Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 15

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 1996, 29, 519–533 NUMBER 4 (WINTER 1996)

EVALUATION OF A MULTIPLE-STIMULUS PRESENTATION


FORMAT FOR ASSESSING REINFORCER PREFERENCES
ISER G. DELEON AND BRIAN A. IWATA
THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA

We compared three methods for presenting stimuli during reinforcer-preference assess-


ments: a paired-stimulus format (PS), a multiple-stimulus format in which selections were
made with replacement (MSW), and a multiple-stimulus format in which selections were
made without replacement (MSWO). Results obtained for 7 participants showed mod-
erate to high rank-order correlations between the MSWO and PS procedures and a similar
number of identified reinforcers. In addition, the time to administer the MSWO pro-
cedure was comparable to that required for the MSW method and less than half that
required to administer the PS procedure. Subsequent tests of reinforcement effects re-
vealed that some stimuli selected in the PS and MSWO procedures, but not selected in
the MSW procedure, functioned as reinforcers for arbitrary responses. These preliminary
results suggest that the multiple-stimulus procedure without replacement may share the
respective advantages of the other methods.
DESCRIPTORS: assessment, developmental disabilities, reinforcer preference

Several methods have been developed by choose one. Assessment continued until each
which therapists can identify stimuli that item had been paired with every other item.
might function as reinforcers for individuals In this procedure, reinforcement effects were
with developmental disabilities. These pro- predicted based on relative preference among
cedures have included a variety of formats, stimuli: Greater preference was attributed to
such as caregiver interviews (Green et al., stimuli that were selected on a high per-
1988; Windsor, Piche, & Locke, 1994), sin- centage of the trials in which they were avail-
gle-stimulus approach methods (Pace, Ivan- able. Fisher et al. compared this method to
cic, Edwards, Iwata, & Page, 1985; Smith, that developed by Pace et al. (1985), in
Iwata, & Shore, 1995), and arrangements which relative preference was based on the
that involve selection from among concur- percentage of trials in which a participant
rently available stimuli (Fehr, Wacker, Tres- approached items that were presented singly.
ize, Lennon, & Meyerson, 1979; Paclawskyj After assessing the same 16 items using both
& Vollmer, 1995). methods, two classes of stimuli were com-
One example of a procedure using con- pared in a concurrent-operants arrangement:
currently available stimuli was described by (a) those selected on at least 80% of trials in
Fisher et al. (1992). Their paired-stimulus both types of assessments (high-high stimu-
method involved presenting two stimuli si- li), and (b) those selected on more than 80%
multaneously and allowing participants to of the single-stimulus trials but selected on
60% or less of the paired-stimulus trials (sp-
This research was supported in part by a grant from high stimuli). Results revealed that all 4 par-
the Florida Department of Health and Rehabilitative ticipants distributed their responding more
Services. We thank Julie Burke, Sonya Fischer, Han-
Leong Goh, SungWoo Kahng, Dorothea Lerman, Jodi towards the response option that produced
Mazaleski, Bridget Shore, and April Worsdell for con- high-high stimuli as a consequence, suggest-
ducting various aspects of the study. ing that the paired-stimulus procedure iden-
Reprints may be obtained from Brian Iwata, Psy-
chology Department, The University of Florida, tified reinforcers more accurately than did
Gainesville, Florida 32611. the single-stimulus approach method. Based

519
520 ISER G. DELEON and BRIAN A. IWATA

on these results, the authors suggested that, method may indicate that stable preferences
given an 80% cutoff for approach respond- can be determined in fewer, or even single,
ing, the single-stimulus method may over- sessions.
estimate preference for some stimuli. In addition, the PS method resulted in a
A variation of the paired-stimulus meth- more distinct ranking of the items than did
od, involving selection from among more the MS method. That is, because the most
than two concurrently available stimuli, was preferred items were not available on each
recently described by Windsor et al. (1994). PS trial, participants chose from among less
These authors compared the Fisher et al. preferred items on some proportion of the
(1992) paired-stimulus (PS) method to a trials. By contrast, some participants never
multiple-stimulus (MS) method in which selected some of the available stimuli in the
the entire array of six items was available on MS procedure because they always selected
each trial. Typically, each procedure identi- the same (most preferred) stimuli. If many
fied the same most preferred item. However, stimuli remain unselected, no differentiation
the MS procedure required less overall time regarding relative preference is possible, lead-
(i.e., fewer trials) to administer. A six-item ing to a conclusion that those stimuli would
comparison using the MS presentation be ineffective as reinforcers. Of the 48 stim-
method required five administrations con- uli assessed for the 8 participants by Wind-
sisting of 10 trials each (50 trials), whereas sor et al. (1994), eight (16.7%) were never
the PS method required five administrations selected in the MS procedure even though
consisting of 30 trials each (150 trials) to all stimuli were previously identified as
assess the same six stimuli. Mason, McGee, ‘‘liked items’’ by caregivers. As such, the MS
Farmer-Dougan, and Risley (1989) suggest- method may be prone to the production of
ed that treatment effects might be enhanced false negatives—items that may function ef-
as a function of conducting reinforcer as- fectively as reinforcers if tested directly but
sessments prior to each session. Thus, one whose potential is obscured by the contin-
advantage of briefer assessment procedures uous availability of the most preferred items.
might be to facilitate more frequent sam- In this study, we describe a third proce-
pling, thereby allowing therapists to accom- dure that was developed as an attempt to
modate idiosyncratic shifts in client prefer- combine the best features of the PS format
ence. (Fisher et al., 1992) with those of the MS
However, the PS method produced more format (Windsor et al., 1994). It is a varia-
consistent results across sessions. As mea- tion of the MS format in which selections
sured through Kendall’s rank coefficient of are made without replacement (MSWO):
concordance, the PS method resulted in a When an item is chosen from the array, it
mean coefficient of .631 relative to the .486 is unavailable during the next presentation.
produced by the MS method. This finding This procedure could be repeated until ei-
suggests that although the MS method re- ther all items have been selected or until a
quired less administration time, the more criterion is reached indicating that no more
consistent results produced by the PS meth- selections will be made. The procedure es-
od may actually make it more time efficient. sentially requires individuals to choose
That is, if consistent results are not pro- among the less preferred items, a feature that
duced by the MS method, then several ad- was responsible for the more distinct rank-
ministrations may be required to determine ings in the PS method. Furthermore, by
clear and stable preferences, whereas the yoking the number of trials to that in the
more consistent results produced by the PS MS format used by Windsor et al. (1994),
REINFORCER PREFERENCE ASSESSMENT 521

it can be determined if equally useful infor- limited but functional vocal repertoire, al-
mation can be gathered in a similarly brief though she engaged in frequent echolalia.
assessment. Thus, the present study com- Jeremy was a 43-year-old man who dis-
pared a preference procedure involving mul- played no expressive language skills but who
tiple-stimulus presentation and selection could follow a limited number of requests.
without replacement to the multiple-stimu- Jack was a 39-year-old man who had a lim-
lus method described by Windsor et al., us- ited expressive repertoire, but who respond-
ing the paired-stimulus procedure as the ed well to a variety of verbal requests. Carlos
standard for comparison. Comparisons be- was a 45-year-old man who showed no re-
tween the two MS procedures included ceptive or expressive language skills and had
rank-order correlations with the PS proce- a slight visual impairment. Max was a
dure, consistency of rank orders across ses- 32-year-old man with limited communica-
sions, time of administration, and number tive skills who displayed periodic episodes of
of potential reinforcers identified. In a sec- aggressive and disruptive behavior.
ond experiment, stimuli that were never se- All sessions were conducted in one of sev-
lected during the MS procedure, but were eral therapy rooms at a day-treatment pro-
selected on some proportion of trials during gram on the grounds of the facility. The
the MSWO and PS procedures, were direct- same room was used throughout the study
ly tested for their efficacy as reinforcers. In for each participant. Each room contained a
this fashion, we attempted to determine if table, two chairs, and materials used during
these stimuli were false negatives (based on the course of the study. Prior to each session,
the MS assessment) or false positives (based participants were instructed or prompted to
on the MSWO and PS methods). sit in one of the chairs; the experimenter sat
in the other. Seven items per participant
were chosen for presentation during each as-
EXPERIMENT 1: sessment. The majority of the items were ar-
COMPARISON OF PREFERENCE bitrarily selected by the experimenters with-
ASSESSMENT METHODS out prior knowledge of the participant’s pref-
METHOD erence for those items. A few additional
items were selected based on casual obser-
Participants, Setting, and Materials vation of preferences and caregiver opinion
Seven adults with profound developmen- of preferred and nonpreferred items.
tal disabilities participated in the study. All
lived at a state residential facility for persons Response Measurement and Reliability
with developmental disabilities and were se- For all methods, a selection response was
lected for participation because they had a recorded when the participant made physical
number of behavioral deficits and could ben- contact with one of the presented items. The
efit from the identification of additional re- participant had 30 s to select an item. If the
inforcers. Rupert was a 26-year-old male participant made contact with more than
with Down syndrome. He walked with an one item, the first item contacted was re-
unsteady gait, responded to simple requests, corded as the selection. If no item was se-
and displayed a few simple signs. Rita was a lected within the 30-s period, the trial end-
25-year-old woman with Cornelia deLange ed. The procedures following a no-selection
syndrome. She was ambulatory but dis- trial varied across presentation methods (see
played no receptive or expressive language below). When a selection was made, the trial
skills. Bessie, a 43-year-old woman, had a ended after the participant received 30-s ac-
522 ISER G. DELEON and BRIAN A. IWATA

cess to the item (leisure stimuli) or after the ther removed from the immediate area (lei-
participant had completely consumed the sure item) or was not replaced (food item).
item (edible stimuli). With a few exceptions, Prior to the next trial, the sequencing of the
all participants were able to interact inde- remaining items was rotated by taking the
pendently with the stimuli being assessed. item at the left end of the line and moving
(The exceptions involved turning on electri- it to the right end, then shifting the other
cally operated leisure items. Experimenters items so that they were again equally spaced
assisted the participants in these cases.) In on the table. The second trial then followed
addition to recording stimulus selections, immediately. This procedure continued until
observers also recorded session duration for all items were selected or until a participant
66.7% of the sessions. Duration was mea- made no selection within 30 s from the be-
sured from the moment a participant was ginning of a trial. In the latter case, the ses-
first instructed to select on the first trial until sion ended and all remaining items were re-
the last item was consumed. corded as ‘‘not selected.’’
Observers recorded selections on data Multiple stimulus with replacement (MS).
sheets that were customized for each proce- MS sessions were conducted in a fashion
dure. For most sessions, the experimenter identical to that described above, with one
also served as the observer. On 36.2% of the exception. After the end of each trial, the
sessions, a second observer independently re- item just selected was returned to the array
corded selections for purposes of interob- (in the case of leisure items) or was replaced
server agreement. Agreements were defined in the array by an identical item (in the case
as both observers having recorded the same of edible stimuli).
selection or no selection for each trial. In- Paired stimulus (PS). A similar procedure
terobserver agreement was computed by di- was followed during this assessment, except
viding agreements by agreements plus dis- that only two items were presented during
agreements and multiplying by 100%. Ob- each trial, and the session continued until
servers disagreed on only three trials each item had been paired with every other
throughout the study, yielding an interob- item (21 total trials per session). Stimulus
server agreement score of 98.9%. pairings followed a predetermined order,
such that the same stimulus was never pre-
Procedures sented on two consecutive trials. Stimuli
Prior to the beginning of the first session, were randomly positioned (left or right side)
participants were given a sample of each of on each trial. In contrast to both multiple-
the edible items and were given 30-s access stimulus methods, failure to select an item
to each of the leisure items. Subsequently, (one of the pair) did not terminate the ses-
participants were exposed to one or two as- sion but simply produced the next trial.
sessment sessions per day.
Multiple stimulus without replacement Experimental Design
(MSWO). For this assessment procedure, Each participant was exposed to five con-
each session began with all items sequenced secutive sessions of each procedure, for a to-
randomly in a straight line on the table, tal of 15 sessions. The order of procedures
about 5 cm apart. While a participant was varied across participants: MSWO, PS, and
seated at the table approximately 0.3 m from MS (Rupert, Rita, and Max); MSWO, MS,
the stimulus array, the experimenter in- and PS (Bessie and Jack); MS, PS, and
structed the participant to select one item. MSWO (Jeremy); and PS, MSWO, and MS
After a selection was made, the item was ei- (Carlos).
REINFORCER PREFERENCE ASSESSMENT 523

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION the MS procedure, only 24 items were ever


The primary dependent variable consisted selected across participants. Stated different-
of a percentage score indicating the number ly, 25 items were never selected in the MS
of times an item was selected over the num- procedure.
ber of trials during which the item was pre- A quantitative index of correspondence
sented. These percentages are shown in Fig- across stimulus rankings produced by the as-
ure 1 for all participants. From a clinical per- sessment procedures is provided in Table 1,
spective, the most important outcome of the which shows the Kendall rank-order corre-
lation coefficients between the MS and PS
assessments is the top-ranked stimulus. For
procedures and between the MSWO and PS
4 of the 7 participants (Bessie, Jeremy, Car-
procedures.1 All correlations were positive
los, and Max), all three assessment methods
and, in many cases, the correlation was fairly
identified the same reinforcer as the most
high. Notable exceptions occurred for Jack,
highly preferred. With some variations in
for whom the MS/PS and MSWO/PS co-
the exact rankings, the MSWO procedure
efficients were .24 and .52, respectively. In
matched the top three ranked items of the
addition, the correlation between the MS
PS procedure for 4 of 7 participants (Rupert, and PS procedures for Bessie, .48, was low.
Bessie, Rita, and Max) and two of the top Mean correlations for the MS/PS procedures
three ranked items for the remaining partic- and MSWO/PS procedures were .61 and
ipants. A similar result was found for the MS .72, respectively. For 5 of the 7 participants,
procedure (top three matches for Rita, Jere- coefficients were higher for the MSWO/PS
my, Carlos, and Max; two of top three correlation than for the MS/PS correlation.
matches for Rupert and Bessie). For all stim- The exceptions were Jeremy and Carlos.
uli that were selected at least once during all Consistency of rankings was measured us-
three assessments, rank discrepancies across ing Kendall’s correlation of concordance. Ta-
assessments typically involved only minor ble 2 shows the correlation coefficients for
deviations. The largest discrepancy found for each assessment method and the means of
any participant was coffee for Jack, which the coefficients across participants. Overall,
was ranked first by the MSWO procedure both the MSWO and PS methods produced
and fourth by the PS procedure. No other similar, moderate to high across-session cor-
stimulus (when selected at all) was separated relations, with respective means of .81 and
by more than two rank positions across as- .83. The coefficients were highly significant
sessments. for all 7 participants. By contrast, the MS
The number of stimuli selected at all dur- procedure resulted in somewhat lower cor-
ing each assessment method, and hence relations (M 5 .56). Although the MS cor-
identified as a potential reinforcer, can also relations were at least moderately high for
be determined from Figure 1. For all partic- most participants, the selection patterns by
ipants, the MS method produced more un- 3 participants (Bessie, Jeremy, and Jack) pro-
selected items than did the MSWO or PS duced correlations lower than .50.
methods; in two cases (Bessie and Jack), only Measurements of the time required to
two items were selected throughout the en- conduct each procedure revealed that the en-
tire MS procedure. This same pattern was tire five-session assessment required a mean
evident when the data are combined for all
1 Given the number of correlation coefficients cal-
participants. For both the PS and MSWO
culated in this study, which may inflate the per-ex-
methods, at least 90% of the items were se- periment error rate, all interpretations based on cor-
lected at least once across participants. For relational data should be viewed cautiously.
524 ISER G. DELEON and BRIAN A. IWATA

Figure 1. Percentage of trials on which stimuli were selected, when available, by 7 participants for all three
assessment methods: multiple stimulus without replacement (MSWO), paired stimulus (PS), and multiple
stimulus with replacement (MS).
REINFORCER PREFERENCE ASSESSMENT 525
Table 1 Table 2
Kendall Rank-Order Correlation Coefficients Between Kendall Rank Coefficients of Concordance Across
the Multiple Stimulus with Replacement (MS) and Administration Rankings for the Multiple Stimulus
Multiple Stimulus Without Replacement (MSWO) Without Replacement (MSWO), Paired Stimulus (PS),
Procedures with the Paired Stimulus (PS) Procedure and Multiple Stimulus with Replacement (MS)
Procedures
MS-PS MSWO-PS
correlation correlation MSWO PS MS
Participant t (N 5 7) t (N 5 7) correlation correlation correlation
Participant W (N 5 7) W (N 5 7) W (N 5 7)
Rupert .67* .76*
Bessie .48 .76* Rupert .845** .79** .75**
Rita .62* .71* Bessie .85** .84** .42
Jeremy .81** .62* Rita .58** .79** .64**
Jack .24 .52 Jeremy .76** .64** .47*
Carlos .81** .78* Jack .87** .91** .42
Max .62* .86** Carlos .87** .92** .65**
Group M .61 .72 Max .90** .90** .56*
* p , .05. ** p , .01.
Group M .81 .83 .567
* p , .05. ** p , .01.

of 16.5 min, 21.8 min, and 53.3 min for


the MS, MSWO, and PS procedures, re- nated during the sixth trial. By omitting
spectively. These group means are highly these sessions and recalculating the average
representative of data for individual partici- assessment time for the MSWO assessments,
pants. That is, for 6 of the 7 participants, an estimated mean assessment time of 24.1
the MS assessment required less time to min is obtained.
complete than did the MSWO assessment, Overall, the three assessment formats pro-
which in turn required less time than did duced similar results in identifying the most
the PS assessment. The exception was Car- preferred stimuli. Likewise, similarity across
los, for whom the MSWO assessment re- the entire array is reflected in the generally
quired 21.9 min to complete, whereas the moderate to high, but always positive, cor-
MS assessment required 24.6 min. For only relation coefficients across procedures. How-
1 participant did either of the two multiple- ever, the MSWO and PS procedures gener-
stimulus procedures take more than half the ally produced more consistent rankings
time to complete than did the PS assess- across administrations. Both multiple-stim-
ment. This result occurred for the MSWO ulus procedures required less time to admin-
assessment with Rita, which took 26.0 min ister than the PS assessment. As stated pre-
relative to the 48.1 min required to com- viously, a procedure that provides similar in-
plete the PS assessment. However, the aver- formation in less time affords therapists the
age time to complete the MSWO assessment opportunity to conduct frequent reinforcer
may be somewhat underestimated due to the assessments and perhaps to tailor treatment
termination of sessions. As noted previously, and training programs to idiosyncratic shifts
sessions for both multiple-stimulus methods in preference. It is curious that the MSWO
were terminated if no selection was made procedure generally required slightly more
within 30 s. This never occurred during an time to complete than the MS assessment,
MS session, but occurred in 18 of the 35 given that fewer stimuli were involved across
sessions (51.4%) of the MSWO sessions successive trials in the former. Anecdotal ob-
across participants. Of the 18 terminated servations during the two procedures pro-
sessions, five were terminated during the fi- vide a plausible explanation. Specifically, in
nal trial and the remaining 13 were termi- the MS procedure, given that the most high-
526 ISER G. DELEON and BRIAN A. IWATA

ly preferred items were always available, par- port a conclusion that the MSWO and PS
ticipants often selected those items imme- procedures are more prone to the production
diately. By contrast, the most preferred items of false positives than is the MS procedure
were not always available in the MSWO (i.e., items would be identified as potential
procedure, and participants seemed to take reinforcers when, in fact, they do not func-
more time to scan the array. An accumula- tion as such). Alternatively, increases in re-
tion of the brief periods required to scan less sponding would suggest that the items can
preferred items seemed to account for the function as reinforcers and that the MS pro-
slight increase in time. cedure is more prone to produce false neg-
Based on the assumption that frequent se- atives.
lection of an item during an assessment is
predictive of that item’s reinforcing efficacy, METHOD
the MSWO and PS procedures predicted Participants and Settings
that more items in the arrays would function Experiment 2 included 4 participants
as reinforcers. Of the 49 items evaluated for from the first experiment: Bessie, Rupert,
all participants with the MS procedure, 25 Jack, and Carlos. Each had selected an item
were never selected by participants. Accord- during the MSWO and PS procedures that
ing to the MS procedure then, these stimuli was not selected in the MS procedure. The
would not be predicted to function as rein- items tested were (a) fruit juice (approxi-
forcers. By contrast, 21 of those same items mately 2 oz per delivery in a small paper
(84%) were selected some proportion of the cup) for Bessie, (b) beets (one quarter of a
time in both of the other procedures. One 1/4 in. thick slice) for Rupert, (c) peanut
item not selected in the MS procedure, M&Mt candy (one per delivery) for Jack,
M&Mst, ranked first for Jack in the PS pro- and (d) chewing gum (one quarter of a stick
cedure. These items may in fact function as per delivery) for Carlos. Sessions were con-
reinforcers, but that sort of prediction (using ducted in the same rooms used to conduct
the MS procedure) might have been ob- the initial assessments. These rooms usually
scured by the continuous availability of a contained the same tables and chairs de-
small number of highly preferred items. scribed previously. The sole exception was
Jack, for whom a microswitch panel (de-
scribed below) was mounted on a small
EXPERIMENT 2: movable cart.
EVALUATION OF
REINFORCEMENT EFFECTS Materials and Response Definitions
A second experiment was conducted to Different materials were used for each
verify predictions about stimuli that were participant, depending on the nature of the
never selected in the MS procedure. Rein- response. Bessie placed game pieces (red or
forcement effects were examined by arrang- black checkers) into a Connect Four game.
ing a schedule of contingent delivery for This involved selecting a game piece from a
four of these items to determine if they box and inserting it through the small slot
could support levels of responding above at the top of the game board. Rupert pressed
baseline. All four items had been selected an ink stamper onto a standard-size legal
some proportion of the time in the other pad. Only responses that left a visible mark
two assessment methods. Failure of these were recorded. Jack pressed a response panel
items to increase rates of responding on ar- that activated a microswitch. The panel was
bitrary tasks over baseline levels would sup- made of plastic and measured 12.7 cm by
REINFORCER PREFERENCE ASSESSMENT 527

20.3 cm. Depression of the panel activated Baseline. When participants reliably emit-
a small red light in the center of the response ted the responses, baseline sessions began.
panel. Only responses that produced acti- Participants were seated in front of the re-
vation of the light were recorded. Finally, sponse materials and, when required, were
Carlos picked small wooden blocks off the prompted to emit the response. No other
table and placed them into a plastic bucket. interactions occurred between the partici-
pant and the experimenter. For Carlos, base-
Response Measurement and Reliability line began when the modeling and reinforce-
All sessions lasted 10 min. Data were col- ment procedure had increased rates of re-
lected by the same experimenters who ad- sponding to a stable rate. As such, the base-
ministered the assessments in Experiment 1 line for evaluation of the chewing gum for
with the use of hand-held computers. The Carlos was at the same time an extinction
computers divided each session into 10-s phase relative to the previous training.
bins. During 34.5% of the sessions, two ob- Fixed-ratio schedule of reinforcement. After
servers independently collected data on par- stable rates of responding were observed in
ticipants’ responding. Interobserver agree- baseline, each participant was exposed to a
ment was calculated on an interval-by-inter- fixed-ratio (FR) 1 schedule of delivery of the
val basis by dividing the smaller number of item in question. During sessions, the ex-
responses observed during an interval by the perimenter sat across the table from the par-
larger number. These fractions were summed ticipant (or on the other side of the cart
across intervals and then divided by the total from Jack). Contingent upon each response,
number of intervals in the sessions. Using the experimenter delivered the relevant item
this method, the mean agreement score was by placing it on a plate in front of the par-
98.3% (range, 91.7% to 100%). ticipant (Jack and Carlos), on the table in
front of the participant (Bessie), or directly
Procedures into the participant’s hand (Rupert).
Preexperimental training. Bessie and Ru-
pert each had previous histories with their Experimental Design
respective responses, thus requiring no train- For Rupert and Jack, an A-B-A reversal
ing. The other 2 participants, Jack and Car- design was implemented, in which A was
los, required initial training to shape the re- baseline and B consisted of the FR 1 sched-
sponse. For Jack, a simple prompt (i.e., ule. For Carlos, a B1-A-B2-A reversal design
‘‘Jack, press this panel’’) and modeled re- was used, in which A was baseline, B1 was
sponse were sufficient to get him to begin response training using the candy, and B2
pressing the panel. This prompt was deliv- was the FR 1 schedule of delivery of chewing
ered once at the beginning of each session gum. Thus, following the FR 1 phase, 3 of
throughout the experiment. For Carlos, it the 4 participants experienced a reversal to
was necessary to shape the response with the baseline. For the final participant, Bessie, no
use of modeling and reinforcement. At the changes in response rates occurred as a result
beginning of each session and every 30 s of FR 1 delivery of juice. Therefore, a re-
thereafter for the duration of each session, versal seemed unnecessary. However, to de-
an experimenter modeled dropping a block termine if response rates had not increased
into the bucket. If Carlos imitated the ex- due to a ceiling effect, a single-session probe
perimenter, a small piece of Kit Katt candy was attempted on the 6th day of the FR 1
was delivered. Kit Katt ranked first or sec- condition using Kit Katt candy as the re-
ond in all three assessments for Carlos. inforcer.
528 ISER G. DELEON and BRIAN A. IWATA

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION siderably increased above those observed in


Figure 2 shows the results for all 4 partic- baseline. Unfortunately, no highly preferred
ipants. FR 1 delivery of juice had little effect stimuli were tested, a preparation that may
on Bessie’s performance: Response rates dur- have shed light on the potential for increase.
ing baseline and FR 1 averaged 0.48 per Nevertheless, substantial increases above
minute and 0.66 per minute, respectively. baseline rates were observed for both Jack
However, when candy was made contingent and Carlos. These results suggest that, in at
upon the same response, an immediate in- least some cases, items that remain uniden-
tified as reinforcers in the MS procedure do
crease in responding to 3.2 per minute was
function effectively as reinforcers, and that
observed. FR 1 delivery of beets produced a
the MSWO and PS procedures more readily
slight increase in Rupert’s responding. His
identify those items as reinforcers.
mean rate during both baseline conditions
was 1.29 per minute. During the FR 1
phase, responding increased slightly to a GENERAL DISCUSSION
mean of 1.86 per minute. Jack’s responding
For the 7 participants in this study, an
increased noticeably during FR to a mean of
assessment format in which stimuli were pre-
4.54 per minute from a mean of 0.78 per sented in an array and selections were made
minute during both baseline phases. Finally, without replacement (MSWO) identified
Carlos’ responding also increased substan- more reinforcers than a similar procedure in
tially. During initial training using candy, his which stimuli were placed back into the se-
rate of dropping blocks in a bucket rose to lection array (MS). In addition, the MSWO
0.96 per minute for the last seven sessions. procedure produced results that were similar
After what appeared to be an extinction in terms of overall ranks and consistency of
burst during the first two sessions of base- ranks to a procedure in which stimuli were
line, rates then decreased to zero for five presented in pairs (PS), but in substantially
continuous sessions. This was followed by an less time. If the similarities between the
increase in responding to a mean of 0.86 per rankings produced by the MSWO and PS
minute during FR 1 gum and a subsequent procedures can be deemed acceptable, the
decrease during the return to baseline. MSWO procedure appears to share the re-
Thus, for 3 of the 4 participants, items spective advantages of the other two proce-
that had never been selected during the MS dures.
procedure but had been selected on some The extent to which failure to identify re-
proportion of the trials during the MSWO inforcers occurs in the MS procedure is dif-
and PS procedures produced increases in re- ficult to gauge from these results. Here, it
sponding when delivered on a contingent occurred for three of the four items evalu-
basis. Juice failed to produce increases for ated, but only those four items were tested,
Bessie, and we determined that failure was and no information is available for items
not due to a ceiling effect because delivery that were not selected in the MS procedure
of candy for the same response resulted in a by the other 3 participants. In addition, only
substantial increase. Only modest increases one item was tested per participant, and that
were observed with Rupert, but the extent item was selected because it was the item not
to which rates of stamping could have in- selected in the MS procedure that had the
creased is not clear. Rupert typically released highest combined rank from the other pro-
the rubber stamp following each response, cedures. Thus, it is not clear how items less
and it may be that rates could not have con- highly ranked might have performed if sub-
REINFORCER PREFERENCE ASSESSMENT 529

Figure 2. Results of reinforcer potency evaluations for four stimuli selected by participants during the PS
and MSWO assessments but not during the MS assessment.
530 ISER G. DELEON and BRIAN A. IWATA

jected to reinforcer testing, and a possible include items that were not reinforcing,
limitation of both the MSWO and PS pro- thereby producing a higher proportion of
cedures is that both are too inclusive. Several nonselections.
of the items chosen less frequently in these Alternatively, the discrepancy might be
procedures may not maintain the levels of partially attributable to differences in the as-
behavior maintained by the more frequently sortment of stimuli used in the two studies.
chosen items. Thus, if one’s goal is to find That is, Windsor et al. (1994) used all food
one or two highly potent reinforcers, the MS items, whereas we used a combination of
procedure may more effectively weed out both food and leisure items. Seventeen of
stimuli of questionable utility. However, in the 25 items (68%) never selected in the
terms of evaluating the assessments strictly present study were leisure items, and only
by how many reinforcers are identified, we 24 of 41 (58.5%) leisure items were selected
suggest that the critical demonstration is not at all. It may simply be the case that for this
how often failure to identify occurs, but that population, food items, as a class of rein-
it occurs at all. If so, and given the results forcers, are more potent than the stimuli we
of Experiment 2, we conclude that the identified as leisure items. In addition, pref-
MSWO and PS procedures appear to iden- erences for leisure items may simply be more
tify more stimuli that are at least minimally idiosyncratic than for food items, and the
reinforcing than does the MS procedure. leisure items used in the present study sim-
This feature of the MSWO and PS proce- ply were not items that the participants
dures may be beneficial when attempting to found reinforcing. These speculations sug-
identify a variety of potential reinforcers for gest that if therapists are particularly con-
individuals reported to have few known re- cerned with identifying reinforcing leisure
inforcers. items, food items may have to be omitted
We previously suggested that the MS as- from the array.
sessment is prone to the production of false A general preference for food over non-
negatives due to the continuous availability food items may help to explain another dif-
of the most preferred items. However, ference between our results and those re-
Windsor et al. (1994) used a nearly identical ported by others. Specifically, the PS proce-
MS procedure that resulted in far fewer dure of Windsor et al. (1994) failed to yield
items never being selected. Across their 8 any stimulus that reached the 80% selection
participants, only 8 of 48 items were never criterion used by Fisher et al. (1992) to de-
selected in the multiple-stimulus procedure fine a preferred item. By contrast, Fisher et
(all eight were selected at least once in the al. found at least two stimuli per participant
paired-choice procedure). By contrast, across that met this criterion, and all but 1 partic-
our 7 participants, 25 of 49 items remained ipant in the present study selected one or
unselected. Several explanations for this dis- more items on at least 80% of trials during
crepancy are possible. In the present study, the PS assessment. Because Windsor et al.
more items were used per person, perhaps used food items exclusively, whereas Fisher
making it more likely that a nonreinforcing et al. and we used more diverse arrays con-
item was included in the array. Furthermore, sisting of both food and nonfood items, two
all the items used by Windsor et al. (1994) tentative conclusions follow from these ob-
were selected by caregivers as items that the servations. First, outcomes obtained using
participants seemed to enjoy. No such cri- the PS, and perhaps the MSWO, procedure
terion was placed upon the array in the pres- may vary as a function of the composition
ent study, making the array more likely to of the array. That is, when all the items are
REINFORCER PREFERENCE ASSESSMENT 531

similar and highly preferred (as may be the MSWO assessment and second in the PS
case with food rather than nonfood items), assessment.
less distinct rankings might emerge. Alter- Additional research is needed to more
natively, when food items are included with clearly determine not only the predictive va-
nonfood items in mixed arrays, results of the lidity of outcomes obtained from reinforcer-
present study suggest a high preference for preference assessments but how those out-
food such that selection percentages for food comes should best be interpreted. Initial
reach higher levels at the expense of nonfood steps in this direction have been taken re-
items, resulting in some food items reaching cently by Piazza, Fisher, Hagopian, Bow-
80% selection. In the present study, all items man, and Toole (1996), who reported that
that reached 80% selection in the PS assess- the three highest ranked items or activities
ment were food items. from arrays of at least 12, as selected through
A second conclusion is that it is currently a PS assessment, appeared to function more
difficult to set a cutoff criterion for the pre- effectively as reinforcers than did stimuli
diction of effective reinforcers. Again, Fisher chosen from the middle or bottom of the
et al. (1992) defined items that reached 80% rankings, when compared in a concurrent-
selection in the PS procedure as high-pref- operants arrangement. In that study, rein-
erence items, and this criterion was based on forcer potency was predicted, and later ver-
that set by Pace et al. (1985) for the single- ified, simply by ranking the items based on
stimulus presentation method. However, if relative preference. Thus, until more is
selection percentages can vary as a function known about how the use of specific assess-
of the array composition, stimuli falling far ment procedures or stimuli might actually
short of 80% selection may function at least determine (i.e., set limits on) the manner in
modestly as reinforcers (the stimuli tested in which participants’ selections are made, rel-
the second experiment, gum for Carlos and ative ranking might be preferable to a per-
beets for Rupert, were selected on 40% and centage criterion in making predictions
33.3%, respectively, of the trials during about reinforcer efficacy, although rankings
which they were available in the PS proce- may also be subject to procedural influences.
dure). A selection criterion of 80% would That is, the findings of Fisher et al. may be
have resulted in the rejection of these stimuli specific to preference assessments that in-
as reinforcers. The difficulty of setting a per- volve relatively large stimulus arrays. For
centage criterion for the MSWO procedure smaller arrays, results may have to be inter-
is further compounded by the nature of the preted more cautiously, as evidenced by the
measurement. That is, given a perfectly con- data obtained for Bessie in the second ex-
sistent pattern of selection in which one item periment, which showed that juice (ranked
is always chosen first, another always chosen second and third highest in the PS and
second, and so on, the second highest MSWO procedures, respectively) did not
ranked item will be chosen on only 50% of function as a reinforcer in a free-operant ar-
the trials in which it was available (never on rangement. This cautionary note is particu-
the first trial, always on the second trial, and larly relevant for multiple-stimulus formats,
unavailable thereafter), thus falling below because the array size may be practically lim-
the 80% criterion set for other assessment ited by the number of items that can be pre-
methods. It is noteworthy that, for Bessie, sented simultaneously before position biases,
the item tested (juice) failed to maintain related to the effort of having to reach fur-
rates of behavior above those observed in ther for items on the edges of the array, are
baseline, yet this item ranked third in the expressed.
532 ISER G. DELEON and BRIAN A. IWATA

REFERENCES foundly retarded individuals. Journal of Applied


Behavior Analysis, 18, 249–255.
Fehr, M., Wacker, D., Tresize, J., Lennon, R., & Mey- Paclawskyj, T. R., & Vollmer, T. M. (1995). Rein-
erson, L. (1979). Visual, auditory, and vibratory forcer assessment for children with developmental
stimulation as reinforcers for profoundly retarded disabilities and visual impairment. Journal of Ap-
children. Rehabilitation Psychology, 26, 201–209. plied Behavior Analysis, 28, 219–224.
Fisher, W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian, Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Hagopian, L. P., Bow-
L. P., Owens, J. C., & Slevin, I. (1992). A com- man, L. G., & Toole, L. (1996). Using a choice
parison of two approaches for identifying rein- assessment to predict reinforcer effectiveness. Jour-
nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 1–9.
forcers for persons with severe and profound dis-
Smith, R. G., Iwata, B. A., & Shore, B. A. (1995).
abilities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25,
Effects of subject-versus experimenter-selected re-
491–498.
inforcers on the behavior of individuals with pro-
Green, C. W., Reid, D. H., White, L. K., Halford, R.
found developmental disabilities. Journal of Ap-
C., Brittain, D. P., & Gardner, S. M. (1988).
plied Behavior Analysis, 28, 61–71.
Identifying reinforcers for persons with profound Windsor, J., Piche, L. M., & Locke, P. A. (1994).
handicaps: Staff opinion versus systematic assess- Preference testing: A comparison of two presen-
ments of preferences. Journal of Applied Behavior tation methods. Research in Developmental Dis-
Analysis, 21, 31–43. abilities, 15, 439–455.
Mason, S. M., McGee, G. G., Farmer-Dougan, V., &
Risley, T. R. (1989). A practical strategy for on- Received January 22, 1996
going reinforcer assessment. Journal of Applied Be- Initial editorial decision March 23, 1996
havior Analysis, 22, 171–179. Revisions received June 20, 1996; July 8, 1996
Pace, G. M., Ivancic, M. T., Edwards, G. L., Iwata, Final acceptance July 24, 1996
B. A., & Page, T. A. (1985). Assessment of stim- Action Editor, Wayne W. Fisher
ulus preference and reinforcer value with pro-

STUDY QUESTIONS
1. Describe the paired-stimulus (PS) and multiple-stimulus (MS) assessment formats. What
appears to be the main advantage of the MS format and what is a potential limitation?

2. How does the authors’ MSWO format differ from the MS format described by Windsor et
al. (1994), and what are its potential advantages over the MS and PS formats?

3. The authors compared the PS, MS, and MSWO procedures in the first experiment. What
were the dependent measures, and what results were obtained?

4. What was the purpose of the second experiment and what criteria were used in selecting
the stimuli?

5. Describe the basic design used in the second experiment and the results that were obtained.

6. Results of the second experiment suggested that the MS assessment may be prone to the
production of false negatives, yet Windsor et al. (1994) used a nearly identical MS procedure
that resulted in far fewer items never being selected. How did the authors account for these
discrepant findings?
REINFORCER PREFERENCE ASSESSMENT 533

7. Based on information contained in the article, describe several ways in which either the
assessment methodology itself or the stimuli selected for presentation may affect outcomes
obtained during reinforcer preference assessments.

8. Given the results of this study, indicate which assessment format would be preferred for the
following purposes, and explain your selection(s).
(a) Frequent (e.g., daily or weekly) selection of a single potent reinforcer.
(b) Frequent selection of several reinforcers that might be varied to prevent satiation.
(c) Infrequent selection of several reinforcers.

Questions prepared by Iser DeLeon and Jana Lindberg, University of Florida

You might also like