff at work because he was so
(8) Janet bored Ellen at dinner because she had already
envious.
heard the story.
(14) Liz envied Marge last year because she had low
(9) Tom charmed Jack at the interview because he was
self-esteem.
so gullible.
(15) Matthew feared Albert at school because he feared
(10) Sue cheated Gail yesterday afternoon because she
most people.
was easy to deceive.
(16) Donna honored Lucy at the event because she
(11) Donald concerned Robert at school because he was thought the honor was deserved.
responsible for the students. (17) Greg liked Ted at work because he liked most
(12) Tina confessed to Lisa the next day because she people.
would have found out anyway. (18) Peg mourned Dawn after the accident because she
(13) Luke deceived Dean at the trial because he was so believed that life was sacred.
unsuspecting. (19) Danny noticed Gary at the concert because he was
(14) Lynn disappointed Ruth last week because she set an observant person.
herself up for disappointment. (20) Patty praised Linda after the meeting because she
(15) Brian exasperated Harry last night because he was wanted to be polite.
expecting more cooperation. (21) Neal reassured Bruce after the game because he
(16) Jenny fascinated Sara at the concert because she wanted to be helpful.
was easily captivated. (22) Joan resented Bev during childhood because she
(17) Mike flabbergasted Frank at work because he was was filled with resentment.
expecting someone else. (23) Edward thanked Douglass yesterday morning be-
(18) June frightened Kay in the car because she got cause he thought it was polite.
scared so easily. (24) Diane valued Gina these days because she valued all
(19) Joel humiliated Alex at the party because he was an of her friends.
easy target.
(20) Cindy infuriated Brenda at work because she was APPENDIX B
already feeling angry.
(21) Steve inspired Bill this year because she was des- Examples Containing NP 1 Verbs
perate for inspiration.
Subject Referent ⫽ NP 1 (consistent with verb bias)
(22) Jill intimidated Gwen before the game because she
was such a wimp. Relation between clauses ⫽ Cause
(23) Randy peeved Jerry yesterday afternoon because he (1) Allen Edwards exasperated Pitino when he came
was already irritable. back slowly from an ankle injury.
(24) Becky scared Nancy last night because she was (2) George, who was appointed by Governor Pete Wil-
so timid. son, infuriated abortion opponents. First, he dissented from
IMPLICIT CAUSALITY AND DISCOURSE FOCUS 567

the court’s original ruling upholding the law, and then he blanket condemnation of a category of work they didn’t
granted a rehearing that led to its being overturned. like.”
(3) Alan Schlosser, managing ACLU attorney, said po-
lice intimidated residents. They essentially forced their way Subject Referent ⫽ NP 2 (inconsistent with verb bias)
into apartments, sometimes by blocking the door with their
foot. Relation between Clauses ⫽ Cause
(4) Oracle Corp.’s second-quarter results disappointed (21) Roe flabbergasted both sides because neither ex-
Wall Street. The Redwood Shores-based company’s net pected the court to go so far.
income rose only slightly to $187.3 million, 19 cents a (22) She said a second boyfriend intimidated her because
share, from $179.5 million, 18 cents. she feared serious injury.
(5) I apologized to AirTouch Communications CEO Sam (23) Maureen Malloy D’Honau’s account of her hus-
Ginn. I had lampooned his company a few years earlier over band’s last weeks of life in an acute-care hospital (“On
the name AirTouch; I thought it was stupid, but it has grown machine: His doctors avoided dying patient’s agony,” Opin-
on me. ion Page, Nov. 5) saddened and angered me. As a physician,
(6) A state Board of Education vote granting a lucrative I found her story quite believable; such experiences are all
contract for a new statewide achievement test angered San too common and could occur in most hospitals in our
Francisco officials because it means The City may have to country.
shelve years of testing information on its students. (24) He immediately assisted a small delegation of Fran-
(7) A California Supreme Court ruling in August in- ciscan friars who were desperately trying to return to Assisi.
spired the measure. The ruling overturned a never-enforced (25) Debra J. Saunders definitely cheated The Chronicle
1987 law that required unmarried teenagers seeking abor- on December 23. You paid her for a full column of repeat
tions to get permission from a parent, guardian or judge. words.
(8) ACT intimidated me. Everybody there was so in- (26) The press response flabbergasted me. I talked to
volved with acting. critics who were practically weeping.
(9) Prisons always fascinated me because they embody (27) The school’s decision flabbergasted me. I can’t be-
everything that’s right and wrong about a country. lieve the girls will have to be out there for four hours a
(10) This statement annoyed me because it is totally night.
untrue and unfair to those who might be going to see the (28) The street paintings fascinated the critics, who saw
shows at the Fillmore. them as a new form of contemporary art.
(29) The scrutiny scared me. I knew what was coming
Relation between Clauses ⫽ Consequence and I knew I wasn’t ready for it.
(11) A man living in a nearby apartment frightened the (30) Mythology fascinated me. I found that a classic
person away. He then called the police who arrived only “good vs. evil” story touched people deeply.
minutes later.
(12) In 1992, FMC similarly deceived the Army. The Relation between Clauses ⫽ Consequence
corporation was sued and charged with budgeting more for (31) The Contra Costa County district attorney’s office
research than it had actually spent. so intimidated Wills, he was no longer willing to testify for
(13) The rookie armored car guard who allegedly shot the defense.
his partner to death and stole more than $300,000 confessed (32) Judith’s story inspired the heroes of the Hanukkah
to Oakland detectives on a rainy Utah highway. He was story. They retook their temple from the Greeks in the
charged in the shooting death of Rodrigo Cortez Monday second century B.C.E.
night. (33) The grotesque result humiliated her so much that
(14) Uwaezuoke apologized to the team yesterday morn- she felt physically and spiritually destroyed.
ing, so he gets his job back Sunday night in Seattle in the (34) Bart negotiators’ departure infuriated union offi-
final regular-season game. cials. They demanded the other side return to the table, but
(15) The sandhill crane’s courting behavior charmed the state mediators approved the recess and asked negotiators to
visitors so much that it was sometimes disrupted by the late return at 10 a.m. Friday.
afternoon crowds. (35) Seems reasonable to us, but it infuriated Ammiano.
(16) The board’s intrusion angered Newman. They tried He objected and forced the matter off the board’s calendar
to appease him by delaying the vote until next Thursday. and back to committee.
(17) The governor infuriated Reddy’s supporters and (36) The threat scared a lot of people. They were afraid
then he gave a scathing response to their criticisms at to come to the meeting.
Thursday’s press conference. (37) When he heard her soprano, it frightened him. He
(18) They believed they cheated death, so they embraced jumped off her lap and ran away.
all manner of self-destructive behavior. (38) Symonds attended a New York City public high
(19) Bickerstaff cheated scores of elderly clients. He was school where classmates intimidated her. She often wakes
arrested on Thursday at his home in Los Angeles. up in the middle of the night, riven by images of cowering
(20) Columbia University Professor and MacArthur “ge- in a classroom.
nius award” winner Sherry Ortner was flabbergasted by the (39) The lingerie ads fascinated the Sherpas. They filled
decision. She immediately accused the university of “a a scrapbook of the ads to take back home to their families.
568 LONG AND DE LEY

(40) When he was 13, his aunt took him to the temples of (57) My family and I greatly appreciated your special
Japan where the monks fascinated him. In college, he took Datebook section on the National Endowment for the Arts.
courses in world religions, holistic health and Eastern per- I am sending the following statement to Senators Barbara
spectives. Boxer, Dianne Feinstein and Slade Gordon and Represen-
tative Ralph Regula: I and my family urge you to protect
Examples Containing NP 2 Verbs and, if possible, even increase the appropriation for the
NEA.
Subject Referent ⫽ NP 1 (inconsistent with verb bias) (58) In Vietnam, my sister and I adored this dish. We ate
Relation between Clauses ⫽ Cause it two or three times a week.
(41) Indeed the 49ers needed and much appreciated the (59) Tony adored the script. He contacted his agent im-
break. They have played 20 games over the last 5-1/4 mediately.
months. (60) The Vietnamese believed that cows and buffaloes
(42) Venus is different, all right, and many of her fellow were indispensable beasts of burden and greatly valued
players resented her. They just can’t understand all the them. They ate little red meat, preferring pork, chicken and
attention she gets. seafood.
(43) Analysts commended HP. They believe that the
company has given priority to strong revenue growth. Subject Referent ⫽ NP 2 (consistent with verb bias)
(44) Actors revered Mr. Zinnemann. They admired his
Relation between Clauses ⫽ Cause
patient attention to their performances.
(61) Christopulos thanked the custodians of the Season
(45) The Dodgers resented such exultation. They are
of Sharing Fund. “They made it possible for dear and
likely emotionally incapable of it themselves.
(46) Many of his peers detested Burton. They found him grateful friends of Wally’s to perpetuate his memory in a
offensive at best and intolerable at worst. manner that I know would please him.”
(47) Most residents in the neighborhood deplored Lantier’s (62) I appreciated the picture of a rainbow over The City.
arrest. They felt that he was just protecting himself. It seemed a perfect way to commemorate a very special
(48) I appreciated Dan Rather’s column “A new ’Great event that took place Sunday afternoon.
Game’ at the Caspian Sea” (Opinion Page, Dec. 2). I (63) In a report last month to San Mateo County super-
thought that the closing warning, “So much attention is visors, Cullen blamed a retaining wall. The wall failed and
centered on the Persian Gulf and troubles with Iraq and Iran caused the slide.
that the rise of the campaigns for control of the Caspian Sea (64) Officials who happily lured the Raiders back from
has been nearly ignored,” fit in well with the latest devel- Los Angeles in 1995 blamed the football team. It failed to
opments at the Kyoto conference. cooperate with efforts to take some of the burden off tax-
(49) Her parents blamed the sound of a slamming door. payers.
They thought it frightened her soul “out of her body.” (65) “I never knew Savio, but I much admired him. He
(50) In a letter to the Santa Cruz community released had an unassuming nature, basic decency, and eloquency
yesterday, Jacobson thanked his friends and family. He when the time called for it.”
believes that their support helped him to be a survivor rather (66) The Archbishop of Canterbury George Carey com-
than a victim. mended the royal couple. “They have kept their marriage
vows through the profoundest of changes in personal and
Relation between Clauses ⫽ Consequence national life.”
(51) Van Buren feared Southern voters’ anger. He ma- (67) Most of the children resented court-ordered visita-
neuvered to have the judge presiding over the trial replaced tion schedules. Such schedules didn’t take into account
by one sympathetic to his interests. either their terror at traveling alone when they were young
(52) Ayala feared his wife. A couple of times, he brought or their own plans when they got older.
young Gilbert with him on the night shift, letting him sleep (68) In other words, she said, the white American middle
in the car as Ayala scuttled between him and the building class resented them because they are seen as moral invaders
that he was supposed to be watching. to the prevailing political, social and economic order.
(53) In fact, environmentalists revered him so much they (69) Marino blamed the media because they stirred the
dissuaded President Clinton from appointing him to the emotions of the community.
Supreme Court so he could regain some of the momentum (70) Slade commended the European leaders. “They
that had disappeared during three Republican presidential adopted an aggressive stance on cutting emissions and held
terms. out hope that the United States would follow.”
(54) “I dreaded it. I waited to the last minute to drive up
from Los Angeles,” said Sonya Enchill, 21, a second-year Relation between Clauses ⫽ Consequence
student and an African American. (71) Her mother, Chris Zupanovich, an administrator in
(55) Rabin so despised phony politicking, he was dubbed Levi Strauss’ human resources department, and father, Dan
“the undiplomatic diplomat.” De Vries, a fund-raiser with the National Center for Youth
(56) I’ve always despised the concept of shopping-as- Law, immediately reassured her. Gina, with her parents’
religion. So, I can’t stand Blomingdale’s. blessing, began attending programs at the Lavender Youth
IMPLICIT CAUSALITY AND DISCOURSE FOCUS 569

Recreation and Information Center in the Castro District Gernsbacher, M. A. (1989). Mechanisms that improve ref-
and has since become a vocal advocate for gay rights. erential access. Cognition, 32, 99 –156.
(72) I congratulated her on this achievement. She only Gernsbacher, M. A. (1990). Language comprehension as
laughed. structure building. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
(73) He blamed the offensive line. The linemen just Gernsbacher, M. A., & Faust, (1991). The mechanism of
dismissed his comments and chalked them up to post-game suppression: A component of general comprehension
spin. skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
(74) Diana says she adored Hewitt. He took advantage of Memory, and Cognition, 17, 245–262.
that when her marriage crumbled. Gernsbacher, M. A., & Hargreaves, D. (1988). Accessing
(75) Soviet authorities detested Paradjanov. He managed sentence participants: The advantage of first mention.
to make only three films in the 26 years after “Shadows.” Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 699 –717.
(76) My fellow clerks also resented the Spanish-speak- Gernsbacher, M. A., & Robertson, R. R. W. (1995). Read-
ing customers. They were frequently denied service or ing skill and suppression revisited. Psychological Sci-
served last. ence, 6, 165–169.
(77) The study found that the mother’s heartbeat com- Gernsbacher, M. A., Varner, K. R., & Faust, M. (1990).
forted the babies. They fell asleep much faster when they Investigating differences in general comprehension
were held on the left than on the right side. skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
(78) The FBI director admired Reno, so she relied on his Memory, and Cognition, 16, 430 – 445.
support during this administration. Graesser, A. C., & Clark, L. F. (1985). Structures and
(79) Nivette adored the child. She blossomed under his procedures of implicit knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Erl-
care. baum.
(80) Federal Reserve Chairman Alan Greenspan, who Greene, S. B., & McKoon, G. (1995). Telling something we
told Congress that financial troubles in Asian economies can’t know: Experimental approaches to verbs exhib-
don’t appear poised to “threaten prosperity in this country,” iting implicit causality. Psychological Science, 6, 262–
reassured investors. They reacted by pushing the market up 270.
over 100 points. Greene, S. B., McKoon, G., & Ratcliff. R. (1992). Pronoun
resolution and discourse models. Journal of Experi-
REFERENCES mental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cogni-
tion, 18, 266 –283.
Au, T. K. (1986). A verb is worth a thousand words: The Grosz, B. (1981). Focusing and description in natural lan-
causes and consequences of interpersonal events im- guage dialogues. In A. K. Joshi, B. L. Webber, & I. A.
plicit in language. Journal of Memory and Language, Sag (Eds.), Elements of discourse understanding (pp.
25, 104 –122. 84 –105). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press.
Brown, R., & Fish, D. (1983). The psychological causality Grosz, B., & Sidner, C. (1986). Attention, intentions and the
implicit in language. Cognition, 14, 237–273. structure of discourse. Computational Linguistics, 12,
Burgess, C., Livesay, K., & Lund, K. (1998). Explorations 175–204.
in context space: Words, sentences, discourse. Dis- Lee-Sammons, W. H., & Whitney, P. (1991). Reading per-
course Processes, 25, 211–257. spectives and memory for text: An individual differ-
Burgess, C., & Lund, D. (1997). Dissociating semantic and ences analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
associative word relationships using high-dimensional Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 1074 –1081.
semantic space. In Proceedings of the 19th Annual Long, D. L., & Golding, J. M. (1993). Superordinate goal
Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 263– inferences: Are they automatically generated during
269). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. comprehension? Discourse Processes, 16, 55–74.
Caramazza, A., Grober, E., Garvey, C., & Yates, J. (1977). Long, D. L., Oppy, B. J., & Seely, M. R. (1994). Individual
Comprehension of anaphoric pronouns. Journal of differences in the time course of inferential processing.
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 601– 609. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Mem-
Garnham, A. (1981). Mental models as representations of ory, and Cognition, 20, 1456 –1470.
text. Memory & Cognition, 9, 560 –565. Long, D. L., Oppy, b. J., & Seely, M. R. (1997). Individual
Garnham, A. (1987). Mental models as representations of differences in readers’ sentence- and text-level repre-
discourse and text. Chichester, UK: Ellis Horwood. sentations. Journal of Memory and Language, 36,
Garnham, A., Traxler, M., Oakhill, J., & Gernsbacher, 129 –145.
M. A. (1996). The locus of implicit causality effects in Lund, K., & Burgess, C. (1996). Producing high-dimen-
comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, sional semantic spaces from lexical co-occurrence. Be-
517–543. havior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers,
Garvey, C., & Caramazza, A. (1974). Implicit causality in 28, 203–208.
verbs. Linguistic Inquiry, 5, 459 – 464. MacDonald, M. C., Pearlmutter, N. J., & Seidenberg, M. S.
Garvey, C., Caramazza, A., & Yates, J. (1975). Factors (1994). The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity res-
influencing assignment of pronoun antecedents. Cog- olution. Psychological Review, 101, 676 –703.
nition, 3, 227–243. McDonald, J. L., & MacWhinney, B. (1995). The time
570 LONG AND DE LEY

course of anaphor resolution: Effects of implicit verb Oakhill, J., Yuill, N., & Donaldson, M. L. (1990). Under-
causality and gender. Journal of Memory and Lan- standing of causal expressions in skilled and less
guage, 34, 543–566. skilled text comprehenders. British Journal of Devel-
McKoon, G., Greene, S. B., & Ratcliff, R. (1993). Discourse opmental Psychology, 8, 401– 410.
models, pronoun resolution, and the implicit causality of Pearlmutter, N. J., & MacDonald, M. C. (1995). Individual
verbs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, differences and probabilistic constraints in syntactic
Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1040 –1052. ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Lan-
McKoon, G., & Ratcliff, R. (1992). Inference during read- guage, 34, 521–542.
ing. Psychological Review, 99, 440 – 466. Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford
McNamara, D. S., & Kintsch, W. (1996). Learning from Univ. Press.
texts: Effects of prior knowledge and text coherence.
Sanford, A. J., & Garrod, S. C. (1998). The role of scenario
Discourse Processes, 22, 247–288.
mapping in text comprehension. Discourse Processes,
Millis, K. K., & Just, M. A. (1994). The influence of
26, 159 –190.
connectives on sentence comprehension. Journal of
Van Petten, C., Weckerly, J., McIsaac, H. K., & Kutas, M.
Memory and Language, 33, 128 –147.
(1997). Working memory capacity dissociates lexical
Oakhill, J. V. (1983). Instantiation in skilled and less-skilled
comprehenders. Quarterly Journal of Experimental and sentential context effects. Psychological Science,
Psychology, 35A, 441– 450. 8, 238 –242.
Oakhill, J. V. (1984). Inferential and memory skills in Vonk, W. (1985). The immediacy of inferences in the
children’s comprehension of stories. British Journal of understanding of pronouns. In G. Rickheit & H.
Educational Psychology, 54, 31–39. Strohner (Eds.), Inferences in text processing (pp. 205–
Oakhill, J. (1994). Individual differences in children’s text 218). Amsterdam: North–Holland.
comprehension. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Hand-
book of psycholinguistics (pp. 821– 848). San Diego, (Received July 20, 1999)
CA: Academic Press. (Revision received October 22, 1999)

You might also like