Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 16

The Moderating Effect of Knowledge and

Resources on the Persuasive Impact of


Analogies
MICHELLE L. ROEHM
BRIAN STERNTHAL*

Four experiments were conducted to examine the persuasive impact of new product
appeals containing an analogy. An analogy highlights the similarity in the benefits
offered by a familiar base product and an unfamiliar target product. This device is
found to be persuasive when (a) message recipients have the ability to map at-
tribute relations from some base category to understand the benefits of a target
product, and (b) they allocate the substantial resources needed to complete this
mapping. In the absence of either of these conditions, the persuasive impact of
an analogy is more limited. A variety of devices, including expertise with the base
product, training in how to process base information, and a positive mood, are
shown to improve the comprehension of an analogy and to enhance its
persuasiveness.

W hen personal digital assistants (PDAs) were intro-


duced in the mid-1990s, they offered an unusual
combination of characteristics. Although they resembled and
has a dialing pad, which a PDA does not. However, they
do share relational structures among attributes. Input re-
ceived from the keyboard on a PDA can activate a wireless
functioned like a conventional computer, one feature of modem and direct a transmission to a particular destination.
PDAs was uncharacteristic of traditional computers: they On a cellular phone, the dialing pad provides a parallel
could support wireless communication. A challenge in mar- function of directing information transmission.
keting PDAs and other novel products is to help consumers To comprehend this analogy, surface attributes are set
identify and appreciate their product benefits, particularly aside and structural relations are transferred from base to
those that might not be apparent from an inspection of a target. Because the cellular phone’s dialing pad attribute
product’s surface attributes. cannot be mapped onto the PDA, it is disregarded. However,
An analogy may be useful in this regard. An analogy the cellular phone’s wireless communication function does
compares a known base item to an unknown target item map and thus can be applied to the target PDA.
with which it shares a relational structure among attributes, The characteristic of allowing only relations to be mapped
but not surface features. For example, an analogy might be from base to target distinguishes an analogy from other types
drawn between a PDA and a cellular phone. These objects of comparison that have been described in the consumer
have few surface features in common. A PDA has a key- (see Gregan-Paxton and Roedder John 1997) and psycho-
board, which a cellular phone does not, and a cellular phone logical literatures (see Gentner and Markman 1997). A lit-
eral similarity, for instance, is a comparison that differs from
*Michelle L. Roehm is assistant professor of marketing, Babcock Grad-
uate School of Management, Wake Forest University, Winston-Salem, NC
an analogy in that both attributes and structural relations are
27109 (Michelle.Roehm@mba.wfu.edu). Brian Sternthal is Kraft Professor shared between a base and a target and thus can be mapped
of Marketing, Kellogg Graduate School of Management, Northwestern from base to target (Gentner 1983; Gentner and Markman
University, 2001 Sheridan Rd., Evanston, IL 60208. The authors acknowl- 1997).1 A comparison between a computer and a PDA rep-
edge the helpful input of the editor, the associate editor, and the three
reviewers. In addition, the authors thank Alice Isen for drawing attention
resents a literal similarity. Both computers and PDAs have
to a connection between mood and analogy and Jennifer Gregan-Paxton, attributes such as keyboards, screens, and memory. They
Dedre Gentner, and Harper Roehm for insightful comments on earlier drafts also share structural relations, such as using a keyboard to
of the article. Funding for this research was provided by the Proctor & input information to be stored in memory.
Gamble Research Grant to the Marketing Department at the Kellogg Grad-
uate School of Management and by the Babcock Graduate School of Man- 1
agement Faculty Research Fund. Correspondence may be directed to the Related frameworks utilizing different terminologies have been sug-
first author. gested by Gregan-Paxton and Roedder John (1997) and McQuarrie and
Mick (1996).

257

 2001 by JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH, Inc. ● Vol. 28 ● September 2001


All rights reserved. 0093-5301/2002/2802-0006$03.00

This content downloaded from 155.97.178.73 on Fri, 28 Nov 2014 01:05:55 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
258 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

In the current research, we examine the persuasive power to similarities in mere surface features. Thus, aspects of the
of an analogy. The goal is to identify conditions under which PDA such as display screens may be elaborated, leaving
an analogy’s effectiveness as a persuasion device may either less attention available for elaborating on its structural re-
be facilitated or undermined. To assess the impact of an lations and the associated functions such as information stor-
analogy, we contrast its effects with those prompted by a age. When this occurs, the consumer may learn less from
message that uses a literal similarity. the literal similarity than from the analogy about what kinds
of tasks the PDA can actually help them to accomplish. As
THE PERSUASIVENESS OF AN ANALOGY a result, a literal similarity is likely to be less persuasive
than an analogy.
The extant literature offers a starting point for analyzing Although an analogy can be a highly persuasive device,
the persuasiveness of an analogy. Research reported by there are impediments to processing an analogy that might
McQuarrie and Mick (1999) suggests that comparisons such compromise its impact. Finding common structural relations
as an analogy and a literal similarity prompt elaboration of between a base and target in the absence of attribute com-
message information, that is, the association of message monalities can be challenging. For instance, it might be
content with information that is already in memory. The difficult to map the shared data-transmission function be-
distinction made in the psychological literature between an tween a base cellular phone and a target PDA because sur-
analogy and a literal similarity (Gentner and Markman 1997) face attributes such as a keyboard and a dialing pad do not
extends this observation by suggesting that elaboration may correspond. In this event, what one might learn about the
take different forms, depending on the type of comparison PDA from the comparison with a cellular phone may be
made. somewhat obscure and difficult to discover.
Analogies allow only structural relations to be mapped The difficulty in finding structural relations without as-
from a base to a target. Thus, it seems likely that when sociated attributes is highlighted by considering an alter-
processing an analogy, elaboration would be devoted pri- native case where common surface features would help to
marily to structural relations of the target and not to surface make common structural relations salient. For instance, in
attributes. In contrast, a literal similarity allows mapping of processing a literal similarity between a computer and a
both structural relations and surface attributes. When pro- PDA, the presence of keyboards on both items may offer a
cessing a literal similarity, elaboration is likely to be allo- clue that data-storage functions are provided by both
cated to both surface attributes and structural relations. For products.
a fixed level of cognitive resources, the elaboration of sur- This analysis suggests that two factors are likely to mod-
face attributes prompted by a literal similarity would come erate the persuasive impact of an analogy. One factor is the
at the expense of the elaboration of structural relations. ability to detect and map common relations in the absence
These observations suggest that the mapping of analogies of surface attributes. The other moderator is the resources
may generate more elaboration of structural relations than available for processing an analogy. Substantial cognitive
does the mapping of literal similarities. Further, such a focus resources are needed for this task. It has been found that
should be advantageous because structural relations are comprehending the relations between a base and target takes
thought to be more informative about what benefits a product more time (Goldstone and Medin 1994) and a greater num-
offers than are attributes (Gregan-Paxton and Roedder John ber of stimulus repetitions (Markman and Gentner 1993)
1997). Support for this contention is found in studies show- than does comprehending attributes. Apparently, in the ab-
ing that comparisons involving shared relations rather than sence of attribute cues, mapping the relations becomes more
shared attributes produce inferences that are generally per- effortful (Bassok 1990, p. 532).
ceived to be more sound (Gentner, Rattermann, and Forbus A two-factor theory thus guides the present research.
1993) and more goal-relevant (Read 1984). The implication When a person is capable of detecting common structural
is that analogies, with their emphasis on structural relations, relations in the absence of common attributes, and when
should prompt more favorable elaboration about the advo- substantial cognitive resources are allocated to the pro-
cated product than a literal similarity with its divided focus cessing task, analogies will be highly persuasive and have
on attributes and relations. Because greater elaboration of greater impact than other comparison types such as literal
favorable information generally results in greater message similarities. When either of these conditions is absent, the
persuasion (e.g., Petty and Cacioppo 1981), our prediction persuasive power of an analogy may be undermined. In such
is that an analogy should stimulate greater persuasion than cases, less demanding comparisons such as literal similar-
a literal similarity. ities may be more persuasive than analogies. Each of these
To clarify why an analogy might have greater persuasive factors is considered in greater detail.
power than a literal similarity, consider a message in which
an analogy compares a cellular phone to a PDA. The analogy
highlights common structural relations between base and
The Moderating Effect of Expertise
target, thereby focusing attention on the PDA’s utilities such Because analogies do not provide attribute cues regarding
as wireless communication. In contrast, a literal similarity the structural relations that are appropriate to map from a
comparing a computer and PDA would not only direct at- base to a target, factors that facilitate the detection of these
tention to structural relations but also might call attention relations should enhance the comprehension of an analogy.

This content downloaded from 155.97.178.73 on Fri, 28 Nov 2014 01:05:55 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
KNOWLEDGE, RESOURCES, AND ANALOGIES 259

Expertise with the base is one factor that may help to over- persuaded by an analogy than are novices.
come the challenge of mapping the relations presented in
an analogy (Gregan-Paxton and Roedder John 1997). H1b: Although experts are likely to comprehend both an
McQuarrie and Mick (1999) offer support for this contention analogy and a literal similarity, an analogy is likely
with their observation that an analogy was more persuasive to be more persuasive.
than a control message for nationals who were knowledge-
able about base relations but not for foreigners who were A different outcome is anticipated when novices are mes-
not. sage recipients. A literal similarity is less likely than an
The advantage afforded by base expertise may relate to analogy to elude novices, because it would allow them to
differences in base knowledge structures between experts map the attributes that they do know about a base category.
and novices. Experts have base knowledge that is richly Consider, for example, a literal similarity between a com-
endowed with attribute information as well as abstract in- puter and a PDA. In response to this comparison, a novice
formation about structural relations (Alba and Hutchinson may activate surface attributes that make up the bulk of their
1987). Furthermore, experts are likely to use this knowledge computer knowledge. Inspection of the target PDA will re-
in making decisions. As Spence and Brucks (1997) have veal that attributes such as a keyboard, a screen, and the
noted, “experts use more efficient top-down strategies start- like can be mapped from their knowledge of a computer.
ing with known quantities to deduce unknowns, as opposed It is also possible that mapping surface attributes may
to the bottom-up strategies favored by novices” (p. 234). facilitate novices’ relational mapping between the base and
When experts’ knowledge is activated in response to an target (Bassok 1990; Ross and Kilbane 1997). For example,
analogy, available attributes and/or structural relations mapping a keyboard feature from a computer to a PDA may
should become accessible, and the expert may thus be focus attention on this attribute. This might stimulate a nov-
prompted to search for these attributes and relations in the ice to consider the role it may play for the computer and
target. Inspection of the target will indicate that only rela- realize that there is a data-input relation between a keyboard
tions are to be mapped onto the target, and this mapping and memory in a computer. If the attribute mapping prompts
can then be completed. Thus, using extensive base knowl- the generation of a structural relation for the base, the re-
edge to guide processing may help experts to recognize lation can then be mapped to the target.
common structural relations in the absence of surface at- In sum, novices are likely to be able to comprehend and
tribute cues. make judgments on the basis of literal similarities. This
Novices with regard to a base are likely to have knowl- comprehension might be based on attribute similarities or
edge structures that may include some attribute information on the use of attributes to cue recognition of abstract rela-
but relatively little abstract information about underlying tions between base and target. In either case, a literal sim-
concepts (Alba and Hutchinson 1987). Novices therefore ilarity should allow novices to learn more about the target
tend to rely on surface characteristics of the stimulus in than they can derive from an analogy. Novices thus should
making a judgment (Chi, Feltovich, and Glaser 1981). This be more persuaded by a literal similarity than by an analogy.
description suggests that novices may find analogies difficult H1c: Novices will be more likely to comprehend and to
to process. Lacking relational base information, they may be persuaded by a literal similarity than by an
not seek the corresponding structural relations in the target. analogy.
Furthermore, examination of the target would indicate that
the attributes they do know about the base are not appro-
priate to map to the target. As a result, novices may learn The Moderating Effect of Resources
little from analogies.
Evidence reported by Novick (1988) is consistent with The predictions made to this point assume that adequate
these expectations. Her data indicate that experts can process resources are available to complete the mapping tasks re-
an analogy, whereas novices have limited ability to do so. quired to process an analogy. When substantial resources
In particular, when faced with base attributes that do not are not allocated to processing, even experts may have dif-
correspond to those in the target, novices are less likely than ficulty in comprehending an analogy. This assertion follows
experts to map relations from a base to a target (experiment from the earlier observation that mapping relations in the
1). absence of attribute cues can be relatively effortful. It also
The view emerging from this analysis is that base ex- implies a qualification for our earlier prediction regarding
pertise moderates the persuasive impact of an analogy. An experts’ processing of analogies. Specifically, for experts,
analogy is likely to be more persuasive than a literal sim- analogies should be highly persuasive when substantial re-
ilarity when a message recipient is an expert in the base sources are available, but not when resources are limited.
domain, but not when the recipient is a novice. A novice H2a: Experts are more likely to comprehend an analogy
may be less capable of mapping structural relations and may and to be persuaded by it when resources are sub-
thus learn relatively little from an analogy. These obser- stantial than when resources are limited.
vations suggest the following two hypotheses.
When resources are more limited, comparisons that ac-
H1a: Experts are more likely to comprehend and to be commodate this resource limitation better than an analogy

This content downloaded from 155.97.178.73 on Fri, 28 Nov 2014 01:05:55 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
260 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

are likely to be more persuasive. For example, if a com- their ability to comprehend structural relations, but also on
parison involves a literal similarity rather than an analogy, the presence of substantial resources to do so.
both attributes and structural relations would be common to
the base and the target. The shared attributes could provide H3: Novices are more likely to comprehend and to be
cues about the structural relations that are appropriate to persuaded by an analogy when they are given in-
map and thereby decrease the resources needed to learn structions to focus on the structural relations among
about the target. attributes and allocate substantial resources to the
processing task than when either of these conditions
H2b: Experts with limited resources are more likely to is absent.
comprehend and to be persuaded by a literal sim-
ilarity than by an analogy. Another approach to facilitating novices’ recognition of
base relations is to induce them to be in a positive mood
when they are performing this task (see Isen 1993 for a
Emulating the Effect of Expertise: Training and review). Identifying a relational structure between known
attributes entails generating relations between concepts, such
Positive Mood as dialing pads and mouthpieces, which were previously
The foregoing analysis underscores the utility of expertise viewed as unrelated. Numerous findings suggest that a pos-
in comprehending analogies. This observation raises the is- itive mood fosters such processing. Isen, Daubman, and
sue of how the processing of analogies might be enhanced Nowicki (1987) reported that significantly more respondents
when expertise is lacking. Our earlier discussion of novices were able to solve a problem requiring them to recognize
and experts provides some insights. that an object could serve multiple purposes when they were
Experts were portrayed as having knowledge of structural in a positive mood rather than in a neutral one. Lee and
relations about the base, which facilitates recognition of the Sternthal (1999) observed that those in a positive mood were
same relational structure in the target product. Novices lack more likely than those in a neutral mood to associate brand
this knowledge. For example, a novice with regard to a names with the categories in which they held membership
cellular phone may know that attributes of a cellular phone and to use this categorization as a retrieval cue to enhance
include a dialing pad and a mouthpiece. However, a novice their brand name recall. Along similar lines, investigators
is unlikely to have generated a relation between the two, have noted that individuals in a positive mood are more
such as that the dialing pad directs the transmission of voice likely to relate atypical objects to a particular category than
information spoken into the mouthpiece. If a novice could are their neutral mood counterparts (e.g., Isen and Daubman
be prompted to recognize this relation, it could then be 1984).
mapped onto a PDA, and an analogy between a cellular Applying these observations in the context of an analogy,
phone and a PDA could be understood. it is anticipated that like a focusing instruction, a positive
We consider two ways in which such recognition might mood might facilitate the recognition of relations among
be enhanced. One approach to facilitating novices’ pro- attributes in the base that would otherwise be perceived as
cessing of an analogy is to provide a prompt that focuses unrelated. This recognition, in turn, should facilitate the
their attention on structural relations. Spence and Brucks mapping of structural relations between a base and a target.
(1997) found that giving novices organizational prompts If this analysis is correct, novices will be more persuaded
enhanced the extent to which they responded like experts, by an analogy when they are in a positive mood than when
though this observation was not made in the context of a they are in a neutral mood. However, because such pro-
variation in the type of comparison. Thompson, Gentner, cessing is resource demanding, this outcome is expected to
and Lowenstein (2000) reported that research participants occur only when substantial resources are devoted to the
were more likely to transfer knowledge from a base to a processing task.
target in a negotiation context when they were given in-
structions that focused them on underlying principles rather H4: Novices are more likely to comprehend and to be
than on specific message content. However, whether novices persuaded by an analogy when they are in a positive
would benefit particularly from such prompts was not mood and when they allocate substantial resources
examined. to the processing task than when either of these con-
In the present research, we assess this possibility by giving ditions is absent.
novices instructions that either focus their attention on the
structural relations among attributes of the base object or
on other message details. The expectation is that directing Testing the Effects of Analogies
novices’ attention to structural relations in the base will
facilitate their recognition of base relations like wireless The present research is based on the notion that the pro-
communication. These relations can then be mapped to the cessing and persuasive impact of an analogy depend on two
target, and the novice can learn about the target from the factors. One is message recipients’ ability to map abstract
analogy. However, as predicted for experts, novices’ pro- relations between a base and a target. Variations in expertise,
cessing of an analogy should depend not only on enhancing instructions, and mood may moderate this ability. The other

This content downloaded from 155.97.178.73 on Fri, 28 Nov 2014 01:05:55 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
KNOWLEDGE, RESOURCES, AND ANALOGIES 261

factor is the availability of the resources required for the by it than would novices. It was also anticipated that experts
processing task. would be more persuaded by the analogy than by the literal
Four experiments were conducted to test this theorizing. similarity because the analogy was expected to prompt
Experiment 1 examines the ability to process structural re- greater elaboration of the structural relations present in the
lations as a determinant of an analogy’s impact. This occurs target. Finally, novices were predicted to be more persuaded
in a context where respondents allocate significant resources by a literal similarity than by an analogy. This follows from
to the processing task. Experts are expected to be more the assumption that novices would be more capable of map-
persuaded by an analogy than by a literal similarity, but the ping attributes than structural relations from a base to a
reverse is anticipated for novices (Hypotheses 1a–c). Ex- target.
periment 2 focuses on the importance of resource allocation
to the processing of an analogy. This entails investigating
the impact of varying resource allocation on experts’ eval-
Method
uations of an analogy and a literal similarity. The prediction Respondents. We recruited participants who were
is that when experts allocate substantial resources to mes- likely to be highly motivated to process the information
sage processing, they will be more persuaded by an analogy presented. Cacioppo, Petty, and Kao’s (1984) 18-item Need
than when resource allocation is limited. In the latter case, for Cognition (NFC) scale was administered to screen re-
an analogy will be less persuasive than a literal similarity spondents. Because NFC is thought to reflect an individual’s
(Hypotheses 2a, b). Finally, in experiments 3 and 4, we predisposition toward effortful and elaborative processing,
assess whether novices’ processing of an analogy and its only those with relatively high scores on the NFC scale
persuasive impact are enhanced by the presence of sub- were invited to participate in the experiment. Fifty-seven
stantial resources and the introduction of devices such as individuals took part in the study. The average NFC for this
instructions and positive mood to enhance their ability to group was relatively high (M p 2.60 on a scale ranging
process structural relations (Hypotheses 3, 4). from ⫺4 to ⫹4).
In all experiments, two types of measures are of critical
interest. One is an indicator of comprehension. For this, we Stimuli. Two print ads were developed for fictitious new
examine base association measures for the target product. software products. Both used Quicken financial management
These measures are intended to assess whether respondents software as a base for the comparison message. One ad
report associations to the target that are consistent with the presented an analogy between Quicken and “NutriWatch
base and thus reflect a comprehension of a relationship be- nutritional management software.” The other offered a literal
tween base and target. The other indicator is respondents’ similarity between Quicken and “MoneyWatch financial
evaluation of the target product. A series of bipolar adjective management software.”
items were used to assess responses on this criterion Quicken literature was examined to generate a list of its
measure. attributes and structural relations, such as that Quicken has
expense category labels (attribute) that are input into a sys-
tem for reporting expenditures on groceries, gas, and en-
EXPERIMENT 1: THE EFFECT OF tertainment (a structural relation among the category label
EXPERTISE ON PROCESSING AN attributes). The analogy ad for NutriWatch was developed
ANALOGY by maintaining the structural relations but changing the as-
sociated attributes. For example, the ability to track trans-
In experiment 1, we examined the processing of an anal- actions by category was maintained as a relation, but the
ogy by experts and novices in the base domain of high-tech category labels involved were related to food groups instead
products. We chose a high-tech context because we antic- of financial accounts. In the literal similarity ad for
ipated that research participants could be readily recruited MoneyWatch, both Quicken’s attributes and structural re-
who would differ in their knowledge of this domain. Indi- lations were maintained (see App. A).
viduals who were likely to allocate substantial resources to
the processing task were selected for study so that the ab- Procedure. After reading one of the comparison mes-
sence of resources would not account for the outcomes sages, respondents were asked to complete several measures.
observed. First, they indicated their evaluations of the target brand on
Of central interest in this study was the role of the ability a series of 10 semantic differential type items. These items
to map structural relations in determining the impact of an include like/dislike, useful/not useful, high-/not high-tech,
analogy. We measured experts’ and novices’ reactions to an good/bad, high/low quality, practical/impractical, worth/not
ad that presented a novel product via an analogy. As a worth owning, impressive/unimpressive, valuable/not val-
contrast condition, some respondents were given a message uable, and advanced/not advanced.
featuring a literal similarity rather than an analogy. We an- Message recipients next were asked to complete a base
ticipated that experts would be more capable than novices associations measure for the target software in the ad. The
of mapping structural relations between base and target. If purpose of this measure was to assess the extent to which
this assumption were correct, experts would exhibit greater associations that might be linked to the base (Quicken) were
comprehension of an analogy and would be more persuaded being applied to the target product. The assumption is that

This content downloaded from 155.97.178.73 on Fri, 28 Nov 2014 01:05:55 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
262 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

the greater the number of base associations that respondents FIGURE 1


attached to the target, the greater is the comprehension of
EXPERIMENT 1 MEANS BY TREATMENT
the comparison message in the ad.
Research participants were given six features that the ad-
vertised product might provide but that were not specifically
mentioned in the ad. They were asked to circle the two
features that seemed most likely to be offered by the ad-
vertised product. Three items on the list were characteristics
that a pretest had indicated were often associated with the
Quicken base product: generates reports, tracks gains, and
provides management guidelines. In the analogy condition,
these features were described in nutritional terms, such as
“generates reports on the vitamin values in the foods you
eat,” because the target software was NutriWatch. In the
literal similarity condition, where the target was financial
software, the features were described in financial terms. An
example is, “generates reports on interest paid to credit cards
last year.” The other three items on the list were generic
software features that might be associated with any software
product (e.g., has an on-line help function for troubleshoot-
ing) and that a pretest had indicated were not specifically
associated with Quicken.
Because experts were likely to comprehend the analogy
message more fully than novices, experts were expected to
circle more Quicken-related features than were novices in
the analogy condition. Further, because experts were capable
of processing both the analogy and the literal similarity, they
were expected to circle a similarly large number of Quicken- tern for these groups as was found on the factor analyses
related features in both the analogy and literal similarity for the entire sample. The scale created by combining the
conditions. Novices were expected to circle fewer Quicken- 10 evaluation items is reliable (a p .93 ). These items were
associated features in the analogy condition than the literal averaged to form an evaluation score for each respondent.
similarity condition if their comprehension of the analogy An ANOVA was performed to assess the effects of mes-
were poorer than their understanding of the literal similarity. sage recipients’ expertise (expert, novice) and the type of
Finally, research participants answered five questions de- comparison presented in the advertising message they read
signed to measure their knowledge about the Quicken base. (analogy, literal similarity) on their evaluations of the target
The questions pertained to how often they used Quicken, product. This analysis indicates the presence of the signif-
how familiar they were with it, and how expert they felt icant interaction between these factors depicted in Figure 1
they were with it. Answers to these questions were used to (F(1, 53) p 41.29, p ! .01). Means classified by treatments
categorize respondents as experts or novices. appear in Table 1. Consistent with Hypothesis 1a, simple
effects tests reveal that this outcome obtained because ex-
Results and Discussion perts were more favorable toward the advertised software
than were novices when an analogy was used to describe
Measurement. The five items designed to assess the product (F(1, 53) p 14.60, p ! .01). Further, in accor-
knowledge about Quicken loaded on a single factor that is dance with Hypothesis 1b, experts exhibited more favorable
reliable (a p .89). An expertise scale was constructed by evaluations when the message included an analogy rather
summing each respondent’s score on the five items. A me- than literal similarity (F(1, 53) p 20.31, p ! .01). Finally,
dian split was used to classify respondents as experts or as predicted by Hypothesis 1c, novices were less favorable
novices. Findings indicate that experts (M p 4.72) had when the product was described by an analogy than when
greater knowledge of Quicken than did novices (M p it was described by a literal similarity (F(1, 53) p 20.99,
2.36, F(1, 55) p 128.74, p ! .01). p ! .01).2
Evaluations. An exploratory factor analysis was con- Base Associations. The number of Quicken-associated
ducted on the 10 items indicating respondents’ evaluations features chosen by respondents provides an indication of the
of the new software. Results indicated that the items loaded extent to which the ad messages were comprehended. The
on a single factor, and a confirmatory factor analysis was results indicate the presence of a significant interaction be-
consistent with this interpretation. These analyses were re-
peated within the expert segment of our sample and within 2
These findings were also replicated using a different context. Requests
the novice segment. Results indicated the same loading pat- for these data can be made from the senior author.

This content downloaded from 155.97.178.73 on Fri, 28 Nov 2014 01:05:55 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
KNOWLEDGE, RESOURCES, AND ANALOGIES 263

TABLE 1 to map structural relations from base to target. This is sup-


EXPERIMENTS 1–4 TREATMENT MEANS
ported by evidence indicating that experts applied more
Quicken associations to the target than did novices. More-
over, although base association data suggest that experts
Dependent variable
Study and were able to comprehend both the analogy and the literal
treatment Evaluationsa Base associationsb similarity messages, experts found the analogy to be more
persuasive. We explain this outcome by noting that a map-
Experiment 1:c
Literal similarity:
ping of structural relations frequently offers more compel-
Novices 4.53 1.13 ling insights about a target than does a mapping of attributes.
Experts 2.52 1.23 Novices were more persuaded by a literal similarity than
Analogy: an analogy. Our view is that this outcome occurred because
Novices 2.81 .57 novices did not map structural relations from base to target
Experts 4.25 1.47
Experiment 2:d when processing the analogy, but they were able to use
Literal similarity: attribute commonalities to understand the literal similarity
Low NFC 6.03 3.71 ad. This assertion is supported by the greater tendency to
High NFC 5.08 4.13 apply Quicken-associated features to the target after expo-
Analogy:
Low NFC 4.69 1.57
sure to the literal similarity rather than the analogy.
High NFC 6.12 2.50 More broadly, the findings of experiment 1 suggest that
Experiment 3:e an analogy is a highly effective persuasive device when
No training: message recipients have the expertise to map relations. This
Low NFC 2.88 .75 outcome was observed for respondents who were likely to
High NFC 2.99 .64
Training: devote substantial resources to the processing task. However,
Low NFC 2.82 .47 because only high-NFC respondents served as research par-
High NFC 4.95 1.47 ticipants in experiment 1, the role of resources in processing
Experiment 4:e an analogy remains to be explored.
Neutral:
Low NFC 4.92 .50
High NFC 4.79 .33 EXPERIMENT 2: THE EFFECT OF
Story:
Low NFC 4.54 .67 RESOURCES ON EXPERTS’ PROCESSING
High NFC 6.32 1.86
Music: In experiment 2, experts’ responses to an analogy and a
Low NFC 4.66 .50 literal similarity were examined when the resources allo-
High NFC 5.85 1.83 cated to message processing were either substantial or lim-
a

b
Possible response range for all evaluations was 1–7. ited. It was anticipated that when NFC was high, experts
Possible response range for experiments 1, 3, and 4 base associations was
0–2. Possible response range for experiment 2 base associations was 0–5.
would be more persuaded by an analogy than by a literal
c
Sample was composed of high NFCs. similarity, replicating the result found in experiment 1 (Hy-
d
Sample was composed of high-tech experts. pothesis 1b) and suggesting that the availability of substan-
e
Sample was composed of high-tech novices.
tial resources affected the outcomes reported in the previous
experiment. In addition, experts were expected to better
tween expertise and type of comparison (F(1, 53) p 4.40, comprehend and be more persuaded by an analogy when
p ! .04). Simple effects tests reveal that, in accordance with NFC was high rather than low (Hypothesis 2a). For low-
Hypothesis 1a, experts chose more Quicken-related features NFC experts, however, the analogy was likely to be less
than did novices (F(1, 53) p 11.31, p ! .01) in the analogy well understood and less persuasive than the literal similarity
condition, indicating that experts had greater comprehension (Hypothesis 2b). These predictions were tested in a context
of the analogy. Further, experts were equally likely to choose that differed from experiment 1 so as to assess the robustness
Quicken-associated features in the analogy and literal sim- of our findings.
ilarity conditions (F ! 1). Consistent with Hypothesis 1b,
these findings suggest that experts comprehend the com- Method
parison between Quicken and the advertised software,
whether this comparison involved an analogy or a literal Stimuli. Two new print ads were developed for a fic-
similarity. Novices selected fewer Quicken-associated fea- titious PDA product called the Voyager. Both the analogy
tures in the analogy condition than in the literal similarity and literal similarity versions are presented in Appendix B.
condition (F(1, 53) p 4.46, p ! .04), implying that novices In the analogy treatment, the ad began by comparing the
comprehended less from the analogy than from the literal Voyager to a cellular phone and specifying Ameritech as its
similarity (Hypothesis 1c). producer. A structural relation associated with cellular
The results of experiment 1 are consistent with expec- phones was then attributed to the Voyager by asserting that
tations. Experts were more persuaded by the analogy than it had “wireless technology that lets you record and send
were novices, presumably because of experts’ greater ability voice messages at your convenience.”

This content downloaded from 155.97.178.73 on Fri, 28 Nov 2014 01:05:55 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
264 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

In the literal similarity treatment, Voyager was introduced cellular phone associations a respondent would have applied
by comparing it to a computer and indicating that it was to the PDA, and thus the greater the number of phone items
made by IBM. A list of computer-like characteristics of the that would be labeled “true.” Similarly, the more the literal
Voyager was then provided. Attributes included a high-res- similarity was understood, the greater the number of com-
olution screen, keyboard, calendar and address book, spread- puter items that would be marked “true.”
sheet software, RAM, a fax/data modem, a peripheral port We expected that high-NFC experts would have better
for a printer or external keyboard hookup, and a variety of comprehension of the analogy than would low-NFC experts.
preinstalled on-line services. A computer-like relation was Therefore, in the analogy condition, we expected more
also presented. It indicated that the “keyboard allows you “true” responses to the cellular phone items among high
to input written messages that Voyager can transmit to any NFCs than low NFCs. Further, low-NFC experts were ex-
Internet address.” pected to be less able to comprehend the analogy than the
literal similarity. Hence, for these respondents we anticipated
Procedure. Twenty-nine people who had substantial fewer true responses to the cellular phone items in the anal-
expertise with regard to technological innovations partici- ogy condition than to the computer items in the literal sim-
pated in the experiment in small groups. As compensation ilarity condition.
for their time, participants were entered into a lottery for
five cash prizes ranging from $50 to $250.
The study was represented as an opportunity to offer re- Results
actions to advertising copy for a new product. Respondents Measurement. The NFC scale items were subjected to
read a print ad that presented the fictitious Voyager PDA. a factor analysis, which indicated that responses to the items
The message included either an analogy to a cellular phone loaded on a single factor. The scale is reliable (a p .90).
or a literal similarity with a computer. After reading the ad Each participant’s responses were combined and averaged
copy, respondents reported their evaluations of the Voyager to form an overall NFC score. A median split was used to
on 13 seven-point semantic differential items: like/dislike, assign respondents to high (M p 2.96) and low NFC
useful/not useful, low-/high-tech, good/bad, dependable/not (M p 1.09). These groups differed significantly in their
dependable, high/low quality, practical/impractical, worth/ NFC score (F(1, 27) p 32.39, p ! .01).
not worth owning, impressive/not impressive, simple/com-
plex, valuable/not valuable, advanced/not advanced, and Evaluations. An exploratory factor analysis was con-
convenient/inconvenient. ducted on the 13 items indicating respondents’ judgments
These measures were followed by two individual-differ- of the new software. Results indicate that the items loaded
ence scales. One was a definition task that was used to verify together on one factor, and a confirmatory factor analysis
our participants’ expertise in the base domain of high-tech was consistent with this interpretation. These analyses were
gadgetry. Eight terms such as “telnet” and “Fortran” were then repeated within the high-NFC segment of our sample
listed, and participants were asked to define as many of the and within the low-NFC segment. Results indicate the same
terms as they could. The overall mean number of correct loading pattern as the factor analyses on the entire sample.
definitions suggests a substantial level of expertise (M p The scale created by combining the 13 evaluation items is
5.34). The other scale was the 18-item version of the NFC reliable (a p .93). Responses on the individual items were
scale. combined to produce a single evaluation measure.
The final measure was respondents’ base associations. An analysis of variance on the evaluation scores reveals
This measure is conceptually similar to the one used in the presence of a two-way interaction between the type of
experiment 1. Again, the intention was to assess the extent comparison presented in the ad and NFC (F(1, 25) p
to which associations that might be linked to the base were 14.05, p ! .01). Means categorized by treatments appear in
being applied to the target product and thereby infer the Table 1. High-NFC respondents’ evaluations of the Voyager
degree to which the comparison was understood. were more favorable when they were exposed to an analogy
Participants were asked to respond true or false to 15 than to a literal similarity (F(1, 25) p 5.21 , p ! .03). This
statements about the Voyager. Of these 15 statements, five outcome replicates the finding in experiment 1 and thus
directly contradicted the information presented in the Voy- offers testimony for the claim that respondents had signif-
ager message and thus were expected to be easily identified icant expertise with regard to the base. In the analogy con-
as false.3 The remaining 10 statements concerned charac- dition, it was found that high-NFC respondents’ evaluations
teristics that could plausibly be true of the Voyager, although of the Voyager were more favorable than those of low-NFC
they were not specified in the message. Five of these char- respondents (F(1, 25) p 9.71, p ! .01), which is consistent
acteristics represented typical cellular phone properties, and with Hypothesis 2a. Finally, as predicted by Hypothesis 2b,
five represented typical computer properties. We reasoned the evaluations of low-NFC respondents were less favorable
that the greater the comprehension of the analogy, the more when the ad contained an analogy than when it featured a
literal similarity (F(1, 25) p 9.22, p ! .01).
3
Examination of the answers to the five false statements indicates the
absence of significant treatment effects (F’s ! 1.10). These items are not Base Associations. A base association score was cre-
discussed further. ated for each respondent by counting the number of phone-

This content downloaded from 155.97.178.73 on Fri, 28 Nov 2014 01:05:55 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
KNOWLEDGE, RESOURCES, AND ANALOGIES 265

associated items endorsed in the analogy condition, in which structural relations. The emphasis on strategies for enhanc-
a cellular phone provided the base, and the number of com- ing the mapping of structural relations rather than on stim-
puter-associated items endorsed in the literal similarity con- ulating resource allocation is warranted because vehicles for
dition, in which a computer served as the base. An ANOVA enhancing resource availability, such as message repetition
on this measure reveals main effects for message condition and issue involvement, are well established, whereas it is
(F(1, 25) p 42.38, p ! .01) and NFC (F(1, 25) p 6.18, less apparent how novices’ ability to map structural relations
p ! .02). As expected, in the analogy condition (Hypothesis might be improved.
2a), high-NFC respondents exhibited higher base association
feature scores than did low-NFC respondents (F(1, 25) p
4.55, p ! .02). Also as predicted (Hypothesis 2b), experts EXPERIMENT 3: USING TRAINING TO
with low NFC exhibited lower base association scores for ENHANCE NOVICES’ ABILITY TO
the analogy condition than for the literal similarity condition PROCESS ANALOGIES
(F(1, 25) p 26.27, p ! .01).
The results of experiment 1 suggest that novices had dif-
ficulty in comprehending the implications of the analogy
Discussion and thus were not persuaded by it. If lacking the ability to
The results of experiment 2 offer evidence for the mod- map relations from base to target underlies novices’ diffi-
erating effect of resources in experts’ responses to an anal- culty with analogies, a device such as training, which
ogy. It appears that processing an analogy is a resource- prompts them to recognize structural relations in the base,
demanding task even for highly knowledgeable people. might help them to map relations to the target and com-
Thus, when the Voyager PDA was advertised via an analogy, prehend the analogy. In experiment 3, we investigate the
high-NFC experts evaluated it more favorably than did those effectiveness of training in this regard. Our expectation is
low in NFC. Further, the base associations measure suggests that training will result in more favorable evaluations of
that these different evaluative responses were related to dif- target products presented to novices via analogies.
ferences in comprehension of the analogy. High-NFC ex- We adapted our training intervention from an instructional
perts also exhibited greater comprehension of the analogy approach that was used by Thompson et al. (2000). These
than did low-NFC experts. investigators asked research participants to read two cases.
When the resources allocated to processing were limited, For some respondents, the goal was to give advice to the
experts were more persuaded by a literal similarity than by protagonist in each case. For other respondents, the goal
an analogy. This outcome appears to be attributable to dif- was to derive a principle that was represented by the two
ferences in comprehension. The base association measure cases. Despite the fact that both groups processed the same
suggests that low-NFC experts had better comprehension of case information, those who were prompted to derive the
the message presenting a literal similarity than one pre- principle were significantly more likely to adopt it in a sub-
senting an analogy. sequent face-to-face bargaining situation than were those
In sum, the outcomes of experiment 2 indicate that the who offered advice for each case separately.
persuasiveness of an analogy is contingent upon resource We reasoned that similar instructional procedures might
availability and that analogies are more resource demanding help novices to recognize base structural relations that can
to process than are literal similarities. This is because anal- be transferred to a target product to facilitate comprehension
ogies rely on the mapping of structural relations that can be of an analogy. Thus, in experiment 3, novices read the same
difficult to detect and resource demanding to map. That said, NutriWatch ad that was used in experiment 1, but prior to
we do not wish to overstate the generality of this outcome. receiving this message, some were given training about the
Indeed, it should be possible to calibrate an experimental Quicken base product. All participants were exposed to a
task so that a literal similarity is more demanding than an description of Quicken. In the training condition, respon-
analogy. For example, encouraging individuals to focus on dents were asked to read the description and then list the
every element that can be mapped from base to target might kinds of things they felt they could accomplish with
make the demands of a literal similarity greater than those Quicken. As in Thompson et al.’s (2000) derivation con-
of an analogy. This is because a literal similarity permits dition, this question was intended to prompt respondents to
the mapping of both attributes and structural relations, consider the underlying structural relations among
whereas an analogy entails the mapping only of structural Quicken’s attributes. In the no-training condition, respon-
relations. In most everyday circumstances, however, an anal- dents were asked to identify any typos that appeared in the
ogy is expected to be more resource demanding than a literal Quicken description. Here, as in Thompson et al.’s advice
similarity. condition, the goal was to have respondents focus on mes-
Taken together, the results of experiments 1 and 2 suggest sage content other than relations among attributes.
that the persuasive impact of an analogy is moderated by Although training has the possibility of enhancing nov-
the ability to map structural relations and by the presence ices’ use of an analogy, the results of experiment 2 suggest
of the substantial resources to accomplish this task. In our that the ability to map structural relations is not sufficient
subsequent experiments, we expand on these findings by to process an analogy. The availability of substantial re-
considering mechanisms that may facilitate the mapping of sources also seems necessary. Thus, in experiment 3 we

This content downloaded from 155.97.178.73 on Fri, 28 Nov 2014 01:05:55 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
266 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

examined the impact of the presence and absence of training NFC respondents (M p 2.55, F(1, 60) p 221.17, p ! .01).
under conditions of low and high NFC. Our prediction was Finally, exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses were
that novices who were high in NFC and received training conducted on the 10 evaluative items, using the full sample,
would be persuaded by an analogy, whereas the persuasive the high-NFC subsample, and the low-NFC subsample.
impact of an analogy would be more limited in the absence These analyses all indicate that the items form a unidimen-
of either of these conditions (Hypothesis 3). sional scale that is reliable (a p .90 ). An evaluation score
Support for this prediction would provide further evidence was developed by averaging each respondent’s score on the
that the ability to map structural relations and the availability 10 items.
of substantial resources help a person to comprehend an
analogy and use it in making a judgment. Further, such
results would indicate that training can compensate for a Evaluations. The effect of the treatments on the eval-
lack of expertise in mapping relations and thereby can
uation measure indicates the presence of a significant train-
prompt novices to act in the same manner as we had ob-
ing # NFC interaction (F(1, 58) p 53.84, p ! .01). Means
served for experts.
categorized by treatment appear in Table 1. As predicted by
Hypothesis 3, when NFC was high, those who received
Method training were more persuaded by the analogy than those
Using the same screening questions as were used to detect who were not trained (F(1, 58) p 95.49, p ! .01). When
Quicken expertise in experiment 1, the 62 research partic- NFC was low, the evaluations for training and no training
ipants were identified as individuals who have limited did not differ (F ! 1). Moreover, those who received training
knowledge about Quicken. When these novices arrived at were more persuaded when NFC was high than when it was
the laboratory, they read an ad that described the types of low (F(1, 58) p 123.79, p ! .01).
tasks that Quicken is helpful in carrying out. At the end of
this description, respondents were asked some questions. In
the training condition, the question was, “If you owned Base Associations. The number of Quicken-associated
Quicken software, what kinds of things would you do with features that were circled provides an indication of the extent
it?” Respondents recorded their answers on a lined sheet of to which base associations were applied to the target and
paper. In the no-training condition, the question was, “Were thus the degree to which the analogy was comprehended.
there any typos in the ad? Please go back and circle any The results indicate the presence of a significant interaction
that you identify, so that they can be corrected before the between training and NFC (F(1, 58) p 12.27, p ! .01).
ad runs.” There were three typos in the ad. Consistent with Hypothesis 3, simple effects tests reveal that
When respondents had finished this task, they were asked when NFC was high, more Quicken-associated items were
to read the ad for NutriWatch and answer the same 10 ev- circled by those who received training than by those who
aluative items as were used in experiment 1. This served as did not (F(1, 58) p 12.85, p ! .01). When NFC was low,
the criterion measure. Research participants also were asked there was no difference in the number of Quicken-associated
to complete the same base associations measure as was used items circled by those in the training condition and those
in experiment 1. This involved selecting two of the six fea- in the no-training condition (F(1, 58) p 1.68, p 1 .20). Fi-
tures presented that they would expect the NutriWatch soft- nally, those who were trained circled more Quicken-asso-
ware to possess. Choice of Quicken-associated items would
ciated items when NFC was high than when it was low
offer evidence that they comprehended the analogy. Finally,
(F(1, 58) p 20.67, p ! .01).
individuals completed the 18-item NFC inventory, with re-
sponses ranging from 1 to 7.

Results and Discussion Mediation Analysis. The pattern of responses to the


base associations measure thus resembles the pattern ob-
Measurement. Responses on the five-item Quicken served for evaluations. This parallel is consistent with the
knowledge measure indicate that research participants had notion that increases in comprehension of the analogy led
limited knowledge of this software (M p 2.55). This level to more favorable attitudes among high-NFC novices who
of performance is the same as that for novices in experiment received training.
1 (M p 2.36, F(1, 89) p 1.01, p 1 .32). It is also signifi- A mediation analysis was conducted to examine this ex-
cantly weaker than the performance of experts in that study planation, following the approach recommended by Baron
(M p 4.72, F(1, 88) p 109.29, p ! .01). These data suggest and Kenny (1986). These analyses indicate that training and
that the respondents were novices with regard to the Quicken NFC significantly predict base associations (F(1, 58) p
base used in the NutriWatch analogy ad. 7.76, p ! .01) and evaluations (F(1, 58) p 31.00, p ! .01).
Respondents’ scores on the NFC scale were computed. In addition, base associations significantly affect evaluations
They were categorized as high or low in NFC on the basis (F(1, 57) p 29.27, p ! .01), such that the effect of training
of a median split. High-NFC respondents produced scores and NFC on evaluations is reduced (F(1, 57) p 20.42,
(M p 4.97) that were significantly higher than those of low- p ! .01). Thus, the results support the idea that differences

This content downloaded from 155.97.178.73 on Fri, 28 Nov 2014 01:05:55 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
KNOWLEDGE, RESOURCES, AND ANALOGIES 267

in comprehension mediated differences in evaluations for were introduced in the time between experiment 2 and the
novices processing the analogy message.4 present study.
In sum, experiment 3 offers evidence that there are cir- At the outset of the study, those in the neutral mood
cumstances under which novices can process and are per- control condition were exposed to the message for Voyager
suaded by an analogy. This outcome emerged when novices without any prior mood intervention. For the remaining par-
allocated substantial resources to the processing task and ticipants, a positive mood was introduced in one of two
training led these resources to be directed toward an as- ways. Some respondents were asked to create an autobio-
sessment of the structural relations among attributes. In the graphical story (hereafter referred to as the story condition).
absence of either of these conditions, novices were less able This entailed asking respondents to “recall an episode in
to comprehend the analogy and thus were less persuaded your life that made you feel very happy and continues to
by it. Thus, experiment 3 provides additional evidence that make you happy whenever you think about it even today.”
the persuasive impact of an analogy requires the ability to This induction was selected because it has successfully
map relations and substantial resources to do so. prompted a positive mood in previous research (Lambert et
al. 1997).
Positive mood was also induced using music. Respon-
EXPERIMENT 4: USING POSITIVE MOOD dents in the music condition listened to about one minute
TO ENHANCE NOVICES’ ABILITY TO of upbeat music at the outset of the experiment. Music was
PROCESS ANALOGIES chosen as a second means of inducing a positive mood
because it “has long been considered an efficient and ef-
Experiment 4 tested the robustness of the finding reported fective means for triggering moods” (Bruner 1990, p. 94)
in experiment 3 by investigating an alternative mechanism and has been shown to induce a positive mood (Gorn, Gold-
for helping novices to process an analogy. Rather than in- berg, and Basu 1993). More specifically, a positive mood
troducing training to prompt novices to focus their resources has been found to correlate with music that is played in a
on the structural relations among attributes, a positive mood major mode (Alpert and Alpert 1990), has consonant har-
was used for this purpose. Mood was selected because it monies (Wedin 1972), has a relatively high register (Rigg
represents an induction that is likely to occur in everyday 1940), and has a brisk tempo (Alpert and Alpert 1990).
contexts. Following these criteria, the first author composed and re-
Positive mood was expected to have an effect on novices’ corded a short piece of disco-style music. An original com-
processing of an analogy that would be similar to that found position was used to minimize the effects of song familiarity
in response to training in experiment 3. Because positive on respondents’ reactions. As they listened to the taped mu-
mood has been shown to promote the generation of inter- sic, respondents were asked to consider the kinds of thoughts
concept relations (Isen 1993; Lee and Sternthal 1999), it or feelings that came to mind.
seems reasonable to suggest that it will draw attention to Research participants were given 10 items to help assess
relations that can be mapped to comprehend an analogy. their mood at the time of exposure to the message. These
Thus, we predicted that when novices had ample resources
items included happy, excited, positive, pleased, calm, alert,
and were in a positive mood, they would better comprehend
tired, stimulated, gloomy, and irritated. End points for each
and be more persuaded by an analogy than when either of
of these seven-point items were labeled “not at all” and
these conditions was absent (Hypothesis 4).
“extremely.” For participants in the story and music treat-
ments, these items were encountered immediately after the
Method mood induction, following the procedures frequently used
in previous mood studies (e.g., Wegener, Petty, and Smith
Thirty-nine respondents participated in experiment 4. 1995). For control respondents, the initial task was to com-
They were asked to read advertising copy for a new product plete the mood manipulation checks.
that they would be asked to evaluate. The product was de- At this point, all respondents read the information de-
scribed in a message that was similar to the Voyager analogy scribing the Voyager and were asked to evaluate this product
ad used in experiment 2. However, the list of product fea- on 10 attitudinal items that had been used in the previous
tures was modified to make the Voyager seem contemporary experiments. Of these, eight were subsequently found to load
with Palm Pilot and other hand-held electronic products that on a single factor. These items included like/dislike, useful/
not useful, good/bad, impressive/not impressive, worth/not
4
Mediation analyses were not reported for experiments 1 and 2, because worth owning, practical/impractical, valuable/not valuable,
they would have been inappropriate. Although comprehension is expected
to be a mediator for novices and individuals with low NFC, it is not
and advanced/not advanced.
purported to mediate differences in judgments for experts with high NFC. Respondents next completed a base associations measure.
High-NFC experts are anticipated to comprehend both analogy and literal As in the prior experiments, the base associations were in-
similarity but to be more persuaded by an analogy because the information tended to detect treatment differences in comprehension of
that analogies deliver is more compelling than that of literal similarity. We
thus present mediation analyses only for experiments 3 and 4, where com-
the analogy. This entailed selecting two features that seemed
prehension is theorized to mediate evaluations of all subjects for which most appropriate for the Voyager from a list of six. The six
data are reported. items included three phone-associated features (phone head-

This content downloaded from 155.97.178.73 on Fri, 28 Nov 2014 01:05:55 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
268 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

set, local calling plan, calling card) and three computer- .90). Means categorized by the independent variables are
associated features (printer, America Online trial member- reported in Table 1.
ship, external keyboard). If respondents comprehended the We conducted ANOVAs to examine the effects of NFC
analogy, they were expected to select more phone associ- and mood on novices’ attitudinal evaluations. When the
ations than if they did not. story intervention is used to operationalize mood, a signif-
Two individual-difference measures were then adminis- icant two-way interaction is found (F(1, 21) p 21.81, p !
tered. One was the 18-item NFC scale, which was used to .01). Tests of simple effects reveal that this outcome occurs
assess the likely level of resource allocation to the processing because a positive mood prompted more favorable evalu-
task. Responses were given on scales with values ranging ations than a neutral mood when NFC was high
from ⫺4 to ⫹4. Respondents also completed the same def- (F(1, 21) p 29.22, p ! .01). When NFC was low, evalua-
inition task used in experiment 2 to assess knowledge about tions were relatively unfavorable and not affected by mood
high-tech products. In the present study, the word list was (F(1, 21) p 1.63, p 1 .22). Finally, those in a positive mood
updated with new terms such as “Perl” and “Java” in order were more persuaded when NFC was high rather than when
to enhance discriminative power between relative experts it was low (F(1, 21) p 39.56, p ! .01). These findings are
and novices. consistent with Hypothesis 4.
A similar pattern of outcomes emerged when a positive
Results and Discussion mood was induced by music rather than a story. There is a
significant interaction between NFC and mood
Measurement. Measurement issues were addressed (F(1, 22) p 9.46, p ! .01). A positive mood led to more
first. The 10 items designed to assess mood were factor favorable evaluations than a neutral mood when NFC was
analyzed. Five items (happy, excited, positive, pleased, stim- high (F(1, 22) p 11.44, p ! .01). Evaluations were rela-
ulated) were found to load on one dimension and were there- tively unfavorable and unaffected by mood when NFC was
fore averaged to produce a single mood scale. This scale is low (F ! 1). Respondents in a positive mood were more
reliable (a p .85). The score on this scale was significantly persuaded when NFC was high than when it was low
lower for those in a neutral mood (M p 4.77) than it was (F(1, 22) p 16.61, p ! .01).
for those in a positive mood, regardless of whether the pos-
itive mood induction involved the story (M p 5.74, Base Associations. Particular interest centered on re-
F(1, 23) p 8.10, p ! .01) or the music (M p 5.46, spondents’ propensity to select phone-related features be-
F(1, 24) p 5.13, p ! .03). These outcomes are consistent cause the analogy message compared the Voyager to a cel-
with the view that both interventions were successful in lular phone. The greater the extent to which respondents
varying respondents’ mood. expected Voyager to have phone features, presumably the
The NFC scale is unidimensional. The NFC scores were greater the likelihood that they understood the analogy.
computed by averaging across the 18 items for each re- An interaction between NFC and mood is found when
spondent. This scale is reliable (a p .87). A median split the story was used to manipulate mood (F(1, 21) p 6.44,
was used to classify research participants as either high p ! .02). Simple effects tests reveal that this outcome occurs
(M p 2.92) or low (M p 1.57) in NFC. The differences in part because high-NFC novices chose more phone-as-
between these groups on NFC are significant (F(1, 37) p sociated features when in a positive mood than when in a
61.00, p ! .01). neutral mood (F(1, 21) p 13.81, p ! .01). For low-NFC
Performance on the definition task by those deemed nov- novices, the number of phone-associated features chosen
ices (M p 3.54) supported their classification as such. Their was uniformly low, whether the novices were in the positive
score is significantly lower than that found for experts in or neutral condition (F ! 1). In addition, for respondents in
experiment 2 on a similar measure of knowledge (M p a positive mood, more phone-associated features were se-
5.34, F(1, 66) p 13.69, p ! .01). lected when NFC was high rather than low (F(1, 21) p
The effect of each of the mood inductions was examined 12.97, p ! .01). These findings are consistent with Hypoth-
separately for novices who were high and low in NFC. Thus, esis 4.
the story condition was contrasted with the neutral mood Results are similar when music was used to operationalize
control. In a replicate design, the story was replaced by mood. A significant two-way interaction between NFC and
music as a positive mood induction, and the music treatment mood is found (F(1, 22) p 10.80, p ! .01). This interaction
was contrasted with the neutral mood control condition. This occurs because high-NFC novices had a greater propensity
approach was followed to simplify exposition and because to choose phone features when in a positive mood than when
the story and music are intended to serve the same purpose in a neutral mood (F(1, 22) p 16.33, p ! .01), whereas low-
of prompting the activation of a positive mood. NFC novices did not significantly differ by mood condition
in the number of phone features chosen (F ! 1). Finally,
Evaluations. Participants’ evaluations of Voyager were those in a positive mood chose more phone-associated fea-
examined first. Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses tures when NFC was high rather than when it was low
within the full sample, within the high NFCs, and within (F(1, 22) p 23.16, p ! .01).
the low NFCs indicate that the eight items compose a un-
idimensional evaluation scale. This scale is reliable (a p Mediation Analyses. Mediation analyses were con-

This content downloaded from 155.97.178.73 on Fri, 28 Nov 2014 01:05:55 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
KNOWLEDGE, RESOURCES, AND ANALOGIES 269

ducted to examine the role of comprehension in persuasion lations is to be successful. Consistent with this view, we
(Baron and Kenny 1986). For the story design, it was found found that an analogy was more persuasive for experts when
that mood and NFC significantly predict base associations they devoted substantial resources to its processing than
(F(1, 21) p 5.04, p ! .04) and evaluations (F(1, 21) p when resources were limited. Further, when experts allo-
15.37, p ! .01). In addition, base associations have a sig- cated few resources to message processing, an analogy was
nificant effect on evaluations (F(1, 20) p 19.59, p ! .01), less persuasive than an appeal featuring literal similarity
such that the effect of mood and NFC on evaluations is (experiment 2).
reduced (F(1, 20) p 3.40, p ! .08). Although our evidence suggests that expertise and sub-
Findings were similar for the music design. Mood and stantial resources are sufficient conditions for an analogy to
NFC significantly predict base associations (F(1, 22) p produce superior evaluations, expertise is not necessary for
6.82, p ! .02) and evaluations (F(1, 22) p 4.05, p ! .057). an analogy to be an effective persuasive device. There are
Base associations significantly affect evaluations circumstances in which novices are capable of processing
(F(1, 21) p 26.42, p ! .01) such that the effect of mood an analogy and being persuaded by it. This outcome occurs
and NFC on evaluations becomes insignificant (F ! 1). when devices are presented that prompt recognition of struc-
Thus, the results of both sets of mediation analyses support tural relations among attributes of a base product. Specifi-
the view that comprehension mediates evaluations for nov- cally, training respondents to engage in such processing (ex-
ices processing an analogy message. periment 3) or prompting them to be in a positive mood
The outcomes reported in experiment 4 reinforce the find- while performing the comparison task (experiment 4) are
ings of experiment 3. Both suggest that allocating substantial shown to enhance novices’ ability to process analogies and
resources does not in itself enhance novices’ processing of be persuaded by them, provided that substantial resources
an analogy. Also required is a prompt for novices to deploy are allocated to the task. In effect, these outcomes suggest
their resources in a manner that allows them to recognize that expertise, training instructions, and a positive mood
how base relations can be mapped onto the target. In ex- represent the same construct: all facilitate the mapping of
periment 4, high- and low-NFC novices had similar and structural relations between a base and target.
relatively unfavorable evaluations in the neutral mood con-
dition. In contrast, a positive mood induced by a story or Contributions
by upbeat music facilitated high-NFC respondents’ mapping
of base relations to the target. This apparently enhanced The primary contribution of this research is to identify
their ability to process the analogy and the extent to which factors that affect the processing and persuasive impact of
they were persuaded by it. messages that contain analogies. We theorize that an ability
to map structural relations and the availability of resources
GENERAL DISCUSSION to perform this task moderate the impact of analogies. Al-
though we have focused on literal similarity as an alternative
The present research suggests that analogies can be pow- comparison mechanism to test our view, our theorizing pro-
erful communication devices. They offer the prospect of vides a starting point for examining other types of com-
using existing product knowledge to acquire an in-depth parison or juxtaposition. Thus, future work might further
understanding of a target product’s benefits, particularly compare comprehension and persuasion between analogies
those that might not be apparent otherwise. In the four ex- and, say, mere appearances, metaphors, puns, and the like
periments reported here, it is shown that an analogy can be (Gentner 1983; Gentner and Markman 1997; McQuarrie and
highly persuasive, and experiments 1 and 2 suggest it can Mick 1996, 1999), on the basis of mapping requirements
be more persuasive than an appeal featuring a literal simi- and resource demands.
larity. However, it is also apparent that the effectiveness of The present work also contributes to an understanding of
an analogy is moderated by the message recipient’s ability how novices might become more expert. Spence and Brucks
to map structural relations and the availability of cognitive (1997) found that providing respondents with an aid that
resources to perform the comparison task. helped organize information for making a decision enhanced
A message containing an analogy is better comprehended novices’ ability to operate like experts. When this finding
and is more persuasive when the recipient has expertise with is considered along with our evidence that training and pos-
regard to the base product. Presumably, expertise facilitates itive mood can enhance the processing of an analogy, the
the mapping of structural relations from base to target and emerging view is that any device stimulating the processing
thereby helps one to learn about the target product. Findings of structural relations has the potential to help novices be-
from experiment 1 indicate that experts can more readily come experts.
map structural relations in this manner than can novices. Further, the observations that training and positive mood
However, without expertise—even when substantial re- helped novices suggest that a deficit novices must overcome
sources are devoted—persuasiveness is lower for an analogy in becoming more expert may often relate to a lack of pro-
than for a literal similarity. cedural knowledge rather than simply a lack of semantic
Our data also indicate that expertise is not sufficient to knowledge. That is, novices appear able to comprehend anal-
ensure the persuasiveness of an analogy. Substantial re- ogies when given guidance about how to manipulate attrib-
sources must be allocated if the mapping of structural re- ute knowledge they already hold, whether this is achieved

This content downloaded from 155.97.178.73 on Fri, 28 Nov 2014 01:05:55 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
270 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

through training or the presence of a positive mood while APPENDIX A


processing an analogy. This suggests that helping novices
to focus on how various bits of information are related may STIMULI FOR EXPERIMENT 1
sometimes be a more viable strategy for promoting their
understanding of persuasive messages than adding infor- NEW NUTRIWATCH PERSONAL
mation to their existing and limited knowledge structure. At
the same time, there are likely to be situations in which
NUTRITIONAL MANAGEMENT
novices are so lacking in attribute knowledge that they will SOFTWARE [NEW MONEYWATCH
be unable to process an analogy, even with the prompts we PERSONAL FINANCIAL MANAGEMENT
have suggested. SOFTWARE]5
Finally, the present research adds to the growing literature
documenting the impact of positive mood on message per- NutriWatch is a new nutrition management [MoneyWatch
suasion. A positive mood has been found to promote the is a new financial management] software package that is
like the popular Quicken financial management software
accessibility of associations that relate different individual
package.
objects (Isen et al. 1987; Lee and Sternthal 1999). The pre-
Research shows that many people are bad at managing
sent research suggests that this property extends to the struc-
their personal nutrition [finances], simply because they don’t
tural relations among attributes of a single base object. In
realize what they eat and drink [spend] on a daily basis.
experiment 4, a positive mood appears to have facilitated
NutriWatch [MoneyWatch] remedies this by helping you
the generation of relations between surface attributes of a
keep track of your diet [money].
base product. This in turn seems to have helped respondents
NutriWatch [MoneyWatch] offers an impressive array of
to process an analogy when substantial resources were al-
attributes and benefits, including:
located to the processing task. However, as promising as
positive mood appears to be in facilitating the processing • An electronic register for recording calories taken in and
of persuasive messages, to date, demonstrations of this mood exercised away [recording regular checking account
effect have focused on contexts requiring the generation of transactions].
relations. Whether a positive mood has a similar effect in • A nutrition labeling system for tracking food eaten from
other contexts, say, those that involve learning of unrelated the basic food groups [recurrent expenses such as gro-
details, awaits investigation. ceries, gas, and entertainment].
• Periodic reports of your nutrition intake by food group
category [expenditures in your expense categories].
• Comparisons of eating [spending] habits to stated goals.
• Online access to your doctors’ offices [bank].
Managerial Implications • Online access to a nutrition counseling web site [to your
broker].
Although the use of analogy messages by advertisers ap- • Free technical software support, 24 hours a day.
pears to have become widespread, our findings suggest that • NutriWatch [MoneyWatch] helps you to better manage
caution is warranted in using analogies in advertising. Anal- your physical [financial] health.
ogies are likely to be effective devices when the target au-
dience is composed of consumers who have substantial
knowledge of the base product (for a similar position on the APPENDIX B
importance of background knowledge in interpreting visual STIMULI FOR EXPERIMENT 2
metaphors, see McQuarrie and Mick 1999). Further, given
our evidence that even experts can experience difficulty in
processing an analogy when resource availability is lacking, ANALOGY CONDITION MESSAGE
efforts to ensure ample resource availability are warranted. For many years, Ameritech has produced a wide variety
This latter observation implies that marketers should be of business and personal telephone products and services.
cautious about the media placement of ads featuring an anal- In fact, Ameritech is one of the leading phone companies
ogy. The presentation of ads that include an analogy by in the world. Now Ameritech is introducing a new product
several advertisers within the same medium may tax con- that is like a cellular phone—but for people who have a
sumers’ resources and thus undermine persuasion. This sug- variety of communication needs. It is called Voyager.
gestion appears to be at odds with current practice. For What makes the Ameritech Voyager different from ex-
example, many consulting services and high-tech firms that isting products is the wireless technology that lets you record
advertise in Business Week and Fortune often describe their and send voice messages at your convenience without plug-
brands’ benefits in analogical terms. Although this practice ging into a phone line or an electrical outlet. Do it from the
may be justified because of the abstract and complex nature car, a park, the airport, a taxi, or wherever. The 4-inch-square
of their work, it may impose resource demands that com-
promise the persuasive impact of analogical messages. 5
Analogy condition wording [literal similarity condition wording].

This content downloaded from 155.97.178.73 on Fri, 28 Nov 2014 01:05:55 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
KNOWLEDGE, RESOURCES, AND ANALOGIES 271

Voyager helps you to stay in touch when you’re away from Mood and Purchase Intentions,” Psychology and Marketing,
your home or office. 7 (July/August), 109–133.
Ameritech’s new Voyager has a special high-resolution Baron, Reuben M. and David A. Kenny (1986), “The Moderator-
screen. Its on-screen keyboard allows you to input written Mediator Variable Distinction in Social Psychological Re-
search: Conceptual, Strategic and Statistical Considerations,”
messages that Voyager can transmit to any Internet ad- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51 (Decem-
dress, fax machine, or pager in the world. Several on- ber), 1173–1182.
line services are pre-installed, including America Online, Bassok, Miriam (1990), “Transfer of Domain-Specific Problem-
AT&T Magic Mail, OAG’s Flightline, Radio Mail, Eu- Solving Procedures,” Journal of Experimental Psychology:
dora, and e-shop. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16 (May), 522–533.
Additional Voyager features include 1 MB of RAM, a Bruner, Gordon C. II (1990), “Music, Mood and Marketing,” Jour-
fax/data modem that transmits at about half the speed (4800- nal of Marketing, 54 (October), 94–104.
bps) of most desktop modems, a peripheral port for printer Cacioppo, John T., Richard E. Petty, and Chuan Feng Kao (1984),
or keyboard connection, a rechargeable nickel cadmium bat- “The Efficient Assessment of Need for Cognition,” Journal
tery, and a carrying weight of only 1.5 lbs. Priced at only of Personality Assessment, 48 (June), 306–307.
Chi, Michelene T. H., Paul Feltovich, and Robert Glaser (1981),
$1,000, the Voyager also provides a calendar, an address “Categorization and Representation of Physics Problems by
book, and spreadsheet capabilities. Experts and Novices,” Cognitive Science, 5 (April/June),
121–152.
LITERAL SIMILARITY CONDITION Gentner, Dedre (1983), “Structure-Mapping: A Theoretical Frame-
work for Analogy,” Cognitive Science, 7 (April/June),
MESSAGE 155–170.
——— and Arthur B. Markman (1997), “Structure Mapping in
For many years, IBM has produced a wide variety of Analogy and Similarity,” American Psychologist, 52 (Janu-
business and personal computers. In fact, IBM is one of the ary), 45–56.
leading computer manufacturers in the world. Now IBM is ———, Mary Jo Rattermann, and Kenneth D. Forbus (1993), “The
introducing a new product that is like a laptop com- Roles of Similarity in Transfer: Separating Retrievability from
puter—but for people who have a variety of communication Inferential Soundness,” Cognitive Psychology, 25 (October),
needs. It is called Voyager. 524–575.
IBM’s new Voyager has a special high-resolution screen. Goldstone, Robert L. and Douglas L. Medin (1994), “Time Course
Its on-screen keyboard allows you to input written messages of Comparison,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learn-
that Voyager can transmit to any Internet address, fax ma- ing, Memory, and Cognition, 20 (January), 29–50.
chine, or pager in the world. Several on-line services are Gorn, Gerald J., Marvin E. Goldberg, and Kunal Basu (1993),
“Mood, Awareness and Product Evaluation,” Journal of Con-
pre-installed, including America Online, AT&T Magic Mail,
sumer Psychology, 2 (July), 237–256.
OAG’s Flightline, Radio Mail, Eudora, and e-shop. Gregan-Paxton, Jennifer and Deborah Roedder John (1997), “Con-
Additional Voyager features include 1 MB of RAM, a sumer Learning by Analogy: A Model of Internal Knowledge
fax/data modem that transmits at about half the speed (4800 Transfer,” Journal of Consumer Research, 24 (December),
bps) of most desktop modems, a peripheral port for printer 266–284.
or keyboard connection, a rechargeable nickel cadmium bat- Isen, Alice M. (1993), “Positive Affect and Decision Making,” in
tery, and a carrying weight of only 1.5 lbs. Priced at only Handbook of Emotion, ed. Michael Luis and Gannett M. Hav-
$1,000, the Voyager also provides a calendar, an address iland, New York: Guilford, 261–277.
book, and spreadsheet capabilities. ——— and Kimberly A. Daubman (1984), “The Influence of Af-
What makes the IBM Voyager different from existing fect on Categorization,” Journal of Personality and Social
products is the wireless technology that lets you record and Psychology, 47 (December), 1206–1217.
send voice messages at your convenience without plugging ———, Kimberly A. Daubman, and Gary P. Nowicki (1987),
“Positive Affect Facilitates Creative Problem Solving,” Jour-
into a phone line or an electrical outlet. Do it from the car,
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52 (June),
a park, the airport, a taxi, or wherever. The 4-inch-square 1122–1131.
Voyager helps you to stay in touch when you’re away from Lambert, Alan J., Saera R. Khan, Brian A. Lickel, and Katja Fricke
your home or office. (1997), “Mood and the Correction of Positive versus Negative
Stereotypes,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
[Received August 2000. Revised January 2001. David 72 (May), 1002–1016.
Glen Mick served as editor, and Punam Anand Keller Lee, Angela and Brian Sternthal (1999), “The Effects of Positive
served as associate editor for this article.] Mood on Memory,” Journal of Consumer Research, 26 (Sep-
tember), 115–127.
Markman, Arthur B. and Dedre Gentner (1993), “Structural Align-
REFERENCES ment during Similarity Comparisons,” Cognitive Psychology,
25 (October), 431–467.
Alba, Joseph W. and J. Wesley Hutchinson (1987), “Dimensions McQuarrie, Edward F. and David Glen Mick (1996), “Figures of
of Consumer Expertise,” Journal of Consumer Research, 13 Rhetoric in Advertising Language,” Journal of Consumer Re-
(March), 411–454. search, 22 (March), 424–437.
Alpert, Judy I. and Mark I. Alpert (1990), “Music Influences on ——— and David Glen Mick (1999), “Visual Rhetoric in Ad-

This content downloaded from 155.97.178.73 on Fri, 28 Nov 2014 01:05:55 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
272 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

vertising: Text-Interpretive, Experimental, and Reader Re- chology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23 (March),
sponse Analysis,” Journal of Consumer Research, 26 (June), 427–440.
37–54. Spence, Mark T. and Merrie Brucks (1997), “The Moderating Ef-
Novick, Laura R. (1988), “Analogical Transfer, Problem Similarity fects of Problem Characteristics on Experts’ and Novices’
and Expertise,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learn- Judgments,” Journal of Marketing Research, 34 (May),
ing, Memory, and Cognition, 14 (July), 510–520. 233–247.
Petty, Richard E. and John T. Cacioppo (1981), Attitudes and Per- Thompson, Leigh, Dedre Gentner, and Jeffery Lowenstein (2000),
suasion: Classic and Contemporary Approaches, Dubuque, “Avoiding Missed Opportunities in Managerial Life: Ana-
IA: Brown. logical Training More Powerful than Individual Case Train-
ing,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Making,
Read, Stephen. J. (1984), “Analogical Reasoning in Social Judg-
82 (May), 60–75.
ment: The Importance of Causal Theories,” Journal of Per-
Wedin, Lage (1972), “A Multidimensional Study of Perceptual-
sonality and Social Psychology, 46 (January), 14–25. Emotional Qualities in Music,” Scandinavian Journal of Psy-
Rigg, Melvin G. (1940), “The Effect of Register and Tonality upon chology, 13 (December), 241–257.
Musical Mood,” Journal of Musicology, 2 (September), Wegener, Duane T., Richard E. Petty, and Stephen M. Smith (1995),
49–61. “Positive Mood Can Increase or Decrease Message Scrutiny:
Ross, Brian H. and Matthew C. Kilbane (1997), “Effects of Prin- The Hedonic Contingency View of Mood and Message Pro-
ciple Explanation and Superficial Similarity on Analogical cessing,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69
Mapping in Problem Solving,” Journal of Experimental Psy- (July), 5–15.

This content downloaded from 155.97.178.73 on Fri, 28 Nov 2014 01:05:55 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like