Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Quality of Life Research (2005) 14: 1345–1355  Springer 2005

DOI 10.1007/s11136-004-6013-z

Quality of life of children with language delays

Heleen M.E. van Agt1, Marie-Louise Essink-Bot1, Heleen A. van der Stege2, J.G. de Ridder-Sluiter2 &
Harry J. de Koning1
1
Department of Public Health, Erasmus MC, Rotterdam (E-mail: h.vanagt@erasmusmc.nl); 2Dutch
Foundation for the Deaf and Hard of Hearing Child, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Accepted in revised form 3 November 2004

Abstract

We investigated health-related quality of life (HRQOL) of children with language problems and controls.
Data on language development (Language Screening Instrument 3-years-olds, Van Wiechen items) and
HRQOL by means of the TNO-AZL Pre-school children Quality of Life-questionnaire (TAPQOL) were
collected at age 3 in a population-based cohort by parental questionnaire (n=8877, response 78%; mean
age 39.1 months (SD 2.0), 4347 were girls). Cronbach’s a (internal consistency) ranged between 0.63 and
0.85. Dependent on the definition of language problem, 131 to 316 children appeared to be language
impaired. Receiver Operating Characteristic analyses (ROC-curves) to assess the discriminative ability of
six TAPQOL scales revealed that the Communication scale and Social Functioning scale discriminated best
between children with language problems and children without these problems. Language-impaired chil-
dren had significantly lower scores on the Communication scale and Social Functioning scale as compared
to children without language problems (p<0.01). The findings indicate that language problems at age three
can have an impact on children’s social life. These results provide additional evidence for the importance of
monitoring the language development and its consequences during childhood.

Key words: Health Related Quality of Life, Language development, Pre-school children

Introduction educational performance and social development.


However, at the time the child’s language devel-
Language development of young children shows opment is relatively behind as compared to its
large variability. A variety of methods based on peers this may also have an impact on the pres-
different reference tests have been proposed to ent, for instance resulting in problems in com-
assess serious language delays in young children municating with other people, problems in social
[1–3]. However, there is no generally accepted functioning in general and in emotional or
method of case definition or gold standard in this behavioural problems. Most studies focus on the
field [4]. Moreover, there seems to be not a clear consequences of language delays for learning
distinction between delayed or deviant language abilities and school performance or for behaviour
development [5]. Among children aged 0–7 years, [9–12], although some research more specially
the prevalence of serious language delays is esti- concerned social skills and pragmatic ability [13,
mated to be 5–10%, depending on the criterion of 14]. Few studies have investigated the general
language delay used [4, 6–8]. impact of language delays on the child’s daily life
Some language delays seem to spontaneously or quality of life.
recover in the pre-school period; some delays Despite the availability of instruments to mea-
persist and may have consequences for future sure Health Related Quality of Life (HRQOL) in
1346

children [15–18] HRQOL of children with lan- Specialists’ judgement


guage problems has not been reported. From the regional Speech and Hearing Centres
We investigated HRQOL of children with lan- (SHC) we obtained the personal details of chil-
guage delays in a population-based cohort of dren who visited one of these SHC’s before the
11.000 children aged 3 years old using the TNO- age of three. Linking these files with the cohort
AZL Pre-school children Quality of Life-ques- data resulted in matched files of 338 children.
tionnaire (TAPQOL). The aim of the study was to After that, detailed file inspection was employed
determine HRQOL incurred by language delays resulting in diagnostic conclusions about 261 of
and the suitability of the TAPQOL in this cohort, these children.
using parental report of HRQOL and language In addition, to obtain diagnostic conclusions
development, and additional information from about children who visited specialists elsewhere
specialists and an expert-panel. we collected follow up data on a group of chil-
dren with (possible) speech/language problems.
Therefore we selected all children whose parents
Method had indicated in the questionnaire that their
child had visited a specialist because of speech
Population and data collection procedures en/or language problems (n=524). We asked the
parents by postal letter for the address details of
We used data from a large randomized controlled the specialist involved, provided the parents gave
trial to assess the accuracy of language-screening their written consent. The parents of 239 chil-
at age 1.5 and 2 and to assess the effect of specific dren supplied specialist’s addresses (for 7 of
language-screening on language development and these 239 children the written consent was lost,
HRQOL at age 3 [19]. Child health care centres, for 17 children the specialist address was incor-
providing preventive care to children in the 0– rect en the specialists of 55 children were not
4 years age range, in 6 regions in the Netherlands approached because of logistic reasons). The
with about 15,000 children in care participated. specialists (comprising speech therapists, ear/nose
To identify children with a diagnosed language and throat specialists and paediatricians) of in
problem we used three criteria based on (1) parent total 160 children received a questionnaire in
report, (2) specialists’ judgment, and (3) an expert- which they could report their diagnostic results
panel. Details of the data collection procedures and value the child’s language development (on
have also been described elsewhere [19]. three aspects: language production, receptive
language and communication skills) on a 5-point
Parent report scale (‘fine’ to ‘much below standard’). Accord-
The parents of 11,412 children were sent a postal ing to this criterion children were having a
questionnaire at the time of their child’s third diagnosed language problem if the diagnosis was
birthday. The questionnaire included six scales of ‘language problem’/‘late start of language devel-
the TNO-AZL Pre-school children Quality of Life- opment’ (following the ICIDH coding system) or
questionnaire (TAPQOL) [20], three validated ‘language development below standard’. This
language instruments (or parts thereof) for the resulted in specialists’ judgments of in total 123
relevant age group [21–23], and questions con- children.
cerning any language problems or related treat-
ment in the first 3 years of life. In total 8877 Expert-panel
questionnaires were returned (response 78%). Finally, the same group of children with (possible)
Children were identified as having a diagnosed speech/language problems plus children who failed
language problem if parents reported: My child on the screening instrument (n=554) was assessed
has been assessed by a specialist: My child’s by an expert-panel of 6 professionals in the field of
vocabulary contained too few words for his/her children’s language development. For each child a
age/My child had little to no spontaneous speech/ computer generated overview was compiled, which
My child could not understand others, or could contained information from the parent question-
understand others only with difficulty. naire. The overview included the child’s language
1347

‘‘history’’, the assessment of health professionals Health Related Quality of Life


as reported by the parents, and the scores on the
language measures. Three experts had assessed The TAPQOL is a generic, multidimensional
each child independently by random assignment, instrument covering 12 domains of the Health
so that each expert assessed 279 children. No Related Quality of Life (HRQOL) of 1–5 year old
information was given about the child’s status (i.e. children. The TAPQOL was normed and validated
intervention group, control group, screened or not on the basis of a sample of 362 children aged 1–
screened) nor about the screening (outcome). Each 5 years from the general population [20]. In the
expert was asked to assess independently whether parental questionnaire at age 3 we include six do-
there was or had been a language delay (on a 5- mains of the TAPQOL relevant for HRQOL of
point scale: ‘no language delay’, ‘probably no language problems, namely Communication, So-
language delay’, ‘I don’t know’, ‘yes, probably cial functioning, Anxiety, Positive mood, Problem
language delay’, ‘yes, definitely’) and seriousness behaviour and Liveliness. Each item of the Com-
(‘minor’, ‘rather’, ‘major’, ‘unknown’, ‘not appli- munication scale comprises an objective and a
cable’). We first conducted a pilot to ensure that subjective part. Firstly, a question is asked about
they all used a similar criterion. For each child, the the frequency (‘never’, ‘sometimes’ and ‘often’) of
three individual expert assessments were integrated the symptom, for example: ‘Did your child have
into a combined (average) assessment. Children (any) difficulties in understanding what other
with a diagnosed language problem were identified people say?’ Secondly, the subjective part of the
if the (average) assessment was ‘yes, definitely item is assessed by asking about the emotion of the
language problem’ or ‘yes, probably language child if the child is having the symptom ‘some-
problem’; or – if the panel’s report was ‘I don’t times’ or ‘often’ (‘at that time, my child felt’: ‘fine’,
know’ – the specialists’ judgment was inputted. ‘not too well’, ‘rather badly’, ‘badly’) (Appendix I
Table 1 shows the number of children with shows all items of the Communication scale). The
language problems and children without language score on each item can range from 0 (frequency of
problems according to the three grouping vari- complaint ‘never’) to 4 (frequency of complaint
ables. Some characteristics of children with and ‘sometimes’ or ‘often’, and emotional reaction to
without language problems (according to parents) the complaint: ‘badly’). Summing up the scores on
are given in Table 2. the four items of the Communication scale results
in the scale score, which ranges from 0 to 16. The
items of the other five scales refer to specific states,
Table 1. Number of children with and without diagnosed moods or activities of the child, each consisting of
language problems according to three grouping variables a single question about the frequency of these
(n=8877) items. Problem behaviour is measured by asking
Parents Specialists Panel
how often the child is conducting short-tempered,
hostile, irritated, angry, restless or impatient,
Language problem 252 131 316 rebellious and obstinate; Positive mood is indi-
No language problem 8625 8747 8561 cated by the frequency that the child is cheerful,
Total 8877 8877 8877
content, and happy. Being afraid, tense and anx-
ious are items of the Anxiety scale; being full of
Table 2. Characteristics of children with and without diag-
energy, active, and brisk refer to Liveliness; and
nosed language problems (according to parents) indications of Social Functioning are the frequency
of the child is playing with other children pleas-
Language No language All antly, being at ease with other children and feeling
problem problem
N=252 N=8625 N=8877
sure of him/herself with other children. The score
on each item ranges from 0 (‘never’) to 2 (‘often’).
Mean age (months) 39.6 (SD 2.6) 39.0 (SD 2.0) 39.1 (SD 2.0) For each scale the scale score is constructed by
% girls 27.0 49.7 49.0 summing up the item scores. Finally, all six scale-
Education mother:
scores are linearly transformed to a 0–100 scale,
% High vocational 12.8 20.3 20.1
where higher scores indicate a better HRQoL.
1348

Measures of language development internal consistency of the scales of the TAPQOL


was calculated with Cronbach’s a-coefficient [24].
The measures concern the productive and recep- Cronbach’s a of 0.70 or higher are considered
tive language development pertaining to the social being sufficient for group comparisons. The con-
use of language as well as the child’s linguistic struct validity of the TAPQOL scales was exam-
knowledge. The parental questionnaire included a ined by inspecting the Pearson correlation
Dutch language screening instrument called Lan- coefficients (PCC) between the TAPQOL scales
guage Screening Instrument for 3 to 4-year-olds and the scores on the language comprehension
(LSI), and Van Wiechen items (VW) [22, 23]. The measure (LSI-CT) and the language production
LSI consists of a parent form (LSI-PF), with measure (LSI-PF and VW). We assumed that the
questions about productive language as fluency, TAPQOL scales were conceptually different from
articulation and vocabulary (‘finding adequate the language development measures and hence
words’), and a child test (LSI-CT) to assess the expected low correlation coefficients between these
child’s receptive language development, for in- measures. At first, the patterns of PCC were
stance the ability to name objects or understand examined. It was hypothesised that scales which
orally given instructions. The items of VW concern refer directly to the ability of using language, as
questions about productive language as audibility, the Communication and Social Functioning scales,
vocabulary (‘Does your child make comprehensi- would exhibit higher correlation coefficients with
ble ‘‘sentences’’ of three words or more?’) and the language development measures than scales
grammar skills (‘Does your child use the following which are supposed to be not related to language
words to make questions? Where, what,…etc’) (see development in a direct way. In addition, in the
Appendix II for an overview of the items of LSI- Communication scale the relation between the
CT, LSI-PF and VW). The LSI-CT and LSI-PF questions about the frequency of language prob-
were normed and validated in an unselected pop- lems (objective part) and the language develop-
ulation of 1565 3-, 4- and 5-year old children from ment measures was compared with the relation
nursery schools, day care centres and child health between the questions about the (negative) impli-
centres. In the 3-year-old age-group (n=486), the cations of the reported problems (subjective part)
internal consistency and test–retest reliability of and the language development measures. The
the LSI-CF was found to be respectively around hypothesis was that reported problems (without
0.90 and 0.82; for the LSI-PF the internal consis- the associated implications) correlate higher with
tency was 0.73) [22]. A correlation of 0.60 was the language measures than the questions about
found between the LSI-CF scores and data on the (negative) implications of the reported prob-
speech therapy from a group of children with lems, as the language measures are supposed to
serious language delays. The VW fits into the measure the child’s language development in an
internationally accepted method of individual objective way without taking account of its
developmental surveillance as an approach of implications.
early detection of developmental delays, in com- Secondly, a factor analysis was performed to
bination with anamnesis, physical examination, examine the relationships among six scales of the
observation and information from the parents [23]. TAPQOL and the total scores of three measures of
The child’s language comprehension was measured language development, including LSI-CT, LSI-PF
by the LSI-CT and language production by sum- and VW.
ming up the scores of VW and LSI-PF. The discriminative ability of the TAPQOL
scales was examined in two ways. Firstly, the mean
Analyses scale-scores were compared between groups of
children who are supposed to differ in language
We used the number of missing values in the total development. We used the Mann–Whitney U test
sample as an indicator of the feasibility of the to test for statistically significant differences be-
TAPQOL-scales. Psychometric properties were tween these groups because of the nonnormal
determined by assessing the internal consistency, distribution of the data. Subsequently, receiver
construct validity and discriminative ability. The operating characteristic (ROC) curves were con-
1349

structed. The discriminative abilities of the TAP- in general, the percentages of missing values in all
QOL-scales were compared by calculating the area groups were low, ranging from 1.9 to 6.7 %, which
under the ROC curves [25, 26]: a value of 0.50 indicates that the TAPQOL is feasible in this
means that discrimination is equal to chance; if population. Five from the six scales appeared to
values are between 0.50 and 1.0, this means that meet the standard of Cronbach’s a for group
discrimination is better than chance. comparison.

Construct validity
Results
Table 4 shows the PCC correlation coefficients
Feasibility and reliability between the TAPQOL-scales and the language
development measures (comprehension and pro-
Table 3 presents an overview of the missing values duction) based on the data from all children in the
and Cronbach’s a of the TAPQOL-scales in the response group. It appeared that the Communi-
total population and in subgroups of children cation scale correlated relatively high with both
identified with language problems (according to language development measures, whereas for all
the expert-panel or clinicians and according to other scales including Social Functioning the cor-
parents). As expected, the completion of the 2-step relation between these measures was very low.
items of the Communication scale (Appendix II) Table 4 also presents the correlations between the
was more complex than the other items, yielding objective and subjective part of the Communica-
the highest percentage of missing values. However, tion scale and the language development measures.

Table 3. Cronbach’s as of 6 TAPQOL-scales in all children and in subgroups of children identified as having diagnosed language
problems according to two grouping variables

Scale No. of items All Diagnosed language Diagnosed language


N=8877 problems (parents) problems (panel)
N=252 N=316

a % missing a % missing a % missing

Communication 4 0.78 5.3 0.80 6.7 0.82 6.5


Problem behaviour 7 0.76 3.8 0.80 6.1 0.79 5.2
Positive mood 3 0.80 2.0 0.88 3.3 0.87 4.8
Liveliness 3 0.73 1.9 0.79 3.9 0.79 3.9
Social functioning 3 0.75 1.7 0.80 4.4 0.82 4.3
Anxiety 3 0.63 2.3 0.67 6.1 0.69 6.1

Table 4. Construct validity: Pearson correlation coefficients between TAPQOL scales and the measures of language comprehension
(LSI-CT) and language production (LSI-PF + VW) in all children

Scale Language comprehension (LSI-CT) Language production (LSI-PF + VW)

Communication 0.30 0.61


Communication: objective part 0.29 0.60
Communication: subjective part 0.16 0.34
Social functioning 0.11 0.16
Positive mood 0.11 0.12
Problem behaviour 0.08 0.11
Liveliness 0.09 0.09
Anxiety 0.02 0.07
1350

The common factor analysis of the 6 TAPQOL-

Area under ROC


scales and the two language development measures
(LSI-CT and LSI-PF + VW) revealed a two-fac-
tor solution (eigenvalue>1.0), which explained
44.9% of the common variance (data not shown).

0.83
0.52
0.52
0.51
0.53
0.60
The first factor seemed to represent a language
development dimension and the second appeared

p-Value (MWU)
to reflect a HRQOL dimension. All TAPQOL

Table 5. Discriminative ability of TAPQOL and differences in TAPQOL-scores between children with and without language problems (N=8877)
scales except for Communication had high factor
loadings (P0.30) on the second dimension and low
factor loadings on the first one. The language

0.25
0.40
0.44
0.01
<0.01

<0.01
measures and the Communication scale had high
factor loadings on the first factor and low factor
loadings on the second factor.
In the second factor analysis, the separate scores

No (N=8561)/Language
of the ‘objective’ and ‘subjective’ part of the

(according to panel)
Communication scale were included. Both Com-

problem (N=316)

(19)
(19)
(22)
(11)
(16)
(23)
munication scores had high factor loadings on the

(13)/68.4
(16)/59.3
(19)/69.5
(10)/96.6
(12)/93.1
(15)/83.4
‘language development’ factor (0.57 and 0.43).

Mean (SD)

97.4
89.0
62.8
71.8

95.7
92.2
Discriminative ability
Area under ROC

Table 5 gives an overview of the ability of the


TAPQOL scales to discriminate between groups of
children, which are supposed to differ in language
development (according to the expert-panel and
0.52
0.85
0.52
0.52

0.54
0.62
according to parents). It shows that the Commu-
nication scale and the Social Functioning scale are
p-Value (MWU)

able to discriminate between children with and


children without language problems (area under
ROC >0.50). The other scales cannot discriminate
between these groups any better than chance as
0.10
0.43
0.43

0.01
<0.01

<0.01

indicated by values of the area under ROC of


around 0.50. Figure 1 reproduces, as an example,
problem (N=252) (according

the ROC curves of the groups for the scale Com-


munication. The Communication scale discrimi-
No (N=8514)/Language

to parents) Mean (SD)

nates best between children with and without


(12)
(19)
(20)
(22)

(17)
(24)

language problems (data not shown for all


criteria).
(10)/96.1
(13)/67.2
(16)/59.5
(19)/68.9

(12)/92.1
(15)/82.3

MWU, Mann–Whitney U test.

HRQOL-scores of children with and without


97.5
88.9
62.7
71.8

95.7
92.2

language problems

The scores of children with language problems on


Problem behaviour

Social functioning

the Communication and Social Functioning scales


Communication

Positive mood

were significantly lower than the scores on these


Liveliness

scales of children without language problems


Anxiety

(Table 5). The scores on the Problem Behaviour,


Scale

Anxiety, Positive Mood and Liveliness scales did


1351

100 examined the HRQOL of language problems in


children in a comprehensive way. We found that
even at the age of three children with language
80 problems have a lower health-related quality of life
than children without these problems, irrespective
of the criteria for language problems used in this
60 study.
sensitivity

Considering the complexity of the filling out


instruction of the Communication scale the per-
40 centage of missing values were relatively low, not
exceeding 7%, and comparable to other research
in which HRQOL measures were used [20, 26].
20 The internal consistency of the used TAPQOL
scales was, in general, satisfactory or acceptable,
with 5 out of 6 alphas higher than 0.70 in the total
0 sample. From the analysis of the construct valid-
100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0
ity, we may conclude that the used TAPQOL
specificity scales clearly measure something different than the
language problems (parents)
language performance tests. In discriminating be-
language problems (panel or clinician or SHC)
tween children with and without language prob-
lems the Communication scale and the Social
Figure 1. Discriminative ability (ROC) of the TAPQOL
Functioning scale seemed to be the most relevant.
Communication scale.
Whether the TAPQOL is able to measure changes
(sensitivity to change over time) in the HRQOL of
children with language impairments is one of the
not differ between children with and without remaining issues for additional investigations.
language problems. The inclusion of 6 out of 12 TAPQOL scales
in the parent questionnaire is a potential limi-
tation of the study. Generally, selection of one
Discussion or more single scales of a standardized instru-
ment is not recommended, because the internal
We investigated the HRQOL of a large, population- conceptual structure may have been affected with
based cohort of children aged 3 years in which 4% possible serious consequences for the validity
had language problems. The TAPQOL, a generic and reliability, and for the interpretation of the
HRQOL instrument for 1–5 year-old children, was results. Psychometrically, the selected part should
developed to measure the impact of health problems be regarded as a new instrument. That is, the
in children in clinical settings. The TAPQOL has validity and reliability of the selected scales
proved to be able to discriminate between healthy should then be assessed afresh. At the start of
and less healthy children [20, 27]. This is the first the study we were aware of this limitation and
time that the TAPQOL was used to assess the discussed it with the developers and found the
HRQOL in children without strictly medical prob- selection of 6 of the 12 TAPQOL scales justifi-
lems. Many studies investigated the consequences of able in this special situation. Firstly, the primary
language problems in children by focussing on the outcome measure of the study was language
impact of these problems on learning skills or development. Including the full TAPQOL was
behavioural aspects. Only a few case-studies considered to be a serious respondent burden. As
examined the influence of language impairments on an example, in the parent questionnaire – con-
one particular aspect of quality of life, such as sisting of no more than 10 sheets – , the inclu-
friendship relations in children around 4 years of sion of a detailed child test had to precede the
age and self-esteem in children aged 6 to 13 [28, 29]. inclusion of the full TAPQOL. Nevertheless, we
There are no published studies so far which considered the possibilities of selecting part of
1352

the TAPQOL, as it was a unique opportunity to However, we could not use this instrument, as it
collect data on HRQOL in a new population, is only suitable for children up to 30 months of
moreover taking account of the large scale of the age (we sent the parent questionnaire just around
study. Secondly, we selected the scales in a the child’s third birthday). However, besides the
careful way with the help and consent of the items in the language measures there were more
designers of the TAPQOL. As a result, the scales items in the questionnaire measuring explicitly
referring for the most part to physical health, linguistic knowledge, e.g. one item inquiring
namely gastric problems, appetite, skin problems, about the type of language problem of the child
motor functioning, pulmonary problems and (‘My child’s vocabulary contained too few words
sleep were not selected. Thirdly, we re-examined for his/her age’ was one of the answer categories)
reliability and validity of the included scales and another item asked ‘does your child say more
which proved to be satisfactory as was shown in than 50 words?’. Other items more implicitly refer
this paper. Given these careful considerations, to linguistic knowledge. So, we think the parent
we think the selection of scales is justified in this questionnaire contained a sufficient number of
particular instance. However, we certainly should items referring explicitly or implicitly to linguistic
not recommend this in general. knowledge.
In defining language problems the gold stan- Differences in HRQOL between children with
dard is less clearly stated as for well-defined language problems and children without these
diseases such as cancer or diabetes. With devel- problems may have been (partly) due to differences
opmental disorders such as language delays the in socio-demographic characteristics between these
distinction between delays that fall within the groups. Children with language problems were
natural biological variation and delays or disor- more often boys and had less often highly educated
ders that require intervention is much more dif- mothers than children without language problems.
ficult. There is a norm for language production However, adjusting for these factors in the analyses
(>50 words at 2 years of age) [3] which was did not affect the observed differences in TAPQOL-
used to identify serious language delays at the scores between children with and without language
SHC in our study, but we had to make use of problems. This means that differences in HRQOL
proxies to assess whether there was or had been can be attributed to language problems.
a language problem or not in the total cohort. If the severity of language problems differed
To identify children with language problems we between responders and non-responders, non-re-
made use of parent reports as well as specialists sponse could have caused bias. Therefore we
and an expert-panel. Although two of our cri- compared the scores on the language production
teria to identify children with language problems score and the language comprehension score
consisted of external sources, namely informa- between responders and non-responders in chil-
tion from specialists who had actually seen the dren who were assessed for language delay at
child (at SHC or otherwise) and an expert-panel, Speech and Hearing Centres. We found that the
who blindly assessed written case descriptions, average scores on both language measures in the
we ourselves did not actually see these children non-response group were significantly lower than
which could be a limitation of the study. Inter- the average scores in the response group. So, the
estingly, despite these different sources it turned impact of language problems on HRQOL might
out that the results seem to apply irrespective of in fact be higher in the total group than esti-
the criterion used. mated in this study.
Many items of the language measures in the We think the results are generalisable, because
parent questionnaire pertain to the social use of our population consisted of children from the
language (as ‘speech fluency’, ‘speech understood general population. All children resident in the
by strangers’ or understanding questions), working area of the health centres in the selected
whereas the number of items concerning linguistic regions were included. The selection of regions
knowledge seemed to be limited. The MacArthur was pragmatic and not based on knowledge
CDI should be a useful instrument to measure about the prevalence of language problems in
productive vocabulary and grammar skills [30]. these areas.
1353

The results seem to indicate that children with and may lead at a later age to behavioral
and without language problems did not differ in problems and learning problems at school. These
problem behaviour like short-tempered or results provide additional evidence for the
hostility, nor did they differ in being cheerful, importance of monitoring the language develop-
content and happy, nor in being afraid, tense and ment during childhood [31].
anxious. However, it is not possible to determine
whether this was actually true or because these
scales were less sensitive, as all children had high
scores on these scales. This seems to be an issue Acknowledgements
for further investigation. However, at age three,
children with and without language problems not The Health Care Insurance Board (‘College voor
only appeared to differ in communication abilities zorgverzekeringen’) financially supported this
like understanding what other people say and study. The authors wish to thank all personnel of
expressing one’s will, they also seemed to differ the child health care centres, the staff of the six
on social functioning capabilities like being at Home Care institutions (Het Groene Kruis
ease with other children and feeling sure with Oostelijk Zuid-Limburg, Het Groene Kruis
other children. In some subgroups, children with Heuvelland, Stichting Thuiszorg Midden Lim-
language problems seemed to be less full of en- burg, Stichting Thuiszorg Westelijke Mijnstreek,
ergy, less active and less brisk than children Stichting Thuiszorg Midden Brabant and Stich-
without these problems. ting Thuiszorg Den Haag), the staff and per-
We demonstrated that language problems at sonnel of the Speech and Hearing Centres
the age of three can have an impact on chil- (Stichting Audiologisch Centrum Hoensbroeck,
dren’s social life. Parents say they experience Audiologisch Centrum Eindhoven, Stichting Au-
more problems in understanding other people, in diologisch Centrum voor Tilburg en omstreken,
speaking clearly and coherently and in expressing Haags Audiologisch Centrum ‘Effatha’, Centrum
one’s will. Moreover, they seem to be less voor Auditief en Communicatief Beperkten ‘St.
capable in playing with other children pleasantly, Marie’ Eindhoven and Stichting Thuiszorg Rot-
they are less at ease with other children and they terdam), as well as the General Practitioners,
feel less sure of themselves with other children. If Speech and Language therapists, ear-, nose- and
children’s social life is affected this may have throat- specialists, paediatricians and other pri-
serious consequences for their cognitive and mary-care workers in the research area for their
social-emotional development and well being, collaboration.

Appendix I. Items of the TAPQOL Communication scale

Item Answer categories

Objective part Subjective part

In the last 3 months, did your child have.. Never/sometimes/often


If answer is ‘sometimes’ or ‘often’:
At that time, my child felt.. ‘fine’, ‘not too well’, ‘rather badly’,
‘badly’
1. Difficulty in understanding what others
said?
2. Difficulty in talking clearly
3. Difficulty in saying what he/she meant
4. Difficulty in making clear what he/she
meant
1354

Appendix II 3. Rescorla L, Hadicke-Wiley M, Escarte E. Epidemiological


investigation of expressive delay at two. First Language
Items of Language Screening Instrument – Child Test (LSI-CT) 1993; 13: 5–22.
Uses own name?
4. Law J, et al. Screening for Speech and Language Delay: A
(choosing from four objects in picture): Systematic Review of the Literature. Health Technol Assess
– Where is the train? 1998; 2(9): 1–184.
– Where are the spectacles? 5. Curtiss S, Katz W, Tallal P. Delay versus deviance in the
– Where is the high chair
language acquisition of language-impaired children. J
What is this? (show picture of object) Speech Hear Res 1992; 35(2): 373–383.
– chair 6. Wright NE, et al. The speech and language development of
– plane low birth weight infants. Br J Dis Commun 1983; 18(3):
– dog
187–196.
– cat 7. First LR, Palfrey JS. The infant or young child with
– boat developmental delay. N Engl J Med 1994; 330(7): 478–483.
Does your child do the following when asked?
8. Reep-van den Bergh CMM, et al. Prevalentie van ta-
– Placing toys in the box alontwikkelingsstoornissen bij kinderen. Tijdschr Gezond-
– Placing cup on the table heidswet 1998; 76(6): 311–317.
– Handing candy over to mother 9. Tomblin JB, et al. The association of reading disability,
Where is/are your…?
behavioral disorders, and language impairment among
– knee second-grade children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2000;
– nails 41(4): 473–482.
– lips 10. Coster FW, et al. Specific language impairments and
Where is ….(show picture with the following objects):
behavioural problems. Folia Phoniatr Logop, 1999; 51(3):
– the cupboard? 99–107.
– the pan 11. Kaderavek JN, Sulzby E. Narrative production by children
– the water running from the tap
with and without specific language impairment: oral nar-
– the hall-stand ratives and emergent readings. J Speech Lang Hear Res
– the candle 2000; 43(1): 34–49.
– the doghouse 12. Conti-Ramsden, G, et al. Follow-up of children attending
– the parachute
infant language units: Outcomes at 11 years of age. Int J
Soup is warm and ice is…. Lang Commun Disord 2001; 36(2): 207–219.
Birds fly in the sky and fishes swim in the… 13. Kaiser AP, et al. Teacher-reported behavior problems and
A giant is big and a dwarf is…. language delays in boys and girls enrolled in Head Start.
John is a boy and Mary is a ….
Behav Dis 2002; 28(1): 23–39.
What must you do when you cut your finger? 14. Fujiki M, et al. Withdrawn and sociable behavior of chil-
Why must n’t you play with matches? dren with language impairment. Language, -Speech, -and-
Items of Language Screening Instrument – Parent Form (LSI-
Hearing-Services-in-Schools 1999; 30(2): 183–195.
PF)
15. Ravens-Sieberer U, Bullinger M. Assessing health-related
Understood by strangers quality of life in chronically ill children with the German
Fluency KINDL: first psychometric and content analytical results.
Adequate words
Qual Life Res 1998; 7(5): 399–407.
Articulation 16. Rosen CL, et al. Health-related quality of life and sleep-
Items of Van Wiechen (VW) disordered breathing in children. Sleep 2002; 25(6): 657–666.
Uses own name 17. Loonen HJ, et al. Quality of life in paediatric inflammatory
Puts 3 + words together bowel disease measured by a generic and a disease-specific
Understood by family questionnaire. Acta Paediatr 2002; 91(3): 348–354.
Asks who/where/what/how questions 18. Stolk EA, Busschbach JJ, Vogels T. Performance of the
Understood by strangers EuroQol in children with imperforate anus. Qual Life Res
Asks why/when/how much questions 2000; 9(1): 29–38.
19. Koning de, et al. A cluster-randomised trial of screening for
language disorders in toddlers. J Med Screen 2004; 11(3):
109–116.
References 20. Fekkes M, et al. Development and psychometric evaluation
of the TAPQOL: A health-related quality of life instrument
1. Reynell J. A Developmental Approach to Language Dis- for 1–5-year-old children. Qual Life Res 2000; 9(8): 961–972.
orders. Br J Disord Commun 1969; 4(1): 33–40. 21. Burden V, et al. The Cambridge Language and Speech
2. Dunn LM, Hottel JV, Peabody picture vocabulary test Project (CLASP). I .Detection of language difficulties at
performance of trainable mentally retarded children. Am J 36 to 39 months. Dev Med Child Neurol 1996; 38(7):
Ment Defic 1961; 65: 448–452. 613–631.
1355

22. Gerritsen FME. VTO Language-screening 3- to 6-years 27. Veen S, et al. Quality of life in preschool children born
Olds: The Development of Language Screening Instru- preterm. Dev Med Child Neurol 2001; 43(7): 460–465.
ments to Use in Youth Health Care (in Dutch). Leiden: 28. Stoneham G. Friendship skills in children with specific
Rijksuniversiteit Leiden, 1988. language impairment. Int J Lang Commun Disord 2001;
23. Brouwer-de Jong E, Burgmeijer R, Laurent de Angelo M. 36(Suppl): 276–281.
Monitoring Development at the Child Health Care Cen- 29. Jerome AC, et al. Self-esteem in children with specific lan-
tre. Manual for the revised Van Wiechen examination (in guage impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2002; 45(4):
Dutch). Assen: van Gorkum, 1996. 700–714.
24. Streiner DL, Norman GR. Health Measurement Scales: A 30. Fenson, L., et al., Variability in early communicative de-
Practical Guide to their Development and Use. Oxford/ velopment. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 1994; 59(5): 1–173;
New York/Tokyo: Oxford University Press, 1989. discussion 174-185.
25. Hanley JA, McNeil BJ. The meaning and use of the area 31. Law J, Conti-Ramsden. Treating children with speech and
under a receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve. language impairments. Br Med J 2000; 321(7266): 908–909.
Radiology 1982; 143(1): 29–36.
26. Essink-Bot ML, et al. An empirical comparison of four
generic health status measures. The Nottingham Health Address for correspondence: Heleen M.E. van Agt, Department
Profile, the Medical Outcomes Study 36-item Short- of Public Health, Erasmus MC, P.O. Box 1738, 3000 DR
Form Health Survey, the COOP/WONCA charts, and Rotterdam, The Netherlands
the EuroQol instrument. Med Care 1997; 35(5): 522– Phone: +31-0-10-4087714; Fax: +31-0-10-4089449
537. E-mail: h.vanagt@erasmusmc.nl

You might also like