Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 8

CLINICAL STUDIES

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL EFFECTS OF THIRD VENTRICLE


TUMOR SURGERY
Melissa A. Friedman, Ph.D. OBJECTIVE: This study assessed the neuropsychological outcome of patients after
The Brain and Spine Center, The surgical treatment for third ventricle brain tumors. Neuropsychological consequences
University of Texas M. D.
Anderson Cancer Center, and
of surgical intervention can have a major impact on patients’ quality of life and
Department of Psychology, therefore have important implications for treatment planning.
University of Houston, Houston, METHODS: A retrospective analysis of 33 patients’ neuropsychological data was
Texas
performed. All patients received a comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation after
Christina A. Meyers, Ph.D. treatment for a primary brain tumor in the third ventricular region. Twenty-six patients
The Brain and Spine Center, The
underwent surgery, 14 via the transcallosal approach and 12 via a subfrontal, left
University of Texas M. D. transcortical, right pterional, or infratentorial supracerebellar approach. Seven patients
Anderson Cancer Center, Houston, were not treated by surgical intervention.
Texas
RESULTS: There was a significantly elevated frequency of cognitive impairment rela-
Raymond Sawaya, M.D. tive to normative values in memory, executive functioning, and fine manual speed and
The Brain and Spine Center, The dexterity. There were no differences in mean neuropsychological scores between
University of Texas M. D. patients who underwent surgery and those who did not. There were no differences in
Anderson Cancer Center, Houston, mean performance on the basis of surgical approach, tumor infiltration, or history of
Texas
cranial irradiation. Repeated measures data available for two patients revealed mem-
Reprint requests: ory impairment before and after surgery, and one patient experienced major improve-
Christina A. Meyers, Ph.D., ment after surgery on a measure of mental flexibility and problem solving.
Department of Neuro-oncology,
University of Texas M. D. CONCLUSION: Patients with third ventricle tumors are at risk for developing impair-
Anderson Cancer Center, Box 431, ments in memory, executive function, and fine manual speed and dexterity, which are
1515 Holcombe Boulevard, domains associated with frontal subcortical functions. In the current study, different
Houston, TX 77030.
Email: cameyers@mdanderson.org. types of treatment were not associated with differential cognitive sequelae, and
surgical intervention did not account for cognitive deficits.
Received, May 7, 2002.
KEY WORDS: Cognition, Neuropsychology, Surgery, Third ventricle, Tumors
Accepted, November 11, 2002.
Neurosurgery 52:791-798, 2003 DOI: 10.1227/01.NEU.0000053367.94965.6B www.neurosurgery-online.com

T
he objective of this study was to evaluate and describe mus, mamillary bodies, mamillothalamic tract, fornix, and
the neuropsychological status, relative to normal func- internal medullary lamina (31).
tioning, of patients with tumors in the third ventricle of Cognitive impairments may result from tumor effects as
the brain after surgery and to identify factors that may account well as from treatment effects. Direct and indirect tumor ef-
for the variability in performance in such patients. Neuropsy- fects include increased intracranial pressure, tumor compres-
chological outcome is highly relevant to treatment planning sion of structures important for memory (25, 27), and tumor
for patients with third ventricle brain tumors because of the infiltration of memory structures (22, 34). Treatment effects
risk of damaging brain structures in the third ventricular may include surgical manipulation and resulting damage to
region during treatment. Group studies and individual case structures important for cognitive functioning. In addition,
reports suggest that cognitive deficits, particularly in memory, many patients receive cranial irradiation, which is known to
are associated with tumors in this region (11, 37, 39, 45). exert protracted and irreversible demyelinating effects on sub-
Studies of nontumor pathological entities, such as infarcts and cortical white matter. Such effects are associated with cogni-
alcoholic Korsakoff’s syndrome, associate similar memory tive slowing and decline in memory and executive functioning
deficits with damage to structures in the third ventricular (13, 35). The neuropsychological effects of chemotherapy are
region (10, 14, 20, 30, 32, 41). The third ventricular structures thought to be acute and reversible except in cases of intra-
and tracts crucial to memory functioning include the thala- arterial or intraventricular administration; protracted treat-

NEUROSURGERY VOLUME 52 | NUMBER 4 | APRIL 2003 | 791


FRIEDMAN ET AL.

ment toxicity is considered attributable to cranial irradiation, of a ventricular wall such as that of the posterior third
although the synergistic toxicity of radiation administered ventricle.
concurrently with chemotherapy has yet to be delineated in A biopsy procedure had been performed on seven patients
the literature (36). before their neuropsychological evaluations. Of these, four
The purpose of the present study was to evaluate and underwent a needle biopsy in which a cannula was passed
describe the neuropsychological outcome of a relatively large through the cortex under either stereotactic or endoscopic
sample of third ventricle tumor patients and to identify factors visual guidance. The remaining three patients underwent an
influencing neuropsychological outcome. This objective in- open biopsy, one via a right pterional approach, and two via
cluded evaluating the differential effects of surgical treat- a left frontal transcortical approach. When patients were
ments, tumor type, and radiation therapy on patients’ neuro- grouped into surgical categories for comparisons, the latter
psychological performance. three patients were grouped in the category corresponding to
their biopsy approach, as the cranial and brain manipulation
required for the open biopsy were similar to those performed
PATIENTS AND METHODS for the surgical resection.
Thirty-three patients, including 17 females and 16 males, Patients were grouped on the basis of three variables: tumor
were studied retrospectively. Their average age was 31 years type, surgical treatment, and whether they had a history of
(standard deviation [SD], 13 yr; range, 13–63 yr). The average receiving cranial irradiation therapy. Table 1 lists the number
education level of the patients was 14 years (SD, 3 yr; range, of patients in each of these groups. Fourteen patients under-
8–20 yr). All patients had been diagnosed with tumors in the went cranial irradiation (with or without chemotherapy), and
third ventricular region of the brain. The average time be- 19 did not. Regarding tumor type, patients were grouped
tween diagnosis and evaluation was 31 months (SD, 56 mo; according to their pathological tumor diagnosis in one of two
median, 8 mo; range, 1 wk to 250 mo). Twenty-six patients categories: extra-axial intraventricular tumors (n ⫽ 18), which
underwent surgical resection of their tumors, and postopera- usually are well-circumscribed and slow-growing tumors; or
tive neuropsychological data were available for all of them; infiltrative tumors (n ⫽ 15), which tend to be faster-growing
preoperative data also were available for two patients. The tumors that are invasive of surrounding structures or infiltra-
seven patients who did not undergo surgical resection also tive of white matter and densely cellular. Table 2 lists the
received a neuropsychological evaluation. For the two patients pathological diagnoses within each category.
who had both pre- and postoperative evaluations, the postop- As listed in Table 1, there were three categories for surgical
erative evaluation was used in the statistical analyses. treatment: no surgery (n ⫽ 7), surgery via the anterior tran-
Before their neuropsychological evaluation, 20 patients had scallosal approach (n ⫽ 14), and surgery via other approaches
symptoms of and were treated for increased intracranial pres- (n ⫽ 12). The “other surgery” category included patients who
sure associated with acute hydrocephalus. Treatment of these received the subfrontal (n ⫽ 2), left frontal transcortical (n ⫽
patients consisted of a cerebrospinal fluid diversion procedure 4), right pterional (n ⫽ 3), or infratentorial supracerebellar
to improve cerebrospinal fluid circulation, such as an external approach (n ⫽ 2). As this study required a retrospective re-
ventriculostomy, a ventriculoperitoneal shunt, or fenestration view of records, there was one patient whose surgical ap-

TABLE 1. No. of patients in the surgical, radiation, and tumor type groups
No cranial irradiation Received cranial irradiation
Surgical approach Tumor type Total
(n ⴝ 19) (n ⴝ 14)

No surgery (n ⫽ 7) Extra-axial intraventricular 3 2 5

Infiltrative 1 1 2

Total 4 3 7

Transcallosal (n ⫽ 14) Extra-axial intraventricular 3 2 5

Infiltrative 4 5 9

Total 7 7 14

Other (n ⫽ 12) Extra-axial intraventricular 6 1 7

Infiltrative 2 3 5

Total 8 4 12

792 | VOLUME 52 | NUMBER 4 | APRIL 2003 www.neurosurgery-online.com


THIRD VENTRICLE TUMOR SURGERY

problem solving (Comprehension), verbal reasoning (Similar-


TABLE 2. Tumor pathology by tumor type group ities), and graphomotor speed (Digit Symbol). Verbal memory
Tumor No. was assessed using the Verbal Selective Reminding Test
(VSRT) (9, 21), which provides measures of storage and de-
Extra-axial intraventricular layed recall of a word list. Some patients also were tested with
Ependymoma 2 the Hopkins Verbal Learning (8) and the immediate and de-
Craniopharyngioma 5 layed measures of the Logical Memory tests from the Wech-
Colloid cyst 2 sler Memory Scale (43). Executive functioning was measured
Central neurocytoma 3 using the Booklet Category Test (16, 18), the Trail Making Test
Germinoma 6 Part B (18, 38), and Digit Span Backward (28, 44). Motor speed
and dexterity were tested with the Grooved Pegboard (33).
Infiltrative
Visuoperceptual measurements included the Trail Making
Pineoblastoma 2
Test Part A (18, 38) for simple visual tracking speed and the
Low-grade glioma 8
Benton Test of Facial Recognition (7) for discrimination of
Anaplastic glioma 3
complex visual stimuli. The Benton Visual Retention Test (5,
Glioblastoma multiforme 2
49), Administration A, was used as a measure of working
memory for visual forms. Language functioning was mea-
sured using tests of the Multilingual Aphasia Examination (6),
proach could not be determined, and this patient was included including Controlled Oral Word Association as a test of rate of
in the “other” category. These subgroups were combined for fluency, the Visual Naming test, and the Token Test of aural
the purpose of conducting statistical analyses because individ- comprehension.
ually they were very small.
The no-surgery group consisted of patients who had not
undergone surgical resection of their tumor. For these pa-
tients, it was determined by their physician that the tumor RESULTS
type had a high likelihood of responding to radiation or
Independent sample t tests revealed no differences between
chemotherapy, or its effects would be treated best by palliat-
neuropsychologically unimpaired and impaired patients with
ing symptoms of increased intracranial pressure with a cere-
respect to in age, education, or time since diagnosis. Table 3
brospinal fluid diversion mechanism.
lists the mean standardized score and the percentage of pa-
One patient underwent two operations via the anterior tran-
tients displaying mildly and severely impaired performance
scallosal approach before the neuropsychological evaluation.
for each neuropsychological test. (Standard scores are pre-
Documentation clearly indicated that the second operation
sented in the form of z scores or percentiles. A z score repre-
used the same approach and reopened the incisions made
sents, in SD units, the degree to which a score deviates from
during the previous operation. This patient’s neuropsycholog-
the mean of the normative population. With the population
ical performance was later evaluated individually to ensure
mean set at a z score of 0.00, 68.26% of individuals are ex-
that her status in this regard did not confound the results of
pected to obtain a z score between ⫺1.00 and 1.00. Approxi-
the study.
mately 13.59% of individuals are expected to obtains a z score
Determination of which surgical approach would be used
between ⫺1.00 and ⫺2.00, 2.14% between ⫺2.00 and ⫺3.00,
for each participant was made by the neurosurgeon. Such
and 0.13% lower than ⫺3.00. The same frequency expectations
determination was made according to the objective of obtain-
apply to the respective positive z scores.) Mildly impaired
ing maximal exposure of the lesion, with minimal injury to
performance was defined as a score at least 1.5 SDs below the
normal neural and vascular structures. The location of the
published, adjusted normative mean for that test. By use of
tumor within the ventricle, therefore, was a primary consid-
this criterion, in the normal population, mildly impaired per-
eration in determining the surgical approach.
formance would be expected to occur in approximately 6.7%
of people. Severely impaired performance was defined as a
Measures score at least 2.5 SDs below the published, adjusted normative
Selection of neuropsychological tests was in accordance mean for that test. In the normal population, severely im-
with the objective of providing a comprehensive description paired performance would be expected to occur in less than
of cognitive performance in this population. Only tests with 1% of people. Binomial tests were conducted to determine the
standardized procedures, published evidence of reliability extent to which observed impairment frequencies differed
and validity, and normative data were selected. General intel- from normative expectation. As listed in Table 3, greater than
lectual functioning was measured using subtests of the Wech- expected frequency of mild impairment was demonstrated on
sler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised (WAIS-R) (44), including measures of memory, executive function, and manual speed
those measuring patients’ fund of worldly information (Infor- and dexterity, and greater than expected frequency of severe
mation), attention span (Digit Span), sustained concentration impairment was demonstrated on measures of memory and
(Arithmetic), nonverbal reasoning (Block Design), real-world manual speed and dexterity.

NEUROSURGERY VOLUME 52 | NUMBER 4 | APRIL 2003 | 793


FRIEDMAN ET AL.

TABLE 3. Mean z scores, percentage mildly impaired, and percentage severely impaired by neuropsychological testa
Percentage Percentage
Mean z score
Test No. mildly impaired severely impaired
(SD)
(z score <ⴚ1.5) (z score <ⴚ2.5)

Intellectual function (WAIS-R)


Verbal IQ 25 ⫺0.68 (0.77) 12 0
Information 28 ⫺0.51 (0.98) 14 4
Digit Span total (forward and backward breakdown, 33 ⫺0.37 (0.83) 6 0
below)
Arithmetic 31 ⫺0.28 (0.84) 0 0
Comprehension 24 ⫺0.68 (0.83) 13 4
Similarities 32 ⫺0.34 (1.04) 9 3
Block Design 32 ⫺0.19 (0.84) 3 0
Digit Symbol 33 ⫺0.56 (1.07) 27b 0
Verbal memory
Word Recall-Immediate (VSRT/CLTR) 21 ⫺2.06 (1.56) 67b 43b
Word Recall-Delayed (VSRT) 21 ⫺2.38 (2.14) 67b 43b
Word Recall-Immediate (Hopkins/sum of trials) 6 ⫺2.24 (1.62) 67b 67b
Word Recognition (Hopkins/Discrimination Index) 6 ⫺2.01 (2.86) 50b 33b
Story Recall-Immediate (WMS-R/Logical Memory) 7 ⫺1.55 (1.18) 57b 29b
Story Recall-Delayed (WMS-R/Logical Memory) 7 ⫺3.46 (3.50) 57b 43b
Visual Memory (BVRT error score) 19 ⫺0.93 (1.78) 26b 16b
Attention/executive functioning
Digit Span Forwardc (WAIS-R) 32 6 (raw span) 6 0
Digit Span Backwardc (WAIS-R) 32 4 (raw span) 28b 3
Booklet Category test 16 ⫺1.16 (1.32) 44b 13b
Trail Making test A 30 ⫺1.23 (2.08) 27b 20b
Trail Making test B 29 ⫺1.89 (2.83) 35b 28b
Manual speed and dexterity
Grooved pegboard-Right 25 ⫺2.46 (3.23) 52b 40b
Grooved Pegboard-Left 25 ⫺1.69 (2.27) 36b 24b
Visual perceptual ability
Facial recognition 20 45th percentile 10 5
Language ability (MLAE)
Visual naming 25 51st percentile 20 12b
Fluency (COWA) 33 35th percentile 24b 9b
Language comprehension (Token test) 24 42nd percentile 13 4
a
SD, standard deviation; WAIS-R, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised; IQ, intelligence quotient; VSRT, Verbal Selective Reminding Test; CLTR; continuous
long-term retrieval; WMS-R, Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised; BVRT, Benton Visual Retention Test; MLAE, Multilingual Aphasia Examination; COWA, Controlled
Oral Word Association.
b
Observed impairment frequency greater than normative expectation, binomial test; P ⬍ 0.01.
c
Forward span of 4 is considered mildly impaired, and 3 or less is considered severely impaired. Backward span of 3 is considered mildly impaired, and 2 or less
severely impaired. Adapted from, Lezak M: Neuropsychological Assessment. New York, Oxford University Press, 1995, ed 3 (28).

Independent sample t tests revealed no significant differ- only one word. This was the comparison between the tran-
ence with regard to any of the major grouping variables (i.e., scallosal group, with a mean z score of ⫺1.90 (or 7 of 12
tumor type, surgical intervention, or whether patients re- words), and the no-surgery group, with a mean z score of
ceived cranial irradiation) for any neuropsychological test. For ⫺2.72 (or 6 of 12 words).
example, on the Delayed Recall measure of the VSRT, which is The four patients who underwent the left frontal transcor-
expected to be sensitive to damage to memory structures in tical approach did not display greater language impairment
the third ventricular region, mean performance was similar than any other group. None of these four patients in the left
across groups, as was impairment frequency (Table 4). The frontal transcortical group displayed any impairment on nam-
largest mean difference in that measure, for any of the com- ing of visual stimuli or aural comprehension. One patient in
parisons, amounted to an approximate difference in recall of the transcallosal group underwent two operations before the

794 | VOLUME 52 | NUMBER 4 | APRIL 2003 www.neurosurgery-online.com


THIRD VENTRICLE TUMOR SURGERY

TABLE 4. Comparison of mean z scores across group variables on Verbal Selective Reminding Test delayed recalla
Tumor type Surgical approach Cranial irradiation

Extra-axial Received
Infiltrative Transcallosal Other approach No surgery No irradiation
intraventricular irradiation
(n ⴝ 12) (n ⴝ 8) (n ⴝ 6) (n ⴝ 7) (n ⴝ 12)
(n ⴝ 9) (n ⴝ 9)

Mean ⫾ SDb ⫺2.65 ⫾ 2.10 ⫺2.01 ⫾ 2.27 ⫺1.90 ⫾ 2.22 ⫺2.61 ⫾ 1.80 ⫺2.72 ⫾ 2.53 ⫺2.41 ⫾ 2.25 ⫺2.35 ⫾ 2.16

Percentage impaired 56% 75% 63% 83% 57% 67% 67%


(z score ⱕ⫺1.5)
a
SD, standard deviation.
b
Independent sample t tests comparing patients by tumor type, surgical group, and radiation group revealed no statistically significant differences between
comparison groups.

neuropsychological evaluation. According to the surgical re- except verbal recall, which declined into the severely impaired
ports, the second operation reopened the incisions made dur- range; this decrement could be attributable to the delayed
ing the first procedure. Scrutiny of this patient’s performance effects of cranial irradiation, surgical intervention, or other
revealed that all of her scores were at ⫺0.33 SDs or better, factors.
which suggests average or better performance across cognitive
domains. DISCUSSION
The effect on patient performance of shunting as a remedy
for hydrocephalus was evaluated. Independent sample t tests The results presented here indicate that patients with third
revealed no differences on any neuropsychological test be- ventricle tumors have a greater frequency of impairments in
tween patients who did and those who did not undergo a memory, executive functioning, and manual speed and dex-
shunting procedure. terity than the normal population. Other studies of patients
Preoperative neuropsychological data were available for with third ventricle tumors also detected impairments in these
two patients. One of these patients was a 36-year-old woman cognitive domains (12, 15, 17, 19, 24, 26, 29, 40, 42, 46–48).
who underwent the supracerebellar approach for resection of These findings are consistent with known functional neuro-
a low-grade glioma and fenestration of the posterior third anatomy, as the third ventricle lies among various tracts that
ventricle to relieve hydrocephalus; she did not receive adju- interconnect brain regions and structures known to be associ-
vant radiation or chemotherapy. Her preoperative neuropsy- ated with such memory, executive, and motor functions.
chological evaluation revealed severe impairment in verbal Elevated frequency of mild impairment, defined by perfor-
memory (immediate and delayed measures of the VSRT), mance more than 1.5 SDs below the normative mean (below
higher-order reasoning and problem solving (Category Test), the 7th percentile), was demonstrated in memory (57–67% of
and bimanual speed and dexterity (Grooved Pegboard). Her patients), executive functioning (27–44% of patients), and
performance was generally intact on WAIS-R intellectual manual speed and dexterity (36–52% of patients). A large
subtests. Follow-up evaluation, conducted 2 months after sur- subset of the patients displayed particularly severe impair-
gery, revealed stability of impairment in memory and in man- ment in memory and in manual speed and dexterity, with 43%
ual speed and dexterity, but improvement by more than 3 SDs of patients performing more than 2.5 SDs below the normative
in higher-order reasoning and problem solving, which prob- mean (below the 1st percentile) on memory tests (Table 3). Our
ably was largely attributable to the relief of hydrocephalus impairment frequencies are higher than those reported in
and a practice effect. previous research studies, in which, for instance, only 7 to 23%
The other patient for whom pre- and postoperative data of patients were impaired in memory performance (17, 23, 47).
were available was a 63-year-old woman diagnosed with an This suggests that the risk for cognitive impairment in this
anaplastic astrocytoma who had a history of cranial irradia- population may be greater than previously thought.
tion and chemotherapy before undergoing resection via the Amnestic syndromes generally are characterized by prom-
transcallosal approach; she required no intervention for hy- inent memory deficit with relatively preserved general intel-
drocephalus. Her preoperative neuropsychological evaluation lectual functioning (3). The preserved abilities in our third
revealed severely impaired verbal recognition memory (Hop- ventricle tumor patients were those often associated with in-
kins Verbal Learning Test, Discrimination Index) and border- tellectual and higher cortical functions. For instance, the
line performance on a measure of working memory (Digit WAIS-R Verbal intelligence quotient scores, as well as scores
Span Backward), with intact performance in all other cognitive on WAIS-R subtests assessing fund of information, mental
domains. Follow-up evaluation conducted 13 months after arithmetic, real-world problem solving, abstract verbal rea-
tumor resection revealed stability of performance in all areas soning, and visuospatial analysis revealed no statistically sig-

NEUROSURGERY VOLUME 52 | NUMBER 4 | APRIL 2003 | 795


FRIEDMAN ET AL.

nificant elevation in impairment frequency. Nor was signifi- reasoning and problem solving that resolved after surgery.
cant impairment frequency observed in measures of complex The improvement was attributed to relief of hydrocephalus
visual discrimination (Facial Recognition) or aural language and the likelihood of a practice effect. This postoperative
comprehension (Token Test). Together, these findings suggest improvement on the measure of higher-order reasoning, in the
that a subset of patients displayed an amnestic syndrome, context of stable impairment in memory and fine manual
which probably was secondary to diencephalic disruption. speed and dexterity, serves as an example of the possibility for
The finding of a pattern of impairment in some cognitive dissociation within individuals of impairments across cogni-
domains with intact performance in others highlights the im- tive domains and demonstrates that different pathological
portance in this field of research of comprehensive neuropsy- mechanisms in patients with third ventricle tumors may affect
chological evaluations, with selection of tests sensitive to im- different cognitive domains.
pairment across domains. In this patient sample, for instance, Although patient performance did not differ significantly
use of the WAIS-R intelligence quotient score alone would not by comparison group, it is possible that the detection of group
have allowed for the detection of impaired versus preserved differences was impeded by low statistical power. A larger
functions. sample would confer greater statistical power. On the Delayed
In this study, mean performance did not vary according to Recall measure of the VSRT, for instance, the difference be-
whether patients underwent surgery as treatment for their tween the mean z score in the transcallosal surgery group and
tumors. In fact, the seven patients who did not undergo sur- the no-surgery group (Table 4) was 0.82, equal to a difference
gical intervention, and the two patients evaluated preopera- in recall of approximately 1 word on the 12-item list. A power
tively, were as impaired in memory, executive, or motor func- analysis revealed that a sample of 130 patients per group
tions as were patients who did undergo surgery. Among would be required to detect a statistically significant differ-
patients who underwent surgery, mean performance did not ence of this magnitude. However, even if such statistical sig-
vary according to whether they underwent the transcallosal nificance were established in a study, the clinical significance
approach or the other approaches combined. Performance also of such a difference would be debatable.
did not vary according to whether patients underwent radia- Although mean group differences were not significant,
tion treatment or whether they had an infiltrative versus in- there was very large variability of performance within groups
traventricular extra-axial tumor. Finally, patients who under- (Table 4), which suggests that variables other than those iden-
went the left frontal transcortical approach exhibited no tified in this study may have accounted for variability in
evidence of greater language impairment than those who un- cognitive performance. For instance, this study did not control
derwent any other approach. These results suggest that poorer for the effects of time since diagnosis, which may have mod-
performance could not be attributed to surgical intervention, erated the effects of tumor type; tumor infiltrativeness may
radiation therapy, or tumor type. have had its greatest effects on patients with longer duration
Previous studies using pre- and postoperative evaluations since diagnosis, in whom there had been more time for infil-
support the finding that surgical intervention does not neces- tration to occur. Similarly, the ability of this study to detect
sarily induce cognitive impairments. Honegger et al. (23) stud- delayed radiation effects might have been improved if it had
ied 13 patients treated surgically for craniopharyngiomas and included an evaluation of the effects of amount of time since
reported that 12 of the patients displayed no memory impair- radiation or dose of radiation.
ment after surgery, and mean scores were stable or improved The following recommendations are made for future stud-
from pre- to postoperative evaluations. Bellotti et al. (4) stud- ies. First, to test the effects of surgical intervention, future
ied 31 tumor patients evaluated after third ventricular sur- studies should use a prospective design with pre- and post-
gery. Although postoperative memory deficits were detected, operative evaluations and a no-surgery comparison group.
all 12 patients who received a preoperative evaluation dis- Second, studies should control for the effects of different ma-
played the presence of the deficit at that time. Donnett et al. nipulations within surgical groupings, particularly for tran-
(17) studied 22 patients, 12 of whom received preoperative scallosal surgery patients. The transcallosal approach may
evaluations, and found that 50% of those who received pre- include one of several possible routes for entry into the third
operative evaluations displayed impairments in memory and ventricle from the lateral ventricle (e.g., via an interforniceal
executive functioning, and 66% of those with baseline impair- route [1, 2], via a dilated foramen of Monro, via incision of a
ments displayed resolution of impairments after surgery. forniceal column to enlarge the foramen of Monro, and via a
Hutter et al. (24) studied 18 patients treated for third ventricle subchoroidal route [48]), and these routes involve manipula-
tumors with transcallosal surgery; of the four who received a tion of different memory structures. In the current study,
preoperative evaluation, all exhibited improvement, particu- combining patients who underwent different entry routes in
larly in memory, after surgery. the transcallosal group may have masked the unique effects of
The current study provided pre- and postoperative data for any one route. Third, the effects of tumor and treatment vari-
two patients. Both displayed preoperative memory impair- ables, in addition to those evaluated here, should be consid-
ments that were not resolved at the postoperative evaluation. ered, and future studies may control for variables such as
One of these patients, a 36-year-old woman, also displayed extent of tumor resection, degree of preoperative impairment,
preoperatively impaired performance on a test of higher-order time since diagnosis, and time since cranial irradiation.

796 | VOLUME 52 | NUMBER 4 | APRIL 2003 www.neurosurgery-online.com


THIRD VENTRICLE TUMOR SURGERY

CONCLUSION 19. Geffen G, Walsh A, Simpson D, Jeeves M: Comparison of the effects of


transcortical and transcallosal removal of intraventricular tumours. Brain
103:773–788, 1980.
In summary, patients in this study displayed a high inci-
20. Graff-Radford NR, Tranel D, Van Hoesen GW, Brandt JP: Diencephalic
dence of impairment after surgery in the domains of memory, amnesia. Brain 113:1–25, 1990.
executive function, and fine manual speed and dexterity, 21. Hannay HJ, Levin HS: Selective reminding test: An examination of the
which are associated with frontal subcortical circuitry. These equivalence of four forms. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 7:251–263, 1985.
results are consistent with those of prior neuropsychological 22. Heilman KM, Sypert GW: Korsakoff’s syndrome resulting from bilateral
fornix lesions. Neurology 27:490–493, 1977.
outcome studies in this population, in which cognitive deficits
23. Honegger J, Barocka A, Sadri B, Fahlbusch R: Neuropsychological results of
were present after surgery but could not be attributed to the craniopharyngioma surgery in adults: A prospective study. Surg Neurol
direct effects of surgical intervention. Results of this study 50:19–29, 1998.
suggest that cognitive outcome is not differentially affected by 24. Hutter BO, Spetzger U, Bertalanffy JM, Gilsbach, JM: Cognition and quality
the treatment approaches used. of life in patients after transcallosal microsurgery for midline tumors.
J Neurosurg Sci 41:123–139, 1997.
25. Ignelzi RJ, Squire LR: Recovery from anterograde and retrograde amnesia
after percutaneous drainage of a cystic craniopharyngioma. J Neurol
REFERENCES Neurosurg Psychiatry 39:1231–1235, 1976.
26. Jeeves MA, Simpson DA, Geffen G: Functional consequences of the tran-
1. Apuzzo MLJ (ed): Surgery of the Third Ventricle. Baltimore, Williams & scallosal removal of intraventricular tumours. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychi-
Wilkins, 1998, ed 2. atry 42:134–142, 1979.
2. Apuzzo MLJ, Chikovani OK, Gott PS, Teng EL, Zee CS, Giannotta SL, Weiss 27. Kahn EA, Cosby EC: Korsakoff’s syndrome associated with surgical lesions
MH: Transcallosal, interfornicial approaches for lesions affecting the third involving the mammillary bodies. Neurology 22:117–125, 1972.
ventricle: Surgical considerations and consequences. Neurosurgery 10:547– 28. Lezak M: Neuropsychological Assessment. New York, Oxford University Press,
554, 1982. 1995, ed 3.
3. Bauer RM, Tobias B, Valenstein E: Amnestic disorders, in Heilman KM, 29. Lobosky JM, Vangilder JC, Domasio AR: Behavioural manifestations of third
Valenstein E (eds): Clinical Neuropsychology. New York, Oxford University ventricular colloid cysts. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 47:1075–1080, 1984.
Press, 1993, ed 3, pp 523–602. 30. Malamut BL, Graff-Radford N, Chawluk J, Grossman RI, Gur RC: Memory
4. Bellotti C, Pappada G, Sani R, Oliveri G, Stangalino C: The transcallosal in a case of bilateral thalamic infarction. Neurology 42:163–168, 1992.
approach for lesions affecting the lateral and third ventricles: Surgical 31. Markowitsch HJ: Diencephalic amnesia: A reorientation towards tracts?
considerations and results in a series of 42 cases. Acta Neurochir (Wien) Brain Res Rev 13:351–370, 1988.
111:103–107, 1991. 32. Markowitsch HJ, von Cramon DY, Schuri U: Mnestic performance profile
5. Benton AL: The Revised Visual Retention Test. Dubuque, William C. Brown of a bilateral diencephalic infarct patient with preserved intelligence
Co., 1974. and severe amnesic disturbances. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 15:667–652,
6. Benton AL, Hamsher KdeS: Multilingual Aphasia Examination. Iowa City, 1993.
AJA Associates, Inc., 1983. 33. Matthews CG, Kove H: Instruction Manual for the Adult Neuropsychology Test
7. Benton AL, Hamsher KdeS, Varney NR, Spreen O: Contributions to Neuro- Battery. Madison, University of Wisconsin Medical School, 1964.
psychological Assessment: A Clinical Manual. New York, Oxford University 34. McEntee WJ, Biber MP, Perl DP, Benson DF: Diencephalic amnesia: A
Press, 1983. reappraisal. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 39:436–441, 1976.
8. Brandt J: The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test: Development of a new verbal 35. Meyers CA: Neuropsychological aspects of cancer and cancer treatment.
memory test with six equivalent forms. Clin Neuropsychol 5:125–142, 1991. Physical Rehabilitation and Rehabilitation: State of the Art Reviews
9. Buschke H, Fuld PA: Evaluation of storage, retention, and retrieval in 8:229–241, 1994.
disordered memory and learning. Neurology 11:1019–1025, 1974. 36. Meyers CA: Issues of quality of life in neuro-oncology, in Vecht J (ed):
10. Butters N: Alcoholic Korsakoff’s syndrome: An update. Semin Neurol Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Amsterdam, Elsevier Science BV, 1997, vol 23,
4:226–284, 1984. pp 389–409.
11. Calabrese P, Markowitsch HJ, Harders AG, Scholz M, Gehlen W: Fornix
37. Palmai G, Taylor DC, Falconer MA: A case of craniopharyngioma present-
damage and memory: A case report. Cortex 31:555–564, 1995.
ing as Korsakov’s syndrome. Br J Psychiatry 113:619–623, 1967.
12. Cavazzuti V, Fischer EG, Welch K, Belli JA, Winston KR: Neurological and
38. Reitan RM, Davison LA: Clinical Neuropsychology: Current Status and Appli-
psychophysiological sequelae following different treatments of craniophar-
cations. New York, Winston/Wiley, 1974.
yngioma in children. J Neurosurg 59:409–417, 1983.
39. Shiminski-Maher T: Late effects associated with treatment of craniopharyn-
13. Christianson SA, Neppe V, Hoffman H: Amnesia and vegetative abnormal-
giomas in childhood. J Neurosci Nurs 22:220–225, 1990.
ities after irradiation treatment: A case study. Acta Neurol Scand 90:360–
40. Sweet WH, Talland GA, Ervin FR: Loss of recent memory following section
366, 1994.
14. Clarke S, Assal G, Bogousslavsky J, Regli F, Townsend DW, Leenders KL, of fornix. Trans Am Neurol Assoc 4:76–82, 1959.
Blecic S: Pure amnesia after unilateral left polar thalamic infarct: Topo- 41. Victor M, Adams RD, Collins GH: The Wernicke-Korsakoff Syndrome. Phila-
graphic and sequential neuropsychological and metabolic (PET) correla- delphia, F. A. Davis Co., 1971.
tions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 57:27–34, 1994. 42. Villani R, Papagno C, Tomei G, Grimoldi N, Spagnoli D, Bello L: Transcal-
15. Colangelo A, Ambrosio A, Colangelo C: Neurological and behavioral se- losal approach to tumors of the third ventricle: Surgical results and neuro-
quelae following different approaches to craniopharyngioma. Childs Nerv psychological evaluation. J Neurosurg Sci 41:41–50, 1997.
Syst 6:379–382, 1990. 43. Wechsler D: Wechsler Memory Scale. New York, The Psychological Corpora-
16. DeFilippis NA, McCampbell E: The Booklet Category Test. Odessa, Psycho- tion, 1945.
logical Assessment Resources, Inc., 1979. 44. Wechsler D: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised. San Antonio, The Psy-
17. Donnett A, Schmitt A, Dufour H, Grisoli F: Neuropsychological follow-up of chological Corporation, 1981.
twenty two adult patients after surgery for craniopharyngioma. Acta 45. Williams M, Pennybacker J: Memory disturbances in third ventricle
Neurochir (Wien) 141:1049–1054, 1999. tumours. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 17:115–123, 1954.
18. Fromm-Auch D, Yeudall LT: Normative data for the Halstead-Reitan neu- 46. Williams MJ, Medwedeff CH, Haban G: Memory disorder and subjective
ropsychological tests. J Clin Neuropsychol 5:221–238, 1983. time estimation. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 11:713–723, 1989.

NEUROSURGERY VOLUME 52 | NUMBER 4 | APRIL 2003 | 797


FRIEDMAN ET AL.

47. Winkler PA, Ilmberger J, Krishnan KG, Ruelen H-J: Transcallosal appropriate control groups to distinguish between the effects
interforniceal-transforaminal approach for lesions occupying the third ven- of treatment versus tumor.
tricular space: Clinical and neuropsychological results. Neurosurgery 46:
879–890, 2000. Jeffrey N. Bruce
48. Wochiewsky C, Vogel S, Lehmann R, Staudt J: Transcallosal removal of
New York, New York
lesions affecting the third ventricle: An anatomic and clinical study. Neuro-
surgery 36:117–122, 1995.
49. Youngjohn JR, Larrabee GJ, Crook TH: New adult age and education-
correction norms for the Benton Visual Retention Test. The Clinical Neu-
ropsychologist 7:155–160, 1993.
T he authors describe a very high incidence of neurological
deficit related to the occurrence and treatment of lesions
around the third ventricle. This has been documented in pre-
vious reports. The value of this article is its emphasis on the
Acknowledgment magnitude of the risk and the fact that there is no clearly safer
The authors received no financial support in conjunction with this article. method of avoiding the deficit, at least according to the data
presented. Problems with the article include the heterogeneity
COMMENTS of the patients and the approaches as well as the retrospective
nature of the review. The lack of a negative cognitive effect of

T he anatomic and pathophysiological basis for neurocognitive


deficits in patients with third ventricle tumors can be difficult
to decipher. A myriad of direct and indirect effects, alone or in
radiation is surprising and probably is related to the short time
between radiation and evaluation. Although this study pre-
sents interesting data, it does not answer the questions raised
combination, can contribute to these deficits: hydrocephalus, by its objective. These answers will await a better-planned,
compression or invasion of critical neuronal pathways coursing prospective study.
through or adjacent to the third ventricle, and the effects of
surgery, radiation, or chemotherapy. Optimization of patient Charles J. Hodge, Jr.
management depends on delineating causative effects. Syracuse, New York
Using a battery of tests, Friedman et al. provide an impor-
tant contribution by evaluating the postoperative neuropsy-
chological function of patients with third ventricle tumors.
T his is a descriptive study that is limited by the low number
of patients and the absence of preoperative data for all but
two patients. However, this report illustrates the significant
The cognitive impairments found in these patients are greater
than those reported in previous, less comprehensive studies. It cognitive problems faced by patients with lesions around the
is unfortunate that the retrospective nature of this study was third ventricle. It is slightly surprising that no clear differences
unable to provide more than two patients with pre- and could be identified among the various surgical approaches,
posttreatment evaluations. A larger number of patients with disease processes, and presence of hydrocephalus.
preoperative baseline evaluations would be helpful in sepa- The neurocognitive effects of brain tumors and surgery
rating tumor-related versus treatment-related effects. Al- have not been reported extensively. To the patient, these lim-
though the report provides evidence of interesting trends and itations in cognitive ability are very important in resuming a
a relative lack of differences between patients who underwent “normal” life and returning to the preoperative lifestyle. Fur-
surgery and radiation therapy, the small size of the sample ther studies such as this are needed.
groups may prohibit detection of these differences. The au-
thors provide useful recommendations for future studies, in- Joseph M. Piepmeier
cluding prospective pre- and postoperative evaluations and New Haven, Connecticut

Future Meetings—Congress of Neurological Surgeons


The following are the planned sites and dates for future annual meetings of the
Congress of Neurological Surgeons:
2003 Denver, CO October 18–23
2004 San Francisco, CA October 16–21
2005 Boston, MA October 8–13
2006 Chicago, IL October 7–12
Future Meetings—American Association of
Neurological Surgeons
The following are the planned sites and dates for future annual meetings of the
American Association of Neurological Surgeons:
2003 San Diego, CA April 26–May 1
2004 Orlando, FL May 1–6
2005 New Orleans, LA April 16–21
2006 San Francisco, CA April 22–27

798 | VOLUME 52 | NUMBER 4 | APRIL 2003 www.neurosurgery-online.com

You might also like