Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 24

Article

Journal of Marketing Research


2021, Vol. 58(2) 223-245
Marketing, Through the Eyes ª American Marketing Association 2021
Article reuse guidelines:

of the Stigmatized sagepub.com/journals-permissions


DOI: 10.1177/0022243720975400
journals.sagepub.com/home/mrj

Colleen M. Harmeling, Martin Mende, Maura L. Scott,


and Robert W. Palmatier

Abstract
A consumer’s personal attribute (e.g., disease, body weight) can assume the qualities of a stigma (i.e., become a source of
devaluation by others) in the presence of certain audiences, which can affect consumption and represent a major hurdle to
marketers in many industries (e.g., health care). Two field experiments manipulating the marketing communications sent to 1,453
consumers diagnosed with 87 diseases of varying stigma potential, as well as two Amazon Mechanical Turk studies, reveal that
consumers with potentially stigmatizing attributes distinctly decode aspects of marketing communications as audience cues, to
infer how (un)favorable observers of their consumption will be in light of the potential stigma. When consumers possess
potentially stigmatizing attributes, audience cues influence social devaluation inferences, which influence their beneficial con-
sumption (program enrollment, long-term engagement in health care program; e.g., 64% click-through decrease) and their
interest in detrimental consumption (products that promise to alleviate the stigma but are associated with considerable risks).
Anticipated empowerment may increase beneficial consumption among consumers managing stigmatizing attributes.

Keywords
Consumer stigma, field experiments, social devaluation, consumer empowerment
Online supplement: https://doi.org/10.1177/0022243720975400

A stigma occurs when others judge an attribute of a person as or even detrimental. The same cue may be a positive signal to
negatively deviating from what they consider normal and devalue consumers with nonstigmatizing body weights.1 This research
the person because of this attribute (Goffman 1963). Personal examines how and when stigma shapes consumer responses to
attributes such as health conditions (e.g., HIV, obesity) or finan- marketing communication, including responses that likely bolster
cial status (e.g., poverty) can manifest as stigmas (Goffman their well-being (e.g., join a health program), and those that
1963). Even new diseases (e.g., COVID-19) can quickly become undermine it (e.g., pursue offerings that promise to reduce the
stigmatizing (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention 2020). stigmatizing attribute quickly at considerable risk, such as baria-
We propose that marketers need to understand that consumers tric surgery for weight loss).
with potential stigmas often feel they are constantly “on stage” Subtle and often ignored aspects of marketing communica-
and are unable to be themselves, for fear of being “reduced to the tions can inadvertently serve as audience cues. Because stigma
stigma itself” by unfavorable audiences (Hebl, Tickle, and
Heatherton 2000, p. 289). We theorize that a stigma uniquely
1
transforms consumers’ judgments of and responses to marketing Other examples include HIV-related public health campaigns that often
communications, by making consumers more vigilant in identify- deliberately leverage negative deviations (e.g., fear, shame, disgust appeals);
notably, such campaigns often fail (Fairchild, Bayer, and Colgrove 2015).
ing and reacting to audience cues—facets of the communication
that inform consumers’ inferences about others they may encoun- Colleen M. Harmeling is a Persis E. Rockwood Associate Professor of Marketing,
ter during consumption (e.g., other consumers) and how these Florida State University (email: charmeling@business.fsu.edu). Martin Mende is
others might judge them. For example, consumers with stigma- Professor of Marketing and Jim Moran Professor of Business Administration,
tizing body weights may perceive seemingly neutral information Florida State University (email: mmende@business.fsu.edu). Maura L. Scott is a
Persis E. Rockwood Professor of Marketing, Florida State University (email:
such as the aspirational, fit models in typical gym ads as cues of an maura.scott@fsu.edu). Robert W. Palmatier is John C. Narver Chair of
unfavorable, potentially threatening audience (e.g., due to social Business Administration, Professor of Marketing, Foster School of Business,
devaluation), rendering the marketing communication ineffective University of Washington (email: palmatrw@uw.edu).
224 Journal of Marketing Research 58(2)

can dramatically affect consumers’ presentation of self in front effectively target consumers with potential stigmas, marketers
of others (Goffman 1963), we focus on three aspects of ads that must understand where communications fall along this conti-
enable self-to-other comparisons: a demographic description of nuum (e.g., in Study 4, failing to refine communications in light
co-consumers, visual imagery, and an identified source of the of a stigma reduced click-through rates for a beneficial offering
communication. We use these aspects to determine the breadth by 41%).
of cues that trigger consumers to anticipate an unfavorable Second, we find that certain audience cues can inadvertently
(socially devaluing) versus favorable (benevolent) audience, hinder beneficial and encourage detrimental consumption out-
and how this affects their consumption. comes, illustrating the dangers of stigma in consumers’ deci-
We examine our conceptual model with four experiments in sion making and their future well-being. For example, in Study
health care settings (potential stigma: disease and body 1, across 87 diseases ranging in their level of stigma, ambig-
weight). In these contexts, we observe personal attributes with uous audience cues reduced consumers’ participation in bene-
low or no stigma (e.g., anemia, normal body weight) relative to ficial health programs relative to shared audience attribute
high stigma (e.g., HIV, obesity) and manipulate audience cues cues. Moreover, among consumers managing a potential
in marketing communications. Study 1 is a longitudinal field weight stigma, Study 2 demonstrates that the commonly used
experiment, conducted in collaboration with a health care firm, marketing tactic of aspirational images (e.g., thin models in ads
that includes 483 consumers diagnosed with 87 diseases and for fitness centers) can signal an unfavorable audience (con-
tests the effect of stigma on consumers’ immediate (click- trasting along the stigmatizing attribute of body weight); more-
through) and enduring (e.g., consumer posts, likes over time) over, these aspirational images increase overweight
responses to audience cues in marketing communications. consumers’ interest in offerings that promise to alleviate the
Study 2’s experiment, conducted using Amazon Mechanical stigma quickly (e.g., weight loss surgery) but that often come
Turk (MTurk), then examines consumers’ anticipated social with high physical risks (e.g., malnourishment, even death) and
devaluation as an underlying mechanism mediating the effect financial costs. Thus, our research can help organizations and
of stigmatizing attributes on consumption. Studies 3 and 4 policy makers design more effective communications that pre-
validate consumers’ inferences about a potential audience’s vent (unintentionally) nudging consumers who are managing
intentions toward them (anticipated devaluing or benevolent stigmas away from beneficial and toward harmful consumption
audience intentions) as key processes through which marketing (e.g., Chaney, Sanchez, and Maimon 2019).
communications differentially affect consumer responses Third, we find that consumers prioritize the stigmatizing
among those with potentially stigmatizing versus nonstigmatiz- attribute in their definition of self, such that other typically
ing attributes. more dominant indicators of similarity (e.g., gender, race) are
Our work offers multiple contributions to the marketing subordinate to it (Study 2), thereby providing deeper insights
literature. First, we show that a personal attribute can take on into how people prioritize multiple dimensions of homophily
the qualities of a stigma (i.e., consumers fear it as a source of (e.g., Block and Grund 2014; Lin and Lundquist 2013). Further,
devaluation) and affect consumption in relation to specific we find that consumers managing potential stigmas may oper-
anticipated audiences. Integrating across our studies, we find ate with the assumption that all “others,” even those that might
evidence that consumers estimate threats related to a poten- even be similar to them, may be a threat unless there are cues
tially stigmatizing attribute along a continuum that ranges from that the stigmatizing attribute can be concealed (Study 3) or
minimal threat (anticipated benevolent audience intentions) to cues that directly signal the audience’s benevolent intentions
high threat (anticipated devaluing intentions) depending on the (Study 4). These findings identify theoretical insights and man-
degree to which consumers can accurately assess whether the agerially relevant factors when similarity does not produce
stigmatizing attribute is shared or unshared with the audience, expected homophily effects.
which signals a favorable or unfavorable power differential Fourth, we discover (Study 3) that consumers managing a
between the consumer and the audience. Thus, when managing stigma make not only inferences about anticipated audiences,
potential stigmas, consumers’ threat assessments vary across but also stigma-relevant assessments of (1) the offering and (2)
audience attribute cues, from no information about the audi- the marketer. In terms of the offering, we find that, when con-
ence relative to the stigma, to a shared cue (e.g., HIV patient sumers possess a potential stigma, they respond particularly
with an audience of other HIV patients), to an ambiguous cue favorably to beneficial offerings they perceive as opportunities
(e.g., Type 2 diabetes customer with an audience of people for their empowerment in that the offering may help them
suffering from various diseases), to a contrasting cue (e.g., overcome the adversities associated with their stigma (Shih
obese consumer with an audience of thin people). Both ambig- 2004). However, in terms of the marketer, Study 3 shows a
uous and contrasting cues allow inferences that the potentially detrimental spillover effect: not only do consumers managing
stigmatizing attribute is not shared with the audience; we a stigma consider their consumption audience to be unfavor-
describe these as “unshared cues.” This continuum (from no able toward them, but they also make detrimental inferences
information, to shared cues, to unshared ambiguous cues, to about the organization that promotes the focal consumption.
unshared contrasting cues) is crucial because it reveals insights Overall, our findings suggest that marketers need to better
into how consumers managing stigmatizing attributes screen understand the relevance of seemingly benign cues in their
for and respond to threats. To prevent unwanted effects and marketing communication, which may signal audience
Harmeling et al. 225

characteristics and intentions to consumers managing stigmas, dynamic process in which consumers (must) learn to anticipate
thereby reemphasizing the need for paradigms of positive mar- conditions in which the potentially stigmatizing attribute will
keting (Mick et al. 2012). be problematic (Dovidio, Major, and Crocker 2000).
Revealing a stigmatizing attribute to an audience might be
associated with real or imagined social consequences (e.g.,
Stigma, Consumers, and Inferred Audiences “they treat you like you’re nobody”; stigma: homelessness; Hill
A stigma is an attribute of a person that negatively deviates and Stamey 1990, p. 312), economic consequences (e.g., “not
from what others consider normal in a particular social context; entitled to the same services”; stigma: senior citizens; Tepper
it can be “deeply discrediting,” to the point that its possessor is 1994, p. 505), or physical consequences (e.g., bodily harm by
vulnerable to global devaluation by others (Crocker, Major, others in the marketplace; stigma: sexual orientation; Kates
and Steele 1998; Goffman 1963). However, by definition, not 2002). Thus, for consumers managing potential stigmas, others
all deviant attributes are stigmas, and no attribute in isolation present at the time of consumption can range from merely
can be a stigma (Goffman 1963). Rather, stigma implies a irrelevant bystanders or empathetic co-consumers with bene-
social hierarchy in which the stigma possessor is in last (or a volent intentions to audiences that might challenge their very
lower) place; they are assigned to this position by dominant existence, question their worth to society and entitlement to
others (i.e., an audience) in the focal social context. It is this certain economic benefits, or even inflict physical harm.
audience that defines what is “normal,” determines acceptable Underlying each of these varied threats is the notion that
tolerance around this standard, and assesses what is deviant and certain audiences may socially devalue the stigma possessor by
valued (Lin and McFerran 2016; Wooten 2006). For example, reducing the person’s worth from “a whole and usual person to
extreme intelligence can be a source of pride in certain social a tainted, discounted one” (Goffman 1963, p. 4). Once an audi-
circles but, in others, may be “taken as a sign of social ence devalues a consumer based on a personal attribute, it
ineptitude” and ridiculed (Wooten 2006, p. 189). Thus, an creates psychological conditions that allow them to exclude
attribute’s status as a stigma depends on the audience to which this consumer from basic human protection, “making it permis-
any person is exposed in a given (consumption) setting. sible to treat [her] in a way that would be morally objectionable
In consumption settings, stigma can influence the acquisi- if [she] were fully human” (Goff et al. 2014, p. 527). Thus, fear
tion, use, and disposal of possessions (Wooten 2006). It can act of harm tends to outweigh expectations of benevolence and
as a “psychic boundary” that motivates consumers to hide con- appears to be warranted, because social devaluation removes
sumption practices (Kozinets 2001), avoid participation in pro- the person from the moral confounds of human rights, in the
motions (Tepper 1994), limit their consideration sets, ignore eyes of that focal audience.
being cheated, or refrain from consumption altogether (Adkins
and Ozanne 2005) to avoid stigma-associated risks. To under-
stand the nature of the audience and how consumers sense its Stigma and Sensing the (Un)favorability of an Audience:
favorability toward them, we review extant marketing research,
primarily ethnographic studies, that investigate consumption
Audience Cues
practices that can be sources of stigma (Argo and Main 2008; Real and imagined threats related to possessing a stigma coin-
Kozinets 2001; Muniz and Schau 2005; Tepper 1994) or con- cide with evidence that consumers constantly and vigilantly
sumption as a means to manage stigmas (Adkins and Ozanne attempt to protect themselves from the harm an unfavorable
2005; Coskuner-Balli and Thompson 2013; Hill and Stamey audience may inflict on them (Kates 2002). Because it is
1990; Kates 2002) (see Table 1 for summary). impossible to know with certainty another’s intentions, consu-
mers with potential stigmas learn to sense and interpret subtle
cues and develop context-sensitive judgments to estimate the
Stigma and Consumers’ Inferred Audiences: Social,
nature of an anticipated audience (Kates 2002). As a proxy,
Economic, and Physical Consequences consumers use their stigma to filter visible markers or subtle
Across diverse consumption contexts, prior research hints at behaviors that signal another person shares the focal or similar
the idea of an implied boundary between people who possess a stigma (i.e., audience attribute cues) and use these signals to
stigma and those who do not, with the latter manifesting as an divide the world into those who are presumably sympathetic or
ever-present, potentially threatening audience for whom the empathic to the stigma with benevolent intentions toward them
consumer managing the stigma performs (Kates 2002; Kozi- and those who are not (Adkins and Ozanne 2005; Frable, Platt,
nets 2001). The audience can be family or friends, but in the and Hoey 1998). Yet, attribute similarity is an imprecise proxy
marketplace, it consists of other consumers and service provi- accompanied with a high risk of inaccurately estimating others’
ders too (Sandikci and Ger 2009). Importantly, audience reac- intentions. Consumers often look for more explicit cues of
tions can vary widely from benevolent and empowering to benevolent intentions (i.e., audience intention cues), such as
devaluing and threatening (Shih 2004). Consumers search for body language (e.g., smile; Hill and Stamey 1990) or overheard
those who are sympathetic or empathetic to the stigma often by conversations about someone possessing the stigma (Coskuner-
finding those who share it. However, the extreme variance in Balli and Thompson 2013), to supplement risky inferences
audience reactions makes stigma management a volatile and made by attribute cues alone.
226
Table 1. Stigma and the Ever-Present Audience: Themes and Evidence from Extant Marketing Research.

Theme (Description) Illustrative Quote (Stigma Source; Citation) Explanation

Stigma and consumers’ inferred audiences: social, economic, and physical consequences
Social consequences: consumers make existential inferences “Another dad told how he was often treated as a persona The statement “moms would talk over me, as if I was not
about audiences’ impressions of them, indicating they felt non grata when taking his children to public parks, even there” highlights a threat of nothingness or lack of
insignificant, like they did not exist, or were nothing in the lamenting that ‘moms would talk over me, as if I was not worth. This example also illustrates that similarity without
eyes of certain audiences even there.’ The at-home fathers in attendance bonded gender similarity is not enough to ensure audience
over stories about the myriad disapproving and favorability.
judgmental comments they received from their in-laws”
(at-home fathers; Coskuner-Balli and Thompson 2013, p.
32).
“They [shelter employees] don’t care, they treat you like you The informant identifies shelter employees as critical
nobody. They feel like this—if you in here, you nobody audiences and anticipates they will devalue the person to
cause you don’t want to work” (homelessness; Hill and “nobody” or reduce her or his worth.
Stamey 1990, p. 312).
Economic consequences: consumers voice economic “I was in the store with her, and she bought two boxes of The informant describes an instance with her mother who
concerns, such as ignoring being cheated or not being cereal for $5.00. And, she gave the guy a $10.00 bill. And has low literacy (stigma source) and who was willing to
entitled to the same economic benefits the guy didn’t give her the change back…trying to cheat accept being cheated out of her change rather than risk
her…she wouldn’t wait for her change or nothing” (low exposing the stigmatizing attribute to an unfavorable
literacy; Adkins and Ozanne 2005, p. 103). audience.
“The stigmatizing nature of financially limited Illustrates the potential economic consequences of exposing
characterizations (i.e., senior citizen discount users ‘don’t a stigmatizing attribute, in that onlookers may infer, based
have extra money’ and ‘have very little funds’) was on their senior citizen status, that consumers are “not
reflected in participants’ concerns that others would view entitled to the same services.”
senior citizen discount users as financially dependent,
unable to afford the full price…‘second-class citizens’ who
should not be entitled to the same services” (age; Tepper
1994, p. 505).
Physical consequences: consumers express concern about “So every time I see a black man beaten on TV for no reason, Implies the importance of developing skills to identify cues of
bodily harm the boys have to be taught—[as] my dad used to say— an audience’s (in this case authorities’) favorability, with
‘how to handle yourself’…to avoid or minimize negative potentially fatal mistakes if misread.
interactions with authority” (race; Crockett 2017, p. 565).
There were occasional police reports and continual gossip of Illustrates the real physical harm that can be inflicted on a
physical and verbal attacks on people presumed to be gay person because of the stigmatizing attribute.
or lesbian (i.e., gay bashing) in the area (sexual orientation;
Kates 2002).
“One of our informants…became the victim of bum burning- Provides another illustration of the physical harm others may
a sadistic practice of gangs of teenagers who pour gasoline inflict on a person bearing a stigma.
on a sleeping homeless person and then set him or her on
fire” (homelessness; Hill and Stamey 1990, p. 313).
(continued)
Table 1. (continued)

Theme (Description) Illustrative Quote (Stigma Source; Citation) Explanation

Stigma and sensing the un/favorability of an audience: audience cues


Audience attribute cue: pieces of information that allow the “Baxter: ‘Now coming [to a black-owned restaurant], I don’t Uses the restaurant owner’s race as a cue that the audience
consumer to estimate if an observer possesses the same know about you, but I’m just more relaxed.…I don’t of his consumption at a restaurant will be favorable
or similar stigma worry about, if we get loud somebody’s going to say, “get toward him.
out.” Or, “you’re disrupting my business”’” (race;
Crockett 2017, p. 564)
“Participants frequent[ly] remark[ed] that they hesitated to Implies the use of age and financial status as cues that
use senior citizen discounts in the presence of others who consumers with a potentially stigmatizing age might use to
were younger, financially better-off, or both (as indicated infer the favorability of an audience.
by reference to ‘yuppies’) reflected concerns for others’
evaluations of them as ‘old’ and/or ‘financially limited’”
(age; Tepper 1994, p. 508).
“The gaydar framework later becomes quite useful when Stigmatized consumers learn subtle differences in
identifying gay men in ambiguous social consumption performances “between consumers with the
circumstances…gaydar provides a myriad of subtle and stigma and without” that allow them to identify others
personalized ways of ascertaining the boundaries of the with the stigma in “ambiguous social circumstances” even
subculture.…informants searched for and read when the stigmatizing attribute is concealed or reluctantly
cues.…informants developed holistic, highly context- disclosed.
sensitive judgment…that encompasses not only dress but
also the subtleties in the ways that clothes are displayed”
(sexual orientation; Kates 2002, p. 389).
Audience intention cue: pieces of information that allow the “One young wiper avoids trouble by reading facial Illustrates that consumers managing a stigmatizing attribute
consumer to determine observers’ general benevolence expressions to determine whether to approach a car. often use body language of others to infer their intentions.
or specific acceptance of the stigma You’ve got to really look at ‘em.…If they smile, you shine
their window [BM, thirties]” (homelessness; Hill and
Stamey 1990, p. 311).
“I’ll be out with 2 of my friends and they work, they are full As part of the audience of another person bearing a similar
time workers, engineers in fact, and they might make a stigma, the informant interprets comments about the
belittling comment about a stay home mom you know, I’ll stigmatizing attribute as an indicator of others’ favorability
be standing there wait a second, I am a stay home dad toward him.
what do they think of me?” (at-home father; Coskuner-
Balli and Thompson 2013, p. 28).

227
228 Journal of Marketing Research 58(2)

Marketing
Communication Characteristics

Audience cues
S1, S2, S3, S4

Consumer Inferences Consumption Outcomes

Consumer anticipated
Beneficial consumption
devaluing (vs. benevolent)
behaviors
audience intentions
Consumer’s + S1, S3, S4
S2, S3 +/−
potential stigma
S1, S2, S3, S4
Consumer Detrimental consumption
anticipated empowerment behaviors
S3 S2, S3

Figure 1. Effects of stigma on consumer responses to marketing communications.


Notes: S1 ¼ Study 1, a 2 (audience attribute cue)  measured (disease-related stigma) longitudinal field experiment on 483 patients across 87 diseases. S2 ¼ Study
2, a 3 (audience attribute cue)  measured (stigma source: body mass index) experiment. S3 ¼ Study 3, a 2 (body weight exposure cue)  measured (stigma
source: body mass index) experiment. S4 ¼ Study 4, a 2 (audience attribute cue)  2 (audience intention cue)  measured (disease-related stigma) field
experiment on 970 patients across 87 diseases.

In summary, stigmatized consumers use cues in their envi- propose that consumers managing stigmas use audience cues
ronment such as visible similarities, behavioral subtleties, and (e.g., visual stimuli) in marketing communication to infer
observations as an audience member themselves, to sense the where along the continuum of intentions (from benevolent to
(often hidden) intentions of potential audiences, which can devaluing) a potential audience might fall, with implications
range along a continuum from benevolent to devaluing. Indi- for their consumption (see Figure 1).
cators of safe versus threatening audiences provide an impor- If audience cues are unshared (i.e., either ambiguous with
tant frame of reference for consumers’ stigma management and regard to the focal attribute, or contrasting relative to the con-
related consumption decisions. sumer on the focal attribute), the apprehension that coincides
with managing the stigma in unknown social encounters may
cause the consumer to anticipate more negative audience-
Stigma and Consumer Responses to related effects (e.g., increased risk of social devaluation)
Audience Cues in Marketing Communication (Crocker and Major 1989). In general, ambiguous cues allow
Extant research generally examines the methods consumers use inferences that range from very positive to very negative (Nor-
to infer others’ favorability toward a stigmatizing attribute in ton, Lamberton, and Naylor 2013). However, for consumers
their immediate interactions with them (Crockett 2017; Kates managing stigmas, communications for offerings that might
2002). Yet, consumers often need to use marketing communi- improve their well-being (e.g., wellness programs) that include
cations to predict how future consumption encounters might ambiguous audience cues may allow negative inferences to
unfold, on the basis of limited available information and cues dominate, making them less responsive. Similarly, for consu-
in the communication, along with their memory of prior social mers managing stigmas, unshared cues that are contrasting
encounters (Hoch and Deighton 1989). This is crucial for mar- might elevate interest in high return–risk offerings that they
keters to note, as these predictions typically emerge before the otherwise might consider unnecessary or undesirable (Schou-
consumer has any social interactions with the focal company; ten 1991) (e.g., weight-loss surgery). In these conditions where
thus, these predictions might inherently deter consumers from social devaluation threats are high, consumers may adopt
engaging with the company in the first place. “nothing to lose” mindsets, such that they may believe others
For consumers with potential stigmas, the consumption fea- assign little worth to their current self (Brough et al. 2016) and
tured in marketing communication can be seen as an unknown, discount risks of products that might cause physical harm (e.g.,
threatening social encounter (Adkins and Ozanne 2005). Their surgical procedures). If the focal cues signal that the audience
history and knowledge of the consequences of inaccurately shares the potentially stigmatizing attribute (e.g., health condi-
reading a situation (e.g., experiencing social devaluation) can tions), consumers should derive more favorable inferences
motivate them to go beyond merely considering whether an about this audience’s intentions and possible effect on their
offering fits their economic needs, as is typical (Hoch and Ha well-being (e.g., decreased risk of social devaluation, increased
1986), and also consider their consumption audience and how inferences of benevolent intentions), enabling more beneficial
they can ensure self-preservation in front of this audience. We response to the marketing communication.
Harmeling et al. 229

In contrast, consumers without the potential stigma typically In the studies, we use offers of health care products and
do not harbor concerns about social devaluations and thus do services, such as an online health care program, a platform for
not attend to cues in marketing communication to check for consumers to share health-related content about symptoms,
threats as vigilantly as consumers with the stigma do (Chaney, treatments, and outcomes (Frost and Massagli 2008). An invi-
Sanchez, and Maimon 2019). Taken together, unshared (i.e., tation to a new online program is an unknown social context,
ambiguous or contrasting) audience cues may evoke the idea of requiring consumers to estimate the risks and rewards of
unfavorable audiences that elicit greater fear of social devalua- responding. Online health care platforms are widely used yet
tion and reduce consumers’ likelihood to engage in beneficial remain poorly understood (Wang, Zuo, and Zhao 2015; Young
consumption behaviors but increase their interest in detrimen- 2013). Thus, health care and online health programs offer man-
tal consumption. We hypothesize the following effects: agerially relevant contexts in which to rigorously test our the-
ory. Table 2 summarizes the four studies.
H1a: When consumers with potentially stigmatizing attri-
butes encounter unshared (vs. shared) audience attribute
cues, they have less interest in beneficial consumption
Study 1: Field Experiment Testing Stigma
behaviors (e.g., joining/engaging in a wellness program). and Response to Marketing Communications
These effects are attenuated for consumers with low- Using a longitudinal field experiment and a sample of consumers
stigma attributes. diagnosed with a wide variety of diseases (e.g., HIV, cancer,
H1b: When consumers with potentially stigmatizing attri- rheumatoid arthritis; see Web Appendix A), we analyze whether
butes encounter unshared (vs. shared) audience attribute the degree to which consumers’ diseases are stigmatizing deter-
cues or shared audience attribute cues (vs. no cues), they mines whether they click through on marketing communications
exhibit greater interest in detrimental consumption beha- that vary in their audience cues. We also test these effects on
viors (high risk-high return offers). These effects are actual consumer engagement behaviors (e.g., likes, comments,
attenuated for consumers with low-stigma attributes. friend requests) over a two-week period following their initial
H1c: When consumers with potentially stigmatizing attri- acquisition. Specifically, we examine the interaction between
butes encounter unshared (vs. shared) audience attribute audience attribute cues (ambiguous vs. shared attribute cue) and
cues, inferences of socially devaluing (vs. benevolent) disease stigma on consumers’ likelihood to join a wellness pro-
audience intentions affect their interest in beneficial and gram and longer-term activity within that program.
detrimental consumption. These effects are attenuated
for consumers with low-stigma attributes.
Study 1: Design, Participants, and Procedures
Although we hypothesize the role of audience inferences The study employed a 2 (audience attribute cue: ambiguous,
(ranging from benevolent to socially devaluing) in influencing shared)  measured (disease-related stigma) design. Partici-
consumption decisions, we note that there might be characteris- pants (N ¼ 483) were consumers who agreed to be contacted
tics related to the offering in which a seemingly negative attribute by the collaborating health care firm about their disease and
could be transformed into a source of empowerment (Conrad and opened the invitation from the firm. Participants were ran-
Caldwell 2006; Shih 2004). Experiences and knowledge that can domly assigned to conditions, using stratified random sampling
be acquired only through a history of stigma-related devaluation by disease, so each disease was equally likely to appear in each
can create a unique expertise related to successfully navigating condition. An email with the company name and logo invited
challenging social situations; this expertise might have value in participants to join either a general health group (ambiguous
certain consumption contexts. Providing consumption opportuni- audience attribute cue) or a group of others sharing the same
ties that allow stigmatized consumers to use and share their disease (shared audience attribute cue), as follows:
unique knowledge with others might help them feel empowered
and motivate more positive responses. We therefore also explore “Thanks for signing up to learn about opportunities to connect with
the role of empowerment (in Study 3). others about your personal health experiences with (health con-
dition)! You may think your story is ordinary, but to someone who
is struggling with the uncertainty and fear of a health-related issue,
Empirical Overview and Research Context hearing from someone who has been there can make all the differ-
ence. The [ambiguous group / shared disease group] in the [Com-
We conducted four experiments in a health care context, includ- pany Name] community offers many ways—such as chats, polls,
ing two field experiments (Studies 1 and 4), with an online health and blogs—for you to relate and empathize with people about what
community provider. Health conditions—the main reason con- you’re going through. As a member living with a chronic condi-
sumers seek health care products and services—are deviations tion, I hope you’ll sign up. By joining [Company Name] commu-
from what is considered normal, though only some of them (e.g., nity and the [ambiguous group / shared disease group], you can
HIV, obesity) are potentially stigmatizing. The marketing of shape the experience and create a community of people who care
many health care offerings makes salient the consumer’s health about (health condition). Getting started is easy—just click here or
condition, exacerbating the associated stigma’s effects and dis- on the button below, sign up to join, and create your profile. I look
couraging consumption (DiMatteo 2004). forward to welcoming you!”
230
Table 2. Overview of Empirical Studies.
Designs Objectives Sources of Stigma Audience Cues Dependent Variables Findings

Study 1 Longitudinal field experiment;  Illustrate a long-lasting  Disease: actual patients Audience attribute cue  Actual consumer click-  When cues about the
2 (audience attribute cue: impact of stigma on with various diseases (e.g., (manipulated) through on email potential audience of the
shared, ambiguous)  consumer responses to HIV, cancer, obesity,  Shared—described other invitation to join an focal consumption are
measured (disease-related marketing communications Parkinson’s), supplied by a members of health care online health care ambiguous (vs. specified as
stigma) that is dependent on the health care program program as having the program. sharing the same disease as
audience cues present in provider and rated by two same disease as participant.  Engagement activities the consumer), it generates
the communication. medical doctors.  Ambiguous—no (likes, comments, posts) 64% fewer click-throughs
 Provide proof of concept description of other two weeks after joining for an online health care
for the importance of members of the health care an online health care program among stigmatized
considering stigma by using program. program. consumers.
real marketing  Consumers with
communications linked to stigmatizing diseases who
actual short- and long-term joined the health program
consumer behaviors. after receiving a marketing
communication with an
ambiguous (vs. shared)
audience attribute cue
completed 52% fewer
engagement behaviors (e.g.,
likes, posts, comments) in
the two weeks after the
invitation.

Study 2 Experiment; 3 (audience  Illustrate that possessing a  Body weight; measured Audience attribute cue  Interest in a high return-  Relative to participants in
attribute cue: shared, potentially stigmatizing body mass index (BMI). (manipulated; McFerran risk product meant to the shared and control
contrasting, control)  attribute motivates et al. 2009). quickly alleviate the conditions, participants in
measured (BMI-related consumers to anticipate  Shared: matched stigmatizing attribute the contrasting condition
stigma) between subjects devaluation by others in a participant BMI with photo (i.e., sleeve gastrectomy anticipated more social
future consumption of thin (participant BMI < surgery). devaluation from other
context (social devaluation 30) or heavy (participant consumers, as their BMI
as mediating mechanism). BMI > 30) gym member. approached potentially
 Demonstrate that when  Contrasting: mismatched stigmatizing levels, which
consumers with body participant BMI with photo led to greater interest in
weights at a stigmatizing of thin (participant BMI > high return–risk (e.g.,
levels infer unfavorable 30) or heavy (participant bodily harm) products that
audience cues, they are BMI < 30) gym member. may help manage the
significantly more  Control: no image of other potentially stigmatizing
interested in an offering gym members. attribute (e.g., body
that promises to alleviate weight).
the stigma quickly but  Effects persist when
comes with a high risk. controlling for other forms
 Provide internal validity to of similarity such as gender
field study findings (Study and race, specifying the
1). nature of the cue as an
intention signal rather than
(continued)
Table 2. (continued)

Designs Objectives Sources of Stigma Audience Cues Dependent Variables Findings

general liking through


homophily.

Study 3 Experiment; 2 (stigma  Replicate mediation by  Body weight; measured as Audience attribute cue (held  Likelihood to join an  Consumers with potentially
exposure: yes, no)  anticipated devaluation. body mass index (BMI). constant across all online health care stigmatizing body weight
measured (BMI-related  Test anticipated conditions): Described program. are less likely to join a
stigma) between subjects empowerment as a driver other members of health  Interest in high return– health care program if cues
of stigmatized consumers’ care program as “people risk products meant to in the marketing
decision making. similar to you.” quickly alleviate the communication suggest
 Test the theoretical stigmatizing attribute. there is a high risk of
underpinnings of the exposing that attribute to
phenomenon that others in the program.
consumers with  Effects of stigma on both
stigmatizing (relative to beneficial and detrimental
nonstigmatizing) body behavior are driven by the
weights use cues in amount of devaluation the
marketing communications person anticipates and how
to make inferences about a empowering the offering is
potential audience by in light of the stigma.
manipulating body weight
exposure to the others
present at the time of
consumption, effectively
removing the audience of
the focal attribute or not.

Study 4 Field experiment; 2 (audience  Test of process through  Disease: actual patients Audience attribute cue  Actual consumer click-  Among consumers with
attribute cue: shared, moderation by with various diseases (e.g., (manipulated) through on email stigmatizing diseases, failing
contrasting)  2 (audience manipulating consumer HIV, cancer, obesity,  Shared: source of the invitation to join an to refine communications
intention cue: benevolent, inferences of audience Parkinson’s), supplied by a communication is a patient online health care in light of a stigma reduced
neutral)  measured intentions as either health care program in the health care program. program. click-through rates for a
(disease-related stigma) benevolent toward the provider and rated by two  Ambiguous: source of beneficial offering by 41%.
stigma or neutral. medical doctors. the communication is the
 Replicate effects from the administrator of the health
field study (Study 1) and program.
MTurk studies (Studies 2  Audience intention cue
and 3). (manipulated)
 Benevolent: used terms
such as “community of
people that care”
 Neutral: used terms such
as “platform for learning
from others”

231
232 Journal of Marketing Research 58(2)

A: Immediate Effects: Click-Through Rate B: Long-Term Effects: Engagement Behaviors


Relative to participants who viewed the marketing
communication with an ambiguous audience cue, as Relative to participants who viewed the marketing
disease-related stigma exceeds the scale midpoint communication with an ambiguous audience cue, as
(JN = 4.08), participants who viewed the shared disease-related stigma exceeds the JN point (JN =
40% .40 5.45), participants who viewed the shared audience
audience cue clicked through more.
cue completed more activities in the two weeks
Click-Through Rate (%)

following their joining the program.

Engagement Behaviors
30% .30

Average Number of
20% .20

10% .10

0% .00

1 2 3 4 2 3 7 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Disease-Related Stigma Level Disease-Related Stigma Level

Figure 2. Study 1 (longitudinal field experiment): Johnson–Neyman analysis of audience attribute cues by stigma.
Notes: Engagement behaviors include likes, shares, comments, friend requests, and messages over the two weeks following exposure to the experimental stimuli.
Johnson–Neyman points of significance (p < .05) are marked by the shaded areas.

To measure stigma, two medical doctors, blind to the perceived the group as having a shared illness (MShared ¼
hypotheses and not otherwise involved in the research, assessed 5.79, MAmbiguous ¼ 4.81; F(1, 67) ¼ 11.41, p < .001).
the 87 diseases represented in the sample and rated the extent to
which each disease was stigmatizing.2 The two dependent vari-
ables are click-through rates and long-term consumer engage-
ment behaviors, measured as the number of behaviors (likes,
Study 1: Results and Discussion
friend requests, and comments) undertaken in the two weeks Click-through behavior. We conducted a logistic regression anal-
following entry into the health care program. ysis on click-through behavior as a function of disease-related
stigma, audience attribute cues, and their interaction. The audi-
ence cue main effect is marginally significant (Wald w2 ¼ 2.93,
Manipulation test. We used a sample of 69 U.S. MTurk partici-
p ¼ .09), but the stigma main effect is not (Wald w2 ¼ .68, p ¼
pants (MAge ¼ 35.78 years, 37 women) to test the effectiveness
.41). More important, these effects are qualified by the pre-
of our audience attribute cue manipulations. We described the dicted significant two-way interaction (Wald w2 ¼ 10.20, p <
skin illness psoriasis and asked participants to imagine having it, .001; Figure 2, Panel A). In support of H1a, a Johnson–Neyman
then randomly assigned participants to read either the unshared (JN) analysis reveals that consumers with highly stigmatizing
(ambiguous) or shared email message from the main study. diseases are significantly less responsive if the audience cue is
Next, they indicated the extent to which the audience reflected unshared (ambiguously defined) versus shared (possessing the
the group with a shared illness (“like a generic-health group/like same disease as the recipient), (stigma level  4.08; z ¼ 1.96, p
a specific psoriasis group”; “comprised of people with diseases < .05). This effect reverses for consumers with low-stigma
other than psoriasis/comprised only of people with psoriasis”; diseases (stigma levels  1.83; z ¼ 1.96, p < .05).
“not at all similar to me/very similar to me”; “not specific to
psoriasis/very specific to psoriasis”; “distant to me/close to me”; Long term engagement behaviors. We conducted an analysis of
“not like me/just like me”; “a general health group/a health variance (ANOVA) on long-term engagement behaviors in the
group focused on psoriasis”) on seven-point bipolar scales (a health care program as a function of the audience attribute cue
¼ .90). Participants in the shared audience cue condition factor. The audience cue main effect is significant (F(1, 479) ¼
8.76, p ¼ .003), and the stigma main effect is not (F(1, 479) ¼
2 .86, p ¼ .35). Importantly, these effects are qualified by a
The physicians, one male, one female, came from two top-rated hospital
systems in the northeast and southwest United States. Each responded to the significant two-way interaction (F(1, 479) ¼ 6.70, p ¼ .01;
following items: “Based on the disease or its symptoms, consumers with this Figure 2, Panel B). Providing further support for H1a, the JN
disease often feel that others are uncomfortable with them/treat them as analysis reveals that consumers with highly stigmatizing dis-
inferior/prefer to avoid them/can tell that they have this disease/feel it is the eases engage in fewer activities in response to an unshared
consumer’s own fault for having this disease” (1 ¼ “Not at all,” and 7 ¼ “Very (ambiguous) versus shared (one that signals they share the
much so”) and “Overall this disease is stigmatized” (seven-point scale
anchored by “strongly disagree and “strongly agree”; a ¼ .95) (McLaughlin, same disease) audience cue (stigma level  5.45; z ¼ 1.96, p
Pachana, and Mcfarland 2008). The physicians’ responses were highly < .05). This effect reverses for consumers with low-stigma
correlated (r ¼ .77, p < .001). diseases (stigma levels  1.74; z ¼ 1.96, p < .05).
Harmeling et al. 233

In a field study, using unobtrusive measures of stigma and detrimental purchase decisions. Stigmatized consumers may
real, observable consumer behaviors (e.g., joining an online seek alternatives that (promise to) offer high rewards—physi-
health program, customer engagement behaviors), Study 1 pro- cally substitute the stigma for a desirable attribute in its place,
vides evidence that audience attribute cues alter consumers’ but also entail high risk, because it might be irreversible, painful,
behaviors when they have a potentially stigmatizing disease. or dangerous (Puhl and Brownwell 2006; Schouten 1991).
Specifically, we show that consumers whose diseases are more Therefore, with Study 2 we test whether audience cues in mar-
stigmatizing are less interested in engaging in beneficial con- keting messages influence consumers’ interest in high return–
sumption (e.g., online health program) when audience cues are risk products (e.g., surgical procedures) that promise to quickly
ambiguous versus specified as sharing a stigmatizing attribute alleviate the stigma by reducing excess weight—but come with
with other consumers; when stigma is low, this effect is attenu- risks of bodily harm (Chang et al. 2014).
ated. An invitation with unshared (vs. shared) audience attribute
cues decreased click-through rates among consumers with
highly stigmatizing diseases (þ1SD) by 64%. Moreover, those Study 2: Design, Participants, and Procedure
who joined the program after receiving an invitation with an We employed a 3 (audience cue: control vs. shared vs.
ambiguous (vs. shared) audience cue completed 52% fewer unshared, contrasting) between subjects  measured (BMI:
engagement activities over two weeks. Importantly, these effects stigma source) design. Participants (396 U.S. MTurk partici-
cannot be explained by homophily, as we would expect only a pants; 200 women; MAge ¼ 40.93 years; MBMI ¼ 27.89)4 com-
main effect of audience cues and no interaction by stigma. pleted a two-part study. We use BMI as an indicator of the
degree to which participants’ body weights are stigmatizing
Study 2: Stigma, Audience Cues, and and test anticipated devaluation as a mediator. First, partici-
pants indicated their weight in pounds and height in inches,
Anticipated Social Devaluation
which we used to calculate their BMI (703  weight (in
We theorize the effects observed in Study 1 are caused by pounds) / [height (in inches)]2). Two days later, participants
inferences made by consumers with highly stigmatizing dis- were randomly assigned to one of the three audience attribute
eases, anticipating that others in the ambiguous (vs. shared cue conditions: a heavy, thin, or no image of a woman (images
disease) group observing their consumption may devalue them, adopted from McFerran et al. 2009) who was featured in a
due to the disease. However, messages may have audience cues health facility testimonial, as follows:
that contrast the stigmatizing attribute with a nonstigmatizing
one (e.g., using an ad with a thin model when marketing to “Customer Testimonial: I recently joined [Company]. When I first
obese consumers). In Study 2, we test the mediating role of came to the gym and looked around, the equipment looked great
anticipated social devaluation and examine contrasting audi- and the trainers were friendly. Most of the members were in the
ence attribute cues using a controlled experiment involving same shape that I am in. —Jessica B., [Company] Member”
marketing communications for a full-service health facility
(e.g., fitness, medical weight loss, spa services). Study 1 tested Participants also read that the facility offered access to
verbal audience attribute cues. Study 2 tests a visual audience affiliated surgeons focused on medical weight management
attribute cue. We use body mass index (BMI) as a potential and aesthetics. Using the CDC’s label for “overweight” (BMI
source of stigma, because it is associated with a nationally > 25), the shared weight condition matched participants with
communicated standard for “normal” and deviant categories BMIs greater (less) than 25 to the heavy (thin) consumer
(overweight, obese, and morbidly obese) and is linked to per- image; the unshared, contrasting weight condition matched
vasive societal stigma (Puhl and Brownell 2006).3 Thus, BMI participants with BMIs greater (less) than or equal to 25 to the
provides theory-based levels at which stigma effects are thin (heavy) image; the control condition had no image (Nay-
expected to occur (CDC 2019), as compared with nonstigma- lor, Lamberton, and Norton 2011).
tizing levels of the same attribute. The outcome variable was consumers’ interest in a high
Study 1 focuses on a beneficial choice: actual program enroll- return-risk offering by the health facility (i.e., sleeve gastrect-
ment behaviors. However, stigma also may influence potentially omy surgery) (H1b). To better disguise the focal offering and
reduce demand artifacts, we intermingled it among six other
products designed to manage the stigmatizing attribute (BMI)
3
Overweight and obese people experience stigma (Carr and Friedman 2005; with a brief description of each, in random order (e.g., weight
King et al. 2006), and this stigma becomes more prevalent as they grow loss acupuncture, cool sculpting, body firming detox; see Web
increasingly overweight (Harvey and Hill 2001). To confirm this
assumption, we engaged 61 MTurk participants (38 women, Mage ¼ 34.60
Appendix C for descriptions and stimuli tests). Participants
years; MBMI ¼ 27.82) to respond to 11 items regarding weight stigma such indicated their purchase interest for each product on five-
as “In general, people . . . sometimes stigmatize overweight people;
discriminate against overweight people” (seven-point scale anchored by
4
“strongly disagree and “strongly agree”; a ¼ .93; Meadows and Higgs 2019; Participants: 643 completed part 1, and 398 of those completed part 2 (two
Lewis, Cash, and Bubb-Lewis 1997). Participants expressed beliefs that being days later). Two reported BMIs of < 7 and were removed; the lowest recorded
overweight is stigmatizing; the mean is significantly greater than the scale adult BMI is 7.5. Average BMIs were consistent across time periods (MT1 ¼
midpoint (M ¼ 4.90, t(60) ¼ 6.19, p < .001); see Web Appendix B. 26.93; MT2 ¼ 27.89).
234 Journal of Marketing Research 58(2)

point scales (1 ¼ “Not interested at all,” and 5 ¼ “Extremely interaction is marginally significant (F(2, 388) ¼ 2.91, p ¼
interested”). We included anticipated social devaluation as a .06); see Figure 3, Panel B. For this interaction, the OGRS
mediator: “If I met other people at this gym, I think they would JN point (p ¼ .05) occurs at BMI  29.36, in the overweight
be mortified to meet me; If I met other people at this gym, I range. Exploring the interaction, PROCESS Model 1 shows
think they would be disappointed to meet me” (seven-point, effects of BMI on anticipated social devaluation in the con-
“strongly disagree” and “strongly agree”; randomized; a ¼ .67; trasting (b ¼ .07, t ¼ 4.35, p < .001) and shared conditions (b
Shrauger and Schoeneman 1979). The woman in the images ¼ .02, t ¼ 1.75, p ¼ .08); it is not significant in the control
from McFerran et al. (2009) was of Asian descent, so we con- condition (b ¼ .02, t ¼ 1.32, p ¼ .19). Looked at another way,
trolled for gender and race.5 when BMI is low (1SD), the conditions are not significantly
different from each other (contrasting vs. control, t ¼ .37, p ¼
.71), (shared vs. control: t ¼ .15, p ¼ .88), (contrasting vs.
Study 2: Results and Discussion shared: t ¼ .22, p ¼ .83). When BMI is high (þ1SD), the
Interest in high return–risk product6. An analysis of covariance shared and control conditions are not significantly different (t
(ANCOVA) on interest in the high return-risk product indicates ¼ .04, p ¼ .97); the contrasting condition is significantly
significant BMI (F(1, 388) ¼ 4.32, p ¼ .04) and audience cue greater than the control (t ¼ 2.54, p ¼ .01) and shared (t ¼
(F(2, 388) ¼ 4.68, p ¼ .01) main effects. Importantly, the 2.84, p ¼ .005) conditions.
predicted two-way interaction also is significant (F(2, 388) ¼ Moderated mediation. We examine the mediating role of antici-
6.13, p ¼ .002), see Figure 3A. An omnibus groups regions of pated social devaluation on interest in the high return–risk
significance (OGRS) analysis locates significant JN regions for product (Process Model 107; Hayes 2015). The independent
the effect of a multicategorical variable and continuous vari- variable is BMI, the moderator is audience attribute cues, the
able interaction (Hayes and Montoya 2017). The OGRS JN mediator is anticipated social devaluation, and the dependent
point is at BMI  29.12 (p ¼ .05), a point within the over- variable is interest in the high return–risk product. Supporting
weight category that is approaching the obese category (see H1c, when the audience cue features a consumer with an
Figure 3, Panel A). To examine the two-way interaction fur- unshared, contrasting body type to the participant, anticipated
ther, we use PROCESS Model 1. Notably, the effect of BMI on social devaluation mediates (a  b ¼ .0105, 95% confidence
interest in the high return-risk product is only significant in the interval [CI]: .0041, .0189). It does not mediate when the audi-
unshared, contrasting weight condition (Contrasting: b ¼ .04, t ence cue signals a shared BMI (a  b ¼ .0036; 95% CI:
¼ 3.62, p < .001; Shared: b ¼ .02, t ¼ 1.58, p ¼ .11; Control: b .0007, .0094) or in the control condition (a  b ¼ .0032,
¼ –.02, t ¼ 1.39, p ¼ .16) supporting H1b. To look at this 95% CI: .0012, .0119).
another way, we ran spotlight analysis using planned contrasts The findings in Study 2 suggest that, when consumers’
(control vs. shared; control vs. contrasting; contrasting vs. BMIs are potentially stigmatizing, a popular approach that
shared). When BMI reaches the CDC designation of obese firms use to advertise health facilities (i.e., depicting thin,
(BMI ¼ 30), the shared (F(1, 388) ¼ 3.56, p ¼ .06) and con- aspirational models), may increase their interest in risky prod-
trasting (F(1, 388) ¼ 7.57, p < .01) condition produce greater ucts that promise to alleviate their stigma (e.g., weight-loss
interest in the high return–risk product, relative to the control surgery). In Study 1 we test ambiguous audience cues, which
condition. At higher levels of the obese category (BMI ¼ 37.5), potentially enable inferences about onlookers that vary from
the contrasting condition produces greater interest in the high anticipating others who share or who contrast their stigmatizing
return–risk product than the shared condition ((F(1,388) ¼ condition (Einhorn and Hogarth 1985); in Study 2 we provide a
2.87, p < .09; see Figure 3). contrasting audience cue that should clarify these inferences.
Anticipated social devaluation. An ANCOVA on anticipated Study 2 also provides evidence that a potential stigma can make
social devaluation indicates a significant BMI main effect consumers more sensitive to visual as well as verbal (Study 1)
(F(1, 388) ¼ 18.46, p < .001); the audience cue main effect audience cues.
is not significant (F(2, 388) ¼ 1.79, p ¼ .17). The two-way

Study 3: Manipulating Exposure of the


5
Neither the interest in high return-risk products model nor the anticipated Potentially Stigmatizing Attribute to an
devaluation model showed effects of race (F < 1) and gender (F < 1). When we
removed control variables, the interaction for high-risk return products was Audience
significant (F(2, 390) ¼ 6.28, p ¼ .002), and the interaction for anticipated In Study 2, the shared audience attribute cue produced more
devaluation was marginally significant (F(2, 390) ¼ 2.87, p ¼ .058).
6
As a demonstration of robustness, we averaged participants’ interest in all
interest in detrimental consumption than when the visual audi-
three surgical procedures, which were all perceived as relatively risky (above ence attribute cues were removed (i.e., the control condition),
the scale midpoint, but varied in their perceived effectiveness and consumers’
familiarity with the product). The two-way ANOVA for this risky product
7
interest index reveals main effects of BMI (F(1, 389) ¼ 4.35, p ¼ .04) and We chose Model 10 to accommodate the multinomial nature of our audience
audience cues (F(2, 389) ¼ 4.68, p ¼ .01), and a significant interaction (F(2, cue conditions, which were dummy coded with the control condition as the
389) ¼ 3.57, p < .01). reference category.
Harmeling et al. 235

A: Interest in High Return–Risk Product


2.25 As BMI approaches potentially stigmatizing levels, participants who viewed the
marketing communication with the contrasting audience cue, relative to

Return–Risk Product
participants who were in the control condition (BMI = 30.0) or who viewed the
Interest in High 2.00 shared audience cue (BMI=37.5), were more interested in a high risk (e.g.,
bodily harm) product that may help manage the potentially
stigmatizing attribute (e.g., body weight).
1.75

1.50

1.25

1.00
15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50
Underweight Normal Overweight Obese Morbidly Obese

BMI Level (Potential Stigma Source)

3.25 B: Anticipated Social Devaluation


As BMI approaches potentially stigmatizing levels
Social Devaluation

3.00 (JN = 29.36), participants who viewed the


marketing communication with the contrasting
Anticipated

audience cue anticipated significantly greater risks


2.75 of social devaluation from the potential audience
than those who were in the control condition
or viewed the shared audience cue.
2.50

2.25

15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50
Underweight Normal Overweight Obese Morbidly Obese

BMI Level (Potential Stigma Source)

Spotlight Analysis of Planned Contrasts at Different Levels of BMI for Interest in High Return–Risk Products

BMI (CDC Categorization) Control vs. Shared Control vs. Contrasting Shared vs. Contrasting
15.0 (underweight) F(1, 388)=1.51, p = .22 F(1, 388)= 5.83, p = .02 F(1, 388)=1.60, p = .21
17.5 (underweight) F(1, 388)=1.05, p = .31 F(1, 388)= 4.49, p = .04 F(1, 388)=1.30, p = .25
20.0 (normal) F(1, 388)=0.52, p = .47 F(1, 388)= 2.76, p = .10 F(1, 388)=0.91, p = .34
22.5 (normal) F(1, 388)=0.07, p = .79 F(1, 388)= 0.87, p = .35 F(1, 388)=0.43, p = .51
25.0 (overweight) F(1, 388)=0.16, p = .69 F(1, 388)= 0.04, p = .85 F(1,388)=0.04, p = .84
27.5 (overweight) F(1, 388)=1.44, p = .23 F(1, 388)= 2.51, p = .11 F(1, 388)=0.15, p = .70
30.0 (obese) F(1, 388)=3.56, p = .06 F(1, 388)= 7.57, p < .01 F(1, 388)=0.93, p = .34
32.5 (obese) F(1, 388)=5.16, p = .02 F(1, 388)=11.55, p < .01 F(1, 388)=1.87, p = .17
35.0 (obese) F(1, 388)=5.88, p = .02 F(1, 388)=13.55, p < .01 F(1, 388)=2.51, p = .11
37.5 (obese) F(1, 388)=6.09, p = .01 F(1, 388)=14.36, p < .01 F(1, 388)=2.87, p = .09
40.0 (morbidly obese) F(1, 388)=6.08, p = .01 F(1, 388)=14.63, p < .01 F(1, 388)=3.06, p = .08
42.5 (morbidly obese) F(1, 388)=5.99, p = .02 F(1, 388)=14.65, p < .01 F(1, 388)=3.16, p = .08
45.0 (morbidly obese) F(1, 388)=5.87, p = .02 F(1, 388)=14.58, p < .01 F(1, 388)=3.21, p = .07

Figure 3. Study 2 results: Johnson–Neyman analysis of audience cues by BMI (stigma source).
Notes: BMI ¼ body mass index. Standards for defining stigma levels within BMI come from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, https://www.cdc.gov/
obesity/adult/defining.html. OGRS Johnson–Neyman points of significance (p < .05) are marked by the shaded areas.

suggesting any information about the focal attribute (even suggests it is not general visibility to onlookers that affects
information suggesting attribute similarity with audience mem- behavior, but exposure of a consumer’s stigmatizing attribute
bers) may elicit inferences of threats. An audience implies to others. Thus, factors that reduce exposure of this attribute
some degree of public exposure to others. Yet, our theory could render an audience irrelevant and eliminate the effects. In
236 Journal of Marketing Research 58(2)

Study 3, we manipulate exposure of the potentially stigmatiz- group” (a ¼ .95): anonymity: “People would not know exactly
ing attribute while holding the audience cue constant to test our who I am”; similarity: “People in this group are likely very
theory. We focus again on online health care offers, a context in similar to me”; and effectiveness: “Joining this group would
which typically nonconcealable attributes (e.g., body weight) be an effective means of managing my weight” (all seven-point
become concealable. Thus far, we have focused on negative scales anchored by “strongly disagree and strongly agree”;
inferences (social devaluation), but when managing stigmatiz- random order). The results show that the exposed condition
ing attributes, consumers may also make positive inferences, results in significantly greater feelings of weight exposure
with implications for consumption behaviors. In Studies 3 and (MExposed ¼ 5.48, MNotExposed ¼ 2.86, F(1, 79) ¼ 62.34, p <
4 we look for evidence of more positive inferences (e.g., antici- .001). We observed no difference in perceptions of the effort
pated empowerment and benevolent intentions, respectively) as involved (MExposed ¼ 3.82, MNotExposed ¼3.56, F < 1), effec-
consumption drivers. tiveness (MExposed ¼ 4.78, MNotExposed ¼ 5.08, F < 1), or level
of anonymity (MExposed ¼ 4.70, MNotExposed ¼ 4.98, F < 1),
which rules out the possibility that participants perceived one
Study 3: Design, Participants, and Procedure workshop required more effort to join than the other, one was
more effective than the other, or one was more anonymous than
In Study 3, again using BMI to capture the level of stigma the other. There was no difference in perceived similarity (MEx-
associated with participants’ body weight, we test whether
posed ¼ 4.55, MNotExposed ¼ 4.56, F < 1); thus, we effectively
marketing messages that signal body weight exposure will alter held the audience constant.
consumers’ responses when their weight is potentially stigma-
tizing, as a means to corroborate the prioritization of stigma- Procedures. Over the course of three days, 389 paid U.S.
relevant audience information among these consumers. With a MTurk participants (196 women, MAge ¼ 39.16 years; MBMI
2 (body weight exposure: yes, no) between  measured (BMI: ¼ 26.47) participated in the experiment.8 First, participants
stigma source) design, holding audience cues constant, we test provided demographics and their body weight and height,
anticipated social devaluation and anticipated empowerment as which we used to calculate their BMI. Two days later, the
mediators. We manipulated body weight exposure as follows participants were randomly assigned to assess marketing
[exposed/not exposed]: communications about a health care program (here, a work-
shop) that focused on benefits to the participants. The audi-
We would like your opinion on a new online health workshop. ence cues remained constant across all conditions, indicating
Below are the instructions for how individuals sign up for the “people similar to you.”
program. To Join the Program: The website will use [a full body After reviewing the marketing communication, participants
photo of you to create an exact silhouette (outline) of your body to indicated their likelihood to enroll, on the following items: “I
serve as your avatar / an image you select to serve as your graphic
would like to enroll in this program,” “I would like to join this
logo]. To protect your identity and anonymity, this is used instead
program,” and “I would like to sign up for emails from this
of showing the actual photo of you. The software will use [your full
program” (seven-point scale anchored by “strongly disagree”
body photo to create a silhouette of your body / this line art image],
to represent you when you interact with other members of the
and “strongly agree”; a ¼ .96). We further isolated how much
community. To join: Please upload your photo, then enter the risk participants are willing to assume to quickly alleviate the
following: [Weight, Height, Age, Gender. /Age, Gender.] The stigma by directly measuring their comfort with a given level of
other people in the community will see your: [Silhouette of your risk for a particular return on products recommended through
body based on your photo, Height, Weight, Age, Gender. / Line art the workshop. To this end, we asked the following questions:
graphic of you, Age, Gender.] [A corresponding graphic with “Imagine you could have an Outpatient Medical Procedure
“Support Group” in the header, also showed a red arrow with either (Daily Medication for Three Months). The procedure (medica-
“Your Photo Here. Upload a photo of you . . . ” or “Get Started. tion) is designed to get you your ideal body. Please indicate
Create an account . . . ” to begin sharing and receiving support in a which option you prefer . . . (0) I would not try this product, (1)
nurturing setting. See graphic in Web Appendix D.] 10% chance of success, 5% chance of minor side effects, 1%
chance of serious side effects – (9) 90% chance of success, 45%
Manipulation test. As a test of our manipulations, we randomly chance of minor side effects, 9% chance of serious side effects”
assigned 80 paid U.S. MTurk participants (48 women, MAge ¼ (at each scale point, all percentages were increased by 10%,
35.53 years) to view stimuli from one of the two conditions 5%, and 1%, respectively; a ¼ .84; e.g., Tversky and Kahne-
(exposed vs. not exposed). Next, they responded to items man 1981). We captured anticipated devaluation using the fol-
designed to measure how exposed they would feel: When inter- lowing items: “Thinking about what it would be like to join this
acting with people in this group: “ . . . people could see my group, I feel that I will be negatively judged,” “If I joined this
body”; “ . . . people would know my weight; my weight would
be visible and exposed”; “ . . . other people in the group could 8
Five hundred fifty-two participants completed the first part; we deleted 10
see what physical shape I am in”; “ . . . people could judge my participants because they indicated (impossible) BMIs of < 7, and 389 of them
weight” (a ¼ .96). We also examined effort to join: “It would completed the second part two days later. The average BMIs across both parts
take a lot of effort / time / energy / work for me to join this were highly consistent (MT1 ¼ 26.30; MT2 ¼ 26.47).
Harmeling et al. 237

group, I would feel embarrassed about my weight,” If I joined maximum likelihood estimation in AMOS. Our overall struc-
this group, I would be humiliated by my weight,” and “If I tural model exhibited high goodness of fit (w2(92) ¼ 171.49, p
joined this group, I would feel distressed about my weight,” < .01; comparative fit index ¼ .98; incremental fit index ¼ .98;
(seven-point scale anchored by “strongly disagree” and Tucker–Lewis index ¼ .97; root mean square error of approx-
“strongly agree”; a ¼ .91, Blair and Roese 2013). They also imation ¼ .05; standardized root mean square residual ¼ .05).
indicated anticipated empowerment: “My experiences with my The moderating effect of body weight exposure cues is positive
weight could be a source of insight for other people in this and significant on the relationship between BMI and antici-
group,” “I could really help others in this program by sharing pated devaluation (b ¼ .11, p ¼ .04). The path between antici-
my personal experiences with my weight,” and “My personal pated devaluation and likelihood to enroll was not significant
history with my weight gives me a unique advantage to help (b ¼ .05, p ¼ .26) but was significant for interest in high risk-
others in this program” (seven-point scale anchored by reward products (b ¼ .24, p < .001). The path between antici-
“strongly disagree” and “strongly agree”; a ¼ .91, based on pated empowerment and likelihood to enroll (b ¼ .52, p ¼
Shih 2004). .001) was significant. It was also significant for interest in high
Consumption decisions involve a mix of inferences that risk–reward products (b ¼ .16, p ¼ .01) (see Table 3 and
include how consumers feel others will judge them, how the Figure 4).
offering may benefit their self-concept, and how they perceive
the marketer’s intentions and their persuasion attempts (Fries- Moderated mediation analysis. To test whether the BMI by expo-
tad and Wright 1994). We capture the former in our measures sure cue interaction on consumption is mediated by antici-
of anticipated devaluation and empowerment, but it may be pated devaluation and/or anticipated empowerment, we
that when consumers anticipate devaluation by others, they estimate conditional indirect effects in AMOS using Hayes’s
may also become more skeptical of the ad. Therefore, we also (2015) recommendations. This allows us to test the indirect
capture consumers’ assessments of the manipulative intent of effect of BMI (stigma source) through social devaluation
the ad: “The advertiser tried to manipulate the audience in ways dependent on whether the participant’s weight was exposed
I don’t like,” and “I was annoyed by this flyer because the while simultaneously estimating the effect of anticipated
advertiser seemed to be trying to inappropriately control the empowerment (and vice versa), which is essential when
consumer audience” (seven-point scale anchored by “strongly investigating simultaneous mediators with proposed opposing
disagree” and “strongly agree”; a ¼ .89, Campbell 1995). We effects. When BMI is at potentially stigmatizing levels
also included exploratory measures of guilt and shame; see (þ1SD, BMI ¼ 34.32), weight exposure (versus no exposure)
Web Appendix D for the results and Web Appendix E for list increases anticipated devaluation, which then increases inter-
of variables measured. est in high risk–reward products (a  b ¼ .74, 95% CI: [.139,
1.69]). At low levels (1SD BMI ¼ 18.62), the effects are
weaker (a  b ¼ .44, 95% CI: [.092, 1.02]), providing evi-
Study 3: Results and Discussion dence for H1c. The other mediation effects are nonsignificant
To test both beneficial consumption (joining a wellness (see Table 3).
program) and detrimental consumption (interest in high
risk–reward products) while also modeling both proposed Exploring the role of manipulative intent as a distal mediator. In this
mechanisms (anticipated devaluation, empowerment), we study, anticipated social devaluation does not reduce likelihood
simultaneously estimated a model in AMOS. This method (1) that participants would join the wellness program. One possi-
accommodates multiple mediators and dependent variables, (2) bility for this surprising finding is a spillover effect, such that
enables us to estimate the complex relationships in the model consumers’ beliefs that the audience will be unfavorable
simultaneously, and (3) is robust to the nonnormality of multi- toward them spill over to create suspicion of the advertisers’
plicative terms for testing interactions. As a first step, we con- intentions, which then might impact their likelihood to join. We
ducted confirmatory factor analyses for all multi-item tested this explanation with an alternative model that includes
constructs in our empirical model. The results indicated good participants’ assessments of manipulative intent of the ad as a
overall fit (w2(38) ¼ 70.49, p ¼ .001; comparative fit index ¼ distal mediator such that, conditional on the presence of body
.99; incremental fit index ¼ .96; Tucker–Lewis index ¼ .99; weight exposure cues in the advertisement for the wellness
root mean square error of approximation ¼ .05; standardized program, BMI is expected to influence anticipated social deva-
root mean square residual ¼ .02). All standardized factor load- luation, which then influences perceptions of the marketer’s
ings were .64 or greater and statistically significant at p < .001. manipulative intent to ultimately affect likelihood to enroll.
The model exhibited high internal consistency, with average Indeed, we find a significant moderated mediation effect for
variances extracted (AVEs) from .70 to .92. The AVE for each the highest-order interaction for likelihood to enroll (a  b  c
factor was greater than its squared correlation with any other ¼ .004, 90% CI: [.011, .001]). Body weight exposure is
factor, in support of discriminant validity (see Web Appendix more relevant to consumers’ likelihood to enroll when consu-
D for descriptives). mers’ BMIs are highly stigmatizing, and this effect is mediated
To determine the statistical significance of the parameter by anticipated devaluation, which impacts their assessments of
estimates, we simultaneously estimated the joint effects using manipulative intent of the ad (þ1SD, BMI ¼ 34.32) (a  b  c
238 Journal of Marketing Research 58(2)

Table 3. Study 3 Results: Effects of Stigma and Audience Cues on Beneficial and Detrimental Consumption.
Panel A: Structural Model ␤ t-Value p-Value
Moderating Effects of Stigma Exposure Cues
Consumer BMI (potential stigma source)  weight exposure cue ! anticipated social devaluation .11* 2.08 .04
Consumer BMI (potential stigma source)  weight exposure cue ! anticipated empowerment –.01 .15 .89
Effects of Mediating Mechanisms on Beneficial and Detrimental Consumption Behaviors
Consumer anticipated social devaluation ! likelihood to enroll in health care program .05 1.14 .26
Consumer anticipated social devaluation ! interest in high return-risk products .24** 4.16 .001
Consumer anticipated empowerment ! likelihood to enroll in health care program .52** 10.77 .001
Consumer anticipated empowerment ! interest in high return-risk products .16** 2.82 .01
Effects of Stigmatizing Attribute on Mediating Mechanisms
Consumer BMI (potential stigma source) ! consumer anticipated social devaluation .11* 2.13 .03
Consumer BMI (potential stigma source) ! consumer anticipated empowerment –.02 .29 .77
Controls
Weight exposure cue ! anticipated social devaluation .06 1.12 .27
Weight exposure cue ! anticipated empowerment –.12* 2.36 .02
Consumer gender ! anticipated social devaluation –.06 1.18 .24
Consumer gender ! anticipated empowerment –.14** 2.76 .01
Consumer race ! anticipated social devaluation –.16** 3.10 .01
Consumer race ! anticipated empowerment –.18** 3.39 .01
Weight exposure cue ! likelihood to enroll in health care program –.18** 4.05 .001
Weight exposure cue ! high return-risk product interest –.02 .46 .67
Consumer gender ! likelihood to enroll in health care program .03 .68 .50
Consumer gender ! high return-risk product interest –.05 .28 .78
Consumer race ! likelihood to enroll in health care program –.11** 2.53 .01
Consumer race ! high return-risk product interest –.18** 3.23 .001
Panel B: Moderated Mediation Analysis ␤ LLCI ULCI
Index of Moderated Mediation of Conditional Indirect Effects of Stigma on Consumption Based on Stigma Exposure Cues
Consumer BMI (potential stigma source)  weight exposure cue ! anticipated social devaluation ! likelihood .002 –.001 .011
to enroll in health care program
Consumer BMI (potential stigma source)  weight exposure cue ! anticipated social devaluation ! interest .019 .003 .046
in high return-risk products
Consumer BMI (potential stigma source)  weight exposure cue ! anticipated empowerment ! likelihood –.002 –.020 .020
to enroll in health care program
Consumer BMI (potential stigma source)  weight exposure cue ! anticipated empowerment ! interest in –.001 –.012 .010
high return-risk products
R2 for anticipated social devaluation .05
R2 for anticipated empowerment .08
R2 for likelihood to enroll in health care program .36
R2 for interest in high return–risk products .15

Notes: BMI ¼ body mass index; b represents standardized path coefficient. N ¼ 389. Model fit: w2(92) ¼ 171.49, p < .001; comparative fit index ¼ .98; incremental
fit index ¼ .98; Tucker–Lewis index ¼ .97; root mean square error of approximation ¼ .05; standardized root mean square residual ¼ .05.
*p < .05.
**p < .01.

¼ .22, 90% CI: [.021, .681]). This effect is significantly weaker This study provides further validation that the effects we
at low levels (1SD, BMI ¼ 18.62) (a  b  c ¼ .13, 90% CI: observe are not just audience effects, but “stigma audience”
[.013, .396]).9 effects, which require exposure of the potentially stigmatizing
attribute to others. When we hold the audience cue constant,
consumers with potentially stigmatizing BMIs respond less
9
All hypothesized effects remain marginally significant (p ¼ .06) when we favorably to marketing communications that signal their body
removed covariates from the model. As a robustness test, we also examined
the serial mediation with interest in high return–risk products. We observed a
will be exposed to an audience during consumption, relative to
significant moderated mediation effect for the highest interaction when testing those that are not exposed. Consumers with weights at levels
BMI’s effect on interest in high risk–reward products through anticipated that are not stigmatizing respond indifferently to weight expo-
devaluation and then manipulative intent moderated by body weight exposure sure or no weight exposure.
cues (a  b  c ¼ .009, 90% CI: [.023, .001]). Anticipated devaluation and The results provide additional support for our theorizing and
its impact on manipulative intent exerted a stronger mediating effect when BMI
reached a potentially stigmatizing level (þ1SD, BMI ¼ 34.32) (a  b  c ¼ .12,
a more holistic view of consumers’ decision-making processes
90% CI: [.025, .328]) and a weaker effect at low levels (1SD BMI ¼ 18.62) (a that includes assessments of both opportunities and threats
 b  c ¼ .18, 90% CI: [.024, .535]). relative to the stigmatizing attribute. Consumers with
Harmeling et al. 239

Marketing
Communication Characteristic

Body weight
exposure cues
(exposed, not)

Consumer Inferences Consumption Outcomes

.11*
.05 Beneficial Consumption
Consumer anticipated
.11* Consumer likelihood to enroll in
social devaluation
.52** health care program

Consumer BMI
(Stigma source; .24**
−.01
High, low) Detrimental Consumption
−.02
Consumer
anticipated empowerment .16**
Consumer high return–risk
purchase interest

Figure 4. Study 3 results: effects of stigma on consumers’ beneficial and detrimental responses to marketing communications.
Notes: All effects represent the standardized coefficients. Other effects tested in the model were body weight exposure cues, gender, and race as controls on both
mediators and outcomes.
*p ¼ .05.
**p ¼ .01.

potentially stigmatizing weight generate audience inferences, help . . . ,” and “support in a nurturing setting”) that may have
which in Study 3 were amplified when their weight was guided participants’ inferences about the benevolence of the
exposed to others; the main requirement for an “audience” to audiences’ intentions toward them. Theoretically, this would
exist. This finding cannot be explained by homophily alone, have skewed consumers to infer more benevolent intentions of
which would predict consistent effects across all conditions the potential audience than if these words were not present.
since similarity is held constant (Naylor, Lamberton, and Nor- Yet, even with these benevolent words we find consistent evi-
ton 2011). It provides evidence that anticipated social devalua- dence that consumers inferences of social devaluation vary
tion mediates the effect of a stigma on consumer responses to based on the audience attribute cue they view.
marketing communications. Although empowerment seemed In Study 4, we examine our proposed process further by
unaffected by whether the focal attribute was visible to others, testing whether more direct audience intention cues (i.e., ben-
it emerged as the primary determinant of enrollment intentions. evolent words vs. neutral word choices) are necessary for con-
sumers to make favorable assessments of audience attribute
cues. If removing these direct intention cues in conditions when
Study 4: Test of Process by Moderation, the attribute cue would signal a more favorable audience
Manipulating Audience Intentions (shared attribute cue) reduces consumers positive response,
then this suggests that for consumers managing stigmas, their
Audience attribute cues—explicitly defining the potential audi-
main priorities for identifying similarity to the audience is to
ence as possessing the same disease or not (Study 1), photos of
estimate people’s intentions of them, providing further support
a thin or heavy co-consumer (Study 2)—provide a baseline
of H1c.
from which consumers can estimate an audience’s intentions
in light of the stigmatizing attribute. These inferences inher-
ently contain some level of uncertainty that likely affects deci-
sion making. As demonstrated in Study 2, if a consumer is
Study 4: Design, Participants, and Procedure
managing a stigmatizing attribute, even if an audience cue In Study 4 we partnered with the same health care firm from
signals a shared attribute, they may show greater interest in Study 1 that provided access to consumers diagnosed with a
detrimental behaviors. This may be due to the remaining ambi- variety of diseases (e.g., HIV, cancer, rheumatoid arthritis). We
guity associated with potentially hidden intentions of the audi- analyze the click-through effectiveness of various marketing
ence. In each of the previous studies, all audience attribute cues communications, contingent on the degree to which a consu-
were accompanied by word choices (Study 1: “community of mer’s disease is stigmatizing. We employed a 2 (audience
people who care . . . I look forward to welcoming you”; Study attribute cue: shared vs. unshared, ambiguous)  2 (audience
2: “friendly” staff members; Study 3: “Support Group,” “Get intention cue: benevolent vs. neutral)  measured (disease-
240 Journal of Marketing Research 58(2)

related stigma) design. For the field experiment, we assigned others”; “ . . . helping themselves / helping each other”;
participants to conditions using stratified random sampling by “ . . . learning from others / connecting with others”; and
disease, to ensure each disease was equally likely to appear in “ . . . exchanging insider information / providing support and
each experimental condition. The sample includes all 970 con- encouragement”; randomized, nine-point bipolar; a ¼ .87).
sumers who received and opened an email solicitation from the Participants also responded to the audience attribute cue scale
company that promoted the online health care program. (“The person sending the email seems: “ . . . like a person who
Field study participants received an email with the company has the same illness”; “ . . . like someone with the same con-
name and logo, in which we manipulated audience attribute dition”; “ . . . like a company expert” (R); “ . . . like someone
cues (email sender as either a community member suffering who works for the company” (R); randomized, 1 ¼ “strongly
from a disease (shared) or a manager of the health care program disagree,” and 7 ¼ “strongly agree”; a ¼ .84).
(unshared, ambiguous). We manipulated audience intentions An ANOVA on the audience intention cue index reveals
cues as benevolent or neutral. We manipulated audience attri- an intention type main effect (MBenevolent ¼ 7.25, MNeutral ¼
bute cue [shared / unshared, ambiguous] shown here with ita- 6.07; F(1, 97) ¼ 14.94, p < .001). The audience attribute
lics and audience intention cue [benevolent/neutral] shown cue main effect and the interaction are not significant
here in square brackets. The email read: (Fs < 1). An ANOVA on the audience attribute cue index
reveals an audience attribute main effect (MShared ¼ 4.37,
Thanks for signing up to learn about opportunities to [connect with MAmbiguous ¼ 3.38; F(1, 97) ¼ 16.15, p < .001). The audi-
others / exchange information] about your personal health experi- ence intention cue main effect (p ¼ .25) and the interaction
ences. As [a member of the / the Community Manager for] (Com- (F < 1) are not significant.
pany) community, I’m excited to announce that the new
(Company) online [platform / community] is now up and running!
The online [community / platform] is a place where people can Study 4: Results and Discussion
[connect with a supportive community / exchange information and A logistic regression analysis of click-through behaviors
insights on how] to treat, manage, and cope with (recipient’s con- includes stigma, audience attribute cues, audience intention
dition). The [community / platform] offers many ways—such as
cues, and their higher-order interactions. We find the pre-
groups, chats, polls, blogs, and more—for you to connect with
dicted three-way interaction (Wald w2 ¼ 4.76, p ¼ .03; Fig-
people who [can relate and empathize with / have experience and
ure 5); the other effects in the model are not significant (ps >
information about] what you’re going through. As [a member
.59). As expected, for low stigma diseases (1 SD), the audi-
myself / the Community Manager] I wanted to personally invite
you to join. By being among the first to join (Company) commu- ence attribute  audience intention simple interaction effect is
nity, you’ll have the opportunity to shape the experience and create nonsignificant (a  b ¼ .11, z ¼ .53, p ¼ .60). For highly
a [community of people who care / platform for learning from stigmatizing diseases (þ1 SD), the audience attribute  audi-
others]. In sum: (Company) Community is a [supportive commu- ence intention simple interaction is significant (a  b ¼ .67,
nity focused on / great source for the inside scoop about] (recipi- z ¼ 2.69, p ¼ .007).
ent’s condition). Getting started is easy—just click here or on the We test the interaction between the four combinations of
button below. I look forward to getting to know you! audience-based communications (shared–benevolent, shared–
(Name), [(Recipient’s condition) Patient / Community Manager] neutral, ambiguous–benevolent, ambiguous–neutral) and
stigma. We identify the point at which the omnibus interaction
We captured stigma with the physicians’ ratings from Study becomes significant, namely, when stigma  2.86 (Hayes and
1. The dependent variable is the percentage of participants who Montoya 2017). Consumers with highly stigmatizing diseases
clicked on the Internet link in the invitation email to visit the (þ1 SD) who receive shared–benevolent audience cues (M ¼
online health care program’s website, which we label click- 26.67%) click significantly more often than the other groups
throughs. (average M ¼ 15.66%; z ¼ 2.69, p ¼ .007), as shown in
Figure 5. These results emerge from a highly conservative test,
Manipulation test. To test our manipulations, we randomly because we only changed 9 of 193 words in the audience attri-
assigned 101 participants (MTurk, MAge ¼ 36.91 years, 59 bute cue manipulation in this real-world field experiment, and
women) to one of the four conditions and asked them to actual patients used their own email accounts, which provides
“imagine you are suffering from an illness that you feel impacts additional support for H1c.
your life in a meaningful way. At your doctor’s office, you Consumers with stigmatizing diseases click on a marketing
provided your email address to join a mailing list about the communication more when the message source (1) also pos-
condition. You receive the following invitation on email.” sesses a stigmatizing disease (shared audience cue) and (2)
After reading the email (from the main study), participants provides cues of the benevolent intentions of the potential
indicated the extent to which they perceived the audience inten- audience; rather than the same benevolent intention cues asso-
tion cue to be benevolent (“The people in this group seem more ciated with an ambiguous audience and shared audiences asso-
focused on” “ . . . a selfish intent / a benevolent intent”; ciated with neutral intentions. We find no effects for consumers
“ . . . what they can get out of the group / what they could give with less stigmatizing diseases. These findings provide addi-
to others in the group”; “ . . . caring for themselves / caring for tional support that the effects we observe are due to consumers’
Harmeling et al. 241

Shared audience attribute/


28% benevolent intentions As disease-related stigma increases
(greater than 2.86), marketing
26% communications only generate
significantly higher click-throughs
24% when a shared audience cue is
presented and the intentions of the
Click-Through Rate (%)

22% audience are also explicitly described


as benevolent.
20% Ambiguous audience attribute/
intentions not stated
18%

16% There is no difference among


Shared audience attribute/
these three conditions.
intention not stated
14%

12% Ambiguous audience attribute/


benevolent intentions
10%
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Disease-Related Stigma

Figure 5. Study 4 results (field experiment): analysis of audience attribute cues and audience intention cues by stigma.
Notes: The shaded area indicates the point at which the omnibus analysis of variance becomes significant on the continuous measure of stigma (stigma ¼ 2.86).

inferences of audiences’ intentions and not similarity alone. Stigma, consumer power, and status. Prior work often focuses on
These findings also illustrate a “zero miss” strategy for estimat- how consumers improve their status and power (e.g., Dion and
ing intentions of anticipated audiences among consumers man- Borraz 2017) but overlooks the inferior status that stigma can
aging potential stigma in which ambiguity in cues (e.g., impose on them (Coskuner-Balli and Thompson 2013). By
ambiguous–benevolent) “trigger vigilance for discrimination” definition, stigma implies an undesirable position as “the
(Major and O’Brien 2005, p. 403). They provide guidance to other” in a social hierarchy, and power rests with those without
managers on how to improve responsiveness among consumers the stigma (i.e., the audience). Integrating our findings across
managing potential stigmas. studies, we identify a continuum of threat inferences that con-
sumers managing stigmas make, based on the cues available to
deduce the power differential between them and a potential
audience. This continuum ranges from low threat, shared audi-
General Discussion
ence attribute cues to high threat, unshared (ambiguous or con-
Our studies illustrate a robust interactive effect between a con- trasting) audience attribute cues. It shows how distinct
sumer’s potentially stigmatizing attribute and audience cues in audience cues might (inadvertently) undermine consumer
marketing communications: certain cues fuel fears of social well-being, and it helps reveal a process by which consumers
devaluation, which decreases beneficial consumption (e.g., aim to manage power differentials in the marketplace: they first
click-throughs, enrollment intentions, engagement within a use stigma-relevant audience cues to infer their status relative
wellness program) and increases detrimental consumption to a potential audience and then avoid or approach consump-
(high reward–risk products). These findings point to the need tion encounters. This is important for marketers to understand,
for more nuanced theorizing to explain consumer-marketing as marketing communications often deliberately emphasize
concepts that may operate differently among consumers man- negative deviations in stigma-related campaigns (see Web
aging potentially stigmatizing personal attributes; as such, they Appendix F for examples).
enrich marketing theory on stigma, inform managers, and offer
avenues for further research.
Stigma, inferred audiences, and homophily. Homophily suggests
that people tend to seek out those who are “similar” under the
assumption of a reciprocal sense of “liking” (McPherson,
Theoretical Contributions Smith-Lovin, and Cook 2001). We see some evidence of this,
Our findings directly respond to recent calls for marketing but instead of the expected main effects of audience cues that
research on stigma (e.g., Chaney, Sanchez, and Maimon homophily would predict, we observe an interaction by the
2019; Lamberton 2019; Mirabito et al. 2016; Wooten and stigma potential of a given attribute (e.g., disease, BMI). We
Rank-Christman 2019). They also contribute to research on find systematic distinctions between consumers who (do not)
power, status, and the judgments of others in the marketplace have a potentially stigmatizing attribute in how they assess
(Ratner and Hamilton 2015); homophily (McPherson, Smith- similarity and how that affects decision making. Consumers
Lovin, and Cook 2001) and consumer empowerment; and cus- prioritize the potential stigma over other typical sources of
tomer engagement (Harmeling et al. 2017). similarity (e.g., gender, race) (Studies 2 and 3); notably, the
242 Journal of Marketing Research 58(2)

same attribute (e.g., BMI) does not seem to provide a relevant attribute cues with benevolent audience intention cues (Study
point of similarity when the attribute is not stigmatizing. 4). This is particularly important because it counters the pre-
Thus, we contribute to research on the multidimensionality valent use of aspirational images in marketing campaigns,
of homophily (e.g., Block and Grund 2014; Lin and Lundquist which may inadvertently signal a contrasting audience (Study
2013) by showing that similarity must be calibrated to a 2). For example, a Google image search of “templates for gym
highly prioritized attribute—in this case, the stigmatizing advertisements” produces 502 images on just the first page,
attribute. 95% of which feature a physically fit model (see Web Appen-
We also identify a boundary condition in which mere simi- dix F), which in our studies motivated consumers with BMIs at
larity, even along the prioritized attribute, does not produce the stigmatizing levels to pursue potentially detrimental treatments
intended effects: in Study 2, we find that a shared attribute cue that promised to quickly alleviate the stigma. This illustration
produces more detrimental consumption than the control con- suggests not only that our findings are nonintuitive, but that
dition in which the visual attribute cue was removed, counter to managers also should consider carefully which information
homophily research. This, and the observations in Study 4 in appears in their marketing communications.
which we separate similarity (i.e., shared attribute cues) and
favorability (i.e., benevolent intention cue), suggests an impor- Designing offerings and policies with regard to stigma. Study 3
tant distinction from homophily. When consumers possess shows that requesting consumers to reveal their otherwise con-
potential stigmas, their primary concern may be to reduce cealable stigma, even to an audience that is presumably similar
threats rather than maximize benefits, and similarity is just a to them, can create detrimental effects. This has implications
proxy for assessing potential threats. for product and policy design. The more consistently a partic-
ular cue is tied to a stigma, the greater the risk that it becomes a
Stigma, empowerment, and consumer engagement. Stigmatizing stigma marker (Goffman 1963). This insight is crucial for prod-
attributes might be a source of empowerment (Corrigan, Kosy- ucts, consumption practices, and policies designed to reduce or
luk, and Rüsch 2013; Shih 2004). Findings in Study 3 suggest manage stigmatizing attributes, which might get systematically
that empowerment can affect consumption decisions. Study 1 linked to that stigma (Adkins and Ozanne 2005). For example,
also supports the idea that empowerment triggers downstream redeeming coupons or participating in public welfare programs
effects: we found that consumers with stigmatizing diseases might signal impoverishment, thereby insinuating a stigma of
who joined the health program after receiving a marketing poverty, whether it exists or not. The threat of being assigned a
communication that featured an ambiguous (vs. shared stigma) stigma, even if it does not apply, might reduce consumers’
audience attribute cue completed 52% fewer engagement beha- motivation to engage in that consumption (Argo and Main
viors (e.g., likes, posts, comments) in the two weeks after the 2008). Marketers should consider the potential for their product
invitation. Although more research is needed, these initial to become symbolic of a stigma, its visibility to others, and
insights point to stigma-related empowerment as a potential whether exposing stigmatizing personal attributes is necessary.
platform for a desirable win-win situation that benefits both They might seek to reduce a product’s visibility (e.g., Invisa-
stigmatized consumers and firms. lign for orthodontic issues; Wahl 2005) or minimize its asso-
ciation with the stigma (e.g., birth control pills disguised in
cases that look like cosmetic compacts; Gibbs 2010). Alterna-
Managerial and Policy Contributions tively, marketers could use stigma symbols strategically, to
Explicit descriptions of co-consumers, promotional images, signal acceptance in subtle ways that are undetectable by con-
and the message source in marketing communications all can sumers without the stigma (Chaney, Sanchez, and Maimon
function as audience cues relative to a potential stigma. There- 2019; Shavitt 2019). Our findings lend support to the idea of
fore, our findings offer direct applications to marketing man- having service staff (e.g., health care workers) who possess the
agement and public policy. same stigma as consumers in a focal program (e.g., HIV
patients may prefer programs where some health workers are
Stigma-related marketing and policy communications. Our findings also HIV positive; www.positiveimpacthealthcenters.org).
suggest three possible communication strategies for alleviating
the negative effects of stigma on consumption. First, as illu- Stigma and public policy communications. Our results are relevant
strated in Study 2, marketers can attempt to remove audience for institutions that influence, communicate, and apply public
attribute cues. Even ambiguous audience cues might lead con- policies related to stigma. These institutions (e.g., CDC, World
sumers managing stigmas to inferring greater threats, in stark Health Organization) are particularly legitimizing, and com-
contrast to consumers not managing stigmas, who are likely to munications emanating from them can quickly influence
use ambiguous cues about co-consumers to infer that an audi- beliefs of what is “normal” (Humphreys and Latour 2013). This
ence will be similar to them (Naylor, Lamberton, and Norton standard is the prerequisite for stigmas to emerge. We see
2011). Second, because audience cues are often unavoidable evidence of this in Studies 2 and 3, in which the effects emerge
(e.g., referral programs, group-based offerings), marketers can in line with the categories set by the CDC for abnormal BMIs.
provide signals that the stigmatizing attribute will be concealed Therefore, policy institutions might (inadvertently) affect
(Study 3). Third, marketers can combine shared audience whether and when a stigma emerges. Our research suggests
Harmeling et al. 243

that a requirement of a stigma is an audience that identifies Associate Editor


those bearing the stigma as the “other.” Clarifying misconcep- Katherine White
tions of links between a disease and other categorizations of
people (e.g., HIV/sexual orientation, HPV/women) may be an Declaration of Conflicting Interests
effective means of alleviating people’s assessments of “other” The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to
and reducing stigma. the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding
Limitations and Research Directions The author(s) received no financial support for the research, author-
ship, and/or publication of this article.
Our studies have limitations that point to opportunities for
further research. First, Study 1 showed an unexpected positive
effect of an ambiguous audience cue among consumers with References
less stigmatizing diseases. This might imply a U-shaped rela- Adkins, Natalie Ross and Julie L. Ozanne (2005), “The Low Literate
tionship between stigma and specific targeting (e.g., with no or Consumer,” Journal of Consumer Research, 32 (1), 93–105.
high stigma, specific targeting is more effective, but with low Argo, Jennifer J. and Kelley J. Main (2008), “Stigma by Association in
stigma, generic targeting is more effective); an intriguing idea Coupon Redemption: Looking Cheap Because of Others,” Journal
for further research. Second, Studies 2 and 3 point to another of Consumer Research, 35 (4), 559–72.
important topic: high-risk consumption by people with a stig- Blair, Sean and Neal J. Roese (2013), “Balancing the Basket: The Role
matizing attribute. From a consumer well-being lens, our find- of Shopping Basket Composition in Embarrassment,” Journal of
ing that a single advertising image can nudge stigmatized Consumer Research, 40 (4), 676–91.
consumers toward high-risk choices (e.g., surgical procedures) Block, Per and Thomas Grund (2014), “Multidimensional Homophily
is noteworthy, and more research on such negative implications in Friendship Networks,” Network Science, 2 (2), 189–212.
is needed. Third, Study 4 suggests that service providers are Brough, Aaron R., James E. B. Wilkie, M. A. Jingjing, Mathew S.
crucial audiences. Research should assess how service employ- Isaac, and David Gal (2016), “Is Eco-Friendly Unmanly? The
ees respond to consumer stigmas. A related path is to investi- Green-Feminine Stereotype and Its Effect on Sustainable Con-
gate what makes a particular piece of information a “stigma sumption,” Journal of Consumer Research, 43 (4), 567–82.
marker” for different audiences (e.g., co-consumers, service Campbell, Margaret C. (1995), “When Attention-Getting Advertising
providers). This notion relates back to managerial decisions Tactics Elicit Consumer Inferences of Manipulative Intent: The
about product design and branding (e.g., might “unbranding” Importance of Balancing Benefits and Investments,” Journal of
a product help reduce a product–stigma association?). Fourth, Consumer Psychology, 4 (3), 225–54.
because stigma inherently entails power, studying how consu- Carr, Deborah and Michael A. Friedman (2005) “Is Obesity Stigma-
mers estimate power differentials in new encounters and how it tizing? Body Weight, Perceived Discrimination, and Psychological
shapes their decision to pursue the encounter is important.10 Well-Being in the United States,” Journal of Health and Social
Behavior 46 (3), 244–59.
Fifth, our studies investigate various forms of ambiguity (e.g.,
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2019), “About Adult
along the focal attribute, unspecified similarity) in which a
BMI,” (Accessed August 16, 2019), https://www.cdc.gov/healthy
single cue could produce wide variations in inferences. The
weight/assessing/bmi/adult_bmi/index.html#InterpretedAdults.
degree and nature of ambiguity in audience cues and its impact
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2020), “Reducing
on consumption decision making warrants further investiga-
Stigma,” (Accessed June 12, 2020), https://www.cdc.gov/corona
tion. Sixth, we study consumers who bear a stigma, but others
virus/2019-ncov/daily-life-coping/reducing-stigma.html.
(e.g., their caregivers, family, friends) could be affected by
Chaney, Kimberly E., Diana T. Sanchez, and Melanie R. Maimon
mere association (Argo and Main 2008). These close others
(2019), “Stigmatized-Identity Cues in Consumer Spaces,” Journal
may be more acutely aware of discrediting conditions and more
of Consumer Psychology, 29 (1), 130–41.
sensitive in their responses on behalf of the stigmatized. More
Chang, Su-Hsin, Carolyn R. Stoll, Jihyun Song, J.E. Varela,
research is needed to understand such stigma-by-association.
Christopher J. Eagon, and Graham A. Colditz (2014), “The Effec-
Finally, we narrowed our theory to a single attribute (disease,
tiveness and Risks of Bariatric Surgery: An Updated Systematic
weight), but people may simultaneously possess multiple
Review and Meta-Analysis, 2003–2012,” JAMA Surgery, 149 (3),
potentially stigmatizing attributes, and their interplay indicates
275–87.
a fruitful area for further research. In this context, beyond Conrad, Henry Paul and Marylouise Caldwell (2006), “Self-
social devaluation by an audience, self-devaluation by stigma- Empowerment and Consumption: Consumer Remedies for Pro-
tized consumers also may be relevant. longed Stigmatization,” European Journal of Marketing, 40 (9/
10), 1031–48.
10
For example, Wooten (2006) identifies interesting dimensions (e.g., Corrigan, Patrick W., Kristin A. Kosyluk, and Nicolas Rüsch (2013),
duration, magnitude, type) on which power differentials might vary between “Reducing Self-Stigma by Coming Out Proud,” American Journal
consumers with the stigmatizing attribute and those without it. of Public Health, 103 (5), 794–800.
244 Journal of Marketing Research 58(2)

Coskuner-Balli, Gokcen and Craig J. Thompson (2013), “The Status Harvey, Emma L. and A. J. Hill (2001) “Health Professionals’ Views
Costs of Subordinate Cultural Capital: At-Home Fathers’ Collec- of Overweight People and Smokers,” International Journal of
tive Pursuit of Cultural Legitimacy through Capitalizing Consump- Obesity, 25 (8), 1253–61.
tion Practices,” Journal of Consumer Research, 40 (1), 19–41. Hayes, Andrew F. (2015), Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and
Crocker, Jennifer and Brenda Major (1989), “Social Stigma and Self- Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach.
Esteem: The Self-Protective Properties of Stigma,” Psychological New York: Guilford Press.
Review, 96 (4), 608–30. Hayes, Andrew F. and Amanda K. Montoya (2017), “A Tutorial on
Crocker, Jennifer, Brenda Major, and Claude M. Steele (1998), Testing, Visualizing, and Probing an Interaction Involving a Multi-
“Social Stigma,” in Handbook of Social Psychology, Daniel T. categorical Variable in Linear Regression Analysis,” Communica-
Gilbert, Susan T. Fisk, and Lindzey Gardner, eds. Boston: McGraw tion Methods and Measures, 11 (1), 1–30.
Hill, 504–53. Hebl, Michelle R., Jennifer Tickle, and Todd F. Heatherton (2000),
Crockett, David (2017), “Paths to Respectability: Consumption and “Awkward Moments in Interactions Between Non-Stigmatized
Stigma Management in the Contemporary Black Middle Class,” and Stigmatized Individuals,” in The Social Psychology of Stigma,
Journal of Consumer Research, 44 (3), 554–81. Todd F. Heatherton, Michelle R. Hebl, and Jay G. Hull, eds.
DiMatteo, M. Robin (2004), “Social Support and Patient Adherence to New York: Guilford Press, 275–306.
Medical Treatment: A Meta-Analysis,” Health Psychology, 23 (2), Hill, Ronald Paul and Mark Stamey (1990), “The Homeless in Amer-
207–18. ica: An Examination of Possessions and Consumption Behaviors,”
Dion, Delphine and Stéphane Borraz (2017), “Managing Status: How Journal of Consumer Research, 17 (3), 303–21.
Luxury Brands Shape Class Subjectivities in the Service Hoch, Stephen J. and John Deighton (1989), “Managing What Con-
Encounter,” Journal of Marketing, 81 (5), 67–85. sumers Learn from Experience,” Journal of Marketing, 53 (2),
Dovidio, John F., Brenda Major, and Jennifer Crocker (2000),
1–20.
“Stigma: Introduction and Overview,” in The Social Psychology
Hoch, Stephen J. and Young-Won Ha (1986), “Consumer Learning:
of Stigma, Todd F. Heatherton, Robert E. Kleck, Michelle R. Hebl,
Advertising and the Ambiguity of Product Experience,” Journal of
and Jay G. Hull eds. New York: Guilford Press, 1–28.
Consumer Research, 13 (2), 221–33.
Einhorn, Hillel J. and Robin M. Hogarth (1985), “Ambiguity and
Humphreys, Ashlee and Kathryn A. Latour (2013), “Framing the
Uncertainty in Probabilistic Inference,” Psychological Review,
Game: Assessing the Impact of Cultural Representations on Con-
92 (4), 3–73.
sumer Perceptions of Legitimacy,” Journal of Consumer Research,
Fairchild, Amy L., Ronald Bayer, and James Colgrove (2015), “Risky
40 (4), 773–95.
Business: New York City’s Experience with Fear-Based Public
Kates, Steven M (2002), “The Protean Quality of Subcultural Con-
Health Campaigns,” Health Affairs, 34 (5), 844–51.
sumption: An Ethnographic Account of Gay Consumers,” Journal
Frable, Deborrah E.S., Linda Platt, and Steve Hoey (1998),
of Consumer Research, 29 (3), 383–99.
“Concealable Stigmas and Positive Self-Perceptions: Feeling Bet-
King, Eden B., Jenessa R. Shapiro, Michelle R. Hebl, Sarah L.
ter around Similar Others,” Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Singletary, and Stacey Turner (2006), “The Stigma of Obesity in
chology, 74 (4), 909–22.
Friestad, Marian and Peter Wright (1994), “The Persuasion Knowl- Customer Service: A Mechanism for Remediation and Bottom-
edge Model: How People Cope with Persuasion Attempts,” Jour- Line Consequences of Interpersonal Discrimination,” Journal of
nal of Consumer Research 21 (1), 1–31. Applied Psychology, 91 (3), 579–93.
Frost, Jeana H. and Michael P. Massagli (2008), “Social Uses of Kozinets, Robert V. (2001), “Utopian Enterprise: Articulating the
Personal Health Information Within PatientsLikeMe, an Online Meanings of Star Trek’s Culture of Consumption,” Journal of
Patient Community: What Can Happen When Patients Have Consumer Research, 28 (1), 67–88.
Access to One Another’s Data,” Journal of Medical Internet Lamberton, Cait (2019), “Toward a Dignity Architecture: The Critical
Research, 10 (3), e15. Challenges of Stigmatized-Identity Cues for Consumer Psy-
Gibbs, Nancy (2010), “The Pill at 50: Sex, Freedom, and Paradox,” chology,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 1 (29), 152–59.
Time Magazine (April), available at http://content.time.com/time/ Lewis, Robin J., Thomas F. Cash, and Cristina Bubb-Lewis (1997),
magazine/article/0,9171,1983884,00.html. “Prejudice Toward Fat People: The Development and Validation
Goff, Phillip Atiba, Matthew Christian Jackson, Di Leone, Brooke of the Antifat Attitudes Test,” Obesity Research, 5 (4), 297–307.
Allison Lewis, Carmen Marie Culotta, and Natalie Ann Lin, Ken-Hou and Lundquist, Jennifer (2013), “Mate Selection in
DiTomasso (2014), “The Essence of Innocence: Consequences Cyberspace: The Intersection of Race, Gender, and Education,”
of Dehumanizing Black Children,” Journal of Personality and American Journal of Sociology, 119 (1), 183–215.
Social Psychology, 106 (4), 526–45. Lin, Lily and Brent McFerran (2016), “The (Ironic) Dove Effect: Use
Goffman, Erving (1963), Stigma: Notes on the Management of a of Acceptance Cues for Larger Body Types Increases Unhealthy
Spoiled Identity. New York: Simon and Schuster Inc. Behaviors,” Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 35 (1), 76–90.
Harmeling, Colleen M., Jordan W. Moffett, Mark J. Arnold, and Brad Major, Brenda and Laurie T. O’Brien (2005), “The Social Psychology
D. Carlson (2017), “Toward a Theory of Customer Engagement of Stigma,” Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 393–421.
Marketing,” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 45 (3), McFerran, Brent, Dahl, Darren W., Fitzsimons, Gavan J., and
312–35. Morales, Andrea C. (2009), “‘I’ll Have What She’s Having’:
Harmeling et al. 245

Effects of Social Influence and Body Type on the Food Choices of Sandikci, Özlem and Güliz Ger (2009), “Veiling in Style: How Does a
Others,” Journal of Consumer Research, 36 (6), 915–29. Stigmatized Practice Become Fashionable?” Journal of Consumer
McLaughlin, Deirdre P., Nancy A. Pachana, and Ken Mcfarland Research, 37 (1), 15–36.
(2008), “Stigma, Seizure Frequency, and Quality of Life: The Schouten, John W. (1991), “Selves in Transition: Symbolic Consump-
Impact of Epilepsy in Late Adulthood,” Seizure, 17 (3), 281–87. tion in Personal Rites of Passage and Identity Reconstruction,”
McPherson, Miller, Lynn Smith-Lovin, and James M. Cook (2001), Journal of Consumer Research, 17 (4), 412–25.
“Birds of a Feather: Homophily in Social Networks,” Annual Shavitt, Sharon (2019), “Diversity and Stigmatized Identity in the
Review of Sociology, 27 (1), 415–44. Marketplace: Introduction to Research Dialogue,” Journal of Con-
Meadows, Angela and Suzanne Higgs (2019), “The Multifaceted sumer Psychology, 29 (1), 128–29.
Nature of Weight-Related Self-Stigma: Validation of the Two- Shih, Margaret (2004), “Positive Stigma: Examining Resilience and
Factor Weight Bias Internalization Scale (WBIS-2F),” Frontiers Empowerment in Overcoming Stigma,” The ANNALS of the Amer-
in Psychology, 10 (808), 1–10. ican Academy of Political and Social Science, 591 (1), 175–85.
Mick, David Glen, Simone Pettigrew, Cornelia Connie Pechmann, and Shrauger, J. Sidney and Thomas J. Schoeneman (1979), “Symbolic
Julie L. Ozanne, eds. (2012). Transformative Consumer Research Interactionist View of Self-Concept: Through the Looking Glass
for Personal and Collective Well-Being. New York: Routledge. Darkly,” Psychological Bulletin, 86 (3), 549–77.
Mirabito, Ann M., Cele C. Otnes, Elizabeth Crosby, David B. Wooten, Tepper, Kelly (1994), “The Role of Labeling Processes in Elderly
Jane E. Machin, Chris Pullig, Natalie Ross Adkins, et al. (2016), Consumers’ Responses to Age Segmentation Cues,” Journal of
“The Stigma Turbine: A Theoretical Framework for Conceptualiz- Consumer Research, 20 (4), 503–19.
ing and Contextualizing Marketplace Stigma,” Journal of Public Tversky, Amos and Daniel Kahneman (1981), “The Framing of
Policy & Marketing, 35 (2), 170–84. Decisions and the Psychology of Choice,” Science, 211 (4481),
Muniz, Albert M. and Hope Jensen Schau (2005), “Religiosity in the 453–8.
Abandoned Apple Newton Brand Community,” Journal of Con- Wahl, Norman (2005), “Orthodontics in 3 Millennia. Antiquity to the
sumer Research, 31 (4), 737–47. Mid-19th Century,” American Journal of Orthodontics and Den-
Naylor, Rebecca Walker, Cait Poynor Lamberton, and David A. tofacial Orthopedics, 127 (2), 255–59.
Norton (2011), “Seeing Ourselves in Others: Reviewer Ambiguity, Wang, Xi, Zhiya Zuo, and Kang Zhao (2015), “The Evolution of User
Egocentric Anchoring, and Persuasion,” Journal of Marketing Roles in Online Health Communities–A Social Support
Research, 48 (3), 617–31. Perspective,” in PACIS 2015 Proceedings. Paper 121.
Norton, David A., Cait Poynor Lamberton, and Rebecca Walker Wooten, David B. (2006), “From Labeling Possessions to Possessing
Naylor (2013), “The Devil You (Don’t) Know: Interpersonal Labels: Ridicule and Socialization among Adolescents,” Journal
Ambiguity and Inference Making in Competitive Contexts,” Jour- of Consumer Research, 33 (2), 188–98.
nal of Consumer Research, 40 (2), 239–54. Wooten, David B. and Tracy Rank-Christman (2019), “Stigmatized-
Puhl, Rebecca M. and Kelly D. Brownell (2006), “Confronting and Identity Cues: Threats as Opportunities for Consumer Psy-
Coping with Weight Stigma: An Investigation of Overweight and chology,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 29 (1), 142–51.
Obese Adults,” Obesity 14 (10), 1802–15. Young, Colleen (2013), “Community Management that Works: How
Ratner, Rebecca K. and Rebecca W. Hamilton (2015), “Inhibited from to Build and Sustain a Thriving Online Health Community,”
Bowling Alone,” Journal of Consumer Research, 42 (2), 266–83. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 15 (6), e119.
Copyright of Journal of Marketing Research (JMR) is the property of American Marketing
Association and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a
listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print,
download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like